Trait Associations for Hillary Clinton and Donald Trump in News Media: A Computational Analysis
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
Special Section Article Social Psychological and Personality Science Trait Associations for Hillary Clinton 2018, Vol. 9(2) 123-130 ª The Author(s) 2018 Reprints and permission: and Donald Trump in News Media: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/1948550617751584 A Computational Analysis journals.sagepub.com/home/spp Sudeep Bhatia1, Geoffrey P. Goodwin1, and Lukasz Walasek2 Abstract We study media representations of Hillary Clinton and Donald Trump in the 2016 U.S. presidential election. In particular, we train models of semantic memory on a large number of news media outlets that published online articles during the course of the election. Based on the structure of word co-occurrence in these media outlets, our models learn semantic representations for the two presidential candidates as well as for widely studied personality traits. We find that models trained on media outlets most read by Clinton voters and media outlets most read by Trump voters differ in the strength of association between the two candidates’ names and trait words pertaining to morality. We observe some differences for trait words pertaining to warmth, but none for trait words pertaining to competence. Keywords social perception, semantic representation, political psychology, computational models, media The relationship between trait associations and the perception representations in media coverage pertaining to a particular elec- of individuals and groups has a long history of research in psy- tion. The election that we consider is the 2016 U.S. presidential chological science. Within social psychology, dimensional election, and we attempt to uncover person representations and models of person perception have been dominant, positing that corresponding trait associations with Donald Trump and Hillary there are two (e.g., Cuddy, Fiske, & Glick, 2008; Fiske, Cuddy, Clinton present in a large number of media outlets that published & Glick, 2007; Fiske, Cuddy, Glick, & Xu, 2002) or three (e.g., articles during this election. In addition to helping us character- Brambilla, Rusconi, Sacchi, & Cherubini, 2011; Brambilla, ize media representations for Trump and Clinton, this approach Sacchi, Rusconi, Cherubini, & Yzerbyt, 2012; Goodwin, allows us to test for differences between media outlets favored Piazza, & Rozin, 2014; Landy, Piazza, & Goodwin, 2016; by Trump voters and media outlets favored by Clinton voters. Leach, Ellemers, & Barreto, 2007) key trait dimensions upon In our approach, we utilize distributional models of semantic which individuals and social groups are evaluated. In the con- memory and our contribution is therefore to also showcase how text of political preferences, existing research suggests that the this methodology can be applied to studying person perception most important trait dimension is competence (Ballew & in the context of geopolitical events. Todorov, 2007; Cislak & Wojciszke, 2006; Funk, 1997; Todorov, Mandisodza, Goren, & Hall, 2005), although other research also highlights the importance of the morality dimen- Trait Associations for Political Candidates sion (Goodwin et al., 2014). In general, much of the existing work on person perception in There are two difficulties facing researchers who attempt to the political domain has examined how voter preferences estimate the dimensional structure of representations for real depend on the target’s trait dimensions (e.g., how competent political candidates. Firstly, associations between individuals is a specific candidate perceived to be). In this type of research, and traits or dimensions may vary as a function of political con- participants are often presented, either explicitly or implicitly, text (e.g., Goodwin et al., 2014; Judd, James-Hawkins, Yzerbyt, & Kashima, 2005). Second, determining trait associations with political candidates in the past (i.e., in the context of political 1 Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA 2 events that have already occurred) is nearly impossible using University of Warwick, Coventry, West Midlands, United Kingdom survey-based techniques, making retrospective analyses of per- Corresponding Author: son perception in the political domain very difficult. Sudeep Bhatia, Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, One alternative approach to standard survey-based tech- Philadelphia, PA, USA. niques, and one that we pursue in the present article, is to study Email: bhatiasu@sas.upenn.edu
