Adolescent Girls' Relationships with Mothers and Best Friends

 
CONTINUE READING
Adolescent Girls' Relationships with Mothers
and Best Friends

Leslie A. Gavin
National Jewish Center for Immunology and Respiratory Medicine;
University of Colorado Health Sciences Center

Wyndol Furman
University of Denver

      GAVIN, LESLIE A., and FURMAN, WYNDOL. Adolescent Girls' Relationships with Mothers and Best
      Friends. CHILD DEVELOPMENT, 1996, 67, 375-386. The present study examined factors associated
      with harmony in adolescent girls' relationships with their mothers and their best friends. A
      framework was proposed in which relationship harmony was expected to be related to individual
      characteristics of each partner and the match between the individual characteristics of each
      partner. 60 adolescent girls, their mothers, and their best friends participated in self-report and
      observational tasks. Harmonious mother-daughter partners (vs. disharmonious ones) had more
      similar needs, felt their needs were better met, perceived their partners as more socially skilled,
      and had more similar interests. Harmonious friends (vs. disharmonious ones) had more similar
      needs, and target adolescents perceived partners to be more socially skilled and better at meeting
      their needs. Observational ratings of attunement, positive affect, and power negotiation were
      greater in harmonious relationships with both mothers and friends. Discussion focuses on the
      value of a common framework for studying different relationships.

      Researchers have devoted considerable                The purpose of the present study was
attention to both parent-adolescent relation-         to use a common theoretical framework to
ships and adolescent friendships (see Savin-          identify factors associated with adolescents'
Williams & Berndt, 1990; Steinberg, 1990).            perceptions of harmony in relationships
However, with some notable exceptions                 with mothers and best friends. By harmony,
(Furman & Buhrmester, 1985; Gold &Yanof,              we mean frequent supportive interactions
1985), most investigators have focused on             and infrequent conflictual interactions. The
only one relationship at a time. Moreover,            constructofharmony was of interest because
investigators often employ different con-             it incorporates a wide range of supportive
structs to explore characteristics ofthese two        and conflictual relationship features, can be
kinds of relationships. For example, family           used to examine both family and peer rela-
researchers have extensively examined con-            tionships, and is associated with perceptions
flict between parents and adolescents, but            of adjustment in adolescence (Furman,
we know relatively little about conflict be-          1987).
tween friends. Conversely, more is known
about the role of similarity in friendships           Relationship Harmony
than in parent-adolescent relationships. As a              What factors may be associated with har-
consequence, research on these two rela-              monious relationships? We expected such
tionships has remained relatively isolated            variables to fall into two categories (Furman,
from one another. Often we do not know if             1984). First, having certain individual char-
results pertaining to one relationship are            acteristics, such as social skill, may be asso-
specific to that relationship or may be ap-           ciated with being a good relationship part-
plied to adolescents' relationships in                ner. Second, harmony may stem from
general.                                              similarity between partners' individual char-

