Selfless First Movers and Self-Interested Followers: Order of Entry Signals Purity of Motive in Pursuit of the Greater Good - Ike Silver
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
This article is part of the issue on “Consumer Psychology for the Greater Good” Selfless First Movers and Self-Interested Followers: Order of Entry Signals Purity of Motive in Pursuit of the Greater Good Ike Silver Brooke A. Kelly The Wharton School, University of Pennsylvania Association of American Medical Colleges Deborah A. Small The Wharton School, University of Pennsylvania Accepted by Aparna Labroo and Kelly Goldsmith Guest Editors; Associate Editor, Alexander Chernev When do brands get credit for contributing to the greater good? Prior research has shown that consumers reward brands for prosocial initiatives (e.g., corporate philanthropy, social responsibility, cause marketing) in line with the perceived purity of their motives. We report four experiments demonstrating that consumers interpret the order in which brands launch prosocial initiatives as a signal of their underlying motivations for doing good. Whereas prosocial first movers appear to care genuinely about the causes they support, prosocial followers (i.e., those that imitate the prosocial actions of other brands) typically seem to have more selfish intentions, even if their initiatives have an equal or greater social impact. This discrepancy in perceived motives, in turn, serves as an additional source of first mover preference, above and beyond previously stud- ied first mover effects and unique to greater good marketing contexts. It also leads consumers to reward fol- lowers for launching more original prosocial initiatives, even if these seem less impactful for society. These findings bridge literatures on prosocial behavior and entry order, and they offer practical insight for brands looking to combine profits and purpose. Keywords First mover advantage; Plagiarism; Pioneering; Motive inference; Prosocial behavior; Corporate social responsibility TOMS Shoes was established in 2006 after founder Skechers’ own “charitable shoe collection” (Skechers, Blake Mycoskie’s vacation to Argentina led to eye- 2018). BOBS shoes looked similar and offered an opening encounters with barefoot children. TOMS equivalent buy-one, give-one pledge. Yet although billed itself as a for-profit brand on a genuine social their products and social impact were nearly identical, mission: For every pair of shoes sold, TOMS would consumer responses to TOMS and BOBS diverged donate a pair to a needy child. This “buy-one, give- dramatically: While TOMS became an industry model one” model earned widespread acclaim, and TOMS for purpose-driven marketing, BOBS’ entry provoked went on to succeed both commercially and philan- outrage and ridicule (Timberlake, 2012). thropically, selling over 95 million pairs of shoes, and Imitating previously established business models, donating another 95 million, since its founding product features and appearances, and even brand (TOMS shoes, 2019). In 2010, Skechers tried to emu- names is common (Sayman, Hoch, & Raju, 2002); late TOMS’ winning formula, launching BOBS, and in many cases, consumers actually prefer fol- lower products which can often fulfill similar needs at lower prices (Wilcox, Kim, & Sen, 2009). Why Received 17 July 2019; accepted 31 January 2021 Available online 15 February 2021 then was the reaction to BOBS and their donation The authors thank Amanda Montoya, Gideon Nave, Tom program so negative? As one industry expert put it, Robertson, Corey Cusimano, Alex Shaw, and Sydney Scott for the TOMS/BOBS case illustrated “a powerful dis- helpful feedback on this research, and they thank The Wharton Behavioral Lab, Wharton Risk Management Center, and Wharton tinction between those that do good because of the Social Impact Initiative for financial support. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Ike Silver, The Wharton School, University of Pennsylvania, 3730 © 2021 Society for Consumer Psychology Walnut St., Philadelphia, PA 19104, USA. Electronic mail may be All rights reserved. 1057-7408/2021/1532-7663 sent to isilver@wharton.upenn.edu. DOI: 10.1002/jcpy.1228
2 Silver, Kelly, and Small meaning behind it and those that do it simply for across 60 countries, Nielsen finds that 66% of con- marketing” (Mainwaring, 2010). It seems the two sumers prefer socially responsible brands and pay brands were seen as doing good for different rea- more for their products (Nielsen, 2015). Indeed, cor- sons. Whereas TOMS appeared genuinely commit- porate prosocial initiatives can boost brand equity ted to the cause, something about being a follower (Brammer & Pavelin, 2006; Green & Peloza, 2011; led BOBS’ motives for donating shoes to appear Sen & Bhattacharya, 2001), strengthen investor con- suspect. In a pilot study, we found support for this fidence (Sen, Bhattacharya, & Korschun, 2006), and conjecture: Consumers judged BOBS charitable shoe improve perceived product quality (Chernev & collection to be more self-interested and evaluated Blair, 2015). Consequently, for-profit firms face the BOBS brand less favorably when they knew growing incentives to incorporate prosocial activi- BOBS was a follower to adopt a buy-one-give-one ties—cause marketing, corporate philanthropy, sus- pledge (see Appendix StudyA). tainability, etc.—into their business models. Prior research has established that motive infer- Yet as the BOBS case illustrates, brands do not ences guide consumer reactions to corporate proso- always benefit from doing good: Positive market cial behavior. But what factors lead consumers to responses depend critically on consumers believing see corporate philanthropic initiatives, cause-mar- that brands actually care about the causes they sup- keting campaigns, or social responsibility programs port (i.e., that their motives are “pure”; Newman & as motivated by a genuine desire to contribute to Cain, 2014; Yoon, G€ urhan-Canli, & Schwarz, 2006). the greater good? We propose that, holding other When prosocial brands seem motivated by a genuine factors constant, consumers often use the order in desire to do good, their actions are judged favorably, which brands launch prosocial initiatives as a cue to with positive consequences for brand image, sales, their underlying motivations. We hypothesize that and market share. However, when prosocial actors whereas the first brand to launch a prosocial initia- seem motivated by extrinsic rewards like reputation tive (a “prosocial first mover”) will typically seem or profit, their actions appear “tainted” (Lee, Bolton, & motivated by genuine concern for the greater good, Winterich, 2017; Newman & Cain, 2014; Small & Cry- later brands (“prosocial followers”) will typically der, 2017). In fact, consumers will actually condemn seem to have more selfish motives, even if their brands that accomplish a great deal of good for the prosocial efforts have an equal or greater impact. world if their motives appear self-interested (Bhat- We theorize that cynical motive inferences about tacharjee, Dana, & Baron, 2017; Pallotta, 2008). In followers provide an additional edge for prosocial first extreme