The Continuum from Temperament to Mental Illness: Dynamical Perspectives
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
Review Article Neuropsychobiology Received: February 13, 2020 Accepted: June 19, 2020 DOI: 10.1159/000509572 Published online: August 27, 2020 The Continuum from Temperament to Mental Illness: Dynamical Perspectives William Sulis Department of Psychiatry and Behavioural Neuroscience/McMaster University, Hamilton, ON, Canada Keywords characteristics that present considerable challenges for cur- Temperament · Mental illness · Continuum · Dynamics · rent dynamical systems approaches: transience, contextual- Time series · Dynamical phases · Phase transitions · Process ity and emergence. This review discusses the need for time algebra series data and the implications of these three characteristics on the formal study of the continuum and presents a dy- namical systems model based upon Whitehead’s Process Abstract Theory and the neurochemical Functional Ensemble of Tem- Temperament in healthy individuals and mental illness have perament model. The continuum can be understood as sec- been conjectured to lie along a continuum of neurobehav- ond or higher order dynamical phases in a multiscale land- ioral regulation. This continuum is frequently regarded in di- scape of superposed dynamical systems. Markers are sought mensional terms, with temperament and mental illness lying to distinguish the order parameters associated with these at opposite poles along various dimensional descriptors. phases and the control parameters which describe transi- However, temperament and mental illness are quintessen- tions among these dynamics. © 2020 S. Karger AG, Basel tially dynamical phenomena, and as such there is value in examining what insights can be arrived at through the lens of our current understanding of dynamical systems. The for- mal study of dynamical systems has led to the development Introduction of a host of markers which serve to characterize and classify dynamical systems and which could be used to study tem- Existing approaches to the classification of mental ill- perament and mental illness. The most useful markers for ness have come under considerable criticism of late, and temperament and mental illness apply to time series data there is an urgency to identify biomarkers of mental ill- and include geometrical markers such as (strange) attractors ness that will facilitate diagnosis and individualized treat- and repellors and analytical markers such as fluctuation ment. Temperament of healthy individuals and mental spectroscopy, scaling, entropy, recurrence time. Tempera- illness have been conjectured to lie along a continuum of ment and mental illness, however, possess fundamental neurobehavioral regulation [1–5]. Temperament refers © 2020 S. Karger AG, Basel William Sulis Department of Psychiatry and Behavioural Neuroscience Collective Intelligence Laboratory, McMaster University karger@karger.com 255 Townline Rd E, Cayuga, ON N0A 1E0 (Canada) www.karger.com/nps sulisw @ mcmaster.ca
to the biological basis of consistent patterns of normative ample, psychomotor retardation has been shown to be neurobehavioral regulation. Mental illness, in contrast, regulated mostly by dopaminergic systems [15] which re- refers to dysfunctional or pathological patterns of neu- ceive modulatory influences from mu-opioid receptor robehavioral regulation, and in both classes of behavioral [16, 17] and delta-opioid receptor systems [18, 19]. Close- patterns neurochemistry plays a crucial role. ly related, fatigue has been linked to serotonin systems Early attempts to explore this possible continuum be- which are major regulators of the energetic maintenance tween temperament and mental illness focused on estab- of behavior [10, 11]. Long held to play a role in depression lishing correlations between individual temperament [20], the serotonin system has been implicated in the en- traits and the presence or absence of (symptoms of) men- durance aspects of activities [21–23]. Impulsivity appears tal illness. A search for causal relationships involves an linked to interactions between delta opioid systems and examination of the underlying neurobehavioral regula- dopamine and mu opioid systems [18, 19, 24, 25]. Dys- tory systems (NBRS), and in particular the neurochemi- phoria shows links to dysregulation in the mu-opioid re- cal regulatory systems (NCRS) upon which the phenom- ceptor systems [16, 26]. Anxiety appears also to be linked ena of temperament and mental illness are thought to su- to impaired regulation of kappa opioid receptor systems pervene. The NCRS include monoamine, acetylcholine, by mu opioid receptor systems and arises due to their ef- neuropeptide and opioid receptor systems. fect upon GABA [26, 27], noradrenaline release and HPA The most detailed neurochemical model describing axis arousal [27–34]. the contribution of NCRS to consistent behavioral pat- Since temperament and some psychopathology ap- terns is the Functional Ensemble of Temperament (FET) pear to share NCRS, a model of temperament that is sen- model. The FET summarizes experimental studies in sitive to changes in NCRS should show differential effects neurochemistry and psychopharmacology that have in the presence of mental illness if the continuum hypoth- demonstrated a central role for the NCRS in determining esis is valid [5, 35]. In fact, the FET has shown differential those consistent behavioral patterns known as tempera- effects in the presence of several categories of mental ill- ment traits [6–11]. It describes 12 traits, 9 traits classified ness (depression, anxiety, personality disorders) with by dynamical features (endurance, variability, orienta- much more differential power than lexical approach tion) and by domain (intellectual, physical-motor, social- models (such as Big Five) or Positive/Negative Affect verbal), together with 3 emotionality traits (self-confi- models [4, 35–37]. Temperament profiles obtained using dence, neuroticism, impulsivity). Every trait within the the FET-related Structure of Temperament Question- FET model is associated, not with a single neurotransmit- naire 77 (STQ-77) [9, 38–41] using samples of patients ter system, but with a specific, distinct ensemble of NCRS. suffering from specific mental illnesses showed distinct A useful metaphor for conceptualizing the continuum patterns in the presence of: Major Depression (lower between temperament and mental illness comes from scores on physical/motor traits) [4, 37], Generalized Anx- meteorology, a field in which many modern concepts in iety Disorder (lower scores on social/verbal traits) [4, 36], nonlinear and complex systems dynamics originated and Comorbid Depression and Anxiety (significantly lower which deals with dynamics across multiple time scales: scores on cortical traits) [4, 35] and a range of other dis- very short, short and long duration. Behavior per se oc- orders [38]. curs at very short time scales, resembling individual in- The FET model is notable for describing traits that re- stances of weather. Temperament and chronic mental ill- fer to the dynamics of behavior, particularly endurance ness both describe long-term patterns of behaviors. In and variability, which may account for its sensitivity to that they resemble climate, which also describes long- alterations in the dynamics of the NCRS. The effects men- term dynamical patterns. Acute mental illness resembles tioned above are intriguing and lend support to the hy- frontal weather and storms, in that they all represent pothesized continuum between temperament and mental short duration pathological or outlier dynamical pat- illness. However, although mental illness does appear to terns. The transition from temperament, which is norma- impact on temperament in a differential manner, the al- tive, to mental illness, which is pathological, is analogous tered temperament scores are still within the normative to climate change. range. A simplistic dimensional interpretation of the con- Multiple temperament traits such as impulsivity, sen- tinuum is not supported. Missing are specific biomarkers sation seeking, neuroticism, endurance, plasticity, socia- for pathological states that could be used to determine bility have been linked to NCRS [6–14]. Likewise, several whether or not an individual is functioning within nor- DSM symptoms have been linked to these NCRS. For ex- mative, at-risk, or pathological regimes [5, 42, 43], as well 2 Neuropsychobiology Sulis DOI: 10.1159/000509572