124 Social Psychological and Personality Science 9(2) with trait information about different individuals. Less is sources that were read by Trump or Clinton voters. For this pur- known, however, about the origin of trait–candidate associa- pose, we first ran a pilot study of media consumption tions, and how these associations are influenced by the imme- preferences. diate political context. Existing evidence strongly suggests that news media play an important role shaping people’s voting atti- Method tudes and preferences (DellaVigna & Kaplan, 2007; Gerber, Karlan, & Bergen, 2009; also Margetts, 2017). Media coverage In the first session of this study, we asked 200 U.S. citizens, is often used as a source of information about social norms, recruited through the online experiments website, Prolific Aca- which can shift views of individuals and lead to the polarization demic, about the media outlets they visited regularly and and homophily of attitudes (Bennett, 2012; Slater, 2007). It is trusted for information about politics and current affairs. Parti- also well known that people exhibit strong confirmation bias in cipants in this study were shown the names of the 250 media consuming political information and that selective exposure outlets we had in our news corpus (see corpus description in can drive the emergence of polarization and compartmentaliza- later sections of this article) and were asked to select as many tion in political thought. Thus, causality is likely to go both outlets as they wished. After this task, participants were asked ways, with the preferences for selective consumption on one to indicate which of the political candidates they voted for in side, and growing personalization and selective marketing by the election. Due to the imbalance in Clinton versus Trump vot- media to meet demands of specific groups of customers on the ers in this session, we ran a second study session, in which we other (Bakshy, Messing, & Adamic, 2015; Van Aelst, Sheafer, recruited an additional 100 self-reported Republicans and 100 & Stanyer, 2012). More generally, the content of news media self-reported Democrats from Prolific Academic (who hadn’t both reflects and influences public sentiment and, for that rea- taken part in our first session) and asked them about their media son, can yield useful insights about the psychological correlates preferences in the same way as described above. Thus, our of voters’ preferences (De Vreese & Sementko, 2004; Hop- eventual study had a total (both sessions) of N ¼ 400 partici- mann, Vliegenthart, De Vreese, & Albaek, 2010). pants (mean age ¼ 33.73, SD age ¼ 11.87, 38% female). In In order to examine representations for Hillary Clinton and total, we had 111 Trump voters and 185 Clinton voters in our Donald Trump in news media, we adopt the insights of distri- sample. butional models of semantic memory. These models propose that people’s semantic representations for objects and concepts Results can be approximated by examining the statistical structure of the natural language environments that they are exposed to We pooled the data from these two sessions and used it to cal- (Griffiths, Steyvers, & Tenenbaum, 2007; Jones & Mewhort, culate the degree to which Clinton and Trump voters relied on 2007; Landauer & Dumais, 1997; Mikolov, Sutskever, Chen, each media outlet for political information. For each media out- Corrado, & Dean, 2013; Pennington, Socher, & Manning, let i, we calculated rC i and rT i , which is the total number of 2014; see Firth, 1957; Harris, 1954, for early theories). This Clinton voters and Trump voters in our study who stated that insight has been successfully used to study a range of psycho- they relied on that media outlet. We then used rC i and rT i to cal- linguistic and cognitive phenomena (see Bullinaria & Levy, culate the extent to which Clinton and Trump voters relied on 2007; Jones, Willits, & Dennis, 2015, for a review). Addition- that outlet relative to each of the other media outlets, to P P250 T ally, Bhatia (2017a, 2017b) has applied distributional models obtain RC i ¼ rC i = 250 C T T j¼1 r j and R i ¼ r i = j¼1 r i . Finally, of semantic memory, trained on news media, and online ency- we calculated a measure of relative voter reliance, clopedias, to predict high-level judgments including forecast- RVRi ¼ RC i RT i . Strongly positive values of RVRi indicate ing, social judgment, and factual judgment. Similarly, that the outlet i was relied on more by Clinton voters than Holtzman, Schott, Jones, Balota, and Yarkoni (2011) and Trump voters, whereas strong negative values of RVRi indicate Dehghani, Sagae, Sachdeva, and Gratch (2014) have applied that the outlet was relied on more by Trump voters than Clinton distributional models to study political bias in media. The abil- voters. The 10 media outlets with the highest and lowest RVR ity of distributional models to approximate actual human scores are listed in Table 1. semantic representations suggests that training these models on news media published during the 2016 U.S. presidential election could reveal key similarities and differences in repre- Study of Trait Associations in Media Outlets sentations and trait associations for the two candidates across For our main study, we obtained online news articles published different types of media outlets. This could, in turn, shed light by 250 U.S.