Object Shape, Object Function, and Object Name
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
JOURNAL OF MEMORY AND LANGUAGE 38, 1–27 (1998) ARTICLE NO. ML972533 Object Shape, Object Function, and Object Name Barbara Landau University of Delaware and Linda Smith and Susan Jones Indiana University We investigated the roles of shape and function in object naming. Two-, three-, and five-year- olds and adults heard novel or familiar objects named; some participants also were instructed about the objects’ functions. Then they were asked to generalize the names to new objects that preserved shape or functional capability; some participants also judged the objects’ potential for carrying out the designated function. Children generalized the name by object shape regardless of instruction, but adults did so only in the absence of instruction or for familiar objects. Knowledge of function independent of naming became increasingly stronger and diverse over age. The strong developmental changes in the role of function bear on mechanisms of object naming. q 1998 Academic Press Common sense tells us that if we know function. As Miller and Johnson-Laird (1976) what an object is we often know what it does. argued, function is a critical aspect of catego- If asked ‘‘What is a chair?’’, one might re- rizing most human artifacts, so much so that spond ‘‘Something you sit on’’; if asked ‘‘function—at least for artifacts—is more ba- ‘‘What is a knife?’’, a plausible answer might sic to the definition than form’’ (p. 229). begin ‘‘Something you cut with.’’ Indeed, the In this paper, we consider the role of object importance of function in our knowledge of form and function in the early development named artifact categories is so deeply in- of object naming—specifically, the naming of grained that it often serves as a starting as- artifacts. Interest in the relative roles of object sumption in discussions of the ontogeny and appearance and function has a long history in nature of naming: Assuming that artifact both the developmental and adult literatures names are cover terms for artifact categories, on object naming and concept representation and that function is a critical component of (Bloom, 1996; Gentner, 1978; Malt & John- many such categories, it follows that artifact son, 1992), partly because these properties names should be generalized on the basis of tend to be viewed as major criteria for mem- bership in artifact categories. In our view, This research was supported by NICHD Grant PHS however, the contrast is an especially interest- RO1 HD-28675, NIMH Grant PHS RO1 MH-55240, and ing one because sensitivity to the properties Social and Behavioral Sciences Research Grant 12-FY93- themselves stems from different sources, their 0723 from the March of Dimes. We thank C. E. Wright for advice on statistical matters, the University Montes- relationship is complex, and there may be de- sori, Irvine Child Development Center, and the University velopmental changes between them that yield of Delaware Preschool for cooperating with us, and Diane insight into the development and mechanisms Beck, Jamie Chosak, Jennifer Nolan, and Melissa of object naming. Although function might be Schweisguth for help in conducting the studies. more important than form in adults’ lexical Address reprint requests and correspondence to Barbara Landau, Department of Psychology, Wolf Hall, Univer- categorization of artifacts, it may not be in sity of Delaware, Newark, Delaware 19716. E-mail: children’s. Further, tracking possible develop- blandau@udel.edu. mental changes in form and function in nam- 1 0749-596X/98 $25.00 Copyright q 1998 by Academic Press All rights of reproduction in any form reserved. AID JML 2533 / a010h$$$81 12-17-97 08:40:26 jmla AP: JML
2 LANDAU, SMITH, AND JONES ing may yield new insights into ontogenetic Landau, Smith, & Jones, 1992; Smith, Jones, & process. Information about form originates Landau, 1992). In these studies, adults show principally through the visual system, and an extreme bias: They reject objects that vary function through an understanding of the even a small amount in shape while accepting physical principles (such as gravity or force) objects that are radically different from the involved in the interactions of people with ob- standard in either texture or size. jects and objects with each other. The existence of a shape bias in no way One hypothesis about the relationship be- implies that it is the only mechanism at work tween these sources of information is that the during object naming. On the contrary, the early development of object naming princi- bias is highly context dependent, occuring in pally engages the perceptual systems, and only the specific context of object naming, but not secondarily the systems of general world- in nonnaming contexts that also require judg- knowledge about objects. Because of this, ments of similarity (see Jones & Smith, 1993; early object naming may be relatively ‘‘im- Smith, 1994, for reviews). Furthermore, in mune’’ to the influences of young children’s contexts where similarity of shape is pitted knowledge about objects, including for arti- directly against taxonomic category member- facts an object’s functions (Smith, Jones, & ship (an unusual situation in the real world), Landau, 1996). We ask whether this early im- the importance of shape declines with age (be- munity occurs only in naming tasks, whether tween three and five) and with familiarity of it occurs over a variety of object types, and category (Baldwin, 1992; Imai, Gentner, & whether and how lexical categorization comes Uchida, 1994). Thus, while shape is not the to incorporate information about function. only information that children use to establish In the following sections, we review evi- lexical membership, it does seem to enjoy a dence supporting the importance of shape in special status during children’s first encoun- early object naming, the importance of func- ters with newly named objects. tion in mature object naming, and ways of The contextual dependence of the shape evaluating the relative importance of these bias and its decline in the context of familiar over development. objects suggests that an emphasis on shape is modifiable when alternative bases for general- The Importance of Shape in Early Object ization become known. Object function pres- Naming ents itself as an obvious source of information In a variety of studies, we have shown that which might critically influence lexical cate- young children and adults tend to generalize gory formation. a novel artifact’s name to other objects that are similar to the exemplar object in shape The Importance of Function in Mature rather than texture, substance, or size. The Object Naming findings of preference for same-shape objects There is considerable evidence consistent are consistent with the findings on the taxo- with the idea that functional information is a nomic basis for categorization in young chil- critical, even core, aspect of our mature named dren and infants (Markman & Hutchinson, object categories. Rosch, Mervis, Gray, and 1984; Waxman, 1995), as many objects in the Boyes-Braem (1976) found that basic level same taxonomic category do share overall objects share a high degree of overlap in func- shape (Imai, Gentner, & Uchida, 1994; Rosch tion. Malt and Johnson (1992) showed that et al., 1976) and part structure (Tversky, object function—while not sufficient—can 1989). The preference for same shape in ob- play an important role in adults’ decisions ject naming appears in fragile form by the age about category membership for novel arti- of 2, becomes stronger by the age of 3, and facts, for example, their judgments of when is very strong in 5-year-olds (Jones, Smith, & an object can legitimately be called a ‘‘boat.’’ Landau, 1991; Landau, Smith, & Jones, 1988; Finally, Barsalou (1989) argued that adults AID JML 2533 / a010h$$$82 12-17-97 08:40:26 jmla AP: JML
OBJECT SHAPE, OBJECT FUNCTION 3 readily create novel ‘‘ad hoc’’ categories ‘‘hat’’ to other objects—such as keys—as he based on function, e.g., ‘‘things to take on a places them on his head. Nelson suggested picnic.’’ that once such categories are formed on the Evidence also shows that school-aged chil- basis of action, the corresponding category la- dren tend to rely on an object’s function in bels may be generalized on the basis of per- making lexical category assignments (Keil, ceptual properties such as form. Thus function 1989; Merriman, Scott, & Marazita, 1993; would serve as the core of the infant’s con- Richards, Goldfarb, Richards, & Hassen, cept, while perceptual similarity would follow 1989). Keil found that such older children as the basis for generalizing the object’s name. would select as a ‘‘hammer’’ an object that Knowledge of object function is indeed ac- was heavy enough to pound objects rather than quired before or during the earliest object one that had the overall appearance of a ham- naming, making plausible the idea that func- mer but could not carry out the standard flat- tion might be the core for object naming. In tening function due to a hole placed strategi- habituation tasks, Kolstad and Baillargeon cally in the hammer’s head. Richards et al. (1991) found that 10- and 12-month-old in- (1989) found that fourth to sixth graders and fants who were given experience with a func- adults judged object category membership on tional property (bottoms that serve to contain the basis of function rather than form. For a substance) then generalized on the basis of example, they were unlikely to accept an ob- that property rather than overall similarity of ject that was ‘‘just like a fork but had no appearance. Madole and Cohen (1992) found tines’’ as ‘‘still a fork.’’ that 14-month-olds were sensitive to form– Might object function also serve as a critical function correlations: After habituating to ob- basis for naming during the early years of jects exhibiting a given form together with a word learning? One framework, which we will given function (carried out by particular object call ‘‘action-based,’’ sees the foundation for parts), they dishabituated to objects pos- categories as the product of sensory and motor sessing novel form–function combinations. schemas that develop as the infant grasps, ma- In nonhabituation tasks, there are similar nipulates, and otherwise discovers an object’s findings. Brown (1990) reported that as early properties through action. According to Piaget as 18 months, children will select an object to (1954), the infant’s sensori-motor activities perform a function on the basis of its function- become organized over the first year of life ally relevant properties rather than other sa- as the infant discovers enduring properties of lient features. In one experiment, children objects such as their shape, size, and mallea- were enticed to retrieve a toy from some dis- bility. Discovering the physical effects of ob- tance away using a long rigid cane. Then they jects on each other constitutes some of the were given a choice of several objects to use infant’s earliest knowledge, and could thus as a tool. The children chose objects that pos- serve as a first step in the acquisition of names sessed both sufficient length and rigidity for for objects. the task in preference to objects that looked In this vein, Nelson (1974) argued that ob- more like the standard that had previously ject categories underlying early words are ini- been shown to work. Brown argued that chil- tially formed on the basis of action—what the dren bring to this problem-solving task an un- infant does with an object (see also Werner & derstanding of what is relevantly ‘‘similar’’ Kaplan, 1963). These functional categories are to the standard: here, length and rigidity, not the prelinguistic categories that serve as the surface texture or overall shape. basis for first words, which are often words These ideas about the early importance of for moveable and manipulable objects. Nelson function are also consistent with current ‘‘the- gave the example of the child who first utters ory-driven’’ views of word learning, which the word ‘‘hat’’ in the context of placing it on show that even young children appreciate the his head. Subsequently, he might generalize deeper properties underlying an object’s AID JML 2533 / a010h$$$82 12-17-97 08:40:26 jmla AP: JML
4 LANDAU, SMITH, AND JONES named category and that this understanding shape-based generalization among 2- to 5- allows them to bypass superficial (often ap- year-olds and adults (see Jones & Smith, pearance-based) similarities (e.g., Gelman, 1993; Landau, 1994, for review). In Gentner’s 1988). Of particular relevance is a study by study, the only indication of function-based McCarrell and Callanan (1995) which showed responses in adults was their spontaneous ten- that 2- and 4-year-olds were sensitive to corre- dency to call the hybrid a ‘‘jiggy-zimbo’’ spondences between an animal’s form and upon first seeing it. Their use of the function- its ‘‘function’’ (here, a behavior such as based ‘‘zimbo’’ as the head noun suggests that ‘‘stretches tall to eat cherries’’). This sensitiv- they thought the object was a member of ity was sufficient to promote inductive infer- the ‘‘zimbo’’ category, but one with ‘‘jiggy’’ ences about the behavior of new category properties (Lyons, 1977). members, suggesting that knowledge of func- Other studies also have found a dominance tion may play a critical role in tasks of catego- for an object’s overall appearance compared rization other than naming. For artifacts, func- to its function among young children and have tion may be the key conceptual property (see suggested a developmental increase in the im- Keil, 1989) and thus within this general view portance of function. Tomikawa and Dodd of word learning, an