           This research was supported by a W. T. Grant Faculty Scholar Award to Wyndol Furman.
     Appreciation is expressed to Lawrence Epstein, Beverly Morris, Judy Pomeranz, and Ken Wyatt
     for their invaluable assistance. We also appreciate the cooperation of the Gherry Creek and
     Englewood School systems. Reprints of this article and copies ofthe measures can be obtained
     from Leslie Gavin, National Jewish Center for Immunology and Respiratory Medicine, K-802,
     1400 Jackson Street, Denver, CO 80206 or Wyndol Furman, Department of Psychology, Univer-
     sity of Denver, Denver, CO 80208.
          [Child Development, 1996,67, 375-386. © 1996 by the Society for Research in Child Development, Inc.
          Alt rights reserved. 0009-3920/96/6702-0025$01.00]
376    Child Development
acteristics, such as interests or perceived        lationships than in relationships with par-
socioemotional needs. In the present study,        ents. Furthermore, it is important not to
we considered both individual characteris-         equate these relationships. For example, we
tics of each partner and similarity of charac-     expected relationship harmony to be related
teristics between partners in our examina-         to how well the adolescents perceived their
tion of harmony in adolescents' relationships      needs were met by each partner, although
with peers and parents.                            the specific needs could vary across parent
                                                   and peer relationships.
     Individual     characteristics.—Although
few studies have specifically examined in-         The Present Study
terpersonal harmony, many variables have                In the present study, adolescent girls'
been related to interpersonal attraction. For      relationships with their mothers and best
example, social skill has received consider;       friends were investigated. For both parent-
able attention as an important variable in'        adolescent and friendship pairs, relationship
friendship formation and maintenance.              harmony was hypothesized to be related to
Buhrmester (1990) found that social compe-         (1) certain individual characteristics of each
tence in peer relationships was associated         partner that lend themselves to maintaining
with greater intimacy in friendships. To           harmonious relationships (i.e., positive so-
date, social skills have not been examined         cial skills and prosocial interpersonal char-
as extensively in parent-adolescent relation-      acteristics, ability to meet partners' needs)
ships, although clinical investigators have        and (2) similarity of certain characteristics of
taught parents and adolescents communica-          the two people (i.e., similarity of interests
tion skills as a way of improving their rela-      and socioemotional needs).
tionships (Robin & Foster, 1989). Other po-
tentially important individual characteristics
include emotional attunement, display of           Method
positive affect, high self-esteem, psychologi-     Subjects
cal mindedness, and ability to manage                   The participants were 60 adolescent
power and jealousy within the relationship.        girls, their mothers, and best female friends.
                                                   The age range for the focal adolescents was
     Similarity of partners' characteris-          15-18 years (mean = 16.5), for friends was
tics.—Similarity of interests and similarity       12-18 (mean = 16.2), and for mothers
of attitudes are also major predictors of inter-   was 34-58 (mean = 43.5). The sample com-
personal attraction (see Hinde, 1979; Huston       prised 57 Caucasian, one African-American,
& Levinger, 1978). Children emphasize the          and two Hispanic females. The participants
importance of shared activities in their con-      were primarily from middle-class urban and
ceptions of friendship (Bigelow, 1977; Fur-        suburban families, with the mean family in-
man & Bierman, 1984), and iu their reasons         come being $30,000-$40,000 per year. Fifty-
for liking friends (Rubin, 1980). Attitudes are    eight percent of the target adolescents' par-
more similar among adolescent friends than         ents were married, 3% were separated, 37%
among those who are not friends, and               were divorced, and 2% ofthe mothers were
friends' attitudes become more similar to          widowed.
one another over time (Kandel, 1978). Al-
though similarity has been shown to be im-         Procedure
portant in friendships, it has not been exam-           Questionnaires were administered to a
ined empirically in parent-adolescent              large pool of subjects to identify adolescents
relationships. Relationship harmony may            with particularly harmonious or disharmoni-
also be related to the degree to which the         ous relationships. The questionnaires were
two partners have similar socioemotional           distributed through local school systems and
needs and the degree to which each partner         through televised public service announce-
 perceives that their needs are being met in       ments. Approximately 450 interested adoles-
 the relationship.                                 cents completed a 16-item short version of
                                                   the Network of Relationship Inventory
     We believe that a common framework            (NRI; Furman & Buhrmester, 1985), which
may be used to understand harmony or dis-          assessed supportive and negative interac-
harmony in relationships with both parents         tions with their mothers and their best fe-
and friends. We generally expected the same        male friends. The adolescent's perception
factors to be associated with harmony in the       of harmony was defined as the degree of