cases, prosocial efforts that seem self-inter- movers specifically—above and beyond previously ested can backfire altogether, painting brands that do studied drivers of first mover preference. Indeed, we good more negatively than those who do not do any predict that consumers will prefer first movers over good at all (Lin-Healy & Small, 2013; Newman & Cain, followers more in the context of prosocial initiatives 2014). In sum, observers are skeptical in the domain of relative to other kinds of firm actions—that is, typical prosocial behavior (Critcher & Dunning, 2011) and profit-seeking ones—because having pure intentions assign “charitable credit” to prosocial brands in line matters more when doing good. Moreover, cynicism with the perceived purity of their motives (Gershon & about prosocial followers may lead consumers to pre- Cryder, 2017; Small & Cryder, 2017). fer prosocial initiatives that accomplish less good for Thus, while brands are increasingly expected the world, provided they seem more original and thus and incentivized to contribute to the greater good, more genuine. We report four experiments which they are not guaranteed to benefit from doing so. support these predictions and discuss their practical As prosocial initiatives become more commonplace, implications for brands that aim to combine profits understanding the factors that predict whether con- and purpose. Before describing these studies, we sumers will reward or admonish them is para- review past work on corporate prosocial behavior mount. The present studies investigate one salient and first mover advantages. factor—entry order—as a cue to motive inference. First Movers and Followers Background First mover brands often enjoy more desirable The Demand for Brands that Do Good. . . and Mean It market positioning and larger, more sustained mar- Consumers increasingly expect brands that they ket shares than later entrants (Hotelling, 1929; support to contribute to the greater good. In a glo- Robinson, 1988; Robinson & Fornell, 1985; Urban, bal survey of consumer attitudes and sales data Carter, Gaskin, & Mucha, 1986). For example, first
Selfless First Movers 3 movers can form partnerships, attain patents, and plagiarism provides some support for this idea. build infrastructure which raise barriers to entry for Specifically, recent studies suggest that people’s followers (Sammut-Bonnici & Channon, 2015). Ear- objection to copying ideas is driven in part by a lier and more frequent exposure to first mover concern that copycats are seeking to benefit unde- brands also causes consumers to remember first servingly from the ideas and efforts of others movers better and form more favorable attitudes (Altay, Majima, & Mercier, 2020; Silver & Shaw, towards them (Kardes & Kalyanaram, 1992; Kardes, 2018). Because seeming self-interested and oppor- Kalyanaram, Chandrashekaran, & Dornoff, 1993; tunistic is especially damning when doing good, we Robinson, Kalyanaram, & Urban, 1994). First predict that copying prosocial acts might provoke movers often define product categories and dictate stronger backlash than imitating other sorts of firm consumer tastes and preferences (Carpenter & actions (i.e., typical, profit-seeking ones). Nakamoto, 1989; Kamins & Alpert, 2004; Moreau, To our knowledge, no previous literature has Markman, & Lehmann, 2001). examined the possibility that first mover advan- On the other hand, it sometimes pays to be a fol- tages can arise because of consumer perceptions of lower (Shankar & Carpenter, 2012; Shankar, Car- discrepant motives between first movers and fol- penter, & Krishnamurthi, 1999), allowing first lowers or that preferences for first movers might be movers to test and prime the market for later entry. amplified in the context of good deeds specifically. In practice, imitation is ubiquitous (Sayman et al., Our experiments address this gap, aiming to estab- 2002) and consumer responses to later entrants are lish motive inference as an independent driver of not categorically unfavorable (Van Horen & Pieters, order-based preference. 2012, 2017). In fact, follower brands and products can become quite popular, particularly when they fulfill consumer needs more effectively or at a Present Research lower price (Wilcox et al., 2009). A number of beloved market leaders (a majority by some esti- We investigate a novel first mover advantage mates; Golder & Tellis, 1993) are actually followers, unique to greater good settings, where motives such as Nabisco’s Oreo or Sony’s PlayStation. In matter. To do this, we first seek to establish that, in summary, previous research on whether it is prefer- line with our account, first mover preferences are able to be first or to follow paints a mixed picture. larger in prosocial contexts. We reason that if Importantly, prior work on entry order has little motive inferences matter above and beyond other to say about prosocial innovation specifically, and it first mover effects, then we should expect to would predict neither the powerful consumer back- observe larger first-mover preferences in the context lash that BOBS faced in our motivating example of prosocial brand actions (where motives matter nor more generally that prosocial followers would more) than in the context of nonprosocial brand be seen as having selfish intentions. If anything, actions (i.e., typical profit-seeking actions, where one might expect prosocial followers to be cheered motives matter less). By contrast, alternative sources for “joining the cause” by imitating the prosocial of first mover preference—perceived creativity or products, policies, and practices of other brands. innovativeness, unfairness to first movers, more After all, such initiatives generate positive externali- general dislike of copying—make no predictions ties (i.e., charitable impact) which should paint fol- about the relative size of first mover advantages lowers in a positive light (Chernev & Blair, 2015). across prosocial vs. nonprosocial contexts. Thus, we Indeed, many cultures and religions explicitly aim first to demonstrate an effect specific to greater encourage emulating the behavior of moral role good marketing, where consumers place a particu- models (Bucher, 1998; Kristj ansson, 2006), such that lar premium on having the right kinds of (non- imitating the prosocial behavior of other brands selfish) motives. may seem like an innocuous way to enter the prosocial space and meet the growing consumer Hypothesis 1: Preferences for first mover brands (over demand for social impact. followers) are stronger in prosocial (vs. On the contrary, we suggest that people will typ- typical profit-seeking) contexts. ically respond more negatively to prosocial follow- ers, in part because relative to first movers, We turn next to measuring the drivers of first followers are thought to be doing good for the mover preference in prosocial contexts specifically, wrong reasons. A related line of psychological testing whether motive inferences can explain in research concerning people’s moral judgments of part why people prefer prosocial first movers to