emphasis on function (1982) found that 2- and 3-year-olds more over shape might be expected in naming as readily learned names for perceptually similar well as other kinds of categorization. objects (that had different functions) than for objects sharing a common function (but hav- ing different shapes). Merriman (1993) found Evaluating the Importance of Shape and an increase between ages 3 and 6 in the ten- Function in Early Object Naming dency to generalize a novel name on the basis Previous studies on naming suggest that the of function (as opposed to appearance), but two different kinds of information may inter- even in 6-year-olds function was weighted act differently at different points in develop- only slightly more strongly than appearance. ment. In one of the earliest studies pitting ap- Although there are no comparable studies of pearance against function, Gentner (1978) in- novel object naming among older children and troduced children and adults to two different adults, related findings suggest that at these complex novel objects each possessing a very ages function may often prevail in the naming salient function (a lever dispensed jellybeans of familiar objects (Richards et al., 1989; for the ‘‘jiggy’’; an identical lever moved Rosch et al., 1976). parts of a face for the ‘‘zimbo’’). Participants These findings suggest that overall appear- were encouraged to manipulate each object ance, primarily similarity of shape, dominates repeatedly, viewing the function; during this, function in early object naming. However, each object was labeled. Later, participants there is an alternative interpretation. It is pos- were shown a hybrid object which possessed sible that, given the particular stimuli used, the form of the zimbo but produced jellybeans the appearances (shapes) of the objects were like the jiggy. They were asked whether it much more salient overall than the properties should be called a ‘‘jiggy’’ (following the ob- supporting the functions. If so, children might ject’s overall appearance) or a ‘‘zimbo’’ (fol- have generalized on the basis of object shape lowing its function). no matter what kind of task they were given. Gentner found a U-shaped function: 2- to To argue that similarity of shape not only 5-year-olds and adults chose strongly on the dominates object function, but specifically basis of appearance, but 5- to 15-year-olds does so in the context of object naming, one chose more than half the time on the basis of would need a test that discovers whether chil- function. The strong reliance on appearance dren are sensitive to the objects’ functional among 2- to 5-year-olds and adults is consis- properties and then a measure of whether this tent with our previous findings showing robust sensitivity enters into the object naming task. AID JML 2533 / a010h$$$82 12-17-97 08:40:26 jmla AP: JML
OBJECT SHAPE, OBJECT FUNCTION 5 A recent set of experiments assessed this knowledge of the objects’ functions. We did possibility. Smith, Jones, and Landau (1996) this using simple novel artifacts with simple showed 3-year-olds and adults highly complex novel functions (Experiment 1). Keeping in novel artifacts built of a distinctive base object mind that the findings may vary widely with with salient appended parts. In a Name task, object and function type, we then proceeded participants were asked whether test objects to ask the same questions using functions had the same name as the exemplar. In a Simi- which are known to be accessible to very larity task, participants were asked whether young children (Experiment 2). Finally, be- test objects were ‘‘the same as’’ the exemplar. cause the form and function relationship in Half of the participants were also shown a naming may be different for familiar than for function for either the base object or the sa- novel objects (Merriman, 1993), we investi- lient parts. Note that both tasks—Name and gated these roles in children’s and adults’ Similarity—required participants to make knowledge of familiar everyday objects (Ex- judgments on the basis of some kind of simi- periment 3). larity. The question was whether the basis would be the same for both tasks. EXPERIMENT 1 Results showed that adults’ judgments in We tested children’s and adults’ attention to the Name and Similarity tasks were influenced shape over material in object naming (Name by the functional information. Three-year- task), where the object’s function could be olds’ judgments in the Similarity task were carried out by virtue of its material. All parti- also influenced by the functions. However, cipants heard a novel object named. Half of children’s judgments in the Name task were them were explicitly shown a material-based immune to influence from information about function and half were not, and then all were function. Instead, their judgments were influ- asked to extend the standard object’s name enced by changes in the relative perceptual to new objects. Those who were shown the salience of the base vs the parts. These results function were also directly asked whether led Smith et al. to suggest that early object each object could be used to carry out the naming may in some way be so tightly tied designated function (Function task). Our ques- to certain aspects of perception that it is cut tion was whether the instructions about object off from other influences of world knowledge function could influence participants’ atten- that the child has. tion to shape vs material, and whether the pat- These results suggest a strong hypothesis tern of responses in the Name task would dif- for the development of object naming. Spe- fer from those of the Function task. cifically, knowledge of function—while criti- cal for the formation and use of mature object Method representations that underlie naming—may Participants. Seventy-two participants were play a quite minor role in early object naming. tested, including twenty-four 3- and 5-year- Further, the results suggest that immunity to olds and adults, with each age group balanced functional knowledge should be seen earlier for gender. Mean ages of the children were 3; than age 3, but may begin to change with 5 and 5; 4 (Ranges Å 3; 0–3; 10, 4; 11–5; development after this point. 11). Children were drawn from preschools in The purpose of the present experiments was the area around Irvine, California, and adults to begin to test this account, using a wider were undergraduates at the University of Cali- developmental window and a broader range fornia, Irvine, who participated for class of objects and functions than those used by credit. Half of each age group was randomly Smith et al. (1996). We began by challenging assigned to each of the two experimental con- children’s and adults’ attention to object shape ditions described below. in a naming task and comparing it to their Design and materials. In the No-Function responses in a task that directly assessed their condition, participants were shown the stan- AID JML 2533 / a010h$$$83 12-17-97 08:40:26 jmla AP: JML