two kinds of relationships, but some differ-       perceived support minus the degree ol
ences may occur. For example, similarity of        perceived negative interaction in that rela-
interests may be more important in peer re-        tionship. Harmonious relationships were
Gavin and Furman            377
 considered to be those with scores at least       and nurturance of the other) and (2) Nega-
 one-half of a standard deviation above the        tive Interactions (conflict, punishment, and
 mean, whereas disharmonious were those at         irritation). Cronbach's alphas of the factors
 least one-half of a standard deviation below      exceeded .90.
 the mean. Although the terms harmonious
 and disharmonious are used to describe                 The NRI was used because it is one of
 these relationships, it should be noted that      the few validated measures available that
 they are defined in terms of their relative       poses parallel questions across relation-
 degree of harmony and not in any absolute         ships, thus providing comparable data. Addi-
 sense of harmony or disharmony.                   tionally, the NRI also taps a totai of 10 differ-
                                                   ent supportive or negative features of
     Using these criteria, we identified equal     relationships, permitting us to examine a rel-
numbers of adolescents who had relatively          atively broad construct. The harmony scores
harmonious relationships with both part-           used for assigning subjects to groups were
ners, relatively disharmonious relationships       calculated by subtracting standardized nega-
with both, relatively harmonious relation-         tive interaction scores from the standardized
ships with mothers and relatively disharmo-        support scores.
nious relationships with friends, and vice
versa. Thus, our screening procedure                    The focal adolescents' and their part-
yielded a group of adolescents with diverse        ners' reports of harmony were highly related
relational experiences with mothers and            (adolescent-friend r — .55; adolescent-
friends.                                           mother r = .61). We chose to use only the
                                                   one rater, the focal adolescent, so that the
     At the time of the actual study, the re*      rater was consistent across relationships.
cruited adolescents again rated their rela-
tionships using the full version of the NRI,            Activities and Interests Inventory.—
and final assignment to the four groups was        This questionnaire was used to assess the
done by median splits on these full harmony        similarity of each set of partners' activities
scale scores. In particular, the 60 adoles-        and interests. Each of the three partici-
cents were assigned to the following groups:       pants was asked how much they enjoy a range
(1) those with relatively harmonious rela-         of 66 activities, including various sports,
tionships with both mothers and friends            artistic hobbies, and recreational and daily
(N = 17), (2) those with relatively disharmo-      activities. The format for each item was a
nious relationships with both {N - 16), (3)        five-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not
those with relatively harmonious relation-         at all) to 5 (extremely much). The similarity
ships with mothers and relatively disharmo-        between the adolescent's and partner's in-
nious relationships with friends (N = 14),         terests was determined by correlating the
and (4) vice versa (N = 13).                       adolescent's ratings with those of each of her
                                                   partners.
     During their visit to the laboratory, ado-
lescents, parents, and friends all completed             Emotional      Needs      Inventory.—The
sets of questionnaires, and adolescent-             Emotional Needs Inventory comprised a list
mother and adolescent-friend dyads were             of 16 social and emotional needs derived
observed interacting. Adolescents and their         from the work of Maslow (1954), Murray
friends were each paid $15.00. Mothers par-         (1938), Sullivan (1953), and Weiss (1974)
ticipated voluntarily.                              Initially, participants were asked to rate how
                                                    important each need was within a particular
Questionnaire Measures                              relationship. Mothers rated the items in
     Network of Relationships         Inventory    terms of tfieir parent-adolescent relation-
(NRI).—The NRI (Furman & Buhrmester,               ship, best friends rated items in terms of
1985) was used to assess the adolescent's          their best friend relationship, and focal ado-
perceptions of supportive and negative in-         lescents did both. Ratings for each item were
teractions with mother and best friend. For        done on a five-point Likert scale, ranging
example, subjects were asked, "How much            from 1 (not at all important) to 5 (extremely
does this person help you when you need to         important). Having rated all the items, par-
get something done?" Ratings were done on          ticipants were asked to choose their three
standard five-point Likert scales, and anchor      most important needs in a particular rela-
points ranged from 1 (little or none) to 5 (the    tionship and rate how well the partner met
most). The NRI includes 10 three-item              those needs. These latter ratings were used
scales that load on two factors: (1) Support       to assess need fulfillment, whereas an index
(affection, admiration, reliable alliance, inti-   of needs similarity was derived by corre-