as recognition of the different effects on the dynamics of and bipolar subjects. There are striking differences in behavior which occur in the expression of temperament time series between bipolar and control subjects in terms and mental illness. The goal of the dynamical approach of the size and frequency of fluctuations, the spectral described herein is to identify dynamical biomarkers structure of fluctuations, the entropy, complexity and de- whose presence or absence provides information about gree of persistence. Qualitatively, the time series of nor- the dynamics of these NCRS and which can then be used mal controls look much like white noise, while those of to determine whether an individual’s functioning is nor- clinical subjects show periods with prolonged dwell times, mative, at-risk, or pathological [5, 42, 43]. recurrence and persistence, intermittency, slow wave and long wave patterning. These dynamical features clearly distinguish the two sets of subjects, but are not addressed Dynamics and Time Series with commonly used assessment tools, which are mostly self-report questionnaires referencing either single points Cross-disciplinary research inevitably encounters dif- in time or the subjective estimation of averages. It is un- ferences in the use of terminology. In biology, the term likely that these dynamical features can be assessed by an behavior generally refers to the motor activities of an or- individual subject, let alone represented by means of a ganism. In dynamical systems theory, the term behavior simple Likert scale. It is now possible to acquire real time refers simply to whatever it is that a dynamical system data about behavior and mood using personal monitor- does over time, regardless of the nature of its individual ing devices such as Fit Bits and smartphone apps. How- states. The study of complex systems which consist of ever, the effectiveness of such time series depends upon multiple subsystems extending across multiple spatio- the models developed to explain the time series and the temporal scales and layers of organization inevitably fac- analytical tools used to examine them. es the problem that various disciplines have laid claim to Formally, a dynamical system consists of a space X of these different subsystems and developed their own ter- states, a space T of times and a function f, the dynamic, minologies and methodologies. Dynamical systems theo- which maps from X x T to X. The state f(x,t) represents ry offers a generic language describing temporal change the state of the system after a duration t and which began within any domain – motor, sensory, cognitive, affective, in the state x. A time series represents the succession of electrophysiological, neurochemical. The same language states of a dynamical system over discrete times (e.g., can thus be used across multiple spatio-temporal scales f(x,t0), f(x,t1),f(x,t2),f(x,t3),…) [44–48]. A time series may and across multiple neural systems. In what follows, the also be formed from measurements on states. Since be- term behavior should be understood in this generic sense. haviors are sets of behavioral acts, what is truly needed is A behavioral act is by definition dynamical in nature, a dynamics of sets [49, 50], but this is not yet available, the referring to a pattern in time of states (which could be standard approaches dealing only with trajectories of in- physical or psychological) of an individual (referenced to dividual states. a specific context), having finite duration and satisfying The formal study of dynamical systems seeks out sym- some ecological or functional criteria. Behavior refers to metries and universal characteristics of dynamics in gen- a set of individual behavioral acts which possess the same eral. The classification of trajectories forms the basis for ecological and functional properties. Temperament and the geometrical approach to dynamical systems. Trajecto- mental illness do not refer to specific behaviors (as broad- ries may be described in terms of their final destination, ly conceived here) but rather to distinct patterns of behav- their attractors. The most common attractors are (1) fixed ior which persist over extended periods of time and im- point, (2) periodic, (3) quasiperiodic, (4) strange (chaot- pact on the functional capacity of the individual. Tem- ic). Trajectories which do not trend towards an attractor perament and mental illness refer to second (or higher) may be divergent, transient, recurrent or ergodic. order dynamics (patterns of patterns), which determine The analytical approach has been effectively applied to the temporal patterning of behaviors, which are in turn the analysis of time series data [46–48]. Analytical tools, generated by the lower level dynamics of NCRS (NBRS currently in widespread use and suitable for studying be- more generally) responsible for determining individual havioral data, include: (1) fluctuation spectroscopy, (2) behavioral acts. fluctuation distributions (these tend to be power law rath- The pioneering work of Gottschalk et al. [44] demon- er than Gaussian), (3) Lempel-Ziv complexity, (4) Hurst strated the existence of these second order dynamical pat- exponent, (5) Lyapunov exponent, (6) Shannon and Re- terns in their seminal study of mood variation in normal nyi entropy, (7) Ap entropy, (8) mutual information, (9) Temperament-Illness Continuum Neuropsychobiology 3 DOI: 10.1159/000509572