-based media outlets, over the course of the 2016 on how Trump and Clinton voters represented these two election. We trained semantic memory models individually candidates over the course of the election. on each of these news outlets. This yielded 250 different mod- els, each with unique representations and associations for a very large set of words and phrases. These words and phrases Pilot Study of News Preferences included Hillary Clinton, Donald Trump, and a large number A critical component of our analysis involves studying differ- of trait words, and thus for each model (i.e., each media outlet) ences in trait associations for Trump and Clinton in media we were able to calculate the strength of association between
Bhatia et al. 125 Table 1. The 10 Media Outlets With the Highest and Lowest Read- close to 1 capturing words which are very negatively ership Among Clinton and Trump Voters. associated. Media Outlet RVR Note that since the work of Landauer and Dumais, there have been a number of technical advancements in uncovering CNN .027 semantic representations from text (e.g., Mikolov et al., Washington Post .021 2013; Pennington et al., 2014). These techniques differ in terms NPR .021 of how they specify word context, and how they use contextual The New Yorker .014 similarities in word use in the absence of direct word The Atlantic .014 CBS Local .009 co-occurrence. We do not expect the key qualitative results FOX Sports .009 of our analysis to vary with the use of different techniques NFL.com .010 (though more recent approaches may lead to lower variability Breitbart News .025 and better quality word representations). Fox News .047 Note. Readership is quantified using relative voter reliance (RVR) scores, News Corpora explained in the main text. Positive (negative) values indicate that the outlet was relied on by Clinton (Trump) voters and not Trump (Clinton) voters. We trained our models on the NOW corpus (http://corpus. byu.edu/now/), a very large data set of news articles published the two candidates’ names and the various trait words and online and linked to by Google News. For the purpose of the dimensions. We then examined both the absolute associations present study, we restricted our analysis to articles published between the two candidates and trait dimensions, as well as the by U.S. media outlets between June 16, 2015 (the date that differences in these associations for media outlets predomi- Donald Trump declared his candidacy) and November 7, nantly relied on by Clinton voters and media outlets relied on 2016 (the day before the election). Our final data set consisted by Trump voters, based on the ratings from our pilot study. of 322,699 online articles with a total of 82,784,212 words. To facilitate our analysis, we lowercased all words in the corpus of news articles, removed all punctuation, and replaced Method each mention of donald trump and hillary clinton with donald_trump and hillary_clinton, respectively. We then iden- Latent Semantic Analysis (LSA) tified unique words based on white space (using donald_trump The semantic model used in this article is LSA (Landauer & and hillary_clinton ensured that we retained representations for Dumais, 1997; Landauer, Foltz, & Laham, 1998). The core the full names of the candidates rather than splitting them up insight of LSA is that words can be represented using multidi- into first and last names). mensional vectors, which are obtained by performing a dimen- sionality reduction on word distribution data. The relationship or association between words is subsequently captured by the Candidate–Trait Associations proximity of the vectors for their corresponding words. We trained LSA models on each of the 250 media sources sep- More formally, for a natural language environment with N arately, yielding 250 models. For each model, we considered different words occurring in K contexts, we can represent the each article to be a unique context. Thus, for media outlet i, structure of word distribution using an N K matrix S. S cap- we obtained the matrix Si capturing the occurrence of words tures word context co-occurrence, so that the number of times across the articles in this outlet. We also applied a term fre- word n occurs in context k is indicated by the value of the cell quency inverse document frequency (tf idf) transforma- in row n and column k of S. LSA recovers vector representa- tion to each matrix S i (which is recommended in order to tions for each of the words by decomposing S into some control for word frequency effects) prior to performing singular M
