Mindfulness Training as a Clinical Intervention: A Conceptual and Empirical Review
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
Mindfulness Training as a Clinical Intervention: A Conceptual and Empirical Review Ruth A. Baer, University of Kentucky Interventions based on training in mindfulness skills are (Goleman & Schwartz, 1976; Kabat-Zinn, 1982). The cur- becoming increasingly popular. Mindfulness involves in- rent mindfulness literature describes numerous meditation tentionally bringing one’s attention to the internal and ex- exercises designed to develop mindfulness skills (Hanh, ternal experiences occurring in the present moment, and 1976; Kabat-Zinn, 1990, 1994; Linehan, 1993b). Many is often taught through a variety of meditation exercises. encourage individuals to attend to the internal experiences This review summarizes conceptual approaches to mind- occurring in each moment, such as bodily sensations, thoughts, and emotions. Others encourage attention to fulness and empirical research on the utility of mindfulness- aspects of the environment, such as sights and sounds based interventions. Meta-analytic techniques were in- (Kabat-Zinn, 1994; Linehan, 1993b). All suggest that corporated to facilitate quantification of findings and mindfulness should be practiced with an attitude of non- comparison across studies. Although the current empir- judgmental acceptance. That is, phenomena that enter the ical literature includes many methodological flaws, find- individual’s awareness during mindfulness practice, such as ings suggest that mindfulness-based interventions may perceptions, cognitions, emotions, or sensations, are ob- be helpful in the treatment of several disorders. Method- served carefully but are not evaluated as good or bad, true ologically sound investigations are recommended in order or false, healthy or sick, or important or trivial (Marlatt & to clarify the utility of these interventions. Kristeller, 1999). Thus, mindfulness is the nonjudgmental Key words: mindfulness, meditation, meta-analysis, observation of the ongoing stream of internal and external treatment outcome. [Clin Psychol Sci Prac 10: 125–143, stimuli as they arise. 2003] Until recently, mindfulness has been a relatively unfa- miliar concept in much of our culture (Kabat-Zinn, 1982), Mindfulness is a way of paying attention that originated in perhaps because of its origins in Buddhism. Kabat-Zinn Eastern meditation practices. It has been described as (2000) suggests that mindfulness practice may be beneficial “bringing one’s complete attention to the present experi- to many people in Western society who might be un- ence on a moment-to-moment basis” (Marlatt & Kris- willing to adopt Buddhist traditions or vocabulary. Thus, teller, 1999, p. 68) and as “paying attention in a particular Western researchers and clinicians who have introduced way: on purpose, in the present moment, and nonjudg- mindfulness practice into mental health treatment pro- mentally” (Kabat-Zinn, 1994, p. 4). The ability to direct grams usually teach these skills independently of the reli- one’s attention in this way can be developed through the gious and cultural traditions of their origins (Kabat-Zinn, practice of meditation, which is defined as the intentional 1982; Linehan, 1993b). In the current empirical literature, self-regulation of attention from moment to moment clinical interventions based on training in mindfulness skills are described with increasing frequency, and their popu- larity appears to be growing rapidly. According to Salmon, Send correspondence to Ruth A. Baer, Department of Psychol- Santorelli, and Kabat-Zinn (1998), over 240 hospitals and ogy, 115 Kastle Hall, University of Kentucky, Lexington, KY clinics in the United States and abroad were offering stress 40506-0044. E-mail: rbaer@uky.edu. reduction programs based on mindfulness training as of DOI: 10.1093/clipsy/bpg015 2003 AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGICAL ASSOCIATION D12 125
1997. Mindfulness training is also a central component of described in this review often are directed toward the in- dialectical behavior therapy (Linehan, 1993a, 1993b), an ner experiences of the individual (e.g., thoughts, emo- increasingly popular approach to the treatment of border- tions) and emphasize a less goal-directed, nonjudgmental line personality disorder. The empirical literature on the observation. Langer (1989) has cautioned against drawing effects of mindfulness training contains many method- unwarranted parallels between the two forms of mindful- ological weaknesses, but it suggests that mindfulness inter- ness, noting that they are derived from very different his- ventions may lead to reductions in a variety of problematic torical