6 LANDAU, SMITH, AND JONES and therefore provide a comparison point to previous findings of a shape bias in the context of naming. However, the materials were novel and designed specifically to support clear functions. The test set for each standard included nine objects: two subsets of four plus a replica of the standard. Each subset contained two Same-shape and two Same-material objects. The Same-material objects were the same ma- terial as the standard but different shapes; and the Same-shape objects were the same shape as the standard but were made of different materials—ones that could not support the standard’s function. In the Rif set, the novel materials were nonporous, either styrofoam or a tightly woven, glossy fabric covering a thick soft pad. In the Dax set, these materials were hard and impenetrable, either brick-shaped Lego parts assembled into the U shape or plas- ter painted to a high gloss. In both the Rif and Dax sets, each Same-shape object was matched in color to one Same-material object. Procedure. Participants in the No-Function FIG. 1. Objects used in Experiment 1. The ‘‘standard’’ condition were shown one of the standards Rif and Dax are shown at the left. Test objects included two Same-shape (different material) and two Same-mate- and told, ‘‘See this? This is a dax.’’ Then the rial (different shape) objects (see text for full description). standard was placed to the side, still in full view, and each of the nine test objects was removed from a box one at a time and shown dard object and heard it labeled, but no in- to the participant, who was asked ‘‘Is this a formation about function was provided. In rif/dax?’’ (Name question). After he or she the Function condition, participants saw the answered yes or no to each object, it was put standard and heard it labeled, but they were back in the box and the next object was also told and shown what the standard was brought out. Each object was queried twice, used for. followed by the second standard and test set, Two sets of objects were constructed (see for a total of 18 test trials per set and 36 in Fig. 1). The standard for the Rif set was a total. roughly 2’’ 1 2’’ roundish object made of Participants in the Function condition un- pale yellow sponge used in standard sink derwent the same procedure, with the follow- sponges. The Rif’s function was to wipe up ing exception. They were shown the standard water. The standard for the Dax set was a 2’’ and told, ‘‘See this? This is a rif. Rifs are 1 2’’ squarish U-shaped object made of dark made by a special company so they can do brown cork, similar to that used for bulletin this (demonstrate function). See? So rifs are boards. The Dax’s function was to support made so you can mop up water with them.’’ stick pins of the type used to hold messages Exactly parallel instructions were given for on cork boards. The shapes of these objects the Dax standard. To demonstrate the function were the same as those used in previous stud- for the Rif set, the experimenter took a small ies of the shape bias (e.g., Jones, Smith, & bottle of water, spilled some on the table, and Landau, 1991; Landau, Smith, & Jones, 1988), mopped up the puddle with the object, saying AID JML 2533 / a010h$$$83 12-17-97 08:40:26 jmla AP: JML
OBJECT SHAPE, OBJECT FUNCTION 7 ‘‘I can mop up water with it.’’ The Rif was The reliability of these findings was con- then set next to the bottle of water, where it firmed by a mixed analysis of variance con- remained during the test trials. To demonstrate ducted on the numbers of ‘‘yes’’ responses, the function for the Dax set, the experimenter with age and condition as between-subjects took a push pin and stuck it into the surface factors and property (Same shape/Same mate- of the object, saying ‘‘I can stick pins into it rial) the within-subjects factor. This analysis and they’ll hold.’’ The object with the embed- revealed a main effect of property, F(1,66) Å ded pin was left visible during the test trials. 35.04, p õ .01, and interactions between age After the entire naming procedure was com- and property, F(2,66) Å 18.52, p õ .01, and plete, participants in the Function condition condition and property, F(1,66) Å 58.15, p were asked directly whether they thought each õ .01, which were subsumed by a three-way of the test objects could support the designated interaction among age, condition, and prop- function of the relevant standard (Function erty, F(2,66) Å 11.86, p õ .01. Planned com- question). Participants were directed to look parisons within age showed reliable prefer- at the standard and were asked ‘‘Do you re- ences for Same-shape over Same-material ob- member what I did with this one?’’ If they jects for the 3’s in both the No-Function and did not respond promptly, the experimenter Function conditions, t’s (66) Å 5.91, 4.04, re- reminded them, saying ‘‘I mopped up water spectively, p’s õ .05, and for the 5’s and with it (stuck a pin in it so it would hold).’’ adults in the No Function condition, t’s (66) Then they were asked either ‘‘Could you mop Å 5.78, 4.79, respectively, p’s õ .05. The up water with this one?’’ (for the Rif set) or adults in the Function condition showed a reli- ‘‘Could you stick a pin in this one so it would able preference for material over shape, t(66) hold?’’ (for the Dax set). In this part of the Å 06.66, p õ .05. Planned comparisons were procedure, each test object was queried just also carried out within age to compare shape once. The test items were presented in one of and material responses across No-Function two randomized orders, and the presentation and Function conditions. Only the adults order of object sets was counterbalanced over showed a reliable difference between the two participants. conditions, although the fives approached sig- nificance, t’s (66) Å .66, 1.86, 4.06 for the Results and Discussion 3’s, 5’s, and adults, critical t Å 1.99, p õ .05. Table 1 shows the mean proportions of An examination of individual performances ‘‘yes’’ responses to the Name question for provides greater insight into the develop- Same-shape and Same-material objects over mental trend. Figure 2 shows the scatterplots age and condition (No Function and Func- of individual performances—the number of tion). The table also shows the results for the ‘‘yes’’ responses to Same-shape vs Same-ma- Function question (Function condition only). terial responses for each individual over age Preliminary analyses revealed no differences and condition. The left panel shows the data between object sets, so the results for both for the Name question. Each dot represents questions were collapsed across object set one child’s performance. Individuals who said (Dax vs Rif). ‘‘yes’’ to more Same-shape than Same-mate- Name question. As is apparent, 3-year-olds rial objects fall in the lower half of the figure, generalized the name strongly on the basis whereas those who did the reverse fall in the of shape in both No-Function and Function upper half. Individuals who accepted an equal conditions. Adults, in contrast, generalized on number of the two object types fall along the the basis of shape in the No-Function condi- diagonal. tion but material in the Function condition. Three-year-olds in both conditions and 5- Five-year-olds generalized strongly on the ba- year-olds and adults in the No Function condi- sis of shape in No-Function condition and tion accepted more Same-shape than Same- weakly in the Function condition. material objects. The dominance of shape over AID JML 2533 / a010h$$$83 12-17-97 08:40:26 jmla AP: JML