4 Silver, Kelly, and Small prosocial followers. Specifically, we predict that original prosocial products and initiatives allows whereas the first brand to launch a certain prosocial later entrants to appear more like daring pioneers initiative is typically seen as more purely moti- in their own right and less like followers at all. vated, follower brands that launch similar initia- tives are typically seen as having more tainted Hypothesis 3: Differentiating (vs. copying) can lead motives. Because motives matter in greater good prosocial followers to appear more purely contexts, we also expect to observe downstream motivated. effects of these motive inferences on brand evalua- tions. An interesting implication of Hypothesis 3 is that consumers may sometimes prefer that prosocial fol- Hypothesis 2a: Prosocial followers typically seem less lowers choose less impactful ways to do good, pro- purely motivated (more self-interested) vided these seem more original. Indeed, although than prosocial first movers. scholars and laypeople alike endorse the impor- tance of judging prosocial behavior in terms of out- Hypothesis 2b: Seeming less purely motivated harms comes (i.e., social impact; Singer, 2019; MacAskill, brand evaluations of prosocial followers 2015), consumers often disregard outcome informa- (vs. prosocial first movers). tion in practice (e.g., dollars donated, lives saved), both in their own charitable behavior and in their Why might following lead to more cynical motive judgments of others’ good deeds (Berman, Barasch, inferences? At baseline, people tend to view for- Levine, & Small, 2018). Similarly, we suggest that profit firms as uncaring (Aaker, Vohs, & Mogilner, consumers may prefer more original prosocial ini- 2010; Bhattacharjee et al., 2017) and show skepticism tiatives from followers, even if being original also at their ostensibly benevolent initiatives (Newman means doing less good. & Cain, 2014). But whereas prosocial first movers seem willing to put profits on the line for the cause, Study Overview prosocial followers may seem to be behaving oppor- tunistically, expecting to profit in some way from We report four experiments which examine con- joining the movement. In the eyes of consumers, fol- sumers’ responses to real and hypothetical cases of lowing another firm’s actions typically implies that corporate prosocial behavior and their choices the first mover benefited from taking them (and that between cause-marketing products. These experi- the follower therefore expects to benefit as well). ments manipulate entry order between- and within- While pioneers seldom know how their actions will subjects and operationalize prosocial behavior be received in the marketplace, followers can broadly to include products, programs, and policies observe how consumers respond to first movers, supporting charitable missions from poverty allevi- copying only when it is in their interest to do so. ation to environmentalism to urban revitalization. However, if it appears that the follower, too, is will- Importantly, we do not intend to argue that motive ing to put profits on the line for the sake of the purity is the only driver of order-based preference. cause, they may be perceived as having pure Instead, our studies aim is to show that motive pur- motives after all. For example, if consumers happen ity provides an additional source of first mover to know that a prosocial first mover brand did not advantage that amplifies first mover preferences in profit from doing good (e.g., lost money on an greater good contexts. To this end, we aim not to investment for social impact), following should no remove all other possible drivers of first mover longer appear incentivized or opportunistic, and fol- preference from our stimuli, but rather, to hold lower motives should seem more pure as a result. them constant across conditions and demonstrate What can follower brands do to defend against effects of motive purity above and beyond them. the perception that their motives for doing good All reported experiments were preregistered at are self-interested? We suggest that prosocial fol- AsPredicted.org. All preregistrations, study stimuli, lowers can earn more credit with consumers by dif- data files, and appendix studies are accessible at ferentiating their good deeds from those of prior https://osf.io/97rvc/?view_only=77759b50f8624c entrants (e.g., by “finding their own cause”). Put 539d041071c70aa8ac. The appendix also contains simply, taking steps to identify a more original way five additional studies, which further replicate to do good may signal more genuine motives. This our effects and address related points. is because differentiation makes following seem less In Study 1, using real product choices, we opportunistic: Going out on a limb for more demonstrate that first mover preferences are larger
Selfless First Movers 5 in the context of prosocial behavior, where motive experiment took the first 7 minutes. Participants, purity matters more, relative to nonprosocial (i.e., seated in separate cubicles, read a short (and factu- typical profit-seeking) behavior, where motive pur- ally true) description of a recent trend among major ity matters less. In Study 2, we show that prosocial chocolate manufacturers. Specifically, participants followers (vs. first movers) are specifically judged learned about two real candy bar brands—Cadbury’s as less purely motivated and that this effect medi- Flake and Mars’ Ripple—that have both recently ates broader brand evaluations. In Study 3, we begun sourcing cocoa beans from Ghana (Flake first, moderate our effect, showing that brand evaluation followed by Ripple; Glover, 2009). In other words, penalties for following are weakened when the fol- participants learned about a real first mover brand lower is less likely to profit from doing good and a real follower brand. We selected British candy (which should make their motives appear more bar brands we hoped would be unfamiliar to our pure). In Study 4, we show that finding a more participants in order to minimize the impact of pre- original cause can offset the negative effects of existing preferences on our experiment. being a prosocial follower, even if doing so means We manipulated between-subjects the apparent picking a cause that is less impactful or less obvi- purpose of switching to Ghanaian cocoa beans. In ously connected to the brand. Taken together, our the profit-seeking condition, participants learned studies provide evidence for a novel first mover that these new beans would allow the two brands advantage, driven by moral judgment of underlying to offer a premium-quality product and increase motive, in the growing consumer market for proso- profit margins for shareholders. In the prosocial cial brands and products. condition, participants learned that these new beans would allow the brands to offer a fair-trade product and increase profit shares for local farmers. Partici- Study 1. First Mover Preferences are Stronger in pants were then asked which of the two candy bars Prosocial Contexts they would prefer to take home at the end of the study, Flake—the first mover—or Ripple—the fol- We contend that prosocial followers are evaluated lower. To avoid possible floor effects on choosing more negatively because