and cultural backgrounds. conditions, including pain, stress, anxiety, depressive re- lapse, and disordered eating (e.g., Kabat-Zinn, 1982;Kabat- INTERVENTIONS BASED ON MINDFULNESS TRAINING Zinn et al., 1992; Kristeller & Hallett, 1999; Shapiro, Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction Schwartz, & Bonner, 1998; Teasdale et al., 2000). The most frequently cited method of mindfulness training This review summarizes the recent literature on mind- is the mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) pro- fulness training as a clinical intervention. First, current gram, formerly known as the stress reduction and relax- methods for teaching mindfulness skills are described. ation program (SR-RP; Kabat-Zinn, 1982, 1990). It was Next, conceptual approaches that articulate how mindful- developed in a behavioral medicine setting for populations ness skills may be helpful in treating clinical conditions are with a wide range of chronic pain and stress-related disor- summarized. Finally, the empirical literature on the effects ders. The program is conducted as an 8- to 10-week course of mindfulness training is reviewed. for groups of up to 30 participants who meet weekly for This review does not address transcendental meditation 2–2.5 hr for instruction and practice in mindfulness med- (TM) and other concentration-based approaches, which itation skills, together with discussion of stress, coping, and have been reviewed elsewhere (Delmonte, 1985; Smith, homework assignments. An all-day (7–8-hr) intensive 1975). Concentration-based approaches train participants mindfulness session usually is held around the sixth week. to restrict the focus of attention to a single stimulus, such Several mindfulness meditation skills are taught. For ex- as a word (e.g., a mantra), sound, object, or sensation. ample, the body scan is a 45-min exercise in which atten- When attention wanders, it is redirected to the object of tion is directed sequentially to numerous areas of the body meditation. No attention is paid to the nature of the dis- while the participant is lying down with eyes closed. Sen- traction. Mindfulness meditation, in contrast, involves ob- sations in each area are carefully observed. In sitting med- servation of constantly changing internal and external itation, participants are instructed to sit in a relaxed and stimuli as they arise. wakeful posture with eyes closed and to direct attention to This review also does not address Langer’s (1989, 1997) the sensations of breathing. Hatha yoga postures are used cognitive model of mindfulness, which includes alertness to teach mindfulness of bodily sensations during gentle to distinctions, context, and multiple perspectives, open- movements and stretching. Participants also practice mind- ness to novelty, and orientation in the present (Sternberg, fulness during ordinary activities like walking, standing, 2000). Mindfulness interventions studied by Langer and and eating. colleagues (e.g., Langer, 1989; Langer & Moldoveanu, Participants in MBSR are instructed to practice these 2000) often include teaching participants to consider in- skills outside group meetings for at least 45 min per day, six formation or situations from multiple perspectives or days per week. Audiotapes are used early in treatment, but within new contexts in order to increase learning or cre- participants are encouraged to practice without tapes after ativity. Although this concept of mindfulness shares with a few weeks. For all mindfulness exercises, participants are meditative approaches an emphasis on flexible awareness in instructed to focus attention on the target of observation the present, several important differences can be noted. (e.g., breathing or walking) and to be aware of it in each Langer’s (1989) mindfulness interventions usually involve moment. When emotions, sensations, or cognitions arise, working with material external to the participants, such as they are observed nonjudgmentally. When the participant information to be learned or manipulated, and often in- notices that the mind has wandered into thoughts, mem- clude active, goal-oriented cognitive tasks, such as solving ories, or fantasies, the nature or content of them is briefly problems. In contrast, the meditation-based approaches noted, if possible, and then attention is returned to the CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY: SCIENCE AND PRACTICE • V10 N2, SUMMER 2003 126