8 LANDAU, SMITH, AND JONES TABLE 1 Mean Proportions (SE) of ‘‘Yes’’ Responses to Same-Shape and Same Material Objects (Experiment 1) No function Function Name question Name question Function question Condition,a question, Same Same Same Same Same Same and age shape material shape material shape material 3 yr .92 (.04) .18 (.06) .76 (.07) .25 (.08) .38 (.11) .58 (.12) 5 yr .82 (.08) .09 (.05) .41 (.10) .34 (.08) .04 (.04) .78 (.10) Adults .69 (.09) .09 (.05) .06 (.03) .89 (.06) .06 (.03) .93 (.05) a n Å 12 in each age group and condition. Participants in the No-Function condition answered only the Name question; participants in the Function condition answered both the Name and Function questions. material among 3’s was reliable in both condi- whether the test objects could carry out the tions, n Å 12 vs 0 and 10 vs 0, as it was among designated functions. Results from this ques- 5’s and adults in the No-Function condition, n tion are also shown in Table 1. The first issue Å 10 vs 0 and 9 vs 1, respectively, p’s õ .05, was whether the Function question elicited the Binomial tests (assuming a random probabil- same generalization pattern as the Name ques- ity of .50 in this and all further Binomials). tion. We answered this by directly comparing Adults also showed a reliable dominance of responses to the Name and Function questions material over shape in the Function condition, among participants who had been instructed n Å 11 vs 0, p õ .05, Binomial test, and the about function (the Function condition). Pre- 5’s showed no preference in that condition, liminary analyses revealed no effects of object n Å 4 shape, 4 material, 4 equivocal. The set, so the data were collapsed over this factor. preference for Same-shape objects differed re- The mean proportions of ‘‘yes’’ responses to liably across the two conditions for the 5-year- Same-shape and Same-material objects were olds and adults, p’s õ .05, Fisher Exact tests. submitted to a mixed analysis of variance with Thus, the analysis of individuals was consis- age the between-subjects factor and property tent with the group analysis. and question type (Name vs Function) as To summarize the results of the Name ques- within-subjects factors. The results showed a tion, when a novel object was introduced and main effect of property, F(1,33) Å 49.10, p named with no further information, young õ .01, and interactions between age and prop- children and adults relied on shape as the basis erty, F(2,33) Å 34.93, p õ .01, and question for generalizing the name. When competing type and property, F(1,33) Å 38.57, p õ .01, information about the functional importance both subsumed by a three-way interaction of material was provided, adults switched among age, question type, and property, from naming by shape to naming by material. F(2,33) Å 8.46, p õ .01. Planned comparisons The 5-year-olds were affected by instructional revealed several reliable differences across condition, with a different pattern of prefer- question type: The 3- and 5-year-olds pre- ences when given functional instruction. Un- ferred shape when asked the Name question like adults, however, the 5-year-olds did not but not when asked the Function question; the reliably shift to material when told the objects’ adults showed no such difference, as they pre- functions. ferred material when asked either question, t’s Name vs function question. Participants (33) Å 2.5, 2.86, .14, p’s õ .05. Thus the two who were given instructions about the objects’ questions elicited different responses in 3- and functions were also queried directly about 5-year-olds, but not adults. AID JML 2533 / a010h$$$83 12-17-97 08:40:26 jmla AP: JML
OBJECT SHAPE, OBJECT FUNCTION 9 FIG. 2. Individual participants’ choices of Same-shape vs Same-material objects for the Name Question and the Function question in Experiment 1. Closed circles represent individuals who were not instructed about function (No-Function condition) and were asked the Name question only. Open circles represent individuals who were instructed about function and were asked both the Name and Function questions (Function condition). Individuals who chose on the basis of shape fall into the lower half of the plot whereas those who chose on the basis of material fall into the upper half. The second issue was whether children un- and adults reliably preferred material, t’s (33) derstood the standard objects’ functions, Å 05.3, 06.2, respectively, p’s õ .05. Thus, whether they generalized correctly in the although both the 3’s and 5’s showed different Function question (i.e., on the basis of mate- response patterns in the Name vs Function rial), and most important, whether this under- questions, only the 5’s and adults reliably re- standing affected their generalizations in the sponded reliably ‘‘correctly’’ to the Function Name question. Planned comparisons for the question—that is, on the basis of material. Function question showed that 5-year-olds Comparisons for the Name question showed AID JML 2533 / a010h$$$84 12-17-97 08:40:26 jmla AP: JML
10 LANDAU, SMITH, AND JONES that 3-year-olds reliably preferred shape and quadrant of the right panels in Fig. 2). This that adults reliably preferred material, t’s (33) group included four 3-year-olds and nine 5- Å 3.6, 05.9, respectively, p’s õ .05, just as year-olds. The second group included the re- in the previous analysis of the Name question. maining 11 children. Given this division, we Individual participants’ pattern of shape vs analyzed the number of shape vs material- material responses for the two questions con- based responses in the Name question, using firmed these patterns (see Fig. 2, left and right an analysis of variance with age and group panels). Three-year-olds reliably preferred as the between-subjects factors and property shape when asked the Name question, n Å 10 (shape, material) as the within-subjects factor. shape, 0 material preference, p õ .01, Bino- There was a main effect of property only, with mial test, but showed no reliable preference responses by shape reliably greater than by when asked the Function question, n Å 7 ma- material, M’s Å .58, .29, respectively, F(1,20) terial, 