their motives appear more the follower across conditions, we also told all par- tainted. On this theory, the costs of following ticipants that the follower chocolate bar typically should be steeper (and first mover preferences receives better customer reviews. We preregistered should thus be larger) in the context of prosocial the prediction that participants would more fre- behavior, specifically, where motive purity is more quently pick the first mover (over the better-re- important, than in the context of other kinds of typ- viewed follower) when the brands’ bean-sourcing ical (i.e., profit-seeking) firm actions, where motive initiatives were framed as prosocial, fair-trade pro- purity is less important. In Study 1, using real pro- jects than when they were framed as profit-maxi- duct choices, we test this prediction (Hypothesis 1) mizing projects. by framing the same initiative—making a supply Participants then answered two short multiple- chain change—as either prosocial or profit-seeking choice manipulation checks, reporting which brand and comparing participants’ relative preferences for (Flake or Ripple) had changed their cocoa sourcing first movers between these domains. first (91.2% answered correctly) and identifying the Our aim here is to make a practically important stated purpose for the brands’ sourcing initiatives point: Relative to other sorts of actions where (92.5% answered correctly). Participants also indi- motives matter less, being first matters more for cated whether they had ever encountered either marketing actions taken in pursuit of the greater candy bar previously (87.7% had not). Participants good. picked up their candy bar choice from a hypothesis- and condition-blind research assistant as they exited Method the experimental session. In this and all other studies, we collected basic demographics (e.g., age, gender) Across all studies, sample sizes were determined but did not observe any consistent demographic in advance and any exclusions were preregistered. effects. For all studies, we report all conditions and response variables. For this study, 310 participants (64.5% female, mean age = 27.8) were recruited from a Results northeastern university’s behavioral laboratory for Consistent with our hypotheses, a logistic regres- an hour-long session and received $10. Our sion predicting participants’ likelihood of choosing
6 Silver, Kelly, and Small the first mover candy bar by condition detected a condition in which entry order was omitted to test significant effect. Participants who thought that how consumers respond in the absence of order switching to Ghanaian chocolate was a fair-trade information. In line with prior literature on pioneer- initiative chose the first mover brand more fre- ing effects, our central predictions concern a relative quently (50.0%) than those who thought that it was effect of entry order on brand attitudes and motive a profit-seeking initiative (30.8%; Wald Z = 3.43, inferences (i.e., first movers vs. followers). Never- p < .001). Note that the more general preference for theless, we included this baseline condition to gain the follower product we observed is likely because insight into whether this effect would more closely the follower was said to be better-reviewed, but this resemble a benefit for being first or a penalty for cannot explain differences in first-mover choice following. share across conditions. Method Discussion Nine hundred and four participants (47.7% In Study 1, examining real, consequential choice, female, mean age = 37.5) were recruited from we found results in line with Hypothesis 1: Partici- MTurk. Participants read about a for-profit snack pants displayed stronger first-mover preferences in food company called Shipley’s Harvest. Participants the context of prosocial products (fair-trade choco- learned that Shipley’s Harvest had recently lates) relative to nonprosocial products (premium- launched a corporate philanthropy program donat- quality chocolates). Said differently, in the context ing free healthy lunches to local public schools. of doing good, where motive purity matters more, Participants were randomly assigned to one of the costs of following were amplified. Importantly, three entry-order conditions (first mover, follower, this result is not restricted to the case of fair-trade or baseline) and read an additional excerpt accord- cocoa beans: We replicated it in Appendix Study B, ing to condition. In the first mover condition, par- a scenario study about cause-marketing (vs. cost- ticipants learned that Shipley’s Harvest was “the saving) clothing items. first snack food company” to donate school lunches That first mover preferences would be larger in to local public schools. In the follower condition, greater good contexts is not easily explained by participants learned that Shipley’s Harvest was other previously studied order effects (perceived “following other snack food companies” in doing creativity, unfairness to the first mover, general dis- so. In the baseline condition, we omitted any men- like of copying, etc.) which should not differ across tion of entry order. This baseline condition helped prosocial vs. nonprosocial contexts. By contrast, this us ascertain whether any differences observed pattern is consistent with the notion that percep- between first mover and follower more closely tions of motive purity contribute to first mover resemble positive inferences about first movers or advantages. Where motives matter more, penalties negative inferences about followers. for following are larger. After reading, participants evaluated the Ship- To test our proposed process directly, Study 2 ley’s Harvest brand and made inferences about its measured perceptions of motive purity for first motives for doing good. The brand evaluation task movers and followers in a prosocial context and (adapted from Kamins & Alpert, 2004) asked partic- investigated whether these motive inferences medi- ipants to report their overall attitudes toward Ship- ate effects of order on downstream brand evalua- ley’s Harvest on three 7-point items: liking (1. tions. “Dislike very much” to 7. “Like very much”), favor- ability (1. “Highly unfavorable” to 7. “Highly favor- able”), and positivity (1. “Extremely negative” to 7. “Extremely positive”). These were averaged to cre- Study 2. Prosocial Followers Seem More Self- ate a composite brand evaluation measure interested than Prosocial First Movers (a = 0.95). The motive inference task asked partici- Study 2 sought to demonstrate, via mediation, the pants to rate their agreement with three items on 7- impact of entry order on motive inferences and point Likert scales from 1. “Strongly disagree” to 7. downstream brand evaluations in a greater good “Strongly agree.” These items were: “Shipley’s context (testing Hypotheses 2a and 2b). Participants Harvest’s motives for donating lunches are self- learned about a snack food company that was serving”; “Shipley’s Harvest has an ulterior motive either the first or a follower to donate healthy for donating lunches”; “Shipley’s Harvest is donat- lunches to underserved schools. We also included a ing lunches mainly to look good to customers.”