recurrence and dwell times, (10) correlation, Hausdorff tween them in terms of spatial entropy and largest Lyapu- and Box dimensions, (11) scaling, (12) order parameters nov exponent. Their model focuses on dynamical pat- [41, 42]. terns in behavior as providing the essential differentiation Time series methods have been applied in neurosci- between disorders. ence [48, 51] and have been applied to animal behavioral Heart rate variability is an easily accessible physiolog- data with considerable success [52]. ical measure that can sometimes serve as a proxy for a true Since Gottschalk et al. [44], time series methods are order parameter distinguishing between normative and being used more frequently in the study of bipolar disor- pathological states. Goldberger showed that important der [53–59] and also in schizophrenia [57], obsessive information about physiological functioning can be compulsive disorder [52], generalized anxiety disorder found by examining the complexity of time series data. In [60–62] and social stress [63]. A few studies have em- particular, he showed that the complexity of the R-R in- ployed time series methods in the study of temperament, terval in the ECG is altered during senescence and the personality [53, 54] and monoamine systems [63–65]. transition into pathological states [72]. Fiskum et al. [62] Time series data have been derived from measures of examined time series of heart rate variability in children heart rate variability [62], EEG signals [57], mood ratings and found correlations between sample entropy and in- [56] and levels of various neurotransmitters and their me- ternalizing psychopathology and negative affect, and be- tabolites [64]. A wide variety of analytical tools have been tween detrended fluctuation analysis and internalizing employed: wavelet analysis [65], entropy [56, 60, 62], de- psychopathology. Bornas et al. [60] examined heart rate trended fluctuation analysis [62], fluctuation distribu- variability in high and low anxious adolescents and found tions [58], scaling [61]. Time series have been used as a that the fractal dimension and entropy were lower in the basis for forecasting future mood states [59]. There have high-anxiety group. This is similar to the cardiac results also been some early attempts to take into account the where complexity declined in the context of illness. Heart role of environmental context [66]. rate variability thus serves as a marker for distinguishing The use of time series methods has matured over time, between different states of dynamics. engendering quite sophisticated technical debates around There has been some work to develop formal models data collection and analytical methodologies. The inter- of various NCRS, particularly dopamine and serotonin ested reader is directed to the literature for details. The [63–79]. Most studies have focused on the dynamics of studies mentioned here should be viewed as illustrative the metabolism of the neurotransmitter within a neuron examples of what is possible, even if imperfect, and not as or at a single receptor [73–78]. Stratmann et al. [79] stud- providing any “final answers.” ied the functional role played by monoamines within the The use of time series methods in the study of bipolar CNS, though only for locomotor systems. They found disorder has been quite promising. Ortiz et al. [56] showed that serotonergic systems scale ionotropic signals, en- that first-degree relatives of patients with bipolar disorder abling fast responses to changing contexts. There have had entropies of mood and energy time series that were been a number of papers exploring models of psychopa- more similar to those of the patients than the controls. thology at the symptom level [80–85]. These use a wider Nonlinear analyses have shown that correlations within variety of methods, including neural network modeling mood time series tend to be very short range. Mehraei and [80] and general systems theory [84]. Some papers have Akcay [67] were able to predict future mood based on a explored the personality level in the context of cocaine current time series of mood ratings as have other groups addiction [86] as well as normative function [87]. There [53, 59]. The work of Bonsall et al. [68, 69] is particularly have also been models that have explicitly incorporated interesting as they have not only demonstrated some im- ideas from dynamical systems theory, nonlinear dynam- portant aspects of the dynamics (nonlinearity, identifica- ics and chaos theory [40, 64–67, 87–90]. tion of regions or stability/instability, separation of mood and noisy dynamics) but have also shown the usefulness of time series methods in guiding and measuring out- Challenging Dynamical Features of NBRS comes in treatment [70]. Mandell and Selz [71] applied nonlinear dynamical Understanding the continuum between temperament times series methods to the analysis of time series data and mental illness requires bridging the explanatory gap from subjects with either borderline or obsessive compul- between the behavioral level and the level of the NCRS sive personality disorders. They found differences be- upon which psychological phenomena supervene. The 4 Neuropsychobiology Sulis DOI: 10.1159/000509572
use of dynamical systems theories in this endeavor faces back upon default patterns, but context can induce shifts a formidable challenge due to the presence of three strik- in these patterns. Context includes immediately past ex- ing characteristics of complex adaptive systems which perience, present conditions and future expectations. Or- must be considered in any realistic model. These are tran- ganisms do not merely react to their environments but sience, contextuality and emergence. also to internally generated predictions and expectations concerning those environments [98]. Transience of NBRS Generators The NCRS underlying temperament and mental ill- Behavioral acts are not merely transient in duration, ness, particularly the monoamine systems, are known to they are transient in their expression. This was demonstrat- have complex feedback relationships among themselves ed experimentally by Bernstein [91] who showed that spe- and cannot be considered to be statistically independent cific behavioral acts are not constructed in a stereotypical of one another [5, 10, 11, 79, 84, 92, 93]. Correlations manner, endlessly repeated like a film clip, but are instead among these systems may be time and context dependent, constructed anew each and every time they are expressed local or global [5, 51, 92, 93, 99]. Correlational and di- by an organism. Different neurons, different neural path- mensional analyses are likely to miss these subtleties alto- ways enter into each construction of the behavioral act. gether. Classical probability and statistics are framed This constructive nature of neurobiological regulation led within a single context and thus often misapplied when to the development of the functional constructivism ap- multiple contexts are in play. A significant advance in this proach in several biopsychological disciplines [92]. Trofi- area is the contextual probability theory of Khrennikov mova [92, 93] has proposed several process algebra-based [100] which is being applied to problems in psychology, formalisms for describing the principles of transience economics and biology. which govern processes in functional constructivism. Her approach to process algebra uses several functional differ- Emergence of NBRS Actions entiation classes, a concept of “performance” and several Emergence refers to dynamics exhibited at a higher universal process trends. From this perspective, there is no level that is not a direct consequence of the lower level one-one correspondence between behavioral acts and the dynamics. Emergence demands a multilevel, multiscale dynamics of lower level entities. Instead, many different approach to theory building, modeling and analysis. The processes give rise to similar sets of behavioral acts, so that whole is quite often greater than the sum of the parts. For there is no simple correspondence by means of which one example, studies of nest emigration in Temnothorax albi- can formally move up or down the hierarchy of dynamical pennis [101, 102] have shown that while an individual systems of the organism. In many respects, a behavioral act worker’s decision making may be subject to the non-ra- is like a play, which can be performed with many different tional decoy effect, the decision making of the colony as actors or many different stages. a whole is