3 shape preference, p Å .17, Binomial Å 5.5, p õ .05. There were no interactions. test. Seven of the 3-year-olds switched from These results suggest that object function a shape preference when asked the Name may not enter into object naming early in de- question to a material preference when asked velopment, even when children understand the the Function question; one did the reverse, object properties related to function. First, confirming a reliable difference in their re- there were no reliable effects of instructional sponses across the two questions, p Å .03, condition on the 3-year-olds’ responses to the Binomial test. Five-year-olds showed no pref- Name question and weak effects on the 5- erence when asked the Name question, n Å 4 year-olds’ responses; only adults were reliably shape, 4 material, but reliably preferred mate- and strongly affected, generalizing names dif- rial when asked the Function question, n Å ferently when given instruction about func- 11 material, 0 shape, p õ .01, Binomial test. tion. Second, even children who generalized Three of the 5-year-olds switched from a on the basis of material in the Function ques- shape preference in the Name question to a tion still generalized by shape in the Name material preference in the Function question; question. Thus, although some children under- none did the reverse. Adults reliably preferred stood that material was critical to carrying out material for both questions, n Å 11 material, the function, this knowledge did not enter into 0 shape for the Name question, n Å 12 mate- their naming responses. rial, 0 shape for the Function question, p’s õ .01, Binomial test. Thus, although there were It is possible, however, that functional reliable differences across question type for knowledge did not affect naming because the both the 3- and 5-year-olds, the nature of the objects and functions used in Experiment 1 change was different. Three-year-olds showed were quite novel and perhaps not part of natu- shape-based responses when asked the Name ral, early developing systems of knowledge. question, and diffuse responses when asked Only the adults may have truly understood the the Function question. Five-year-olds showed functions, which were surely familiar to them. divided responses when asked the Name ques- Accordingly, in Experiment 2 we asked the tion and material-based responses when asked Name and Function questions using objects the Function question. whose functions should be understood by age Did the children’s varying understanding of 2: retrieval of one object by another (Brown, the objects’ functions affect their responses to 1990) and containment (Kolstad & Baillar- the Name question? To address this, we di- geon, 1991). If the results of Experiment 1 vided the children into two groups. One group were due to the unusual or complex nature of responded correctly to the Function question, the functions we devised, then more familiar specifically, accepting greater than 50% of the functions might be more likely to enter into time on material and less than 50% of the time naming for 3- and 5-year-olds and perhaps on shape (locating them in the upper left-hand even for younger children. To test this possi- AID JML 2533 / a010h$$$84 12-17-97 08:40:26 jmla AP: JML
OBJECT SHAPE, OBJECT FUNCTION 11 bility, we extended our study downward function: One had a large hole in the bottom, in age. a second was perforated with holes throughout the surface, and the third was made of flimsy EXPERIMENT 2 tissue paper. The object with the hole in the bottom violated the containment function in a Method way similar to the containers used by Kolstad Participants. Ninety-six participants were and Baillargeon (1991). Each of the Same- tested, 24 each of 2-year-olds (M age 2;6, function objects was a different shape from range 24–35 months), 3-year-olds (M age 3;7, the standard. In each set, each Same-shape range 3; –3;11), 5-year-olds (M age 5;5, range object was matched in color to one Same- 5;1–5;11), and adults. Participants were function object. drawn from the same populations as in Experi- Procedures were identical to those of Ex- ment 1 and age groups were balanced for periment 1, except for appropriate modifica- gender. tion of the Function condition instructions for Design, materials, and procedure. The the new object sets. In addition, the instruc- overall design was identical to Experiment 1, tions were simplified a bit to ensure that even with half of the participants in each age group the youngest children would understand them. randomly assigned to the No-Function condi- For the Cane set, participants were told ‘‘See tion and half to the Function condition. How- this? This is a dax (rif). And this is what I ever, all participants were asked both the can do with it. I can pull toys with it.’’ The Name question and the Function question. experimenter took a small toy and placed it at There were two sets of objects (see Fig. 3). the far end of a table, several feet away, then The standard for the Cane set was a 6’’ long hooked it with the head of the cane and pulled blue cane made of hardened clay. The function it toward herself. For the Container set, the of this standard was to retrieve small toys from experimenter introduced the object in the several feet away by hooking and pulling same way, then poured a small amount of wa- them. The test items included a replica of the ter into the standard and lifted the container, standard plus six additional items. Three of moving it along a path and saying ‘‘I can carry these were the Same-shape as the standard, water with it.’’ but could not support the designated function: One was too short (2’’) and the remaining Following this introduction, participants two were made of either lightweight fabric or were tested as in Experiment 1 on the Name beads strung on a cotton thread, each too question (‘‘Is this a dax?’’) with two trials for flimsy to secure and pull small toys. These each of six objects per set, a total of 24 trials. objects violated the relevant function in ways After all items in both sets were queried, parti- similar to those in Brown’s (1990) study. The cipants were asked the Function question. remaining three test items could support the They were shown the standard once more. Same-function as the standard, being the same Those hearing Function instructions were length and material. However, their handle asked ‘‘Do you remember what I did with this pieces and heads were shaped differently. one?’’ In most cases, participants answered The standard for the Container set was a correctly. If they did not respond spontane- 2’’ 1 2’’ 1 2’’ trapezoidal-type container ously, the experimenter prompted by saying made of hardened clay. It stood on short legs ‘‘I pulled the toy (carried water) with it.’’ All and was completely open at the top. Its desig- participants then were asked for each test item, nated function was to carry water. The test ‘‘Could you pull a toy from over there with items included a replica of the standard plus this?’’ or ‘‘Could you carry water with this?’’ three Same-shape and three Same-function ob- Note that although the No-Function partici- jects. The Same-shape objects were identical pants were not given information about the in shape but could not support the designated standard’s function, they could plausibly AID JML 2533 / a010h$$$84 12-17-97 08:40:26 jmla AP: JML