126 Social Psychological and Personality Science 9(2) of overall valence for each trait word (how positive or negative Table 2. Interaction Effects Between Relative Voter Reliance on the each word is), but we did not include these ratings because of Media Outlet and the Dimension Rating of the Trait Word for Predict- their nonspecificity. ing the Association Between That Trait and Clinton Versus Trump, in the Media Outlet. In order to calculate the association between the presidential candidates and these traits, we used the vectors corresponding Regressions b SE z p 95% CI [L, H] to the individual trait words (e.g., honest, warm, and intelli- gent) and calculated their cosine similarity with the vectors cor- Individual regressions Ability .05 .07 0.70 .49 [0.18, 0.09] responding to donald_trump and hillary_clinton. Using these Agency .05 .07 0.63 .53 [0.10, 0.19] methods, we had, for each outlet and for each trait word, a mea- Character .21 .06 3.33
Bhatia et al. 127 Two-dimensional theories posit that these dimensions are Table 3. The Set of High Morality and Low/Neural Warmth, Low/ warmth (which includes morality) and competence, whereas Neutral Morality and High Warmth, and High Competence Words, three-dimensional theories posit that these dimensions are Used to Isolate the Effects of the Morality, Warmth, and Competence, on Trait Ratings. warmth, morality, and competence. In order to examine trait associations in terms of these core dimensions, we generated High Morality and Low/ Low/Neutral and Morality High a composite variable for morality/warmth by averaging ratings Neutral Warmth High Warmth Competence for the character, communion, goodness, morality, and warmth Courageous Warm Athletic dimensions, a composite variable for morality by averaging rat- Fair Sociable Musical ings for the character, goodness, and morality dimensions, and Principled Happy Creative a composite variable for competence by averaging the ratings Responsible Agreeable Innovative for ability and agency dimensions, for each trait. We then ran Just Enthusiastic Intelligent individual regressions testing for the effect of our composite Honest Easygoing Organized dimensions on trait associations. Three new regressions Trustworthy Funny Logical revealed that both the morality/warmth composite and the mor- Loyal Playful Clever ality composite had strong significant interaction effects with the relative voter reliance on the media outlet (p < .001; see Table 2 for details) that survive the Holm–Bonferroni correc- still failed the Holm–Bonferroni correction. We still found a sig- tion for multiple comparisons. In contrast, the competence nificant interaction effect for the morality/warmth composite composite did not display this interaction effect. (p < .01), in a regression with the morality/warmth composite We also ran variants of the above regressions directly com- and the competence composite. The significant interaction paring the composite dimensions against each other. In the first effect of a separate morality composite in a variant of this regres- combined regression, we included the morality/warmth and sion including warmth and the competence composite also per- competence composite. In our second combined regression, sisted (p < .05). A related set of regressions for only negatively we used three composites, for warmth, morality, and compe- valenced words (words below the median valence on Goodwin tence. The former model revealed a strong interaction effect for et al.’s trait ratings) did not yield any significant differences for the morality/warmth composite (p < .001), but not the compe- any dimensions of interest (p > .05). This result could be due to tence composite. The latter model revealed a significant posi- differences in frequency of word use for positively and nega- tive interaction effect for the morality composite (p < .05), tively valenced traits, in news media. but not the warmth dimension or the competence composite. Overall, these results indicate that media outlets read by Clinton (Trump) voters were more likely to associate Clinton Dimension Independence Analysis (Trump) with traits rated highly on the morality composite. Results reported so far suggest that semantic representations of These is no evidence for differences on the warmth or compe- Clinton and Trump present in media read by Clinton and tence composites. The results of these two regressions are also Trump voters