macy, companionship, instrumental help.            lating the two full sets of needs ratings.
378     Child Development
     Social skills.—All participants were           relation with Spearman-Brown correction
asked to rate how well their partners per-          for proportion with composite ratings).
formed a series of 11 social skills. Some                A principal components analysis with an
items were derived from the Perceived In-           oblique rotation was performed on the 240
terpersonal Competencies Inventory (PICI;           sorts. A nine-factor solution accounting for
Buhrmester, Furman, Wittenberg, & Reis,             51.2% ofthe variance was selected because
1988), and others were created specifically         it provided the most theoretically coherent
for this study. Mothers and friends rated           results (see Table 1 for Q-sort items and fac-
their perceptions of the focal adolescent.          tor loadings).
The focal adolescent rated both the mother
and the best friend. A five-point Likert scale           Factors are labeled as follows: Coopera-
(1 = poor to 5 = extremely good) was em-            tive relationship characteristics (15 items,
ployed. The Cronbach's alpha for the                Cronbaeh's alpha = .92); Psychological
mother, friend, and adolescent ratings              Mindedness (five items, Cronbach's alpha =
ranged from .83 to .88.                             .78); Affect (seven items, Cronbach's alpha
                                                     = .86); Self-Esteem (five items, Cronbach's
Observational Measures                              alpha = .66); Self-Centeredness (four items,
      Tasks.—Adolescent-mother and adoles-          Cronbach's alpha - .60); Problem-Solving
cent-friend dyads were videotaped while             (three items, Cronbach's alpha = .73);
participating in three 8-min interactions. Or-      Power (three items, Cronbach's alpha =
der of dyad was counterbalanced across sub-         .66); Attunement (seven items, Cronbach's
jects. In the first segment, the pair was asked     alpha = .86); and Jealousy (three items.
to plan a week-long vacation. This t^k, de-         Cronbach's alpha = .52).
veloped by Grotevant and Cooper (1983),
was used to elicit cooperation and fun in the       Results
relationship. In the second segment, each
partner identified three problems in their re-      Preliminary Analyses
lationship. They were then asked to agree                Measures of similarity of interests and
upon and discuss the most severe problem,           needs were determined by calculating the
including why it was a problem, feelings            correlations between the adolescent's and
about the problem, and possible solutions.          each partner's scores. Next, preliminary
This task is similar to conflict tasks used in      analyses were performed to investigate
previous studies of adolescents' family inter-      questions of discriminant validity among the
actions (Hauser et al., 1984). The third seg-       self-report measures. These analyses re-
ment was designed to examine parent and             vealed that the internal consistency of the
friend supportiveness. Specifically, dyads          different self-report measures (harmony,
were asked to discuss a problem the adoles-         need fulfillment, and social skills) were
cent was experiencing outside ofthe present         higher than their intercorrelations with one
 relationship.                                      exception. Reports of social skills were
                                                    highly related to reports of need fulfillment
     Observational ratings.—Each individ-           (mean r = .71). Other than this, the mea-
ual in each dyad was coded using the Inter-         sures had satisfactory discriminant validity.
actional Q-sort. The Q-set consisted of 58               Next, 2 x 2 MANOVAs (harmonious/
items which focused on (a) characteristics of       disharmonious relationship with modier x
the individual during the interaction (e.g.,        harmonious/disharmonious best friend rela-
affect and mood) and (b) how the individual         tionship) were condw^ted on the set of
manages the relationship with the partner           scores for the mother-adolescent relation-
(e.g., social skills, ability to mwiage confiict,   ship and those for the adolescent-friend rela-
attunement). Haters sorted the items using a        tionship. The MANOVA of the variables
fixed seven-point distribution, with each           concerning the mother-adolescent relation-
pole representing items that are most charac-       ship revealed a significant effect for har-
teristic ofthe individual being coded.              mony in the mother-adolescent relationship,
    There were three raters, each with ex-          Wilks's lambda = .23, j3 < .01, whereas the
tensive clinical and coding experience. To          effects of hannony in the friendship and the
avoid carry-over effects, a rater observed          interaction of harmony in the two relation-
only one of die two sets of dyadic interac-         ships were not significant. Conversely, the
tions for a particular focal adolescent. A sub-     MANOVA of the friendship variables
set of 24 sorts per pair of coders (48 total)       yielded a significant effect for harmony in
was used to calculate interrater reliability.       the friendship relationship, Wilks's lambda
The interrater reliability was .75 (mean cor-        = .36, p < .05, whereas the eflfects of har-
Gavin and Furman 379
                         TABLE 1                                  mony in the mother-adolescent relationship
                        .,^^,,c :.,. r ^.^,,^.
                        ACTORS AND LOADINGS
                                                                  ^^
V 13
                                     •S a

