Men Finally Got It! Rhotic Assibilation in Mexican Spanish in Chihuahua - MDPI
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
languages Article Men Finally Got It! Rhotic Assibilation in Mexican Spanish in Chihuahua Natalia Mazzaro * and Raquel González de Anda Department of Languages and Linguistics, University of Texas at El Paso, El Paso, TX 79968, USA; raquelg@utep.edu * Correspondence: nmazzaro@utep.edu Received: 17 August 2020; Accepted: 3 October 2020; Published: 14 October 2020 Abstract: Rhotic assibilation is a common sociolinguistic variable observed in different Spanish speaking countries such as Argentina, Ecuador, and México. Previous studies reported that rhotic assibilation alternates with the flap and/or with the trill. In this study, we explore three aspects of rhotic assibilation in the Spanish of the state of Chihuahua, Mexico/El Paso, TX, United States: (1) Its diachronic development; (2) the linguistic and social factors that affect this variation and; (3) the possible effect of contact with English in this variable. Fifty-eight participants, including Spanish monolingual and Spanish-English bilingual subjects, performed one formal and two semi-informal speech production tasks. Acoustic and perceptual analysis of the tokens showed that the variation is not binary (standard vs. non-standard variant), but that it includes other rhotic variants with varying degrees of frication. Variation is restricted to phrase-final position and heavily favored by preceding front vowels (/e/ and /i/). These effects have a clear aerodynamic and articulatory motivation. Rhotic assibilation is not receding, as previously reported. It continues to be a prestigious variable prevalent amongst females, but also present in male speakers. The comparison between bilingual and monolingual speakers shows that contact with English does not significantly affect the occurrence of assibilation. Keywords: rhotic assibilation; sociolinguistics; sociophonetics; Mexican Spanish; variation 1. Introduction Rhotic assibilation is a pervasive feature of most varieties of Spanish. It has been reported in Argentina (Colantoni 2006); Bolivia (Morgan and Sessarego 2016); Costa Rica (Vásquez Carranza 2006); Ecuador (Bradley 2004); Spain (Henriksen and Willis 2010); Dominican Republic (Willis 2007); and Mexico (Amastae et al. 1998; Bradley and Willis 2012; Eller 2013; Lope Blanch 1967; Perissinotto 1972; Rissel 1989). Although assibilation is a common feature in all these dialects, social and linguistic factors seem to influence the phonological feature in unique ways. In this study, we focus on rhotic assibilation in the Spanish of the state of Chihuahua, Mexico/El Paso, TX, United States. We explore three aspects of rhotic assibilation: (i) The development of this feature more than two decades after the first (and only) study was conducted in this same dialect (Amastae et al. 1998); (ii) the linguistic and social factors that affect this variation; and (iii) whether contact with English affects the variable in bilinguals. The rest of the paper is organized as follows: Section 1 reviews the phonetic and phonological characteristics of rhotics, the sociolinguistic literature on rhotic assibilation in Mexico City and Ciudad Juárez, Chihuahua. Section 2 details the methodology for the study. Section 3 introduces the results, and Section 4 answers the research questions and relates the findings of the present study with the literature reviewed. Finally, we conclude and discuss the future directions of this research. Languages 2020, 5, 38; doi:10.3390/languages5040038 www.mdpi.com/journal/languages
Languages 2020, 5, Languages x FOR Languages PEER 2020, 5, 2020,REVIEW 38 5, x FOR PEER REVIEW 2 of 21 2 of 21 1.1. Phonological, 1.1. Articulatory, Phonological, and Articulatory, Acoustic Characteristics and Acoustic Characteristics of Spanish Rhotics of Spanish Rhotics Languages 2020, 5, x1.1. FOR Phonological, PEER REVIEW Articulatory, and Acoustic Characteristics of Spanish Rhotics 2 of 21 REVIEW With regard to With rhotic regard distribution, to rhotic there distribution, is a clear there distinction is a clear 2 between of distinction 21 the syllabic between positions the syllabic With regard to rhotic distribution, there is a clear distinction between the syllabic positions that thatposition can be occupied 1.1. Phonological, can can Articulatory, be by occupiedbe tapsoccupied and by and taps trills. Acousticby trills. and taps Taps and/ /ɾtrills. Characteristics Taps /,/,asas inTaps in ofcaro caro /ká/ ɾo/ Spanish /káɾo/ /,‘expensive’, asRhotics in‘expensive’, caroand /káɾo/trillsand /r̄/, astrills ‘expensive’, in carro /r̄/,/kár̄o/ and as intrills carro /r̄/, as in ory, and /kár̄Acoustic o/ ‘car’, Characteristics ‘car’, contrast contrast /kár̄ o/ ‘car’, in of SpanishinRhotics in word-internal word-internal contrast intervocalic word-internal intervocalic Languages position position (Hualde intervocalic 2020, 5, 2005). (Hualde x FOR position In PEER word-initial 2005). REVIEW (Hualde position In word-initial 2005). and after Inposition word-initial and position x FORWith PEER REVIEW regard a to rhoticindistribution, consonant a different thereonly syllable, is a the clear trill distinction occurs: Rosa between /r̄ósa/ ‘rose’theand syllabic positions of 21 /isr̄aél/. 2Israel The that tap after a consonant distribution, there after ina consonant a different in syllable, a different only syllable, the trill occurs: only theRosa trillthat /r̄ occurs: ósa/ ‘rose’ Rosaand /r̄ósa/ Israel‘rose’ /isr̄andaél/.Israel The /isr̄aél/ can be occupied by istaps occurs ina onset clear distinction andclusters: trills. Taps Prosabetween / ɾósa/ /p /, as‘prose’the in syllabic caro and/káɾo/ 1.1. positions Phonological, in ‘expensive’, word-final Articulatory, position and before trills aand /r̄Acoustic vowel: /, asSerin carro amigosCharacteristics of Sp tap occurs/ ɾin/,tap onset occurs clusters:in onset Prosa clusters: /pɾósa/Prosa ‘prose’ /pɾósa/ and in ,‘prose’ asword-final and inposition word-final before positiona vowel: before Ser a vowe and /kár̄ otrills. / ‘car’,Taps l, Articulatory, /sé amígos/ and contrast asword-internal Acoustic in in‘to caro /káɾo/ Characteristics be friends’. ‘expensive’, of Spanish A variable intervocalic rhotic andRhotics position trills appears /r̄/coda in (Hualde in carro position 2005). In(within word-initial a word orposition across word and rd-internal amigos /séɾ amíɡos/ amigos boundaries) intervocalic ‘to /séɾ followed position beamíɡos/ friends’. by a consonant (Hualde ‘to A bevariable 2005). friends’. orIn atrill rhotic pause, Ai.e., word-initial variable appears With when rhotic in coda regard resyllabification position appears to position and rhotic isandindistribution, not coda (within possible. positiona word While therea(within orisacross tap a word a clear or a distinc after a rd to rhotic consonant in a different syllable, only