protected against this effect, remaining rational This form of transience (sometimes referred to as in the absence of any central authority. metastability) has been observed in the place cell system The hippocampal place cell system is also emergent, in the rat hippocampus. The correspondence between in- being not an inherent property of individual hippocam- dividual place cells and actual spatial locations may pal neurons, but rather of the brain-body as a whole [94, change over time, in spite of the animal being able to ac- 95]. So too is long term memory retrieval, a process of curately perform the same spatially dependent tasks [94, reconstruction “on the fly” which is created on demand, 95]. A similar phenomenon has been observed in hippo- emergently, with different top-down and bottom-up campal regions involved in long-term memory, where re- components contributing each time [96]. gions of supporting neurons appear to change over time, migrating along the hippocampus [96]. Dynamical Phase Transitions Contextuality of NBRS Activities Phenomena such as temperament and mental illness, In addition to the considerable challenges involved in which are thought to be enduring features of behavior, trying to access the dynamics of the NCRS directly in hu- whether normative or pathological, are also contextual. mans, transience, contextuality and emergence present Jung [97] noted that the expression of certainly personal- additional complexities that must be addressed both ex- ity traits was contextual. Although the entire behavioral perimentally and theoretically. Contextuality implies repertoire is accessible to any individual, each tends to fall non-stationary time series, which must be subdivided Temperament-Illness Continuum Neuropsychobiology 5 DOI: 10.1159/000509572
into short duration transients, each defined by local con- The idea of dynamical phases and their transitions have text. Emergence, coupled with transience, imply that like- been applied to neurodynamics [107, 108] (where phase is ly there is no simple relationship between the dynamics often taken literally to mean phase-locked synchroniza- of the NBRS and that of the behaviors that they generate. tion of a group of neurons) but only recently have they Faced with such complexity, physicists have sought been applied to the study of large-scale behavior. In the out effective approaches targeting robust, global and if context of neural systems, dynamical phases are frequent- possible, universal features of the system under study so ly recognized as short duration, synchronized bursts of that phenomena can be studied in an approximate (or ef- neuronal activity, which may be localized or distributed. fective) manner at a single level, with the effect of higher Phase transitions are marked by spatially and temporally and lower levels on the dynamics modeled as perturba- distributed phase gradients, “phase cones.” The identifica- tions or fluctuations [103]. Transitional behavior is tion of dynamical phases with phase-locking works at the sought in the distribution of fluctuations and rare events level of neurons, but more complex and subtle forms of [104]. Over time, two level and then multi-level models coordination have been observed in models of complex are developed. In the context of behavior, this suggests a networks of neurons (and complex adaptive systems more shift from studying specific behaviors to studying pat- generally). For example, in exogenously stimulated net- terns of behaviors. Contextually stable patterns of behav- works, a low-frequency external stimulus can induce the ior are referred to here as dynamical phases. These phas- network to produce a stable (across different initial condi- es may be short lived or long lived depending upon the tions and local dynamics), reproducible, patterned global global context but persist long enough to make a differ- network output without synchronization among the ence. Context is thought to induce a dynamical phase, agents [109–112]. This is termed transient induced global while changes in context induce transitions between these response synchronization (TIGoRS). phases. Fluctuations at stable regimes usually obey a Freeman pioneered much of the research into dynam- Gaussian law, but fluctuations near a critical point often ical phases and the role of chaotic dynamics in informa- follow a power law. A 1/f power law distribution is often tion processing by the brain [113–116]. He carried out taken as an indicator of complexity. extensive neurophysiological studies, particularly of ol- This can be illustrated with a simple model, the iter- faction in insects [113], demonstrated the existence of ated logistic map, given by f = μx(1-x) where x lies within these phase synchronized dynamical phases, introduced the interval [0, 1] and μ lies within the interval [0, 4]. Each the concept of phase cones and their relationship to phase value of μ (control parameter) determines a particular dy- transitions and observed them experimentally. The re- namics, given by f. Given an initial value of x, the action sponses of the insect to distinct odors appeared to take the of f is to generate a time series, x,f(x),f(f(x)),f(f(f(x))),… form of phase transitions between dynamical phases cor- As μ increases from 0 to 4, the time series changes from responding to each odor [113, 116]. He introduced the fixed point (period 1) to period 2, period 4, period 8 and idea of patterned attractors [113] which bear some simi- so on until a critical value is reached and a non-periodic larity to TIGoRS. time series appears, following which time series of all pe- Freeman promoted the important idea that the brain riods or aperiodic series begin to occur. Below the critical maintains a state of criticality which enables rapid transi- value, each period is associated with a small interval of μ tions between dynamical phases [117]. This improves in- values, so that these are dynamical phases. Above the crit- formation processing capability. The idea of criticality ical value, each value of μ merely determines a dynamics has gained prominence over time [118–120]. Signatures as change occurs too frequently. The periodicity of the of criticality have been proposed as markers indicating time series serves as a marker of its dynamical phase. the existence of a potential to transition from one dynam- In physics, phase transitions frequently show univer- ical phase to another [121, 122]. A recent paper used such sality [105, 106], whereby the form of the scaling laws de- markers to provide an early warning sign of sudden scribing various physical properties near the transition’s change in the context of psychotherapy [123]. Jiang et al. critical point is independent of the mechanism underly- [124] have developed a set of network-based biomarkers ing the transition. The values of specific parameters in the which can be used in neuroimaging studies to assess the scaling law reflect the mechanism. The presence of uni- level of functional criticality observed in fMRI imagery. versality allows for the creation of general models into They have applied this to the setting of Alzheimer’s dis- which more precise details of mechanism can be added as ease and have demonstrated changes in functional criti- knowledge is acquired. cality, which appear to distinguish between normal cog- 6 Neuropsychobiology Sulis DOI: 10.1159/000509572