Selfless First Movers 7 These items were reverse-coded and averaged motivated in the baseline condition (M = 3.63, (a = 0.89) to form a single measure of perceived SD = 1.47) than in the follower condition (t motive purity. We predicted that Shipley’s Harvest (596) = 2.19, p = .029, d = 0.18). The difference would be seen as more purely motivated and eval- between the baseline condition and the first mover uated more positively in the first mover condition condition was not significant (t(603) = 1.18, p = .24, relative to the follower condition, but we did not d = 0.10). make any predictions about comparisons with the baseline condition. Brand evaluations Next, participants answered a multiple-choice attention check question, selecting between: “Ship- We next subjected participants’ brand evalua- ley’s Harvest was the first snack food company to tions to a one-way ANOVA with entry order (first donate lunches”; “Shipley’s Harvest was following mover, follower, or baseline) as a manipulated fac- other snack food companies in donating lunches”; tor. This procedure revealed a significant omnibus “No information about order was provided.” 79% effect of condition (F(2, 899) = 19.09, p < .001). See answered correctly. Following our preregistered Figure 1. Pairwise planned comparison t tests plan, we included all participants in subsequent revealed that, in line with predictions, Shipley’s analyses (but the same results arise if we focus Harvest was evaluated more positively overall instead on data from correct responses only). when it was described as a first mover (M = 5.71, SD = 1.08) than when it was described as a fol- lower (M = 5.17, SD = 1.10; t(603) = 6.13, p < .001, Results d = 0.50). Shipley’s Harvest was also evaluated more positively overall in the baseline condition Motive purity when no entry order information was provided Participants’ motive purity inferences were sub- (M = 5.44, SD = 1.08) than in the follower condition jected to a one-way ANOVA with entry order as a (t(596) = 3.05, p = .002, d = 0.25). The first mover between-subjects factor, revealing a significant and baseline conditions also differed significantly omnibus effect of condition (F(2, 899) = 5.64, on broader brand evaluations (t(603) = 3.11, p = .004). See Figure 1. Pairwise planned compar- p = .002, d = 0.25). ison t tests indicated that, in line with predictions, Shipley’s Harvest was seen as more purely moti- Mediation vated in the first mover condition (M = 3.77, SD = 1.57) than in the follower condition (M = 3.37, Lastly, we examined whether participants’ SD = 1.35; t(603) = 3.35, p < .001, d = 0.27). Ship- motive purity inferences mediated the effects of ley’s Harvest was also seen as more purely entry order on brand evaluations. We examined 7 *** ** ** 6 *** ns * 5 4 3 2 1 First Mover Baseline Follower First Mover Baseline Follower Motive Purity Brand Evaluations Figure 1. Perceived motive purity (1–7 scale) and brand evaluations (1–7 scale) by entry order condition from Study 2. Error bars repre- sent standard errors. * < 0.05, ** < 0.01, *** < 0.001.
8 Silver, Kelly, and Small each pairwise comparison among all three entry mechanisms. We replicated these results with an order conditions, in each case using a bootstrapping identical design but in different prosocial context procedure with 10,000 samples (Preacher & Hayes, (an eco-friendly jewelry brand) in Appendix Study 2004). These mediation models always included C. entry order condition as the independent variable, Although we made efforts in Studies 1 and 2 to motive purity as a mediator, and brand evaluations describe the follower’s actions as neutrally as possi- as the dependent variable. First, comparing the first ble, we also wanted to know whether a more posi- mover (0) and follower (1) conditions, we found a tive framing of following (i.e., “joining the cause” significant indirect effect of motive purity on brand with the first mover’s invitation) might attenuate evaluations (effect estimate = 0.10, SE = 0.03, 95% the discrepancy in perceived motives between CI = [ 0.16, 0.04]). We also found a significant prosocial first movers and followers. In indirect effect comparing the baseline (0) and fol- Appendix Study D, in the context of disability- lower (1) conditions (effect estimate = 0.07, friendly gaming products, we found that describing SE = 0.03, 95% CI = [ 0.13, 0.01]). However, we following as a response to a direct invitation to did not find a significant indirect effect comparing “join the cause” did not diminish the effects of the baseline (0) and first mover (1) conditions (effect order on motive inference. This result casts doubt estimate = 0.04, SE = 0.03, 95% CI = [ 0.02, 0.10]). on alternative explanations of our effect based on These results are consistent with motive purity perceived harm to the first mover: If the first mover judgments playing a mediating role in the relation- is explicitly inviting followers, following is unlikely ship between entry order and overall brand evalua- to seem harmful. It also suggests that our effects tions, with this effect being strongest when a are relatively insensitive to how order information prosocial brand is known to be a follower. is communicated. Discussion Study 3. Profit Potential Moderates the Impact of The results from Study 2 suggest that prosocial Following on Motive Inference followers are specifically seen as less purely moti- vated (i.e., more self-interested) than prosocial first Study 3 sought to demonstrate the impact of movers and that these motive inferences mediate motive purity on brand evaluations via moderation effects of order on brand evaluation. These findings (testing Hypotheses 2a and 2b). In marketplace con- corroborate Hypotheses 2a and 2b. texts, the presence of followers typically implies Participants’ responses to our baseline condition that the first mover’s actions benefited the brand— are consistent with the possibility that effects on and thus incentivize later entrants. For this reason, brand evaluation derive more strongly from learn- whereas prosocial first movers might appear willing ing that a brand is a follower than from learning to put profits on the line for the greater good, that they are a first mover. A related possibility is prosocial followers may often seem to be behaving that participants simply assumed that Shipley’s in self-interest (i.e., to have more tainted motiva- Harvest was the first (or the only) prosocial actor in tions). In Study 3, we asked whether cynicism the baseline condition and evaluated them accord- about prosocial followers and their motives would ingly. In either case, more explicit knowledge of fol- therefore be lessened in a case where following is lower status appears to be important for producing not incentivized, because the first mover did not our effects: Motive inferences did not differ signifi- benefit from the good deed in question. Specifically, cantly between the first mover and baseline condi- we sought to compare two follower conditions, one tions, but both saw more favorable attributions in which the prosocial initiative being copied—a than the follower condition. Moreover, these motive socially responsible loan program—had profited the inferences mediated brand evaluation effects first mover (implying a potential for the follower to between baseline and follower, but not between likewise profit from it) and one in which it had not baseline and first mover, suggesting that differences profited the first mover (implying the follower in brand attitudes between baseline and first mover would probably not profit from it). We expected may be attributable to other positive inferences that the follower would seem to have purer about pioneering. By contrast, learning that a brand motives, and be evaluated more positively as a is a follower raises suspicion about the brand’s result, in the latter case. motives for doing good, which seems to impact We note that in addition to providing direct evi- brand attitudes above and beyond other dence in favor of our account, this manipulation