12 LANDAU, SMITH, AND JONES FIG. 3. Objects used in Experiment 2. The ‘‘standard’’ for each set is shown at the left. Test objects included three Same-shape (different function) and three Same-function (different shape) objects. The specified function for the Cane set was to pull toys from across a table. The function for the Container set was to carry water. The Same-shape objects for the Cane set violated the specified function by being too short or too flimsy; those for the Container set violated function by possessing holes in the surface or being composed of thin paper. AID JML 2533 / a010h$2533 12-17-97 08:40:26 jmla AP: JML
OBJECT SHAPE, OBJECT FUNCTION 13 judge whether or not each of the test items tions preferred Same-function over Same- could carry out the specified function. shape objects, Bonferroni critical d Å .24, All test items were presented in one of two p Å .05. The adult pattern in the Name ques- randomized orders, and the presentation order tion was responsible for the only reliable pair- of the Cane or Container set was counterbal- wise effects of condition: They preferred anced. Same-shape objects in the No-Function condi- tion, and Same-function objects in the Func- Results and Discussion tion condition, Bonferroni critical d Å .24, In Table 2 are shown the mean proportions p Å .05. of ‘‘yes’’ responses to Same-shape and Same- To assess the differential effects of question function objects over age and condition for type among children, we conducted a separate the Name question (‘‘Is this a dax?’’) and the four-way analysis of variance (age, condition, Function question (‘‘Could you pull a toy/ question type, and property) on the children’s carry water with this?’’). When asked the data only. The relevant means, shown in Table Name question, participants of all ages pre- 2, indicate differences between the two ques- ferred the Same-shape objects, except for the tions among 3- and 5-year-olds. This was re- adults in the Function condition, who general- flected in the principal result from the analy- ized on the basis of function. When asked sis, a three-way interaction among age, ques- the Function question, 3- and 5-year-olds and tion type, and property, F(2,66) Å 9.56, p õ adults preferred the Same-function objects .05. This interaction subsumed all other ef- whereas 2-year-olds showed a slight prefer- fects, which included main effects of age, ence for Same-shape objects. Thus there were F(2,66) Å 7.3, p õ .01, question type, F(1,66) qualitatively different patterns of response for Å 12.39, p õ .01, and property, F(1,66) Å the two questions, and within each question, 10.35, p õ .01, two-way interactions between the developmental pattern was different. age and question type, F(2,66) Å 4.28, p õ Preliminary analyses suggested that there .05, age and property, F(2,66) Å 3.74, p õ were different interactions involving object .05, and question type and property, F(1,66) set for the Name and the Function questions. Å 119.83, p õ .01. There was no effect of or We examined each of these interactions and interactions with condition. found that none of them was relevant to or Planned comparisons evaluated the three- mitigated the results relevant to the principal way interaction by assessing the children’s hypotheses. Therefore, we collapsed over ob- preferences across question type. The 3- and ject set for further analyses. Preliminary anal- 5-year-olds both accepted more Same-shape yses also indicated a four-way interaction objects when asked the Name question than among age, question type, condition, and the Function question, t’s (66) Å 3.62, 3.0, property, F(3,88) Å 4.03, p õ .01, reflecting p’s õ .05, and more Same-function objects the qualitatively different patterns of re- when asked the Function question than the sponse for the two questions. Name question, t’s (66) Å 03.25, 06.75, p’s Planned comparisons were conducted to as- õ.05. Two-year-olds did not accept same- sess these patterns, specifically examining the shape objects differentially over the two ques- different effects of condition and property tions, but did accept more Same-function ob- over age, separately for the two questions. jects when asked the Function question than When asked the Name question, participants the Name question, t’s (66) Å 0.75, 03.87, in all age groups and in both conditions pre- p õ .05. ferred Same-shape over Same-function ob- Individual participant plots for the two jects, except for the adults in the Function questions are shown in Fig. 4. The overall condition, who reliably preferred Same-func- pattern supports the group analysis: When tion to Same-shape. When asked the Function asked the Name question, 2-, 3-, and 5-year- question, 5-year-olds and adults in both condi- olds in both conditions showed a reliable pref- AID JML 2533 / a010h$$$84 12-17-97 08:40:26 jmla AP: JML
14 LANDAU, SMITH, AND JONES TABLE 2 Mean Proportions (SE) of ‘‘Yes’’ Responses to Same-Shape and Same-Function Objects (Experiment 2) No function Function M Conditiona Same Same Same Same Same Same and age shape function shape function shape function Name question 2 yr .73 (.08) .41 (.12) .76 (.08) .41 (.12) .74 .41 3 yr .72 (.11) .25 (.13) .73 (.07) .32 (.13) .72 .28 5 yr .49 (.11) .08 (.08) .52 (.11) .19 (.08) .51 .14 Adults .69 (.09) .33 (.10) .26 (.06) .78 (.06) Function question 2 yr .73 (.08) .62 (.10) .86 (.05) .82 (.10) .80 .72 3 yr .40 (.10) .59 (.11) .46 (.09) .50 (.10) .43 .55 5 yr .22 (.05) .68 (.07) .33 (.08) .69 (.08) .27 .68 Adults .39 (.06) .94 (.04) .19 (.03) .92 (.04) .29 .93 a n Å 12 in each age group and condition (No Function, Function). All participants answered both the Name and Function questions. erence for Same-shape objects, n Å 19 vs 0, the analysis. In the group that accepted Same- 18 vs 1, 19 vs 1 over the three age groups, function objects greater than 50% of the time p’s õ .05, Binomial test. Adults showed a and Same-shape objects less than 50% of the reliable preference for Same-shape objects in time, there were eight 3-year-olds and four- the No-Function condition, n Å 10 of 12, but teen 5-year-olds; these were divided evenly for Same-function objects in the Function con- between the No-Function and Function condi- dition, n Å 11 of 12, x2 (1) Å 13.59, p õ .05. tions. In the second group (who did not meet When asked the Function question, 2-year- the criterion) were the remainder of the chil- olds