nition, mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s dis- The Process Algebra is a specific algebraic system ease [125]. which describes the various ways that processes may cou- The idea of criticality has not been met uncritically ple or interact [129–131]. Couplings describe the timing [126]. Fluctuations in correlated activity in the critical of the generation of actual occasions by each contributing state can be expected to vary widely, and in particular very process. In couplings, processes may generate actual oc- strong correlations are expected to occur. Recently, the casions sequentially (sums) or concurrently (products), simultaneous recordings of 155 cortical motor neurons in share information among occasions (free) or never share macaque monkeys revealed surprisingly weak correla- (exclusive). Two processes may be independent. Interac- tions but wide dispersion, which argues against the stan- tions result in the activation or inactivation of process or dard model of criticality [127]. The authors, however, ar- the creation of new processes. Interactions are described gue for the presence of a second form of criticality that is by functions which map from collections of processes dominated by inhibition yet is nearly unstable due to het- back to the set of processes. Processes interact according erogeneous connectivity [127]. Moreover, recent work to their compatibility [133], dependent upon their attri- has suggested that the brain may exist instead at a sub- butes and functionalities. Interactions among processes critical state, which still provides information processing create superordinate processes. Causal linkages [134] flexibility but avoids runaway states such as may occur in across generations between processes are denoted by con- epilepsy [128]. catenation. In addition, there is the zero process, O, which represents the process which does nothing. The appendix provides a brief example of how the process algebra may Process Approaches to Dynamics be used to construct a dynamical landscape. Currently, there are two complementary dynamical approaches to the problems of transience, contextuality A Dynamical Perspective on the Continuum and emergence: the functional constructivism model of Trofimova [92, 93] and the process algebra model [129– Temperament and mental illness are not specified by 131]. Standard approaches assume that systems are en- any particular behavior or set of behaviors but rather by during, objective, isolated and closed, with fixed dynam- specific dispositions or preferences for, and patterns of, ics and thus are deeply unsuited to understanding tran- behaviors which manifest in different contexts. Recall sient, emergent and contextual complex adaptive systems that the term behavior is used here in the generic sense acting, reacting and interacting. Whitehead’s Process and refers to temporal change in any of motor, cognitive Theory [132] was developed as a theory of organism. In or affective domains. Temperament and chronic mental Whitehead’s view, processes generate informational illness refer to long term, dynamically stable patterns of primitives called actual occasions which come into being, such behavior manifesting across a variety of contexts exist briefly, just long enough to prehend the information and which are normative or pathological, respectively. of previous occasions, then fade away. Processes possess Acute mental illness refers to a transient pattern of dys- transient, contextual and emergent aspects [92, 93, 106, functional or outlier behavior, which may leave the per- 129–131]. son unchanged or induce a dynamical phase transition The functional constructivism approach of Trofimova from a normative to a long-term pathological state. These [92, 93] is a process-oriented approach designed specifi- patterns of behavior may be short term or long term, but cally to take into account the transience, contextuality most importantly, they exhibit a degree of robustness and and emergent nature of behavior and its generation (and stability, and thus appear to be better candidates as phe- other systems as well). It is a global level model to be ap- nomena for study. plied across the multiplicity of scales from the neuro- In dynamical terms, a meta-level approach is needed. chemical to the sociological. Fundamental are concepts of To begin, each behavioral act (or more generally behav- action generation, contingency cycles and the existence of ior) may be considered to be an individual state, and a several universal processes whose actions generate multi- sequence of behaviors becomes a trajectory. A homology scale behavior. These include expansion or diversifica- of trajectories represents a specific dynamical phase. tion, selection or integration, maintenance or replication Next, let finite duration trajectories of behaviors (repre- processes, acting over multiple spatiotemporal scales in a sentative of short duration dynamical phases) become the vertical, horizontal or diagonal manner. new states, and sequences of trajectories (or patterns of Temperament-Illness Continuum Neuropsychobiology 7 DOI: 10.1159/000509572
short term dynamical phases) become the new trajecto- ronment (which consists of other neural systems, somat- ries. This yields a meta-meta-level description. This ic systems and the external environment). Temperament coarse graining can be continued to higher levels. Equal- and chronic mental illness represent distinct long dura- ly, one can fine grain to ever lower levels of dynamics. The tion dynamical phases, while acute mental illness is appendix shows how this might be carried out within the viewed as consisting of short-duration outlier states. process algebra framework. Such a multilevel description Behaviors correspond to first level dynamical phases, is essential when dealing with any complex adaptive sys- of which individual behavioral acts constitute specific in- tem due to the complexity of the feedback relationships stances of these phases. In keeping with the FET model that exist between the various subsystems of the global [10, 11], these phases are determined by the activities of system and the hierarchical organization of these subsys- the NBRS, which itself is a complex adaptive system and tems. so possesses its own dynamical phases. The FET model The transient nature of neural and psychological phe- suggests that many temperament traits result from ensem- nomena allows dynamical transients to serve as legiti- ble interactions among monoamine systems, which in mate, ecologically meaningful states of behaving systems. turn are regulated by opiate receptor systems, among oth- This is certainly true of the NBRS and NCRS that under- er neuropeptides [10, 11]. This suggests that long-dura- lie the generation of behavior in humans. The mono- tion dynamical phases are determined principally by amine and acetylcholine systems exhibit complex feed- NCRS. The precise features of NCRS which give rise to back relationships, and these systems are in turn modu- such long duration dynamical phases are a matter for fu- lated by opiate and other neuropeptide systems, which in ture research. They could be grounded in genetics, local turn possess multiple feedback relationships among and global connectivity, receptor densities, neurotrans- themselves [10, 11, 135, 136]. They act primarily through mitter metabolism among many other possibilities. Short- G-protein coupled receptors which modify the dynamics duration dynamical phases would be expected to be deter- of target neurons, thus acting as control parameters [135]. mined by those systems which