                    •3-S-S
                                                            $ 3

                     «    (0 CB
                    XXX              a: a:

  X
a
—
: n

in Q

          1 O                 tu     ISl CO       IO   to
                                                  (

                                                  (
                                                  (
                              83 (

                                                  {

          1 ^-^               00
                     o   o
                                                  88
                                                  46
                                                  92
                                                  44

                                     t-
                     --. !a

          i 1—1
                                                            92 (
                                                            03 (
                                           50 (
                     29 (
                     47 {
                     94 (
            49 (

                                                       ID
                                                  X

                   "So*'*'
                                           and

                                      u
                   M -3 S %           C3
                      * ' £           1-
                                           -a
       -^S ?.                        •5 £
                                      o
Gavin and Furman            381
with harmonious relationships had more             in both relationships displayed more posi-
similar needs than those in disharmonious          tive affect than adolescents with harmonious
friendships, F(i, 59) = 7.05, p < .01.             relationships with mothers and disharmoni-
                                                   ous friendships; scores for the other two
Reported Social Skills                             cells fell in between. The main effect for
     As reported in Table 2, adolescents who       jealousy was also moderated by a similar in-
reported harmonious relationships with             teraction effect, F(l, 59) = 5.75, p < .05.
their mothers were rated as more socially          Again, adolescents who were satisfied with
skilled by their mothers and in turn rated         both relationships displayed less jealousy
their mothers as more socially skilled, F(l,       with their friends than girls with harmonious
59) = 9.02, p < .01, F(l, 59) = 35.99, p <         relationships with mothers and disharmoni-
.01. Similarly, adolescents who had harmo-         ous relationships with friends.
nious friendships rated their friends as more
socially skillfril, F(l, 59) = 17.18, p < .01.          Analyses of friends' interpersonal char-
Friends' ratings of the adolescent's social        acteristics as displayed with the focal adoles-
skill did not differ as a function of the har-     cents revealed the following. Compared to
mony ratings.                                      those in disharmonious friendships, friends
                                                   in harmonious relationships had higher rat-
Observational Ratings of Interpersonal             ings on cooperative social skills, F{1, 59) =
Characteristics                                    7.29, p < .01, display of positive affect, F(l,
     As described in the "Methods" section,        59) = 4.18, p < .05, ability to not be self-
the 58 Q-sort items were subjected to a prin-      centered, F(l, 59) = 7.25, p < .01, ability to
cipal components analysis. Using the nine          negotiate power in the relationship, F(l, 59)
factors that emerged from this analysis, 2 x        = 6.53, p < .01, attunement, F(l, 59) = 6.22,
2 ANOVAs were carried out for each factor          p < .05, and lack of jealousy, F( 1, 59) = 5.04,
to determine whether certain interpersonal         p < .05. The main effect for lack of jealousy
characteristics differed as a function of the      was moderated, however, by an interaction
adolescent's reports of relationship harmony       effect, F(l, 59) = 5.28, p < .05. FoUow-up
with mother and friend (see Table 3). Analy-       tests indicated that girls with harmonious re-
ses ofthe ratings of interactions with moth-       lationships with mothers and friends had
ers revealed that adolescents with more hai-       friends who were significantly less jealous
monious       relationships   with    mothers      than girls with harmonious relationships
displayed more cooperative relationship            with mothers and disharmonious ones with
skills, F(l, 59) = 17.37, p < .01, more posi-      friends; scores for the other two cells fell in
tive affect, F(l, 59) = 7.81, p < .01, more        between. Once again, this interaction and
attunement to mother, F(l, 59) = 9.58, p <         those described in the preceding paragraph
.01, and better ability to negotiate power,        should be interpreted cautiously as the in-
F(l, 59) - 4.04, p < .05.                          teraction effect was not significant in the
                                                   MANOVA.
     Similarly, mothers who had harmonious
relationships with their adolescents re-
ceived higher ratings on the following vari-       Discussion
ables: cooperative relationship skills, F(l,            Much ofthe literature on adolescent re-
59) = 7.61, p < .01, display of positive affect,   lationships has focused on understanding
F(l, 59) = 4.10, p < .05, ability to problem       normative developmental changes in adoles-
solve, F(l, 59) - 4.95, p < .05, ability to        cents' relationships with parents and
negotiate power with daughter, F(l, 59) --         friends. In contrast, the present study exam-
4.88, p < .05, and attunement with daughter,       ined individual differences in these relation-
F(l, 59) = 7.43, p < .01. Analyses of the          ships. In particular, we tried to determine
interactions with friends revealed that ado-       whether a common framework of variables
lescents with harmonious friendships dis-          could be used to understand variations in
played more positive affect, F(l, 59) = 7.37,      the degree of harmony in adolescents' rela-
p < .01, better ability to share power in the      tionships with mothers and best friends. We
relationship, F(l, 59) = 7.37, p < .01, and        found that both individual characteristics
less jealousy, F(l, 59) = 7.25, p < .01. The       and the match of individual characteristics
main effect for the adolescent's display of        were important correlates of relationship
positive affect, however, was moderated by         harmony. Our findings are strengthened by
a significant interaction between harmony          our reliance on multiple methods and multi-
with friend and harmony with mother, F(l,          ple agents, as the inclusion of observational
59) = 5.62, p < .05. Newman-Keuls tests in-        techniques helps rule out the possibility of
dicated that adolescents perceiving harmony        shared method variance accounting for all
mothe