varied primarily in terms of morality associa- summarized in Table 2. Again, there were no significant main tions. There seems to be some evidence that warmth associa- effects of the dimensional composites on relative associations. tions could have varied as well. However, the warmth dimension is positively correlated with the morality dimension (as well as the morality composite), so this relationship could Valence Analysis be incidental. Our analysis testing for the independent effects Goodwin et al.’s (2014) data set contains negative trait words of both the morality composite and warmth simultaneously that occasionally map on quite strongly to dimensions like mor- found no effect of warmth (but a significant positive effect of ality and warmth (e.g., dishonest has a high rating on the mor- morality). ality dimension as it is strongly reflective of the morality of an Another way to attempt this analysis is to use only a subset individual). It is necessary to rerun the above analysis exclud- of the trait words that Goodwin et al. compiled that are either ing the negatively valenced words, to ensure that the effects high in morality and neutral/low in warmth or neutral/low in documented above did not emerge due to a perverse association morality and high in warmth. There are eight such high moral- between Clinton and immorality-related words in pro-Clinton ity/low warmth words and eight low morality/high warmth outlets, and Trump and immorality-related words in pro- words in Goodwin et al.’s Study 3, and we attempted to repli- Trump outlets. cate our above analysis using these words. For comparability, We did this by performing a median split on the valence rat- we also performed this analysis with the eight high competence ings in Goodwin et al.’s data and excluding trait words below the words in Goodwin et al. The words used in this analysis are median valence. This regression did not change findings for the summarized in Table 3. nine dimensions, except that the interaction effect of character First, we ran three separate regressions, examining the indi- decreased in significance to p ¼ .02, failing the Holm–Bonfer- vidual effects of these sets of words on trait associations. In the roni correction for multiple comparisons. The interaction effect first regression, our dependent variable was relative association of warmth also increased in significance to p ¼ .01; however, it RAij, and the independent variables were the relative voter
128 Social Psychological and Personality Science 9(2) Table 4. Interaction Effects Between Relative Voter Reliance on the possessed by these models between Hillary Clinton, Donald Media Outlet and the Dimension Rating of the Trait Word for Predict- Trump, and various words describing personal traits. Compar- ing the Association Between That Trait and Clinton Versus Trump in ing media outlets favored by Clinton voters against media out- the Media Outlet. lets favored by Trump voters, we found that differences in Regressions b SE z p 95% CI [L, H] representations pertained primarily to morality, with Clinton outlets more strongly associating Clinton with moral traits rela- Individual regressions tive to Trump, and Trump outlets more strongly associating Morality .84 .35 2.39
Bhatia et al. 129 considered in this article to have developed the types of biased Ballew, C. C., & Todorov, A. (2007). Predicting political elections moral associations for the two candidates observed in our tests. from rapid and unreflective face judgments. Proceedings of the Although we have not tested this causal link in our work, there is National Academy of Sciences, 104, 17948–17953. evidence that media bias has this type of causal role (DellaVigna Bennett, W. L. (2012). The personalization of politics. The Annals of the & Kaplan, 2007; Gerber et al., 2009). However, it is also possi- American Academy of Political and Social Science, 644, 20–39. ble that the media biases reflected preheld voter beliefs and that Bhatia, S. (2017a). Associative judgment and vector space semantics. the observed differences in moral associations for Clinton and Psychological Review, 124, 1–20. Trump across outlets favored by Clinton versus Trump voters Bhatia, S. (2017b). The semantic representation of prejudice and were the result of the preexisting associations of the readers of stereotypes. Cognition, 164, 46–60. these outlets. Regardless of the causal direction, our analysis Brambilla, M., Rusconi, P., Sacchi, S., & Cherubini, P. (2011). Look- identifies core differences in representations and associations ing for honesty: The primary role of morality (vs. sociability and across media outlets favored by different types of voters, and competence) in information gathering. European Journal of Social