the occurs: Rosa /r̄ ó sa/ ‘rose’ Israel /isr̄ a él/. The erent word distribution, syllable, boundaries) is more only word the there followed frequently trill boundaries) occurs: isfound a clear by Rosaina distinction followed consonant these /r̄ósa/ contexts,by between ‘rose’ or aan consonant and aemphatic can bethe pause, Israel syllabic i.e., occupiedoracan trill /isr̄ aél/. whenpause, by also positions The resyllabification taps occur i.e.,1and e.g.,that when arte resyllabification trills. Taps a[r̄]te isornot / ɾ]te á[ possible. /, as iniscaro not /káɾ pos tap occurs in onset clusters: Prosa ɾ] /,oras /pɾósa/ ‘prose’ and in word-final position before a vowel: Ser d by taps ers: While Prosa and a tap /pɾósa/ trills. ‘art’, amor is WhileTaps more ‘prose’ amo[ /frequently aandtap in amo[r̄] is in caro more word-final ‘love’. found /káɾo/ frequentlyFollowing in ‘expensive’, these position Hualde found contexts, /kár̄ before oin and /(2005), these ‘car’, an acoda wetrillswill/r̄ emphatic contexts, contrast vowel: use/, the asan in symbol trill Serin word-internal can/r/also carro emphatic to represent trill occur can intervocalic 1 the alsoarte e.g., occur1 e.g. position (H amigos ntrast in /séɾ amíɡos/ ‘to non-distinct be friends’. rhotic found A variable in final rhotic position appears where there in is no possible position (within resyllabification. a word We will or across also use friends’. a[r̄word-internal ]Atevariable or á[ɾ]tea[r̄ ]intervocalic ‘art’, rhotic te or amor á[ɾ]te appears amo[ɾ] position ‘art’, inrather codaamor (Hualde or position amo[r̄ amo[ɾ] ] ‘love’. 2005). orafter (within amo[r̄ In Following word-initial ] ‘love’. aa refer consonant word or Hualde inposition Following across (2005), a different and Hualdewesyllable, will (2005), useonly thewethe symbol will useoccurs: trill the sy word boundaries) nt in a different the followed neutral syllable, term only by ‘rhotic’a consonant the trillthe occurs: than or ‘tap’ a Rosafound pause, and ‘trill’ /r̄ótap sa/ i.e., to ‘rose’ when andto it. resyllabification Israel is not possible. ed by /r/ to represent aa consonant /r/ atothe orfrequently represent pause, non-distinct i.e., when non-distinct rhotic resyllabification 2 share rhotic in occurs is final found not position inpossible. onset in /isr̄ final aél/. where clusters: The position there where Prosa is no there /pɾósa/ possible ‘prose’ isand no inpos w While tap is more Articulatorily, standard found taps in and trills these contexts, the an same emphatic place and trillmannercan of articulation; also occur 1 e.g.,they arte are onset clusters:resyllabification. Prosa /pɾósa/ resyllabification. Werealizedwill ‘prose’ also andthe use We will inneutral word-final also position before a‘to vowel: Ser uently a[r̄ ] te or found á[ɾ]te both in‘art’, these usually amor contexts, amo[ɾ] anas or voiced emphatic amo[r̄ and ] trill use alveolar ‘love’. can term 3 . the Following amigos The also ‘rhotic’ neutral mainoccur /séɾ 1rather term amíɡos/ difference Hualde ‘rhotic’ e.g.,(2005), than between arte ‘tap’ be we rather taps and friends’. will and than ‘trill’ use A‘tap’ trills theistothat variablerefer and symbol ‘trill’ to it. to tapsrhotic refer toi appears íɡos/ ‘to be Articulatorily, friends’. A variable with rhotic Articulatorily, standard tapsappears standard andbetween in coda trills taps 2 share position and trills the (within same 2 share athe word or across amo[ɾ] /r/ to or amo[r̄ represent are] produced ‘love’. the Following non-distinct a single contact Hualde rhotic (2005), found the we in word tip will final of the use the place boundaries) tongue position towards symbol where andsame followed the manner place alveolar there is aof by ridge,and no articulation; manner consonant while possible trillsor of athey articulation; pause, i.e., ies) followed are both byareaproduced usually consonant are both realized with or usually as avoiced several pause, realized (usually andi.e., as two when alveolar voiced or three) resyllabification 3and . The such Whileisa tap alveolar main rapid difference 3. The contacts is is not main (Hualde possible. betweendifference 2005). more frequently found in these contexts, taps Navarro between and trills Tomás taps is that and trills anis n-distinct resyllabification. rhotic We found will in final also use theposition neutral where term there ‘rhotic’ rather no than possible ‘tap’ and ‘trill’ to refer to it. more taps frequently (Tomásfound 1970, pp. inwith these 1, 116) gave contexts, a more an detailed emphatic description trill can of the also Spanish occur trill tip e.g., 1 that specifies arte the position also use theare Articulatorily, produced neutral of the taps term standard tongue are produced ‘rhotic’ tip, taps a single dorsum,rather and with and contact than trillsroot. a single 2 ‘tap’ share He betweenand stated contact the a[r̄ ]the ‘trill’ same that te or the tobetween tip of á[ɾ]te refer place tip of the the to and the ‘art’,tongue it. tongue amor manner oftowards bends the amo[ɾ] of tongue ortheamo[r̄ articulation; upwards to alveolar towards touch ] ‘love’. they the ridge, the Following alveolar r ‘art’, amor while amo[ɾ] are or amo[r̄ ] ‘love’. Following Hualde (2005), we willtwo use orthe symbol ard are taps both andtrills usually trills 2 while share realized upper-most produced trills thevoiced as part are same of with produced the alveolarplace and several and with alveolar ridge, (usually manner the3. The several tongue /r/oftwo main to (usually or articulation; root three) represent difference retracts towards such theythe between rapid three) non-distinct the back taps contacts such rapid of the and oraltrills (Hualde rhotic cavity, is contacts found that and 2005). (Hualde in final 2 nt theNavarro non-distinct Tomás rhotic Navarro 3([1918] found Tomás 1970,difference in final position where there isWeno possible as voiced taps are and alveolar produced the tongue with . The body a main singleadopts a([1918] contact pp. hollow 1, 116) 1970, or between betweengave concave pp.shape. the a1, taps tip more 116) resyllabification. of and gave detailed the trills a more tongue isdescription that detailed will towards alsoofdescription the use thethe alveolar Spanish neutral of trill ridge, the term Spanish that ‘rhotic’tril ra n. We specifies will also the useposition the specifies neutral The realization of the term the position ‘rhotic’ tongue of tip, rather the dorsum, tongue than and‘tap’ tip, and dorsum, root. Articulatorily,He‘trill’and stated to refer root. that standard He tothe it. stated tip taps of that the the trills share the sameb tongue andtongue-tip tip2 of bends the tongue singletrills while contact are between produced the with tipofseveral oftrillstherequires tongue (usually a complex towardstwo combination or the three) alveolar of articulatory such ridge, rapid movements contacts of the (Hualde 2005). 