                                 mothe
                                 mothe
                                 mothe
      mothe
      mothe
                   1
      •5-S               •B      -S-B-B
      $ • $
                   '? s           % $$

                         ony
                   ony
                                  oc a
                                     o ao
      >.      >.

      gg

                                       a :

                                       IS

382
c
                                            o      so
                                                   o so
                                                      e

           CO •* O 10 -* CO

           5?-Hrtt- -^ -H
           00 _^ ^ CD oq _^

in CO Q                        ^* ^& ^* CO C4 ''d* t ^ O O^
"^ °? 'O   COl^ t-;
^ CO CO    CO "^ CO "^ CO CO   COCOCOCO^COCOCOCO

             a^ t * t~
384    Child Development
associations. Moreover, many ofthe signifi-       and remain in authority while giving the ad-
cant questionnaire results were based on          olescent an increasing amount of autonomy
correlations ofthe adolescent's and her part-     and control that is most important. Differ-
ners' responses, and thus were multimethod        entiating the exact nature of these distinc-
in themselves.                                    tions requires further exploration.
     The common framework helped iden-                 Moreover, it is also important to empha-
tify similarities and differences in predictors   size that some results were different for the
of harmony across mother-adolescent and           two relationships. Unlike harmonious
friendship relationships. Harmony in both         mother-daughter relationships, harmonious
relationships was predicted by partners' per-     friendships did not have higher levels of
ceptions that the other person was meeting        common interests and activities. These re-
their socioemotional needs, and by the de-        sults may reflect the fact that all best friends
gree of similarity of their needs. Regarding      have common interests by virtue of being
observed interactional characteristics, har-      adolescents and having chosen each other as
mony in both relationships was related to         friends (Ball, 1981; Kandel, 1978). In fact,
partners displaying positive affect and           the degree of similarity in interests in all
attunement, and being able to share power         four groups of adolescents tended to be
appropriately. Due to the cross-sectional na-     rather high (adolescent-friend interests,
ture of the present study, the causal direc-      mean r = .47) and are higher than the re-
tions ofthese and other relations remain un-      ports of similarity in interests of mother and
clear. For example, need fulfillment may          daughter (mean r = .26).
facilitate harmony and/or vice versa.
                                                       Differences also occurred in the links
     Although much of the literature on ado-      between harmony and social skills in the
lescence emphasizes differences between           two relationships. Both the mother's and
parent and peer relationships, the present        daughter's ability to use cooperative social
results offer a contribution to the literature    skills with one another were related to per-
by demonstrating that many of the findings        ceived relationship harmony. This result
are not specific to one particular relation-      was a robust one, being found in both the
ship. In fact, we suggest that explanations of    self-report and the observational data. The
these findings should not be based on the         social skills data within \he friendship rela-
special properties of one particular kind of      tionship were less consistent. In ttie self-
relationship but, instead, should be based on     report data, the adolescent's report of the
the common processes underlying friend-           friends' social skills differentiated harmoni-
ships, mother-adolescent relationships, and       ous and disharmonious dyads, but the
perhaps other close relationships. For exam-      friend's report ofthe adolescent's skills did
ple, one could argue that positive affect is      not. In the observational data, ratings ofthe
associated with friendship harmony because        friends' cooperative social skills differenti-
positive affective exchanges foster close ties    ated harmonious and disharmonious dyads,
between equals. However, the fact that posi-      but ratings of the focal adolescents did not.
tive affect was also related to harmony in        Although this inconsistency could indicate
mother-adolescent relationships indicates         that social skills are less important within
that such positive affective exchanges play a     the friendship domain, this explanation is
critical role in asymmetrical (parental) as       implausible in light of other research (Asher
well as egalitarian (friendship) relationships.   &Renshaw, 1981; Buhrmester, 1990). It may