thus suggests a relationship between moral trait associations in Psychology, 41, 135–143. peoples’ information environments and voting behavior. Brambilla, M., Sacchi, S., Rusconi, P., Cherubini, P., & Yzerbyt, V. Y. Of course, a more rigorous test of this relationship would (2012). You want to give a good impression? Be honest! Moral involve participant data on trait ratings for Clinton and Trump. traits dominate group impression formation. British Journal of Unfortunately, such data are hard to obtain retrospectively: The Social Psychology, 51, 149–166. election outcomes and subsequent political events may have Bullinaria, J. A., & Levy, J. P. (2007). Extracting semantic representa- changed peoples’ associations with Clinton and Trump, and tions from word co-occurrence statistics: A computational study. it is not clear how survey-based methods could control for these Behavior Research Methods, 39, 510–526. changes. We are also not aware of any studies that elicit views Cislak, A., & Wojciszke, B. (2006). The role of self-interest and com- about Trump and Clinton using the list of traits that we used in petence in attitudes toward politicians. Polish Psychological Bulle- the present study. In fact, the strength of our approach lies in tin, 4, 203–212. the fact that our models are suited for testing any of a range Cuddy, A. J., Fiske, S. T., & Glick, P. (2008). Warmth and compe- of dimensional theories, as the analysis is not constrained by tence as universal dimensions of social perception: The stereotype the choice of trait words. By obtaining a very large corpus of content model and the BIAS map. Advances in Experimental news media data and leveraging theoretical and technical Social Psychology, 40, 61–149. advances in semantic memory research, the present research Dehghani, M., Sagae, K., Sachdeva, S., & Gratch, J. (2014). Analyz- is able to uncover the representations present in different ing political rhetoric in conservative and liberal weblogs related to real-world information environments. This approach can be the construction of the “Ground Zero Mosque.” Journal of Infor- extended to examine more than just trait associations with mation Technology & Politics, 11, 1–14. political candidates. With an expanded media data set, we DellaVigna, S., & Kaplan, E. (2007). The fox news effect: Media bias could extend such analyses to prior presidential elections as and voting. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 122, 1187–1234. well as to social phenomena outside of politics. The increased De Vreese, C. H., & Sementko, H. A. (2004). News matters: Influ- digitization of information has made it possible to rigorously ences on the vote in the Danish 2000 euro referendum campaign. study the social representations present in peoples’ information European Journal of Political Research, 43, 699–722. environments. This has exciting possibilities for our under- Eagly, A. H., & Karau, S. J. (2002). Role congruity theory of prejudice standing of real-world social cognition and judgment, and we toward female leaders. Psychological Review, 109, 573–598. look forward to further contributing to this approach. Firth, J. R. (1957). Papers in linguistics. London, England: Oxford University Press. Declaration of Conflicting Interests Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J., & Glick, P. (2007). Universal dimensions of social cognition: Warmth and competence. Trends in Cognitive The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to Sciences, 11, 77–83. the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J., Glick, P., & Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively Funding follow from perceived status and competition. Journal of Person- The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for ality and Social Psychology, 82, 878. the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: Funding for Funk, C. L. (1997). Implications of political expertise in candidate Sudeep Bhatia was received from the National Science Foundation trait evaluations. Political Research Quarterly, 50, 675–697. Grant SES-1626825. Funding for Lukasz Walasek was received from Gerber, A. S., Karlan, D., & Bergan, D. (2009). Does the media mat- Leverhulme Trust Grant RP2012-V-022. ter? A field experiment measuring the effect of newspapers on vot- ing behavior and political opinions. American Economic Journal: References Applied Economics, 1, 35–52. Bakshy, E., Messing, S., & Adamic, L. A. (2015). Political science. Gervais, S. J., & Hillard, A. L. (2011). A role congruity perspective on Exposure to ideologically diverse news and opinion on Facebook. prejudice toward Hillary Clinton and Sarah Palin. Analyses of Science, 348, 1130–1132. Social Issues and Public Policy, 11, 221–240.