present moment. Thus, participants are instructed to no- view, which postulates that reality consists of opposing tice their thoughts and feelings but not to become absorbed forces. The synthesis of these forces leads to a new reality, in their content (Kabat-Zinn, 1982). Even judgmental which in turn consists of opposing forces, in a continual thoughts (e.g., “this is a foolish waste of time”) are to be process of change. In DBT, the most central dialectic is the observed nonjudgmentally. Upon noticing such a thought, relationship between acceptance and change. Clients are the participant might label it as a judgmental thought, or encouraged to accept themselves, their histories, and their simply as “thinking,” and then return attention to the pres- current situations exactly as they are, while working in- ent moment. An important consequence of mindfulness tensively to change their behaviors and environments in practice is the realization that most sensations, thoughts, order to build a better life. The synthesis of this apparent and emotions fluctuate, or are transient, passing by “like contradiction is a central goal of DBT. waves in the sea” (Linehan, 1993b, p. 87). DBT includes a wide range of cognitive and behavioral treatment procedures, most of which are designed to Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy change thoughts, emotions, or behaviors. Mindfulness Teasdale, Segal, and Williams (1995) proposed that the skills are taught in DBT within the context of synthesizing skills of attentional control taught in mindfulness medita- acceptance and change. Although the skills taught are sim- tion could be helpful in preventing relapse of major de- ilar to those targeted in MBSR, including nonjudgmental pressive episodes. Their information-processing theory of observation of thoughts, emotions, sensations, and envi- depressive relapse suggests that individuals who have ex- ronmental stimuli, the concepts are organized somewhat perienced major depressive episodes are vulnerable to re- differently. For example, Linehan (1993a, 1993b) describes currences whenever mild dysphoric states are encountered, three mindfulness “what” skills (observe, describe, par- because these states may reactivate the depressive thinking ticipate) and three mindfulness “how” skills (nonjudg- patterns present during the previous episode, or episodes, mentally, one-mindfully, effectively). DBT clients learn thus precipitating a new episode. Mindfulness-based cog- mindfulness skills in a year-long weekly skills group, which nitive therapy (MBCT) is a manualized (Segal, Williams, & also covers interpersonal effectiveness, emotion regulation, Teasdale, 2002) 8-week group intervention based largely on and distress tolerance skills. Clients work with their indi- Kabat-Zinn’s (1990) MBSR program. It incorporates ele- vidual therapists on applying skills learned in group to their ments of cognitive therapy that facilitate a detached or de- daily lives. centered view of one’s thoughts, including statements such Linehan (1994) notes that some severely impaired in- as “thoughts are not facts” and “I am not my thoughts.” dividuals may be unable or unwilling to meditate as This decentered approach also is applied to emotions and extensively as Kabat-Zinn’s (1990) MBSR program rec- bodily sensations. MBCT is designed to prevent depressive ommends. Thus, DBT does not prescribe a specific fre- relapse by teaching formerly depressed individuals to ob- quency or duration of mindfulness practice. Instead, goals serve their thoughts and feelings nonjudgmentally, and to for mindfulness practice are established by individual view them simply as mental events that come and go, clients and their therapists. DBT offers numerous mind- rather than as aspects of themselves, or as necessarily accu- fulness exercises from which clients may choose (some rate reflections of reality. This attitude toward depression- adapted from Hanh, 1976). In one example, clients imag- related cognitions is believed to prevent the escalation of ine that the mind is a conveyor belt. Thoughts, feelings, negative thoughts into ruminative patterns (Teasdale et al., and sensations that come down the belt are observed, la- 1995). beled, and categorized. In another exercise, clients imag- ine that the mind is the sky, and that thoughts, feelings, and INTERVENTIONS INCORPORATING sensations are clouds that they watch passing by. Several MINDFULNESS TRAINING variations on observing the breath are taught, including Dialectical Behavior Therapy following the breath in and out, counting breaths, coordi- Dialectical behavior therapy (DBT) is a multifaceted ap- nating breathing with footsteps while walking, and fol- proach to the treatment of borderline personality disorder lowing the breath while listening to music. Some exercises (Linehan, 1993a, 1993b). It is based on a dialectical world- encourage mindful awareness during everyday activities, MINDFULNESS TRAINING • BAER 127