In fact, expression of positive affect may be     be that social skill and friendship harmony
a fundamental correlate of harmony across         are not related in a linear fashion; instead,
many different kinds of relationships.             it may only be important to have some mini-
                                                   mal level of soeial skill in order to have rela-
     Although some of the processes within        tively harmonious friendships.
these two relationships are similar, the vari-
ables may not be operationalized in the                Alternatively, the social skills important
same way in asymmetrical versus egalitarian       in an adolescent friendship may not be
relationships. For example, although power        readily perceived by an outside observer or
management is important in both, one would        may not have been tapped in the structured
expect friends to treat each other as equals,     interactions employed in this study. Infor-
whereas equality of power between parent          mal observations made during the study sug-
and adolescent is probably not expected by        gested that adolescent girls' overt behaviors
either partner. Instead, it may be the par-       and how they behaved socially were not al-
ent's ability to negotiate with the adolescent    ways concordant with their reports of har-
Gavin and Furman             385
mony in the friendship. There appeared to         though our recruiting strategy was designed
be some pressures within the friendship in-       to include a wide range of relationships
teractions to "be nice" and not confront con-     within a nonclinical sample. Third, the work
flict directly, whereas in the mother-            is cross-sectional, thereby preventing us
daughter interactions, conflict was discussed     from specifying the direction of effects be-
openly. Whether or not the adolescents were       tween perceptions of harmony and the vari-
consciously aware of any difficulties in their    ous factors examined. Longitudinal work
friendships, or whether they intentionally        would also help us understand the nature of
disguised difficulties is an interesting ques-    the links between parent-child relationships
tion for interactional researchers that de-       and peer relationships. One should also
serves further investigation.                     not misinterpret the absence of cross-
                                                  relationship effects (e.g., the lack of differ-
     Finally, several observational variables     ences in the friendships of harmonious and
were only associated with reports of har-         disharmonious mother-daughter dyads). The
mony in one of the two relationships. For         inclusion of a relatively equal number of ad-
example, problem-solving ability was re-          olescents in the four cells has the statistical
lated to relationship harmony with mother,        effect of essentially eliminating any relation
but not friends. Compared to peer dyads,          between harmony in mother-daughter dyads
mothers and daughters are likely to have          and harmony in friendships.
more experience and spend more energy
discussing problems and working out day-to-            One interesting aspect of this study is
day hassles, perhaps making it a more impor-      the inclusion of girls with relatively dishar-
tant skill within this domain. In contrast, the   monious best friendships. Although it was
management of jealousy was not related to         beyond the scope ofthe present study to in-
harmony within the mother-daughter rela-          vestigate this group in depth, it represents
tionship, but it was in the adolescent friend-    an interesting subpopulation of girls who
ship. Girls who saw both relationships as         choose to remain in what they acknowledge
harmonious were significantly less jealous        are relatively conflictual, unsupportive best
and had friends who displayed less jealousy       friendships. It would be interesting to study
than girls who only saw their relationships       girls with this pattern of peer relationships
with their mothers to be harmonious. These        more closely.
latter girls may be less secure socially, per-
haps because they are more identified with             These limitations notwithstanding, pres-
adults than with peers. The fact that this        ent results underscore the importance of look-
group of girls was also the least able to dis-    ing at relationships in the broad social con-
play positive affect and have fun with            text and with multiple methods. It is our hope
friends may reflect a pseudo-adult quality        that the framework presented in this study
that may be less attractive to peers.             will extend the current literature on adoles-
                                                  cent relationships by providing one way to
     More generally, fewer variables were         understand harmony in a range of different