130 Social Psychological and Personality Science 9(2) Goodwin, G. P., Piazza, J., & Rozin, P. (2014). Moral character pre- positive evaluation of in-groups. Journal of Personality and Social dominates in person perception and evaluation. Journal of Person- Psychology, 93, 234. ality and Social Psychology, 106, 148–168. Margetts, H. (2017). Political behaviour and the acoustics of social Griffiths, T. L., Steyvers, M., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2007). Topics in media. Nature Human Behaviour, 1, 0086. semantic representation. Psychological Review, 114, 211. Mikolov, T., Sutskever, I., Chen, K., Corrado, G. S., & Dean, J. Harris, Z. S. (1954). Distributional structure. Word, 2, 146–162. (2013). Distributed representations of words and phrases and their Holtzman, N. S., Schott, J. P., Jones, M. N., Balota, D. A., & Yarkoni, compositionality. In C. J. C. Burges, L. Bottou, M. Welling, Z. T. (2011). Exploring media bias with semantic analysis tools: Vali- Ghahramani, & K. Q. Weinberger (Eds.), Advances in neural infor- dation of the Contrast Analysis of Semantic Similarity (CASS). mation processing systems (pp. 3111–3119). Redhook, NY: Curran Behavior Research Methods, 43, 193–200. Associates Inc. Hopmann, D. N., Vliegenthart, R., De Vreese, C., & Albæk, E. (2010). Pennington, J., Socher, R., & Manning, C. D. (2014). Glove: Global Effects of election news coverage: How visibility and tone influ- vectors for word representation. In B. Pang & W. Daelemans ence party choice. Political Communication, 27, 389–405. (Eds.), Proceedings of the Conference on Empirical Methods in Jones, M. N., & Mewhort, D. J. (2007). Representing word meaning Natural Language Processing (pp. 1532–1543). Stroudsburg, and order information in a composite holographic lexicon. Psycho- PA: Association for Computational Linguistics. logical Review, 114, 1–37. Slater, M. D. (2007). Reinforcing spirals: The mutual influence of Jones, M. N., Willits, J. A., & Dennis, S. (2015). Models of semantic media selectivity and media effects and their impact on individual memory. In J. R. Busemeyer & J. T. Townsend (Eds.), Oxford behavior and social identity. Communication Theory, 17, 281–303. handbook of mathematical and computational psychology (pp. Todorov, A., Mandisodza, A. N., Goren, A., & Hall, C. C. (2005). 232–254). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. Inferences of competence from faces predict election outcomes. Judd, C. M., James-Hawkins, L., Yzerbyt, V., & Kashima, Y. (2005). Science, 308, 1623–1626. Fundamental dimensions of social judgment: Understanding the Van Aelst, P., Sheafer, T., & Stanyer, J. (2012). The personalization of relations between judgments of competence and warmth. Journal mediated political communication: A review of concepts, operatio- of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 899–913. nalizations and key findings. Journalism, 13, 203–220. Kahn, K. F. (1992). Does being male help? An investigation of the effects of candidate gender and campaign coverage on evaluations of U.S. senate candidates. The Journal of Politics, 54, 497–517. Author Biographies Landauer, T. K., & Dumais, S. T. (1997). A solution to Plato’s problem: Sudeep Bhatia is an assistant professor of psychology at the Univer- The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, induction, and sity of Pennsylvania. He examines high-level judgment processes representation of knowledge. Psychological Review, 104, 211–240. using computational models. Landauer, T. K., Foltz, P. W., & Laham, D. (1998). An introduction to latent semantic analysis. Discourse Processes, 25, 259–284. Geoffrey P. Goodwin is an associate professor of psychology at the Landy, J. F., Piazza, J., & Goodwin, G. P. (2016). When it’s bad to be University of Pennsylvania who studies thinking, reasoning, social friendly and smart: The desirability of sociability and competence cognition, and moral judgment. depends on morality. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Lukasz Walasek is an assistant professor at the University of War- 42, 1272–1290. wick with interests in social and economic behavior. Leach, C. W., Ellemers, N., & Barreto, M. (2007). Group virtue: The importance of morality (vs. competence and sociability) in the Handling Editor: Jesse Graham
You can also read