such as making tea, washing dishes or clothes, cleaning eliminated. Instead, urges must be accepted as normal re- house, or taking a bath. sponses to appetitive cues. Mindfulness skills enable the client to observe the urges as they appear, accept them non- Acceptance and Commitment Therapy judgmentally, and cope with them in adaptive ways. Acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT; Hayes, Strosahl, & Wilson, 1999) is theoretically based in con- CONCEPTUAL APPROACHES: HOW MINDFULNESS temporary behavior analysis (Hayes & Wilson, 1993). Al- SKILLS MAY HELP though ACT does not describe its treatment methods in The authors of these treatment strategies have suggested terms of mindfulness or meditation, it is included here be- several mechanisms that may explain how mindfulness cause several of its strategies are consistent with the mind- skills can lead to symptom reduction and behavior change. fulness approaches described. Clients in ACT are taught to recognize an observing self who is capable of watching his Exposure or her own bodily sensations, thoughts, and emotions. The first published study of the effects of MBSR (Kabat- They are encouraged to see these phenomena as separate Zinn, 1982) described its application in patients with from the person having them. For example, they are taught chronic pain. MBSR is based, in part, on traditional med- to say, “I’m having the thought that I’m a bad person,” itation practices, which often include extended periods of rather than “I’m a bad person” (Kohlenberg, Hayes, & motionless sitting. Although a relaxed posture typically is Tsai, 1993, p. 588). They also are encouraged to experi- adopted, prolonged motionlessness can lead to pain in ence thoughts and emotions as they arise, without judging, muscles and joints. Mindfulness meditation instructors of- evaluating, or attempting to change or avoid them. Hayes ten encourage students not to shift position to relieve the (1987) describes an exercise in which the client imagines pain, but instead to focus careful attention directly on the that his or her thoughts are written on signs carried by pain sensations, and to assume a nonjudgmental attitude parading soldiers. The client’s task is to observe the parade toward these sensations, as well as toward the various cog- of thoughts without becoming absorbed in any of them. nitions (“this is unbearable”) emotions (anxiety, anger), ACT explicitly teaches clients to abandon attempts to con- and urges (to shift position) that often accompany pain trol thoughts and feelings, but instead to observe them non- sensations. The ability to observe pain sensations non- judgmentally and accept them as they are, while changing judgmentally is believed to reduce the distress associated their behaviors in constructive ways to improve their lives with pain. (Hayes, 1994). Kabat-Zinn (1982) suggests that application of this strategy by chronic pain patients might serve several func- Relapse Prevention tions. For example, prolonged exposure to the sensations Relapse prevention (RP; Marlatt & Gordon, 1985) is a of chronic pain, in the absence of catastrophic conse- cognitive-behavioral treatment package designed to fore- quences, might lead to desensitization, with a reduction stall relapses in individuals treated for substance abuse. over time in the emotional responses elicited by the pain Mindfulness skills are included as a technique for coping sensations. Thus, the practice of mindfulness skills could with urges to engage in substance use. Marlatt (1994) notes lead to the ability to experience pain sensations without that mindfulness involves acceptance of the constantly excessive emotional reactivity. Even if pain sensations were changing experiences of the present moment, whereas ad- not reduced, suffering and distress might be alleviated. diction is an inability to accept the present moment and a Kabat-Zinn et al. (1992) describe a similar mechanism persistent seeking of the next “high” associated with the for the potential effects of mindfulness training on anxi- addiction. The metaphor of “urge surfing” encourages ety and panic. Sustained, nonjudgmental observation of clients to imagine that urges are ocean waves that grow anxiety-related sensations, without attempts to escape or gradually until they crest and subside. The client “rides” avoid them, may lead to reductions in the emotional reac- the waves without giving in to the urges, thus learning tivity typically elicited by anxiety symptoms. This approach that urges will pass. However, the client also learns that is similar to the interoceptive exposure strategy described new urges will appear and that these urges cannot easily be by Barlow and Craske (2000), who instruct clients to in- CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY: SCIENCE AND PRACTICE • V10 N2, SUMMER 2003 128