exert a regulatory influence Disentangling such relationships is a difficult, if not in- upon the NCRS, in particular opiate and other neuropep- tractable, problem and a global approach is required such tide receptor systems. These are again complex adaptive as suggested by Freeman in his notion of “circular causal- systems with their own complicated feedback interactions ity” [114]. and so possess their own dynamical phases which could Meteorologists have developed considerable expertise determine short- and long-duration dynamical phases. in studying long-term dynamical phases (climate), their Functional constructivism provides one approach to short-term phases (weather) and pathologies (storms) modeling dynamical phases. Its core ideas are based upon and their phase transitions (climate change) [137]. They neurophysiological research, and it addresses transience, have made extensive use of time series methods, many of contextuality and emergence, which are fundamental as- which could be adapted for use in neuroscience [138– pects of neurodynamics. Thus, it appears well suited from 141]. the kind of multi-scale modeling necessary to fully cap- The utility of a focus upon markers of dynamical phas- ture an embodied neurodynamics. The process algebra es has already been alluded to previously in the analysis approach, on the other hand, provides a very general for- of fluctuations. These methods have already yielded re- mal framework for describing and analyzing the concept sults for forecasting mood [59, 67, 70], for forecasting the of process, which lies at the heart of naturally occurring possibility of sudden change in therapy [123] and for dis- dynamical systems such as the human nervous system. tinguishing between disease states [53, 56, 60, 124, 125]. Unlike standard dynamical systems theory which begins Thus, although still early, the use of concepts such as dy- with a state space and examines a single dynamic on that namical phases and phase transitions, and the measure- space, the process algebra begins with a space of dynam- ment of their attributes such as fluctuation distributions, ics, in the form of processes, and examines a dynamics on scaling laws, order parameters, show promise in bringing these dynamics. It is thus well suited for studying the spe- about an understanding of the continuum between tem- cific questions addressed here, namely the characteriza- perament and mental illness. tion of dynamical phases (as coherent sets of processes) The continuum between temperament and mental ill- and their phase transitions (process dynamics and inter- ness is hypothesized as a multiscale landscape of second actions). It provides a nuanced unifying language for de- (and higher) order dynamical phases reflecting the activ- scribing multi-scale processes, their constituent sub-pro- ity of NBRS (and NCRS) in interaction with their envi- cesses and their interactions. It is hoped that from such a 8 Neuropsychobiology Sulis DOI: 10.1159/000509572
study, generic or universal features of dynamical phases scales corresponding to individual behaviors, contextu- and their transitions can be discovered and used in the ally dependent short-term patterns of behaviors and study of neurodynamics at any level. long-term patterns of behaviors. This can be carried out The dynamical phase approach can be illustrated using using proxies, such as heart rate and locomotor activity, research involving an animal model of obsessive-compul- collected via smart phone or Fit Bit type technology sup- sive disorder [52]. Long term treatment of rats with the plemented with formal modeling using a process-based D2/D3 quinpirole induced behavioral changes which ex- approach which takes into account the aforementioned hibited characteristics consistent with compulsive check- features of transience, contextuality and emergence. The ing. Single injections of quinpirole induced short-dura- appendix provides a suggestion for how such a system tion locomotor behavior, but long-term injections, twice might be framed within the process algebra. The resulting weekly for 5 weeks, resulted in a persistent change in sev- time series can be analyzed using a variety of measures eral dynamical characteristics. The rats returned to two but with a particular focus upon fluctuation analysis and sites in the open field approximately 5 times more often the search for order parameters for the various dynamical than saline-treated rats. Moreover, the recurrence time to phases. The focus upon phases and phase transitions aris- these two sites was approximately 1/20th that of the sa- es because of the possible presence of universality in the line-treated rats and the dwell time increased. In addition, phase transitions, which can be used to simplify formal there was a contextual effect in that when an object was models, which need not capture all of the details of the placed at a site which previously had not been preferred dynamics but only the most relevant features. by the rat, the recurrence time to that site decreased and the frequency of visits and dwell time both increased (ap- proximately 5-fold for each). In comparing the Lempel- Conclusions Ziv complexity of the saline and treated rats, there was a significant decline in complexity (0.95 ± 0.16 vs. 1.20 ± Temperament and mental illness have been postulated 0.01, p < 0.001). Likewise, there was a decline in entropy to lie along a continuum of neurobehavioral regulation. between the treated and saline rats (0.709 ± 0.005 vs. 0.888 This continuum has been hypothesized, from a dynami- ± 0.003). Both levels were elevated in both treated and cal systems perspective, as a second or higher order land- saline animals showing that the paths of the animals in scape of dynamical processes, grouped together into dis- the open field were fairly random, but there appeared to tinct phases. Each phase in turn consists of distinct pat- be more structure overall in the treated animals. This sug- terns of behaviors, that is, distinct patterns of first order gested that the chronic quinpirole-treated animals were dynamical phase. These phases should be marked by or- in a distinct dynamical phase from that of the saline ani- der parameters and linked through control parameters. mals. This phase was characteristic by a somewhat con- These could be sought empirically through fluctuation textually dependent orientation of the animal’s behavior analysis of appropriate time series of behavioral mea- on two specific sites within the open field possessing sig- sures, and theoretically through the analysis of suitable nificantly shorter recurrence times, significantly higher formal models. The functional constructivism and pro- dwell times and frequencies of visits, and slightly greater cess algebra approaches provide promising candidate structure in their paths. This appears in keeping with the frameworks for such models. The identification of these FET model which attributes a role for dopamine in motor order and control parameters would facilitate the diagno- tempo, plasticity and orientation to probabilities. sis and individualized treatment of mental illness and re- It can be debated whether or not the chronic quin- fine the separation between normative temperament and pirole model represents an induction of an acute or illness. chronic illness state. It does illustrate that this state rep- resents a distinct dynamical phase from that of the con- trol animals, suggesting the validity of the dynamical Acknowledgement phase perspective. Unfortunately, a fluctuation analysis was not carried out during the induction phase which I would like to thank Irina Trofimova for her sharing of her expertise in the field of temperament and her seminal ideas related might have shed some light on the underlying nature of to transience, functional constructivism, compatibility and diago- the dynamical phase transition. nal evolution over the course of many fruitful discussions. Her The goal of the dynamical approach is to study dy- careful critique of this paper was most helpful. namical phases and phase transitions at multiple time Temperament-Illness Continuum Neuropsychobiology 9 DOI: 10.1159/000509572