3 rily, standard upwards taps to touch and and the upwards aerodynamic trills 2 share upper-most to touch forces the (Soléthe same part upper-most 2002). of Due place the to their and alveolar part are manner ofridge, both articulatory theusually of thearticulation; alveolar complexity, tongue ridge, realized trills root asthey the are retracts tongueand voiced mastered towards root late retracts inalveolar the L1 the back.towards The main thed with Navarro several Tomás (usually ([1918] two 1970, or pp.three) 1, such 116) gave rapid a morecontacts detailed (Hualde description 2005). of the Spanish trill that y realized of the asoralvoiced ofand acquisition cavity, the andalveolar andoral are the not cavity, 3 .present tongue The and main inthe body difference thetongue babblinga adopts stage. body between hollow taps Not are only adopts or taps ado concave produced andwith native hollow trills speakers shape. aissingle or concave that find trills shape. contactchallenging,between the tip of 970, specifies pp. 1, the 116) butgave position L2 a more of learners the maydetailed tongue also tip, find description dorsum, them and difficult ofto the root.acquire Spanish He stated and trill some that that may thenever tip of succeedthe tongue in rolling bends their ced with aThe single contact realization The ofbetween realization trills the of requires tiptrills a of the complex requires tongue combination whilea towards complex trills of the combination arebends alveolar articulatory ofridge, movements produced with several (usuallyarticulatory of movements the tongue- two of orthethre ton he tongue upwards tip, to dorsum, touch [r]s the(Solé and upper-most root. 2002). Because He part stated theofproduction the that the alveolar tip mechanism of ridge, the thetongue tongue of trills is quite root retracts complex andtowards requiresthe back precise produced tippart and with several aerodynamic tip andand (usually aerodynamic forces two or (Solé 2002). forcesthree) Due(Solésuch to2002). rapid their Navarro contacts articulatory Due to the Tomás their (Hualde complexity, articulatory ([1918] 1970,2005). trills complexity, pp. 1,are 116) mastered trillsaare gave late more mastered detai er-most of the oral of cavity, theand alveolar articulatory the ridge, tongue aerodynamic the body tongue conditions, adopts root a retracts hollowsmall or towards changes concave in their shape. back production can lead to perceptible s ([1918] in body 1970, the L1adopts pp. acoustic 1, acquisition in athe 116) differences. gave L1and acquisition are a These more notsmall detailed present and areinnot changes description the in present thebabbling specifies production of in stage. the the theofbabbling Spanish Notwhich position trills, only trill stage. of do native the could that Not tongue be due only speakers tip, dodorsum, native findspeakers trills and root. find He tongue osition The of realization the tongue oftip, hollow trills dorsum, or requires concave and a complex root. shape. He combination stated that the of tiparticulatory of the tongue movements bends ofto the contextual tongue- ls requires challenging, a complex but challenging, or prosodic L2features, combination learners but createmay of L2 learners also find a favorable articulatory maythem environment movements also difficult upwards find for the ofthemto to acquire touch instability the difficult tongue- theofand tosound some upper-most this acquire may and and thenever part some of the creation succeed may ofalveolarnever in ridge,succe th uchtip andupper-most the aerodynamic new part forces variants. of the In (Soléalveolar fact, 2002). the rich Due ridge,literature to their the tongue on articulatory rhotic root variationretracts complexity, in towards different trills the dialects back are of mastered Spanish provides late es the (Solé rolling 2002). their Due [r]s rolling toand (Solé their their 2002). [r]sBecause articulatory (Solécomplexity, 2002). the production Because ofthe trills the mechanism are production oral cavity, mastered ofmechanism trillsthe and is quite tongue ofcomplex trills body is quiteand complex adopts requires a hollow andorreq co in L1the ty, andprecise acquisition tongue evidence body for are the adopts not present instability a hollow of in trills or the and, babbling concave most stage. importantly, shape. Notthe onlylate methodologicaldo native speakers challenges they find presenttrills are not present articulatory inL2 the precise babbling and articulatory aerodynamic stage. and aerodynamic conditions, small conditions, The changes realization small in their of trills changes production requires in their a complex canproduction lead to can lea combination o challenging, ation of trills but in requires their learners analysis may a complex (see alsoNot Method combination find only section). themof dodifficult native speakers articulatory to movements acquire find and trills ofsomethe may never succeed in tongue- ers may perceptible also [r]s find(Solé acoustic themperceptible differences. difficult acoustic These differences. small changes Thesetip and smallinaerodynamic the changes production in forces theofproduction trills, (Soléwhich 2002). ofcould trills, Due tobe which duecould their articula be rolling their Acoustically, 2002). theto Because tap acquire and the the and trill production some share the may never same characteristics: mechanism ofsucceed trills in Aislowered quite complex third formant and(Colantoni requires namictoforces contextual (Solé 2001)to 2002). or contextual prosodic Due to their features, orofprosodic articulatory create complexity, trills are mastered late 2). Because precise the production articulatory and and brief periods mechanism aerodynamic trillsfeatures, occlusion, ofconditions, isaquite one favorable occlusion create in the complex small for environment a favorable L1 the changes acquisition tap and and inmore requires environment forthan their the one and instability arefor production not for the the presentof this trill. instability canHowever, insound lead to of the babblingand this soundstage sition and are the creation not present taps of the andnew in creation the variants. differ of babblingnew Induration fact, stage. variants. Not only do native speakers find trills aerodynamic perceptible conditions, acoustic trills differences. small in These the changes small in the of the their changes rich Inproduction fact, literature segment, in the the challenging, with richon tapsliterature can production rhotic but being lead L2shorter oftovariation on than learners trills, rhotic may which in different trills.variation also Quilis find could be due dialects (1993)in them differentof dialec difficult to a ut L2 learners Spanish may provides also Spanish find evidencethem provides difficult for evidence to the instability acquire and some may never succeed in offor the of instability trillstheir and,[r]s ofmost trills importantly, and, 