duce symptoms of panic through exercises such as hyper- decentered view of one’s thoughts encouraged by mind- ventilation and aerobic activity, and to practice tolerating fulness training may interfere with ruminative patterns these sensations until they subside. In contrast, however, believed to be characteristic of depressive episodes (Nolen- mindfulness training does not include the deliberate in- Hoeksema, 1991). That is, mindfulness training may en- duction of panic symptoms. Instead, participants are in- able formerly depressed individuals to notice depressogenic structed to observe these sensations nonjudgmentally when thoughts and to redirect attention to other aspects of the they naturally arise. present moment, such as breathing, walking, or environ- Linehan (1993a, 1993b) describes individuals with bor- mental sounds, thus avoiding rumination. Teasdale has derline personality disorder as emotion phobic. That is, described this perspective on one’s thoughts as “meta- they are often afraid of experiencing strong negative affec- cognitive insight.” Teasdale et al. (1995) also note that a tive states. This fear is understandable, because their neg- practical advantage of mindfulness skills in encouraging ative affective states typically are very intense. However, cognitive change is that they can be practiced at any time, their attempts to avoid these states often have maladaptive including during periods of remission, when depresso- consequences. Linehan (1993a, 1993b) suggests that pro- genic thinking may be occurring too rarely to permit reg- longed observation of current thoughts and emotions, ular practice of traditional cognitive therapy exercises, such without trying to avoid or escape them, can be seen as an as identification and disputing of cognitive distortions. example of exposure, which should encourage the extinc- That is, a mindful perspective about one’s thoughts can be tion of fear responses and avoidance behaviors previously applied to all thoughts. elicited by these stimuli. Thus, the practice of mindfulness skills may improve patients’ ability to tolerate negative Self-Management emotional states and ability to cope with them effectively. Several authors have noted that improved self-observation resulting from mindfulness training may promote use of a Cognitive Change range of coping skills. For example, Kabat-Zinn (1982) Several authors have noted that the practice of mindfulness suggests that increased awareness of pain sensations and may lead to changes in thought patterns, or in attitudes stress responses as they occur may enable individuals to en- about one’s thoughts. For example, Kabat-Zinn (1982, gage in a variety of coping responses, including skills not 1990) suggests that nonjudgmental observation of pain and included in their treatment program. Kristeller and Hallett anxiety-related thoughts may lead to the understanding (1999) suggest that the self-observation skills developed that they are “just thoughts,” rather than reflections of truth through mindfulness training might lead to improved or reality, and do not necessitate escape or avoidance recognition of satiety cues in binge eaters, as well as in- behavior. Similarly, Linehan (1993a, 1993b) notes that ob- creased ability to observe urges to binge without yielding serving one’s thoughts and feelings and applying descrip- to them. Marlatt (1994) suggests a similar effect in patients tive labels to them encourages the understanding that they recovering from addictions. Teasdale et al. (1995) note that are not always accurate reflections of reality. For example, mindfulness training encourages awareness of all cognitive feeling afraid does not necessarily mean that danger is im- and emotional events as they occur, including those that minent, and thinking “I am a failure” does not make it may be early signs of potential depressive relapse. Thus, true. Kristeller and Hallett (1999), in a study of MBSR in mindfulness training may promote recognition of early patients with binge eating disorder, cite Heatherton and signs of a problem, at a time when application of previously Baumeister’s (1991) theory of binge eating as an escape learned skills will be most likely to be effective in prevent- from self-awareness and suggest that mindfulness training ing the problem. Linehan (1993b) suggests that nonjudg- might develop nonjudgmental acceptance of the aversive mental observation and description permits recognition cognitions that binge-eaters are thought to be avoiding, of the consequences of behaviors (e.g., irritating one’s boss such as unfavorable comparisons of self to others and per- with frequent lateness) in place of global judgments about ceived inability to meet others’ demands. the self (e.g., “I am a bad employee”). This recognition Teasdale (1999) and Teasdale et al. (1995), in their may lead to more effective behavior change, including re- discussion of MBCT, suggest that the nonjudgmental, duction of impulsive, maladaptive behaviors. Linehan MINDFULNESS TRAINING • BAER 129