did not show any distinct preference for dren, including sixteen 3-year-olds and ten 5- Same-shape vs Same-function objects, n Å 8 year-olds. Again, these were divided evenly vs 7, respectively, whereas 3’s, 5’s, and adults between the conditions. Given this division, preferred Same-function objects. n Å 12 vs 6, we analyzed the responses to the Name ques- 19 vs 2, 24 vs 0 for each age group, p’s õ tion, using an analysis of variance with age .05, for the 5’s and adults, p Å .07 for the 3’s, and group as between-subjects factors and Binomial tests. The only difference from the property (Same-shape, Same-function) as the overall analysis is the addition that 3-year- within-subjects factor. The results showed a olds reliably preferred Same-function objects main effect of property only, with children when asked the Function question. accepting Same-shape objects more often than As in Experiment 1, we asked whether chil- Same-function objects, M proportions Å .62, dren’s varying understanding of the functions .19, respectively, F(1,44) Å 49.45, p õ .01. affected their responses to the Name question. To summarize, the results of this experi- We again divided children into two groups in ment were consistent with those of Experi- accord with their performance on the Function ment 1. As in that experiment, the Name ques- question (using the same criterion as in Exper- tion (‘‘Is this an X?’’)elicited a preference for iment 1). There were no 2-year-olds who an- Same-shape objects among both children and swered the Function question correctly by this adults when no functional instruction about criterion, so these children were dropped from the objects was provided. When functional in- AID JML 2533 / a010h$$$84 12-17-97 08:40:26 jmla AP: JML
OBJECT SHAPE, OBJECT FUNCTION 15 FIG. 4. Individual participants’ choices of Same-shape vs Same-function objects for the Name question and the Function question in Experiment 2. Closed circles represent individuals who had not been instructed about function (No-Function condition); open circles those who were instructed about function (Function condition). All participants were asked the Name question and the Function question. formation was provided, adults showed a their relationship to the Name question also strong bias for objects that could support the confirmed and extended the results of Experi- same function. These functions depended on ment 1. As in Experiment 1, adults in the material in Experiment 1 and material, length, Name question were strongly affected by in- and global object structure in the present ex- struction about function, whereas they an- periment. swered the Function question correctly The results of the Function question and whether instructed or not. Clearly, the adults AID JML 2533 / a010h$$$85 12-17-97 08:40:26 jmla AP: JML
16 LANDAU, SMITH, AND JONES could determine by inspection whether the ob- of a novel object name. This is consistent with jects could carry out a given function. Also as two possibilities. One is that functional in Experiment 1, 3- and 5-year-olds responded knowledge must be extremely robust to be differently to the Function question than they incorporated into object naming generaliza- had to the Name question, and there was an tions. The second is that the initial links be- increase between age 3 and 5 in the strength tween perception and naming are so strong and accuracy of responses to the Function that even extremely robust functional knowl- question. Two-year-olds showed no evidence edge is excluded from naming judgments. of being able to correctly judge whether the These two possibilities can be tested using objects could carry out their functions. Cru- common objects whose functions are well un- cially, the developmental changes in the 3- derstood. and 5-year-olds’ responses to the Function question did not appear to affect their re- EXPERIMENT 3 sponses to the Name question: There was an Method overall preference for Same-shape objects among children who did respond correctly to Participants. Ninety-two participants were the Function question as well as those who tested, twenty-four 2-year-olds (M age 2;6, did not. range 29–31 months), twenty-two 3-year-olds Although there was no effect of being in- (M age 3;4, range 36–46 months), twenty-two structed about function among 5-year-olds in 5-year-olds (M age 5;3, range 60–70 months), the present experiment, these children were and twenty-four adults. They were drawn from rather conservative in their generalization of the same populations as in previous experi- the name (compared to levels of generaliza- ments, as well as from preschools in the area tion in Experiment 1). Why didn’t they gener- around Bloomington, Indiana and Newark, alize strongly to all Same-shape objects? Delaware. All age groups were balanced for Spontaneous comments suggest that their gender. knowledge about named objects might have Design. The overall design and procedure played a role. For example, in the No-Function were similar to the previous experiments, ex- condition, the cloth and bead canes were cept that question type was a between-subjects sometimes rejected with comments such as variable, with roughly half of the participants ‘‘it’s a piece of towel’’ or ‘‘it’s like a neck- in each age group asked the Name question lace’’ and the pyramid container with ‘‘looks and half the Function question. Participants like a volcano.’’ In the Function condition, who were asked the Name question were the perforated and paper containers were shown a standard, heard it labeled with its real sometimes rejected with comments such as name, and heard its function described. Then ‘‘has holes in it,’’ ‘‘it’s too soft to pull,’’ or they were shown a series of test objects and ‘‘it’s not hard.’’ Although these comments were asked whether each had the same name were not frequent enough to analyze, they sug- as the standard. Participants who were asked gest that the 5-year-olds’ decisions to reject the Function question observed as the experi- some of the Same-shape objects were based menter showed them how the standard carried either on their similarity to other known ob- out its function and then were asked whether jects or, in the Function condition, the inabil- the test objects could be used for that function. ity of the object to support the designated However, they never heard any of the objects function. named. The findings thus support the notion that Materials. There were two sets of objects, there are strong developmental changes in one with a well-known standard and the other children’s knowledge of objects’ functions, with a less well-known standard. The standard and that this knowledge is only slowly incor- for the Comb set was a 4’’ 1 3’’ blue plastic porated into children’s initial generalizations 10-prong comb with no handle. The test set AID JML 2533 / a010h$$$85 12-17-97 08:40:26 jmla AP: JML
You can also read