2002).most importantly, the the methodological the methodolo reported an average of 20 ms. for taps and 60 ms. rolling for trills. The length (Solé of the trill Because is affected by the production mechanis rences. These or small changes in the create production trills, which could be due sto(Solé contextual 2002). challenges prosodic following Because they the challenges present features, vowel: production It in theyis shorter their a favorable before mechanism presentanalysis in [i] andof environment longer trills is before quite [a] for (Solé complex the instability 2002).and requires of this sound and eatures, the creation create of anewfavorable variants. Assibilated environment and Instandard fact,small the for thetheir rich rhotics (see Methodanalysis 4 instability literature tend toin precise alternate ofsection). on (see this rhotic Method articulatory sound synchronically variationand section). and aerodynamic conditions, small ch andin different diachronically dialects (Solé 1992). of atory and Acoustically, aerodynamic conditions, Acoustically, the taponand the the variation tap changes trill and share the the their trill perceptiblesame production share characteristics: the same acoustic can lead characteristics: differences. A loweredto These third A lowered small formant changes thirdin theforp ants. Spanish In fact, the provides These rich Synchronically,literature evidence forchanges apical rhotic the instability trills exhibit in of trills and, non-trilled different variants, dialects mostwhich taps, importantly, approximants, of and the methodological fricatives. Solé (2002) ustic differences. small in the production of trills, could be due ce for(Colantoni the instability 2001) theyperformed (Colantoni ofand atrillsbrief study 2001) and, to periods and brief most replicate of theocclusion, periodsto importantly, phonological of one occlusion, occlusion contextual the methodological variation or between onefor occlusion the prosodicvoiced taptrills and for features, andmore the tapthan create fricatives. and aone Her morefor the favorable than one fo environm rchallenges prosodic features, present create in a their favorable analysis (see environment Method for section). the instability of this sound and their trill. analysis However, results (see trill. taps showed Method However, and thattrills section). taps trills differ may and intrills become the duration differthe fricatives in (orof the the creationduration segment, assibilated) of new ofthe if the with segment, taps variants. finely being controlledInwith shorter fact, taps the being articulatory than literature rich trills. shorter than on newAcoustically, variants. In the fact, tap the and rich the literature trill share on the same rhotic variation characteristics: in different Adialects lowered of third formant and Quilis (1993) Quilis reported the (1993) an average reportedfor ofan 20average ms. forSpanish oftaps 20suggesting and ms. for 60formant provides ms. taps forand evidence trills. 60 involve ms. The for for length the trills. ofThe instability thelength trill is of and, of trills the t or aerodynamic requirements trills are not met, that fricatives a less complex p(Colantoni the trill 2001) share and brief same periods characteristics: of occlusion, A onelowered occlusion thirdfor the the tap and more than one for the des evidence affected for the articulation by affected instability following and allow by the ofa vowel: trills wider following It theand, range is shorter ofmost vowel: importantly, oropharyngeal before It challenges is shorter[i] pressure andone before longer they methodological variation [i]before present and inthan longer [a] trills. their(Solé Thus, before 2002). analysis assibilated [a](see(Solé 2002). section Method f periods of occlusion, andone occlusion infor tap and ofmore than for the ytrill. However, present taps inAssibilated their analysis and trills (see Assibilated differ Method standard and the rhotics duration section). standard 4 tendbeing the segment, rhotics to alternate 4 tendthan Acoustically, with synchronically to alternate taps the tap being shorter than trills. synchronically and the diachronically trill and sharediachronically (Solésame ch the ills differ Quilis (1993) in the duration reported anofshare the average segment, ofsame20 withms. taps for taps andshorter 60 ms. for trills. trills. The length of the trill is lly, the 1992).tap and the Synchronically, 1992). trill Synchronically, apical the trills exhibit apicalcharacteristics: non-trilled trills exhibit (Colantoni A variants,lowered non-trilled taps, third variants, formant approximants, taps, approximants, and fricatives.and Solé fricatives average affected by ofthe 20 1following ms. Thefor taps distribution vowel: and of trills 60 It isms. and shorter taps is forfurthertrills. before discussedThe[i]inand length Hualde longer of 2001) (2005). thebefore trilland is brief [a] (Solé periods 2002). of occlusion, one occlusio 1) and(2002) brief periods performed of (2002) occlusion, a isstudy performed one occlusion for the tap and more than one for the andto replicate a study thetoof[a] phonological replicate trill. the phonological variation However, when wetaps between variation and voiced trills between differ trills wein and thevoiced fricatives. duration trillsofand thefrica segm 2 Since there variation affecting the system rhotics (Hualde 2005), refer to ‘standard’ variants mean prestige vowel: It is shorter before [i] longer 4 before (Solé 2002). taps Assibilated and Her trills results 4 3tend and differforms standard Her in showed used in careful rhotics the duration results andthat speech, prescribed showed trills of themay tend byto segment, thatbecome schools trills alternate and used by with may taps fricatives Quilis synchronically become newscasters. being (1993) shorter (orreported fricatives assibilated) and thanan diachronically trills. (oraverage ifassibilated) the finely of 20 ms. (Solé ifofcontrolled thefor finely taps and contr60 ndard rhotics 1992). Synchronically, Lipski to(1994)alternate apical Hualde trills synchronically (2005) discussed exhibit non-trilled other and diachronically (non-standard) variants, realizations taps, (Solédorsalization including approximants, and and pre-aspiration fricatives. the Solé eported an articulatory average or trill, of articulatory20 aerodynamic neutralization,ms. for taps or aerodynamic retroflexion, and requirements 60 strengthening ms. of for requirements for trills rhotics trills. in affected codas The arefricatives. not forthe and by length met, trills onset of suggesting are not met, clusters. following the trill vowel: thatis suggesting Itfricatives is shorter that involvefricatives before a[i] and involo cal (2002)trills exhibit4non-trilled performed aItstudy Solé to (1992) actually variants, refers totaps, replicate theand word-final approximants, phonological rhotics as “trills”variation and but, between as stated earlier, we Solé voiced prefer trills and to use Hualde’s (2005)fricatives. neutral term following less complexvowel: is less “rhotic”. shorter articulation complex before Solé also refers and [i] articulation allow to assibilated longer avariants wider andvoiced as before allow range “fricatives”. [a] a wider of (Solé oropharyngeal Assibilated range2002). of oropharyngeal and pressure rhotics standard variationpressure 4 tend than variation totrills. alternate thansy to replicate Her the phonological variation between trills and fricatives. d andresults standard showed rhotics that 4 tend trills to may alternate become fricatives synchronically (or and assibilated) diachronically if the(Solé finely controlled t trillsThus, articulatory mayassibilated become Thus, rhotics assibilated fricatives occur (or rhotics when assibilated) the occur vibratingwhen 1992). trillsif approximants, the the tongue-tip finelyvibrating Synchronically, met, controlled failstongue-tip to make apical fails contact trills to exhibit make with the contact palate, non-trilled with orvariants the pala nically, apicalortrills aerodynamic exhibit non-trilled requirements variants, for taps, are (2002) not performed suggesting and fricatives. a study that fricatives Solé involve a to replicate the phonological variat micless requirements for trills and are not met, suggesting that fricatives involve a ed a complex study to replicate articulation the phonological allow a widervariation range of oropharyngeal between Her voiced resultsthan trills showed pressure and variation than trills. fricatives. that trills may become fricatives (o and allow Thus, assibilated a wider range of oropharyngeal pressure variation trills. howed that trillsrhotics may become occur when the vibrating fricatives (or assibilated)tongue-tip if articulatory fails theorto finely make contact aerodynamic controlled with the palate, or requirements for trills are not m ccur when the vibrating tongue-tip fails to make contact with the palate, or aerodynamic1 The distribution 1 requirements The of distribution trills forand trills taps of is are trills further not and met,tapsdiscussed isless suggesting further in Hualde discussed that (2005). fricatives complex articulation and allow in Hualde involve (2005). a a wider range of oroph
Languages 2020, 5, 38 3 of 21 rhotics occur when the vibrating tongue-tip fails to make contact with the palate, or when apical vibration fails, allowing the high velocity air to flow continually through the aperture generating frication (Solé 2002). She further stated that assibilated rhotics in phrase-final position result from the difficulty of sustaining trilling with the lowered decreased subglottal pressure that occurs at the end of a statement. Since this is a natural phenomenon, Solé (2002) argued that the co-occurrence of trilling and frication (or standard and assibilated rhotics) should be a common cross-linguistic pattern. In fact, rhotic assibilation (mainly in phrase-final position) has been reported in many dialects of Spanish (Blecua and Cicres 2019; Canfield 1981; Quilis 1981), in other Romance and non-Romance languages such as Brazilian Portuguese (Silva 1996), Czech (Howson et al. 2014), and in Farsi (Ladefoged and Maddieson 1996). Solé (2002) replicated in the laboratory the variation observed in different languages. 1.2. Sociolinguistic Literature on Rhotic Assibilation in Mexico City Assibilated taps and trills were first studied in Mexico City by Lope Blanch (1967). The author (Lope Blanch 1967) argued that assibilation was a recent phenomenon that appeared after the 1950s in the speech of women. He suggested that assibilation might have been imported from Spain, because it had been observed there and in several other Latin-American countries. Lope Blanch (1967) proposed that rhotic assibilation is a natural process in the evolution of the phonological system of languages. Unfortunately, this last point was not developed further, but it is explored in the laboratory by Solé (2002), summarized above. The first synchronic sociolinguistic analysis of rhotic assibilation in Mexico City was published by Perissinotto (1972). The study was based on 110 h of recorded conversation (the number of participants was not stated) collected between 1963 and 1969. His results showed a high overall percentage of assibilation (68.2%). Female speakers had a higher rate of the assibilated rhotic [ř] compared to their standard variant [r] ([ř] 81.8% vs. [r] 18.2%) while male speakers had a reverted pattern of variation: A higher percentage of the standard variant compared to the assibilated one ([r] 61.1% vs. [ř] 38.9%). By comparing the distribution of the assibilated variant across age and socio-economic status, Perissinotto (1972) found that assibilation was more common in the younger age group and in the high and middle socio-economic levels. In other words, assibilation was a prestigious innovative variant adopted by women of the higher classes. More than three decades after the first study, Martín Butragueño (2006) performed an analysis of rhotic assibilation in 54 native speakers of Mexico City. The overall percentage of assibilation in absolute final position was 27%, compared to the 68.1% reported by Perissinotto (1972). A multivariate analysis of rhotic assibilation showed a number of linguistic and social factors that favored assibilation: (1) Absolute final position; (2) formal style; (3) mid and high education; (4) older generation; and (5) females. Martín Butragueño (2006) claimed that given the lower percentage of assibilation found in his study, as compared to Perissinotto’s (1972), rhotic assibilation seemed to be a receding case of language change. This conclusion was reinforced by the fact that assibilation was more frequent in older women, followed by the adult and younger groups (36%, 32%, and 17%, respectively). This is a tendency in the other direction of what Perissinotto (1972) had found with assibilation being more frequent in younger speakers, followed by adults and older speakers (73.5%, 64.5%, and 31.3%, respectively). The comparison of assibilation found by Perissinotto (1972) and Martín Butragueño (2006) seems to indicate that rhotic assibilation was a short-lived fashion in Mexico City. Martín Butragueño’s (2014) second study analyzed the speech of 54 interviews and reading tasks from the Sociolinguistic Corpus of Mexico City. The subjects were distributed along the different social categories of age, sex, and education level. The study took into consideration all rhotic positions including coda, onset and consonant cluster, word final, and absolute final. A multivariate analysis confirmed what had been reported previously for rhotic assibilation. Presented in order of importance, assibilation was favored by: (1) Phrase-final position; (2) interview (as opposed to reading); (3) consonant clusters (especially /tr/); (4) people with medium level of instruction (10–12 years); (5) female