(1993b) also suggests that learning to focus “one- erated, rather than fearsome and dangerous experiences to mindfully” on the present moment develops control of at- be avoided, even at the cost of significant maladaptive be- tention, a useful skill for individuals who have difficulty havior. completing important tasks because they are distracted by All of the treatment programs reviewed here include ac- worries, memories, or negative moods. ceptance of pain, thoughts, feelings, urges, or other bod- ily, cognitive, and emotional phenomena, without trying Relaxation to change, escape, or avoid them. Kabat-Zinn (1990) de- The relationship between meditation and relaxation is scribes acceptance as one of several foundations of mind- somewhat complex. Several authors (Goldenberg et al., fulness practice. DBT provides explicit training in several 1994; Kabat-Zinn et al., 1998; Kaplan, Goldenberg, & mindfulness techniques designed to promote acceptance of Galvin-Nadeau, 1993) have suggested that mindfulness- reality. Thus, it appears that mindfulness training may pro- based stress reduction may be applicable to stress-related vide a method for teaching acceptance skills. medical disorders, including psoriasis and fibromyalgia. These authors note that meditation often induces relax- Relationship Between Mindfulness Training and Cognitive- ation, which may contribute to the management of these Behavioral Approaches disorders. The induction of relaxation through various This discussion suggests that mindfulness training is con- meditation strategies has been well documented (Benson, sistent with cognitive-behavioral treatment procedures in 1975; Orme-Johnson, 1984; Wallace, Benson, & Wilson, several ways. Training in self-directed attention can result 1984). However, the purpose of mindfulness training is in sustained exposure to sensations, thoughts, and emo- not to induce relaxation, but instead to teach nonjudg- tions, resulting in desensitization of conditioned responses mental observation of current conditions, which might and reduction of avoidance behavior. Cognitive change include autonomic arousal, racing thoughts, muscle ten- appears to result from viewing one’s thoughts as temporary sion, and other phenomena incompatible with relaxation. phenomena without inherent worth or meaning, rather In addition, evidence suggests that relaxation effects are than as necessarily accurate reflections of reality, health, not unique to meditation, but are common to many re- adjustment, or worthiness. Practice of meditation also may laxation strategies (Shapiro, 1982). Thus, although practice lead to relaxation and improved self-management. of mindfulness exercises may lead to relaxation, this out- However, mindfulness training differs from traditional come may not be a primary reason for engaging in mind- cognitive-behavioral treatment in important ways. For ex- fulness skills. ample, mindfulness training does not include the evalua- tion of thoughts as rational or distorted, or systematic Acceptance attempts to change thoughts judged to be irrational. In- The relationship between acceptance and change is a cen- stead, participants are taught to observe their thoughts, to tral concept in current discussions of psychotherapy (Hayes, note their impermanence, and to refrain from evaluating Jacobsen, Follette, & Dougher, 1994). Hayes (1994) sug- them. Another important difference is that traditional gests that acceptance involves “experiencing events fully cognitive-behavioral procedures usually have a clear goal, and without defense, as they are” (p. 30), and notes that such as to change a behavior or thinking pattern. In con- empirically oriented clinicians may have overemphasized trast, mindfulness meditation is practiced with a seemingly the importance of changing all unpleasant symptoms, paradoxical attitude of nonstriving. That is, although a task without recognizing the importance of acceptance. For is prescribed (e.g., sit still, close your eyes, and pay atten- example, an individual who experiences panic attacks may tion), no specific goal is adopted. Participants are not to engage in numerous maladaptive behaviors in an attempt strive to relax, reduce their pain, or change their thoughts to prevent future attacks, including drug and alcohol abuse, or emotions, although they may have sought treatment for avoidance of important activities, and excessive anxious these purposes. They are simply to observe whatever is vigilance toward bodily states. If the individual could ac- happening in each moment without judging it. Finally, cept that panic attacks may occasionally occur and that mindfulness researchers have suggested that effective teach- they are time-limited and not dangerous, panic attacks ing of mindfulness skills by mental health professionals re- would become unpleasant but brief experiences to be tol- quires that they engage in their own regular mindfulness CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY: SCIENCE AND PRACTICE • V10 N2, SUMMER 2003 130