Conflict of Interest Statement ground level could refer to that of fundamental physical processes, neurochemical processes, axo-dendritic processes, neural process- The author has no conflicts of interest to declare. es, neural assembly processes, neurobehavioral regulatory pro- cesses, regional processes, behavioral processes or sociocultural processes, to name a few. Regardless, there is posited a ground level and a set of ground processes, 0P1,0P2,0P3, …Let S be a set of Funding Sources algebraic expressions involving the 8 coupling operations. For ex- ample, one such expression might be (Y × Z) + (Z × W). Each ex- There are no funding sources to declare. pression contains variables and constants. The variables imply universality, the constants specificity. Next, let F be a set of func- tions from tuples of processes to processes which define the inter- Author Contributions action operations. For example, f(0P1,0P2,0P3) = 0P2 × 0P4 means if 0 P1,0P2,0P3 interact, then during the next-generation cycle, 0P1,0P3 The manuscript represents the author’s ideas and opinions and become inactive while 0P2 remains active and 0P4 becomes active. was prepared by the author alone. Finally, there might be superselection rules which are formulae of the form S(x,y,z,a,b,c) = O which states that the combination rep- resented by the form S(x,y,z,a,b,c) never occurs. Using these rules, a next level of processes, Π1 can be constructed. This procedure can Appendix be repeated to create ever higher levels of processes. One can ex- press the dependency upon lower levels by using a functional form. Process Algebra Construction of Dynamical Landscape For example, Π2(Π1) indicates that the second level involves only A landscape of dynamics can be constructed within the Process combinations or operations on first-level processes, an expression Algebra as follows. One begins with a “ground” level of processes of vertical emergence. Π2(Π1, Π0) indicates that the second level Π0. The choice depends upon the number of levels to be included involves combinations of both first and ground processes, an ex- in the analysis and the limits of efficacy expected of the model. This pression of diagonal emergence [69, 70]. References 1 Hirschfeld RM, Klerman GL. Personality at- 10 Trofimova I. The interlocking between func- 17 Johnson SW, North RA. Opioids excite dopa- tributes and affective disorders. Am J Psychi- tional aspects of activities and a neurochemi- mine neurons by hyperpolarization of local atry. 1979 Jan;136(1):67–70. cal model of adult temperament. In: Arnold interneurons. J Neurosci. 1992 Feb; 12(2): 2 Clark LA, Watson D, Mineka S. Tempera- MC, editor. Temperaments: Individual Dif- 483–8. ment, personality, and the mood and anxiety ferences, Social and Environmental Influenc- 18 Comer SD, Hoenicke EM, Sable AI, McNutt disorders. J Abnorm Psychol. 1994 Feb; 103 es and Impact on Quality of Life. New York: RW, Chang KJ, De Costa BR, et al. Convulsive (1):103–16. Nova Science Publishers; 2016. pp. 77–147. effects of systemic administration of the delta 3 Watson D, Naragon-Gainey K. Personality, 11 Trofimova I, Robbins TW. Temperament and opioid agonist BW373U86 in mice. J Pharma- emotions, and the emotional disorders. Clin arousal systems: A new synthesis of differen- col Exp Ther. 1993 Nov;267(2):888–95. Psychol Sci. 2014 Jul;2(4):422–42. tial psychology and functional neurochemis- 19 Jutkiewicz EM, Rice KC, Traynor JR, Woods 4 Trofimova I, Sulis W. The lability of behavior try. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016 May; 64: JH. Separation of the convulsions and antide- as a marker of comorbid depression and anx- 382–402. pressant-like effects produced by the delta- iety. Adv Biosci Biotechnol. 2010;1(3):190–9. 12 Cloninger CR. A unified biosocial theory of opioid agonist SNC80 in rats. Psychopharma- 5 Sulis W. Assessing the continuum between personality and its role in the development of cology (Berl). 2005 Nov;182(4):588–96. temperament and affective illness: psychiatric anxiety states. Psychiatr Dev. 1986; 4(3): 167– 20 Blazer D, Steffens D. Textbook of geriatric and mathematical perspectives. Philos Trans 226. psychiatry. Washington: APA Publishing; R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2018 Apr; 373(1744): 13 Heath AC, Cloninger CR, Martin NG. Testing 2009. 20170168. a model for the genetic structure of personal- 21 Harrington ME. Neurobiological studies of 6 Depue RA, Morrone-Strupinsky JV. A neu- ity: a comparison of the personality systems of fatigue. Prog Neurobiol. 2012 Nov; 99(2): 93– robehavioral model of affiliative bonding: im- Cloninger and Eysenck. J Pers Soc Psychol. 105. plications for conceptualizing a human trait 1994 Apr;66(4):762–75. 22 Krishnan V, Nestler EJ. Linking molecules to of affiliation. Behav Brain Sci. 2005 Jun;28(3): 14 Zentner M, Shiner R, editors. Handbook of mood: new insight into the biology of depres- 313–50. Temperament. New York: Guilford; 1992. sion. Am J Psychiatry. 2010 Nov; 167(11): 7 Gray JA. The neuropsychology of anxiety: an 15 Bennabi D, Vandel P, Papaxanthis C, Pozzo 1305–20. enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippo- T, Hatten E. Psychomotor Retardation in De- 23 Strüder HK, Weicker H. Physiology and campal system. Oxford: Oxford University pression: A Systematic Review of Diagnostic, pathophysiology of the serotonergic system Press; 1982. Pathophysiologic, and Therapeutic Implica- and its implications on mental and physical 8 Kagan J. Galen’s prophecy: Temperament in tions. Biomed Res Int. 2013;2013:158746. performance. Part I. Int J Sports Med. 2001 human nature. Westview Press; 1997. 16 degli Uberti EC, Petraglia F, Bondanelli M, Oct;22(7):467–81. 9 Rusalov VM, Trofimova IN. Structure of Guo AL, Valentini A, Salvadori S, et al. In- 24 Olmstead MC, Ouagazzal AM, Kieffer BL. Temperament and its Measurement. Toron- volvement of mu-opioid receptors in the Mu and delta opioid receptors oppositely reg- to: Psychological Services Press; 2007. modulation of pituitary-adrenal axis in nor- ulate motor impulsivity in the signaled nose mal and stressed rats. J Endocrinol Invest. poke task. PLoS One. 2009;4(2):e4410. 1995 Jan;18(1):1–7. 10 Neuropsychobiology Sulis DOI: 10.1159/000509572