Languages 2020, 5, 38 4 of 21 speakers; and (6) older speakers (55+ years old). Other coda positions were not selected as significant by the multivariate analysis and the author reported very few cases of assibilation in positions other than absolute final and in consonant cluster /tr/. Languages 2020, 5, x FOR PEER REVIEW 4 of 21 Martín Butragueño (2014) presented some spectrograms of trill and taps to illustrate the different types of rhotics found in Mexico selected City Spanish. as significant by the Besides the standard multivariate analysis variants, and the author he alsoreported found approximant very few cases of assibilation in positions other than absolute final and in consonant variants, which the author defined as weakened versions of standard rhotics, and assibilated rhotics. cluster /tr/. He proposed that rhotic Martín Butragueño assibilation be (2014) presented described some spectrograms as change of trill and taps from an approximant to atofricative illustratesound. the different types of rhotics found in Mexico City Spanish. Besides the standard variants, he also found According to theLanguages author, this becomes 2020, 5, xvariants, evident in the cases where the rhotic starts as an approximant4 of 21 FOR PEER which REVIEWthe author defined as weakened versions of standard rhotics, approximant and (with a clear sonority bar and no noise) and continues as a fricative with high frequency noise and an assibilated rhotics. He proposed that rhotic assibilation be described as change from an approximant almost inexistent F0. selected to However, a fricative as demonstrated as significant sound. According to theby by the multivariate Soléanalysis author, (2002) in and this becomes thethe laboratory, author evident there reported in the isvery cases where compelling fewrhotic the casesstarts of assibilation cross-linguistic evidence in positions suggesting(with as an approximant other than that atrills absolute clearalternate final withand sonority bar and in consonant fricatives no noise) (orand cluster assibilated /tr/. continuesvariants), as a fricativeso itwith is high Martín plausible that assibilated frequency Butragueño rhotics noise result (2014) and anfrom presented “a failure almost some spectrograms of sustaining inexistent F0. However, of as trill trilling withandthetapslowered demonstrated to illustrate the decreased by Solé different (2002) in the types of rhotics laboratory, thereatfound isthe in Mexico compelling City Spanish. Besides the standard variants, hewith alsofricatives found subglottal pressure that occurs end of across-linguistic statement” evidence (Solé 2002).suggesting that trills alternate approximant (or assibilated variants, variants), whichso it the author defined is plausible as weakened that assibilated rhoticsversions result from of standard “a failure rhotics, of sustaining and assibilated 1.3. Sociolinguistictrilling Literature rhotics. with on theRhoticHe lowered proposed Assibilation that decreased in rhotic assibilation the Statepressure subglottal be of Chihuahua, described that occurs Mexicoas change from an at the end of a statement” (Soléapproximant to2002). a fricative sound. According to the author, this becomes evident in the cases where the rhotic starts Rhotic assibilation reached the as an approximant (with northa clear of sonority Mexico after bar and theno1960s, noise)with the first study and continues conducted as a fricative with high in Ciudad Juárez1.3.frequency noise inSociolinguistic 1998 (Amastae and an Literature almost et al.on1998). inexistent F0. This studyinshowed Rhotic Assibilation However, as the State ofthat demonstrated assibilation Chihuahua, by Solé Mexicowas present (2002) in the laboratory, there is compelling cross-linguistic in Ciudad Juárez, but with a lower frequency of overall occurrence than reported in Mexico City. evidence suggesting that trills alternate with fricatives Rhotic assibilation reached the norththat of Mexico afterrhotics the 1960s, with the“a first studyofconducted sustainingin The percentage (or of assibilated assibilation Ciudad Juárez variants), so in 1998 in all final it is plausible positions (Amastae et al. (including 1998). assibilated This word study final showed result andthat from phrase-final) failure assibilation was was 6%, present trilling with the lowered decreased subglottal pressure that occurs at the end of a statement” (Soléin while the percentage Ciudadof assibilation Juaréz, but in absolute with a lower final position frequency of was 22%. overall Compared occurrence than to the 68.1% reported in reported Mexico City. The 2002). by Perissinotto (1972),percentage assibilation of assibilation was in much lesspositions all final frequent in Ciudad (including word Juárez. final and Amastae phrase-final) et al.was (1998) 6%, while theSociolinguistic conducted a multivariate 1.3. percentage analysisofLiterature assibilation on 72onspeakers in absolute Rhotic and final Assibilation position found in thethat was State of22%. assibilationCompared Chihuahua, was Mexico to the 68.1% affected by: reported (1) by Gender: Higher rate of assibilated rhotics in women (women 28% and men 16%); (2) social class:(1998) Perissinotto (1972), assibilation was much less frequent in Ciudad Juárez. Amastae et al. Rhotic assibilation conducted a multivariate reached analysisthe northon 72ofspeakers Mexico after and the found 1960s, thatwith the first study assibilation conducted was affected in Greater percentage of assibilation in the higher socio-economic class as compared to the middle class 5by: (1) Ciudad Juárez in 1998 (Amastae et al. 1998). This study Gender: Higher rate of assibilated rhotics in women (women 28% and men 16%); (2) social class: showed that assibilation was present in (21% and 18%, respectively); Ciudad Juaréz, (3) but age: with Adults a lower (36–55 frequency years) of and older overall participants occurrence than (56+) showed reported in Mexico higher City. The Greater percentage of assibilation in the higher socio-economic class as compared to the middle class5 rates of assibilation (31% and percentage (21% and 18%, 19%, of assibilation respectively) respectively); in all(3) final than age:positions younger Adults (36–55 (including participants years)word final(15%); and older and and (4) phrase-final) participants education: (56+)was showed 6%, while higher Assibilation wasthe more percentage of assibilation in absolute final position rates of assibilation (31% and 19%, respectively) than younger participants (15%); and (4) and frequent in speakers with higher levels of was formal 22%. Compared education to the (university 68.1% 23% reported education: by Perissinotto high school 14%).Assibilation All these effects(1972), was more assibilation suggested frequentthat was much assibilation in speakers less