practice (Segal et al., 2002). Professionals using more tra- sure, Mc = the mean of the comparison group on that mea- ditional cognitive-behavioral strategies generally are not sure, and SDp = the pooled standard deviation of the two expected to engage in regular practice of the skills they are groups. If means or standard deviations were not provided, teaching. effect sizes were calculated from the significance level (p). Although the practice of mindfulness generally involves For studies using within-groups designs, effect sizes were acceptance of current reality, rather than systematic at- calculated from t or F (1 df ), or from the significance level tempts to change reality, individuals who practice these when t or F were not reported. Calculations of effect sizes skills may experience reductions in a variety of symptoms. relied on methods described by Rosenthal (1984). The empirical literature addressing this issue is reviewed next. Overview of Treatment Literature Studies examining the effects of mindfulness-based inter- EMPIRICAL RESEARCH ON MINDFULNESS-BASED ventions are summarized in Table 1. The studies are INTERVENTIONS grouped by participant population, beginning with stud- The empirical literature investigating the effects of mind- ies of chronic pain patients. Next are studies of patients fulness-based interventions is reviewed here. Meta-analytic with other Axis I disorders (anxiety, eating, and major de- procedures were incorporated to facilitate quantification of pressive disorders), followed by studies of patients with findings and comparisons across studies. To locate relevant other medical problems (fibromyalgia, psoriasis, and can- studies, a computer search (using PsycInfo and Medline cer). Next are studies with mixed populations, including databases) was conducted of articles and chapters including psychotherapy and medical patients. The last group in- the terms mindfulness or meditation. Reference lists of all cludes studies of nonclinical populations (students and articles were searched for additional articles. Studies were other volunteers). Within each group studies are listed in included if they were published in English and compared order of publication date. a group of participants trained in mindfulness with a group Sample sizes in these studies have ranged from 16 to not trained, or a group who provided data before and af- 142. Mean age of participants has ranged from 38–50 years, ter mindfulness training. Unpublished dissertations and with a mean of 45 years. Gender ratio of the samples has conference papers were excluded, as were studies address- ranged from 0 to 46% male. Education and race/ethnicity ing nonmindfulness forms of meditation, such as transcen- were rarely reported. dental meditation (TM). Studies of mindfulness as defined Nine studies used pre-post designs with no control by Langer (1989, 1997) also were excluded, for reasons group. Nine used between-groups designs with Treatment described earlier. Twenty-one studies meeting these cri- As Usual (TAU) or waiting-list control groups. Most stud- teria were found. ies used the 8–10 week MBSR group intervention (Kabat- For each study, several demographic and methodolog- Zinn,1982, 1990), or a variation of this intervention ical variables were coded, including number, type, and tailored to the population under study. Two studies (Teas- characteristics of participants, research design, the nature of dale et al., 2000; Williams, Teasdale, Segal, & Soulsby, the mindfulness intervention, type of comparison group, 2000) examined MBCT. Dependent variables have in- whether participants were randomly assigned to interven- cluded a variety of self-report measures of pain, other med- tion or comparison groups, the dependent variables re- ical symptoms, anxiety, depression, eating behaviors, and ported, and follow-up intervals and data. general psychological functioning, as well as objective Effect sizes (Cohen’s d) were calculated for all studies measures such as analysis of urine chemistry. that provided sufficient data. Cohen’s d expresses effect size For each study effect sizes were calculated separately in standard deviation