significantly related to perceptions of friend-   relationships.
ship harmony than to perceptions of mother-
daughter harmony. This difference was par-
ticularly true for the measures completed by
the friend and mother. Perhaps mothers are        References
more accurate or insightful reporters be-         Asher, S. R., & Renshaw, P. D. (1981). Children
cause they have known their daughters                  without friends: Social knowledge and social
longer and more intimately. In addition,               skill training. In S. R. Asher & J. M. Gottman
friends may show a positivity bias. Girls may          (Eds.), The developm,ent of children's friend-
be less likely to talk about the negative as-          ships (pp. 273-296), New York: Cambridge
pects of their relationships, due to discom-           University Press.
fort about confiict or a desire to avoid awk-     Ball, S. J. (1981). Beachside comprehensive. Cam-
wardness.                                              bridge: Cambridge University Press.
                                                  Blgelow, B. J. (1977). Children's friendship expec-
Limitations and Future Directions                     tations; A cognitive developmental study,
     First, one must use caution in generaliz-        Child Development, 48, 246-253.
ing from this sample of 60 primarily Gauca-       Buhrmester, D. (1990). Intimacy of friendship, in-
sian, middle-class, adolescent girls and their        terpersonal competence, and adjustment dur-
mothers. Similarly, the sample contained              ing preadolescence and adolescence. Child
few, if any, relationships that could be char-        Development, G\, 1101-1111.
acterized as disturbed in a clinical sense, al-   Buhrmester, D., Furman, W., Wittenberg, M., &
386      CUId Development
    Reis, H. (1988). Five domains of interpersonal     Hinde, R. A. (1979). Towards understanding rela-
    competence in peer relations. Journal of               tionships. New York: Academic.
    Personality and Social Psychology, 55,             Huston, T, L., & Levinger, G. (1978). Interper-
    991-1008.                                              sonal attraction and relationships. Annual Re-
Furman, W. (1984). Issues in the assessment of             view of Psychology, 29,115-156.
    social skills of nonnal and handicapped chil-      Kandel, D. (1978). Similarity in real-life adoles-
    dren. In T. Field, M. Siegal, & J. Roopnarine          cent friendship pairs. Journal of Personality
    (Eds.), Friendships of normal and handi-               and Social Psychology, M, 306-312.
    capped children (pp. 3-30). New York: Ablex.       Maslow, A. (1954). Motivation and personality.
Furman, W. (1987). Social support, stress, and ad-         New York: Harper.
   justment in adolescence. Paper presented at         Murray, H. A. (1938). Explorations in personality.
    the Meeting of the Society for Research in             New York: Oxford.
    Child Development, Baltimore.                      Robin, A. L., & Foster, S. L. (1989). Negotiating
Furman, W., & Bierman, K. L. (1984). Children's            parent-adolescent conflict. New York: Guil-
    conceptions of friendship: A multimethod               ford.
    study of developmental changes. Develop-           Rubin, Z. (1980). Children's friendships. Cam-
    mental Psychology, 20, 925-933.                        bridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Furman, W., & Buhrmester, D. (1985). Children's        Savin-Williams, R. C , & Bemdt, T. J. (1990).
    perceptions of the personal relationship in            Friendship and relationships. In S. S. Feld-
    their social networks. Developmental Psy-              man & G. R. Elliott (Eds.), A* the threshold:
    chology, 21, 1016-1024.                                The developing adolescent (pp. 277-307).
Gold, M., 6E Yanof, D. S. (1985). Mothers, daugh-          Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
    ters, and girlfriends. Journal of Personality      Steinberg, L. (1990). Autonomy, conflict and har-
    and Social Psychology, 49, 654-659.                    mony in the family relationship. In S. S. Feld-
Grotevant, H. D., & Cooper, C. R. (1983). New              man & G. R. Elliott (Eds.), At the threshold:
    directions for child development: Adolescent           The developing adolescent (pp. 255-276).
    development in the family. San Francisco;              Cambridge, MA; Harvard University Press.
    Jossey-Bass.                                       Sullivan, H. S. (1953). The interpersonal theory
Hauser, S. T., Powers, S. I., Noam, F. F., jacobson,       of psychiatry. New York: Norton.
    A., Weiss, B., & Follansbee, D. J. (1984). Fa-     Weiss, R. (1974). Tlie provisions of social relation-
    milial context of adolescent ego development.          ships. In Z. Rubin (Ed.), Doing unto others.
    Child Development, 55, 195-213.                        Englewood Cliffs, NJ; Prentice Hall,
You can also read