frequent withwas higher also in Ciudad a prestigious levels Juárez. of formal education Amastae variant in(universityet al. Ciudad 23% (1998) Juárez. Amastae conducted et al. and high a multivariate (1998) school argued 14%). All analysis that on 72 suggested assibilation these effects speakers in Ciudad and that found Juárez that assibilation wasassibilation a recent was was affected also a “change prestigious from by: (1)in variant Gender: Ciudad Higher rate Juárez. Amastae of assibilated et al. (1998) rhotics in women (women 28% and men 16%); (2) social class: above” (Labov 1972) imported from Mexico City argued by thethat higher assibilation classesinand Ciudad Juárez was transmitted bya women. recent “change Greater percentage from(1972), above”a (Labov of assibilation 1972) importedin the higher from socio-economic Mexico change City by that class as the higher compared classes to the middle and transmitted class 5 by According to Labov (21% and 18%, change respectively); from above (3) age: is Adults a linguistic (36–55from years)aboveand older enters the language from women. According to Labov (1972), a change is a participants (56+) showed higher above the level of ratesconsciousness; of assibilation that (31% is, and speakers 19%, are respectively)generally than younger the linguistic aware ofparticipants linguistic change (15%); form and that enters and (4) they the education: language from above the level of consciousness; that is, speakers are generally aware of the linguistic manipulate its use depending Assibilation was on more thefrequent contextinand/or their interlocutors. The upper classes (university use these 23% form and they manipulate its usespeakers depending with onhigher levels of the context formal and/or theireducation interlocutors. The upper new linguistic formsandclasses use these new linguistic forms in order to differentiate themselves from the classes in high order school to differentiate 14%). All these themselves effects suggested from the that lower assibilation classes, was while also a lower prestigious lowervariant in classes, use these forms Ciudad inwhile order Juárez. to soundAmastae more et al. formal(1998) andargued similar that toassibilation the upper lower classes use these forms in order to sound more formal and similar to the upper classes. in Ciudad classes. Juárez was a recent “change Twenty years fromafterabove” Twenty (Labov Amastae’s years after 1972) study, imported Amastae’s Mazzaro from study, and Mexico González Mazzaro CityGonzález and by dethe Anda higher(2019) (2019) classes and transmitted investigated investigated the by the relationship women. According to Labov (1972), a change from above is a linguistic change that enters relationship between between thethe perception perceptionand and production production ofofrhotic rhotic assibilation assibilation [ř] and [ř] deaffrication and deaffrication of the of the voiceless language from post-alveolaraffricate above the level affricate [[ ʃ ]] in of consciousness; that is, speakers are generally aware of the linguistic the voiceless post-alveolar inthe theSpanish Spanish of Chihuahua, of Chihuahua, Mexico. Thirty-three Mexico. native Spanish Thirty-three native speakers form from and the they state manipulate of Chihuahua its completed use depending on the and production context and/or tasks perception their to interlocutors. establish The upper whether those Spanish speakers from use classes the these state new of Chihuahua linguistic completed production and perception tasks to establish that produced the variants wereforms able to in perceive order to them. differentiate themselves The production data from werethe lowerby elicited classes, asking whether those that while produced lower classes theuse variants these wereinable forms ordertotoperceive sound more them. formal Theandproduction similar data were participants to narrate the fairy tale Caperucita Roja ‘Little Red Riding Hood’ to and thebyupper asking classes. them to elicited by asking participants Twenty years toafter narrate Amastae’s the fairy study, tale Caperucita Mazzaro and Roja ‘Little González (2019) Red Riding Hood’ investigated the and relationship talk about their favorite food. Results showed that the overall percentage of [ř] production was by asking thembetween to talk about greater than their the perception that of [ʃ]favorite and production (17.15% food. Results and 11.83%, of rhotic showed assibilation respectively). that the The[ř] overall and authors percentage deaffrication explained of the that ofvoiceless the [ř] increased production was post-alveolar greater than affricate that of [[ ʃ ] ] in the (17.15% Spanish and of Chihuahua, 11.83%, Mexico. respectively). overall rate of [ř] might be due to assibilation only being investigated in absolute final position, which Thirty-three The authors native Spanish explained speakers that from the increased overall is thethe rate state context of of [ř] Chihuahua might where most beof completed due theto production assibilation variation occurs.only andbeing The perception social tasks investigated factors that to establish in absolute turned out to whether befinal those significant that produced the variants were able to perceive them. The production data were elicited by asking position, which iswere the gender contextand wheregeneration, most of withthefemale variation and younger occurs.speakersThe social favoring factors thethat use of assibilation. turned out The participants to between narrate the fairy tale Caperucita Roja ‘Little Red Riding was Hood’ and subtle; by asking them to to be significant relationship were gender and generation, production and female with perception andofyounger assibilation speakersvery favoring theoverall, use the talk about their perception of the favorite variantfood. was Results very lowshowed (9.1%). that Those the who overall producedpercentage assibilationof [ř] wereproductionnot thewas ones of assibilation. Thegreater relationship than that ofbetween [ʃ] (17.15% production and 11.83%, and perception respectively). Theof assibilation authors explained was that verythe subtle; increased overall rate of [ř] might be due to assibilation only being investigated in absolute final position, which is the context where most of the variation occurs. The social factors that turned out to be significant 5 were did Amastae et al. (1998) gender and generation, not analyze the speech with female and of participants younger of lower speakers favoring socio-economic class. the use of assibilation. The relationship 5 Amastae et between al. (1998)production did not analyze and theperception of assibilation speech of participants of lowerwas very subtle; socio-economic class.overall, the perception of the variant was very low (9.1%). Those who produced assibilation were not the ones 5 Amastae et al. (1998) did not analyze the speech of participants of lower socio-economic class.