25 Pradhan AA, Befort K, Nozaki C, Gavériaux- 39 Trofimova I. Questioning the “general arous- 55 Ortiz A, Bradler K, Garnham J, Slaney C, Alda Ruff C, Kieffer BL. The delta opioid receptor: al” models. Open Behav Sci Psychol. 2010; 4: M. Nonlinear dynamics of mood regulation an evolving target for the treatment of brain 1-8. in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2015 Mar; disorders. Trends Pharmacol Sci. 2011 Oct; 40 Trofimova I; IRINA TROFIMOVA. An in- 17(2):139–49. 32(10):581–90. vestigation into differences between the 56 Ortiz A, Bradler K, Garnham J, Slaney C, 26 Bodnar RJ. Endogenous opiates and behavior: structure of temperament and the structure of McLean S, Alda M. Nonlinear dynamics of 2010. Peptides. 2011 Dec;32(12):2522–52. personality. Am J Psychol. 2010; 123(4): 467– mood regulation in unaffected first-degree 27 Tanaka M, Yoshida M, Emoto H, Ishii H. 80. relatives of bipolar disorder patients. J Affect Noradrenaline systems in the hypothalamus, 41 Trofimova I, Sulis W. Is temperament activi- Disord. 2019 Jan;243:274–9. amygdala and locus coeruleus are involved in ty-specific? Validation of the Structure of 57 Cutler CD, Neufeld RW. Nonlinear Indices the provocation of anxiety: basic studies. Eur Temperament Questionnaire – Compact with Applications to Schizophrenia and Bipo- J Pharmacol. 2000 Sep;405(1-3):397–406. (STQ-77). Int J Psychol Psychol Ther. 2011; lar Disorder. Nonlinear Dynamics Psychol 28 Takahashi M, Senda T, Tokuyama S, Kaneto 11(3):389–400. Life Sci. 2019 Jan;23(1):17–56. H. Further evidence for the implication of a 42 Venkatasubramanian G, Keshavan M. Bio- 58 Sree Hari Rao V, Raghvendra Rao C, Yeragani kappa-opioid receptor mechanism in the pro- markers in Psychiatry– a critique. Ann Neu- VK. A novel technique to evaluate fluctua- duction of psychological stress-induced anal- rosci. 2016 Mar;23(1):3–5. tions of mood: implications for evaluating gesia. Jpn J Pharmacol. 1990 Aug; 53(4): 487– 43 Nelson B, McGorry P, Wichers M, Wigman J, course and treatment effects in bipolar/affec- 94. Hartmann J. Moving from static to dynamical tive disorders. Bipolar Disord. 2006 Oct; 8(5 29 Schwarzer C. 30 Years of Dynorphins – New models of the onset of mental disorder: a re- Pt 1):453–66. insights on their functions in neuropsychiat- view. JAMA Psychiatry. 2017 May;74(5):528– 59 Moore PJ, Little MA, McSharry PE, Geddes ric diseases. Pharmacol Ther. 2009 Sep; 34. JR, Goodwin GM. Forecasting depression in 123(3):353–70. 44 Gottschalk A, Bauer MS, Whybrow PC. Evi- bipolar disorder. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng. 30 Simonin F, Gavériaux-Ruff C, Befort K, Mat- dence of chaotic mood variation in bipolar 2012 Oct;59(10):2801–7. thes H, Lannes B, Micheletti G, et al. kappa- disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1995 Nov; 60 Bornas X, Balle M, De la Torre-Luque A, Fiol- Opioid receptor in humans: cDNA and ge- 52(11):947–59. Veny A, Llabrés J. Ecological assessment of nomic cloning, chromosomal assignment, 45 Guastello S, Koopmans M, Pincus D. Chaos heart rate complexity: differences between functional expression, pharmacology, and ex- and complexity in psychology. Cambridge: high- and low-anxious adolescents. Int J Psy- pression pattern in the central nervous sys- Cambridge University Press; 2011. chophysiol. 2015 Oct;98(1):112–8. tem. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1995 Jul;92(15): 46 Small M. Applied nonlinear time series analy- 61 Tolkunov D, Rubin D, Mujica-Parodi L. Pow- 7006–10. sis: applications in physics, physiology and fi- er spectrum scale invariance quantifies limbic 31 Wittmann W, Schunk E, Rosskothen I, Ga- nance. Singapore: World Scientific; 2005. dysregulation in trait anxious adults using burro S, Singewald N, Herzog H, et al. Prodyn- 47 Kantz H, Schreiber T. Nonlinear time series fMRI: adapting methods optimized for char- orphin-derived peptides are critical modula- analysis. Cambridge: Cambridge University acterizing autonomic dysregulation to neural tors of anxiety and regulate neurochemistry Press; 2003. https://doi.org/10.1017/ dynamic time series. Neuroimage. 2010 Mar; and corticosterone. Neuropsychopharmacol- CBO9780511755798 50(1):72–80. ogy. 2009 Feb;34(3):775–85. 48 Pourahmadi M, Noorbaloochi S. Multivariate 62 Fiskum C, Andersen TG, Bornas X, Aslaksen 32 Reyes BA, Johnson AD, Glaser JD, Commons time series analysis of neuroscience data: PM, Flaten MA, Jacobsen K. Non-linear KG, Van Bockstaele EJ. Dynorphin-contain- some challenges and opportunities. Curr Heart Rate Variability as a Discriminator of ing axons directly innervate noradrenergic Opin Neurobiol. 2016 Apr;37:12–5. Internalizing Psychopathology and Negative neurons in the rat nucleus locus coeruleus. 49 Aubin JP, Frankowska H. Set valued analysis. Affect in Children With Internalizing Prob- Neuroscience. 2007 Mar;145(3):1077–86. Boston: Birkhauser; 2009. https://doi. lems and Healthy Controls. Front Physiol. 33 Reyes BA, Chavkin C, van Bockstaele EJ. Sub- org/10.1007/978-0-8176-4848-0 2018 May;9:561. cellular targeting of kappa-opioid receptors in 50 Dufort PA, Lumsden C. Dynamics, complex- 63 Summers CH. Social interaction over time, the rat nucleus locus coeruleus. J Comp Neu- ity and computation. In: Lumsden C, Brandts implications for stress responsiveness. Integr rol. 2009 Jan;512(3):419–31. W, Trainor EH, editors. Physical Theory in Comp Biol. 2002 Jul;42(3):591–9. 34 Filliol D, Ghozland S, Chluba J, Martin M, Biology. Singapore: World Scientific; 1997. p. 64 Fetissov SO, Meguid MM, Chen C, Miyata, G. Matthes HW, Simonin F, et al. Mice deficient 69–106. Synchronized Release of Dopamine and Sero- for delta- and mu-opioid receptors exhibit 51 Sulis W, Gupta A. Nonlinear dynamics in psy- tonin in the Medial and Lateral Hypothalamus opposing alterations of emotional responses. chiatry. In: Sulis W, Trofimova I, editors. of Rats. Neuroscience. 2000;101(3):657–63. Nat Genet. 2000 Jun;25(2):195–200. Nonlinear Dynamics in Life and Social Sci- 65 Salomon RM, Johnson BW, Schmidt DE. 35 Trofimova I, Sulis W. There is more to mental ences. Amsterdam: IOS Press; 2000. Central neurochemical ultradian variability illness than negative affect: comprehensive 52 Szechtman H, Sulis W, Eilam D. Quinpirole in depression. Dis Markers. 2006;22(1-2):65– temperament profiles in depression and gen- induces compulsive checking behavior in rats: 72. eralized anxiety. BMC Psychiatry. 2018 May; a potential animal model of obsessive-com- 66 Adolf JK, Voelkle MC, Brose A, Schmiedek F. 18(1):125. pulsive disorder (OCD). Behav Neurosci. Capturing Context-Related Change in Emo- 36 Trofimova I, Sulis W. Benefits of distinguish- 1998 Dec;112(6):1475–85. tional Dynamics via Fixed Moderated Time ing between physical and social-verbal as- 53 Perez Arribas I, Goodwin GM, Geddes JR, Ly- Series Analysis. Multivariate Behav Res. 2017 pects of behaviour: an example of generalized ons T, Saunders KE. A signature-based ma- Jul-Aug;52(4):499–531. anxiety. Front Psychol. 2016 Mar;7:338. chine learning model for distinguishing bipo- 67 Mehraei M, Akcay N. Mood and emotional 37 Trofimova IN, Sulis W. A study of the cou- lar disorder and borderline personality disor- states prediction by time series methods. pling of FET temperament traits with Major der. Transl Psychiatry. 2018 Dec;8(1):274. Conference paper. Research Gate;2016. Depression. Front Psychol. 2016 Nov;7:1848. 54 Tsanas A, Saunders KE, Bilderbeck AC, Pal- 68 Bonsall MB, Wallace-Hadrill SMA, Geddes 38 Trofimova I, Christiansen J. Coupling of tem- mius N, Osipov M, Clifford GD, et al. Daily JR, Goodwin GM. Holmes EA. Nonlinear perament traits with mental illness in four age longitudinal self-monitoring of mood vari- time-series approaches in characterizing groups. Psychol Rep. 2016 Apr; 118(2): 387– ability in bipolar disorder and borderline per- mood stability and mood instability in bipolar 412. sonality disorder. J Affect Disord. 2016 Nov; disorder. Proc Biol Sci. 2012 Mar; 279(1730): 205:225–33. 916–24. Temperament-Illness Continuum Neuropsychobiology 11 DOI: 10.1159/000509572
You can also read