Selfless First Movers 9 allows us to rule out multiple alternative explana- All participants were asked to correctly identify tions for our previous effects. A potential for profit which of the two banks they had evaluated (90.0% should have no effect on, for example, perceptions answered correctly). Finally, participants in the two of follower creativity or more general dislike of follower conditions were asked to identify whether copying, but it should certainly impact perceptions the first mover had profited from their prosocial of self-interest. initiative (89.4% answered correctly). Method Results Three hundred and two MTurk participants Motive purity (mean age = 36.2, 40.7% female) were randomly assigned to one of three between-subjects condi- Participants’ motive purity judgments were sub- tions. All participants read about a prosocial first jected to a one-way ANOVA with condition as a mover, a big bank called Madison Trust, that between-subjects factor. Results revealed a signifi- launched a corporate philanthropy initiative to cant omnibus effect of condition (F(2,299) = 9.72, invest in and revitalize underserved urban commu- p < .001). As we predicted, planned comparisons nities. In the first condition, participants simply revealed that the first mover (M = 3.57, SD = 1.38) evaluated the first mover after reading this informa- was evaluated as more purely motivated than the tion. In the other two conditions, participants also follower with high-profit potential (M = 2.89, read about a prosocial follower, Bank of the Atlan- SD = 1.10; t(198) = 3.80, p < .001, d = 0.54). The fol- tic, that launched a similar program, and these par- lower with low-profit potential (M = 3.65, ticipants subsequently evaluated the follower only. SD = 1.46) was also evaluated as more purely moti- The only difference between these two follower vated than the follower with high-profit potential conditions was whether the first mover was (t(197) = 4.14, p < .001, d = 0.59). In terms of motive described as having profited (vs. not profited) from purity, there was no difference between the first investing in underserved neighborhoods. In this mover and the follower with low-profit potential way, we sought to manipulate whether the fol- (t(203) < 1, p > .5). lower’s prosocial initiative seemed incentivized by the potential for profit. We did not specify the fol- Brand evaluations lower’s outcome, that is, whether the follower sub- sequently turned out to profit from doing good. A similar one-way ANOVA on participants’ Thus, all participants either evaluated a prosocial brand evaluations revealed a significant omnibus first mover or one of two prosocial followers—one effect of condition (F(2,299) = 12.91, p < .001). In following a first mover that had profited and the line with our predictions, planned comparison t other following a first mover that had not profited tests revealed that the first mover was evaluated (i.e., a “high profit potential follower” and a “low more positively (M = 5.73, SD = 1.11) than the fol- profit potential follower”), with equal proportions lower with high-profit potential (M = 4.89, randomly assigned to each of three cells. Our key SD = 1.12; t(198) = 5.34, p < .001, d = 0.75). The fol- test concerned the comparison between the two fol- lower with low-profit potential was evaluated mar- lower conditions. After reading, participants evalu- ginally more positively (M = 5.18, SD = 1.34) than ated either the first mover or the follower, the follower with high-profit potential (t(197) = 1.63, according to condition, on the same brand evalua- p = .10, d = 0.23). The first mover was also evalu- tion items (a = 0.96) and the same motive purity ated more positively than the follower with low- items (a = 0.87) used previously. profit potential (t(203) = 3.24, p < .01, d = 0.45). We preregistered two predictions. First, we pre- dicted that the first mover would be evaluated Mediation more positively and seen as more purely motivated than the follower when the follower had a high We estimated two mediation models to examine potential for profit (and thus seemed to be behav- our causal predictions. The first model examined ing opportunistically). Second, we predicted that the comparison between the first mover and the fol- the follower would be evaluated more positively lower with high-profit potential, treating brand and seen as more purely motivated when there evaluations as the dependent variable, condition as was low (vs. high) potential for the follower to the independent variable (first mover = 0, fol- profit. lower = 1), and motive purity as the mediator. A
10 Silver, Kelly, and Small bootstrapping procedure with 10,000 samples incentives). In the real world, consumers seldom revealed a significant indirect effect (effect esti- have direct information about potential for profit. mate = 0.16, SE = 0.07, 95% CI = [ 0.31, 0.04]). Still, the fact that motive inferences and down- This model essentially replicates the causal process stream brand evaluations change when such infor- we have observed previously: Differences in brand mation is provided suggests that these follower evaluations between the first mover and the fol- costs are not driven by other relevant alternatives lower with high-profit potential appear to be driven (such as perceived creativity, innovativeness, gen- by discrepancies in perceived motive purity. eral dislike of copying, etc.), on which potential for The second model examined the comparison profit information should have little effect. In Study between the two different follower conditions, 4, we turn to a more practical question: What can again treating brand evaluations as the dependent prosocial followers do to avoid attributions of self- variable, condition as the independent variable interest? (high-profit potential = 0, low-profit potential = 1) and motive purity as the mediator. This procedure revealed a significant indirect effect (effect esti- Study 4. Finding your Own Cause can Diminish mate = 0.29, SE = 0.09, 95% CI = [0.13, 0.49]). This Skepticism about Prosocial Following result indicates that to the extent that a follower is evaluated more positively for copying when profit We have thus far demonstrated that prosocial fol- potential is low, this effect is driven by differences lowers, relative to first movers, are seen as less in perceived purity of motive. purely motivated and evaluated less positively For completeness, we also analyzed the compar- overall as a result. In Study 4, we ask whether later ison between the first mover (0) and the follower entrants can recoup some of the costs of following with low-profit potential (1). Although the first by differentiating their prosocial behavior from that mover was viewed more positively in terms of of the first mover (i.e., by finding their own cause; brand evaluations, this effect was not mediated by Hypothesis 3). To investigate, we told participants perceptions of underlying motive (effect esti- about two technology brands that each recently mate = 0.02, SE = 0.05, 95% CI = [ 0.09, 0.13]). This launched a corporate philanthropy initiative, with result suggests that additional preferences for first one brand launching their charitable effort movers may be driven by other positive inferences 6 months later than the other; and we varied about pioneering. whether the follower’s initiative was similar to or different from the first mover’s. In all conditions, the first mover brand launched an outreach pro- Discussion gram to teach young women to code. The follower The results of Study 3 provide further evidence then either offered a similar coding program or for Hypotheses 2a and 2b via moderation and instead organized volunteering efforts at local ani- explore a theoretically informative boundary condi- mal shelters. We predicted that differentiating, rela- tion. Prosocial followers face less cynical attribu- tive to copying, would engender more favorable tions when their initiatives seem to lack a potential motive inferences and brand attitudes. for profit (because the first mover’s initiative was To provide an especially conservative test of this not profitable). Followers who emulated a prosocial prediction, we intentionally selected a differentiated initiative that had not been profitable previously initiative that we expected would seem both less were seen as having purer motives, and these socially impactful and a less natural fit with a tech- motive inferences predicted downstream differences nology company’s brand. In other words, we in broader brand attitudes. Indeed, when the first expected that participants would give more credit mover had not profited, the follower and first to a follower firm that differentiated its prosocial mover seemed roughly equally purely motivated. initiative, even if doing so would seem to accom- (Nonetheless, first movers were still preferred on plish less good for society and fit less naturally the broader brand evaluations DV, a result consis- with the brand. Evidence in favor of this pattern tent with the notion that first mover advantages are would suggest that under some circumstances peo- complex and multiply determined.) ple respond more favorably to followers who are This study’s purpose was primarily to test the more original, but less socially impactful, in their underlying psychological mechanism for order- charitable behavior. Note that we make no norma- based motive inferences (a lay intuition that follow- tive claims about the relative societal impact of ing implies the presence of tainting marketplace offering free coding classes vs. volunteering at
Selfless First Movers 11 animal shelters. Of interest here are participants’ impactful and a better fit for a technology brand. perceptions of impact. Nevertheless, we predicted that followers would be judged more negatively for launching such an ini- tiative if doing so appeared derivative of similar Method efforts by a prosocial first mover. Perceived impact and fit pretest Main study We suspected that people would generally view offering free coding classes for young women as A separate sample of 643 MTurkers (mean more socially impactful and a better fit for a tech- age = 38.3, 44.9% female) participated in the main nology brand than organizing volunteering efforts study. All participants read about two technology at animal shelters. To test this prediction, we brands (“Techlogix” and “Businessware”) which recruited 202 MTurkers (mean age = 35.5, 38.1% offer cloud-based storage support for businesses. In female) for a short pretest. Participants were asked all cases, participants learned that in early 2019, to consider a technology brand deciding between Techlogix (the first mover) launched a charitable two possible charitable initiatives to launch. One initiative aimed at increasing gender diversity in was described as “a program aimed at increasing the technology industry: free coding classes for access to software engineering jobs for young young women at local community colleges. Then, women.” The other was described as “a program “six months later,” Businessware (the follower) aimed at caring for neglected and abused animals.” launched their own charitable initiative. We varied Participants then answered two questions: “Which between conditions whether Businessware also program do you think will have a greater positive launched free coding classes (copied condition) or impact on society?” and “Which program do you instead organized volunteering efforts at local ani- think would fit more naturally with the [technol- mal shelters (differentiated condition). To avoid the ogy] company’s brand and message?” on a scale inference that the follower’s coding classes would from 0 to 100, with the two programs as the end- provide a redundant service or somehow compete points of this scale. Which program sat at which with the first mover’s, we described Techlogix as scale endpoint was randomized and coded such based in San Francisco and Businessware as based that higher numbers always indicated a partici- in Boston. pants’ belief that the initiative helping women land Participants then rated both the first mover and jobs in technology was more socially impactful or a the follower on the same motive purity (aFM = 0.89, better brand-cause fit. The scale’s midpoint always aFOL = 0.92) and brand evaluation (aFM = 0.96, read “about the same.” aFOL = 0.98) items used previously. Thus, we With respect to perceived impact, 68% of partici- employed a mixed 2 (order: within-subjects) 9 2 pants gave a response greater than 50, indicating a (follower response: between-subjects) design. We belief that offering coding classes to increase gender preregistered the following predictions. First, we representation in tech was more socially impactful. predicted a main effect of order such that first A one-sample t test comparing participants’ percep- movers would be viewed as more purely motivated tions of social impact to the midpoint of the scale and evaluated more positively than followers. Sec- indicated that coding classes were, on average, seen ond, we predicted an interaction whereby the dis- as having a greater positive impact on society crepancy in perceived motives and brand attitudes (M = 63.1, SD = 27.5; t(201) = 6.77, p < .001, would be larger when the follower copied (vs. dif- d = 0.48). With respect to brand-cause fit, 75% gave ferentiated from) the first mover’s prosocial initia- a response greater than 50, indicated a belief that tive. Third, we predicted that followers who offering coding classes better fit the brand. A one- differentiate would be seen as more purely moti- sample t test comparing participants’ perceptions of vated and evaluated more positive positively than brand-cause fit to the midpoint of the scale indi- followers who copy. cated that coding classes were, on average, seen as Finally, participants were asked to identify which a better fit for the brand (M = 72.9, SD = 28.1; t brand launched their prosocial initiative first as a (201) = 11.58, p < .001, d = 0.82). brief manipulation/attention check. Following our In sum, the pretest results confirmed our intu- preregistered plan, we excluded an additional 19 itions: An initiative aimed at increasing gender participants who missed this simple detail (3% of diversity in tech (relative to one aimed at providing the total sample). All reported results hold if these care for animals) was seen as more socially participants are instead included.