Languages 2020, 5, 38 5 of 21 overall, the perception of the variant was very low (9.1%). Those who produced assibilation were not the ones that perceived it the most, which seemed to suggest that rhotic assibilation was below the level of consciousness and social awareness (Labov 2001). To the best of our knowledge, no other sociolinguistic studies have been done on rhotic assibilation in this geographic area. The present research will further investigate the social and linguistic factors that affect the production of this variable. We also add another layer of analysis to increase the accuracy of coding of tokens, by supplying the auditory classification of tokens with a spectrographic analysis. Acoustic information will allow us to identify if there are other variants that could also be alternating with assibilated rhotics. This study is designed to explore different aspects of the production of rhotic assibilation in the state of Chihuahua/El Paso, TX speech community6 . Our specific questions are the following: i. What is the current state of rhotic assibilation more than two decades after the first (and only) study was done? In this study, we expect to find a lower frequency of rhotic assibilation, since previous studies on this same dialect (Amastae et al. 1998) stated that this variable was receding. ii. Which rhotic variants are found in Chihuahua Spanish? Given that Amastae et al. (1998) suggested that assibilation is receding, we predict that we will find a much smaller percentage of rhotic assibilation than he did in 1998, and a large percentage of standard variants (taps and trills). It is possible that we could find other variants reported in other dialects of Mexican Spanish such as approximants, retroflex, and fricatives (Martín Butragueño 2006). iii. What are the most important linguistic and social correlates of rhotic assibilation? Gender is expected to be one of the most significant factors that influence the variable under study. The fact that female speakers favor assibilation has been consistently reported by Amastae et al. (1998); Lope Blanch (1967); Martín Butragueño (2006); and Perissinotto (1972). Besides gender, we anticipate older speakers to produce more assibilation than the other age groups, which is due to the receding status of the variant (Amastae et al. 1998). The literature on rhotic assibilation agree that the phrase-final position is the most favoring phonetic context for assibilated rhotics. Thus, in this study, we focus on rhotic production in absolute final position. Finally, the preceding vocalic context was found to be significant in our previous analysis of rhotic assibilation (Mazzaro and González de Anda 2016), so we predict that with more data, this factor will show a clearer and more robust effect. iv. Does being a Spanish-English bilingual affect the use of this variable? Previous work (Dalola and Bullock 2017) found that being bilingual affects the social perception and production rates of variables in the L2. However, our participants are all L1 speakers of Spanish, so we do not expect the L2 English to affect the rate of assibilation in bilingual compared to monolingual speakers. v. What is the effect of the formality of the task (style): Reading vs. narrative vs. conversation on the occurrence of assibilation? Given the prestige attached to assibilated rhotics (Amastae et al. 1998; Perissinotto 1972), we predict that tasks that are more formal would elicit higher instances of assibilation. Therefore, the reading task will elicit a higher percentage of assibilation than the narrative and the informal conversation. As the formality of the task decreases, so will the frequency of assibilated rhotics. vi. Does assibilation remain a prestigious feature of speech? We expect that rhotic assibilation will continue to be a prestigious feature of speech. This is based on the vast amount of literature that report the assibilated variant to be used in higher social classes and subjects with higher levels of formal education (Amastae et al. 1998; Martín Butragueño 2006, 2014; Perissinotto 1972). 6 We consider El Paso, TX part of the state of Chihuahua’s Spanish-language speech community because of their geographic proximity (separated only by the Rio Grande River). We are aware that there is lack of research that compares the Spanish of the state of Chihuahua and that of El Paso, but we posit that they are one dialect, at least at the phonetic/phonological level.
Languages 2020, 5, 38 6 of 21 2. Materials and Methods 2.1. Speakers Participants of this study are native Spanish speakers recruited in the El Paso, Texas—Ciudad Juárez, Mexico border area. A total of 58 subjects participated in this study: 36 women and 22 men, with an age range between 18 and 69 years. To compare our data with Amastae et al.’s (1998) generational groups, the participants were divided into four groups: Generation 1: 56. Participants were asked to complete an adult language background questionnaire that elicits information about their place of birth, language(s) of schooling, and language use. The questionnaire contained a section that asked for participants’ self-proficiency ratings in both Spanish and English, and only those who reported to use mostly/only Spanish in their daily everyday interactions (at home, at work, and in social situations) were selected to participate in the study. We considered bilingual those participants who reported to know another language and self-assessed their English knowledge as higher than basic. The demographic information of the participants considered in the statistical analysis is presented in Table 1. Table 1. Participants’ demographic information. Participants’ Information Participants n 58 Age range in years 18–69 Generation 1 (56 years) n 5 (5 females) Male: Female n 22:36 Median/Mean age in years 22/30 Ciudad Juárez (25), From location n El Paso, TX (20), Chihuahua—Capital (5), Chihuahua—Interior (Delicias, Parral, Balleza, Cd. Guerrero, Cuauhtémoc, Jiménez) (8) Bilingual: Monolingual n 29:29:00 All subjects gave their informed consent for inclusion before they participated in the study. The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and the protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Texas at El Paso IRB (Project identification code [274707-10]). Thirty-four participants were recruited at the University of Texas at El Paso (UTEP). Some were students enrolled in beginner-level ESOL (English for Speakers of Other Languages). Seven participants were students at the Universidad Tecnológica de Ciudad Juárez. The remaining 17 participants were contacted using the ‘friend-of-a-friend’ technique (Milroy 1987), whereby potential informants are contacted through common friends, an approach that is particularly appropriate for the community under study. Although rhotic assibilation in Mexican Spanish is a feature mainly observed in higher social classes, specifically mid-high to high class (Amastae et al. 1998; Perissinotto 1972), our study also includes participants of the mid-low or middle social classes. Most of the participants from El Paso who attend UTEP fall within the mid-low or middle social classes. The participants from the state of Chihuahua who attend UTEP are probably from mid to high classes, since it is very expensive for Mexican citizens to afford college north of the border. However, because we do not have an accurate tool to determine participants’ social class, we will not investigate the effect of this factor henceforth.
You can also read