12 Silver, Kelly, and Small Results condition (Mdifferentiated = 5.33, SD = 1.25 vs. Mcopied = 5.01, SD = 1.40; F(1, 641) = 13.33, Motive Purity p < .001, gG2 = 0.015). Again, these main effects We analyzed participants’ perceptions of motive were qualified by the predicted entry order 9 fol- purity for first movers and followers using a lower response interaction (F(1, 641) = 51.78, repeated measures ANOVA with entry order as a p < .001, gG2 = 0.02). within-subjects factor and follower response (copied When the follower copied, the follower was eval- vs. differentiated) as a between-subjects factor. In uated less positively than the first mover (MFol- line with predictions, this approach revealed a sig- Copy = 4.58, SD = 1.46 vs. MFM-Copy = 5.44, nificant main effect of entry order such that first SD = 1.20; paired t(322) = 12.94, p < .001, d = 0.72). movers were seen as more purely motivated than However, when the follower differentiated, there followers across conditions (Mfirst mover = 4.10, was no discrepancy in brand evaluations (MFol-Dif- SD = 1.53 vs. Mfollower = 3.60, SD = 1.62; F(1, fer = 5.28, SD = 1.28 vs. MFM-Differ = 5.39, SD = 1.21; 641) = 67.57, p < .001, gG2 = 0.026). We also paired t(319) = 1.48, p = .14, d = 0.08). detected a main effect of condition on perceived Moreover, while the first mover was evaluated motive purity (Mdifferentiated = 4.05, SD = 1.59 vs. similarly across conditions (t(641) = 0.54, p = .59), Mcopied = 3.65, SD = 1.57; F(1, 641) = 14.37, p < .001, the follower who copied was evaluated significantly gG2 = 0017). Importantly, these main effects were less positively than the follower who differentiated qualified by a significant entry order 9 follower (MFol-Copy = 4.58, SD = 1.46 vs. MFol-Differ = 5.28, response interaction (F(1, 641) = 45.08, p < .001, SD = 1.28; t(641) = 6.40, p < .001, d = 0.50). See gG2 = 0017). Figure 2. Replicating our previous results, when the fol- lower copied the first mover’s prosocial initiative, Mediation the follower was seen as less purely motivated than the first mover (MFol-Copy = 3.20, SD = 1.51 vs. We tested our proposed causal process in two MFM-Copy = 4.10, SD = 1.50; paired t(322) = 11.58, ways. First, we calculated a difference score p < .001, d = 0.64). However, in line with predic- between judgments of the first mover and of the tions, when the follower differentiated their proso- follower on each of the two DVs: brand evaluations cial initiative from that of the first mover, there was and motive purity. These scores represent the mag- no difference in perceived motive purity (MFol-Dif- nitude of the first mover’s advantage on each fer = 4.01, SD = 1.62 vs. MFM-Differ = 4.10, SD = 1.56; dimension for each participant. We then estimated paired t(319) = 0.98, p = .33, d = 0.05). a mediation model with condition (copied = 0, dif- Moreover, independent samples t test revealed ferentiated = 1) as the independent variable, the that while the first mover was regarded similarly motive purity difference score as the mediator, and regardless of the follower’s response (t(641) = 0.06, the brand evaluation difference score as the depen- p = .95), the follower who copied was seen as less dent variable. We found evidence of a significant purely motivated than the follower who differenti- indirect effect (effect estimate = 0.48, SE = 0.08, ated (MFol-Copy = 3.20, SD = 1.51 vs. MFol-Dif- 95% CI = [ 0.32, 0.67]), suggesting that the fol- fer = 4.01, SD = 1.62; t(641) = 6.55, p < .001, lower’s choice to differentiate diminished partici- d = 0.52). pants’ preference for first movers by reducing the discrepancy in their perceived motives for doing good. Brand evaluations Next, we narrowed in on the follower conditions A similar repeated measures ANOVA revealed a and tested whether perceived motive purity medi- similar pattern of results for participants’ brand ated the effect of differentiation on brand attitudes evaluations. See Figure 2. We detected a main (copied = 0, differentiated = 1). Again, we found a effect of entry order such that first movers were significant indirect effect (effect estimate = 0.39, evaluated more positively than followers across SE = 0.06, 95% CI = [0.27, 0.52]), suggesting that conditions (Mfirst mover = 5.42, SD = 1.20 vs. prosocial followers were judged more favorably Mfollower = 4.93, SD = 1.41; F(1, 641) = 89.80, when finding their own cause because differentia- p < .001, gG2 = 0.035). We also detected a main tion attenuated the inference that their motives for effect whereby brands were viewed more positively doing good were impure. For completeness, we in the differentiated condition relative to the copied tested the same mediation pathway comparing first
Selfless First Movers 13 Motive Purity 7 Copy Condition Differentiate Condition 6 5 4 3 2 1 First Mover: Free coding classes First Mover: Free coding classes Follower: Free coding classes Follower: Volunteering for animals Brand Evaluations 7 Copy Condition Differentiate Condition 6 5 4 3 2 1 First Mover: Free coding classes First Mover: Free coding classes Follower: Free coding classes Follower: Volunteering for animals Figure 2. Perceived motive purity (1–7 scale) and brand evaluations (1–7 scale) by entry order (within-subjects) and condition (be- tween-subjects) from Study 4. Error bars represent standard errors. Note that across conditions, the first mover’s behavior is identical, but the follower’s response varies: The follower either launches a prosocial program that is similar to or differentiated from that of the first mover. movers across conditions and detected no indirect Although our pretest participants generally believed effect (effect estimate = 0.00, SE = 0.05, 95% that offering free coding classes for women accom- CI = [ 0.09, 0.09]). plishes more good for society than organizing care for stray animals, participants in our main study evaluated the former as less purely motivated and Discussion more negatively overall when it was enacted by a The results of Study 4 are consistent with follower. This result poses a potential challenge. Hypothesis 3: Follower brands are regarded more From an impact perspective, it would be preferable favorably when they perform more original proso- for follower brands to deploy their resources toward cial acts, even if these seem to have less charitable the most important societal problems and the most impact and seem to fit less well with the brand. impactful charitable causes regardless of entry
You can also read