units; thus, an effect size of 1.0 on a for each dependent measure completed at the conclusion given dependent measure indicates that the treatment of treatment and at all reported follow-up intervals. Post- group scored one standard deviation better, on average, treatment effect sizes then were averaged across dependent than the comparison group on that measure. For studies measures within studies, yielding a single posttreatment using between-groups designs, effect sizes were calculated effect size for each study. Similarly, effect sizes for all de- with the following formula: d = (Mt – Mc)/SDp, in which pendent measures completed at all follow-up intervals were Mt = the mean of the treatment group on a specific mea- averaged, yielding a single follow-up effect size for each MINDFULNESS TRAINING • BAER 131
Table 1. Empirical studies of effects of mindfulness-based interventions Type Mean % Research Treatment Control Rand % Dependent d d Foll Study N Participant Age Male Design Group Group Assn Att Variables Post (Months) Kabat-Zinn 51 Chronic 46 35 Pre-post MBSR None No 12 Pain Rtg Index 0.70 0.35 (1982) pain patients Body Parts PA (2–7) Interfere rtg MSCL POMS total SCL-90-R GSI Kabat-Zinn 90 Chronic 44 33 Pre-post MBSR None No 15 Same as above 0.36 0.51 et al. (1985) pain patientsa (2–15) Part 1 Kabat-Zinn 42 Chronic 48 24 Between group MBSR TAU No — Same as above 0.26 et al. (1985) pain patientsb (n = 21) (n = 21) Part 2 CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY: SCIENCE AND PRACTICE Kabat-Zinn 30–142 Chronic — 31 Series of follow-ups MBSR None No — Pain Rtg Index — 0.53 et al. (1987) pain patientsc 2.5–48 months Body Parts PA (6–48) • post-MBSR MSCL SCL-90-R GSI Randolph 78 Chronic 50 31 Pre-post MBSR None No — Pain rtgs 0.15 0.08 et al. (1999) pain patients BSI-GSI (2–12) POMS total Pain beliefs Kabat-Zinn 22 Anxiety 38 23 Pre-post MBSR None No 8 Hamilton Anx 0.88 1.35 et al. (1992) patients Hanilton Dep (3) BDI; BAI; FSS Mobility Inv Miller et al. 18 Anxiety — — 3-year follow-up MBSR None No — Hamilton Anx — 1.10 (1995) patients (patients from Hamilton Dep (36) V10 N2, SUMMER 2003 from Kabat- Kabat-Zinn et al., BDI; BAI; FSS Zinn et al., 1992) Mobility Inv 1992) Kristeller & 18 Binge eating 46 0 Pre-post Variant of None No 14 Binge freq 1.65 — Hallett (1999) disorder MBSR Binge Eat Sc BDI; BAI Eating rtgs Teasdale 132 Remitted 44 24 Between group MBCT TAU Yes 17 MDD relapse 0.60 — et al. (2000) MDD after (n = 63) (n = 69) medicn tx Williams 41 Remitted 43 27 Between group MBCT TAU Yes — Autobiog 0.71 — et al. (2000) MDD after (n = 21) (n = 20) Memory test medicn txd Kaplan 59 Fibro-myalgia 46 10 Pre-post Variant of None No 23 VAS’s pain, sleep, etc. —e et al. (1993) patients MBSR MSCL SCL-90-R GSI Coping Strat Q Fibro Impact Q 132
Goldenberg 121 Fibro-myalgia 46 7 Between group MBSR Wait list No 9 Vas’s pain, sleep, etc. 0.42 — et al. (1994) patients (n = 79) (n = 18); Fibro Impact Q declined SCL-90-R GSI MBSR (n = 24) Kabat-Zinn 37 Psoriasis 43 46 Between group Mindfulness TAU Yes — Days to clearing 0.63 — et al. (1998) patients tapes during of psoriasis receiving light tx MINDFULNESS TRAINING light therapy • Speca et al. 90 Cancer 51 19 Between group Variant of Wait list Yes 13 POMS 0.60 (2000) patients MBSR SOSI Carlson et al. 54 Cancerf 51 19 6-month follow- Variant of None No — POMS — NS (2000) patients up in Speca et al. MBSR SOSI (6) BAER (2000) Kutz et al. 20 Long-term 38 — Pre-post MBSR None No — SCL-90-R 0.72 0.52 (1985) dynamic POMS (6) therapy clients Interfere rtg Thera rtgs Roth & Creasor 86 Outpatients 43 16 Pre-post MBSR None NO 40 SCL-90-R 0.69 — (1997) inner city BAI Latino/Latina MSCL Mixed dx Self-esteem Reibel et al. 121 Medical 47 29 Pre-post MBSR None No 11 SF-36 0.56 NS (2001) patients, MSCL (12) Mixed dx SCL-90-R GSI Massion et al. 16 Healthy 42 0 Between group Regular Non- No — Melatonin metabolite 0.98 — (1995) women mediators mediators in urine Astin (1997) 19 College — 5 Between group MBSR Wait list Yes 14 SCL-90-R GSI 1.51 — students (n = 12) (n = 7) Control Inv INSPIRIT Shapiro et al. 73 Premed and — 44 Between group MBSR Wait list Yes 3 SCL-90-R GSI 0.50 — (1998) med students SCL-90-R Dep STAI Empathy rtg INSPIRIT Williams 75 Community 43 28 Between group MBSR Wait list Yes 15 DSI 0.67 0.67 et al. (2001) volunteers and info SCL-90-R GSI (3) MSCL Notes. rand assn = random assignment; att = attrition from treatment group; post = posttreatment; foll = follow-up; MDD = major depressive disorder; medicn = medication; tx = treatment; dx = diagnosis; MBSR = Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction; MBCT = Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy; TAU = Treatment As Usual; rtg = rating; PA = problem assessment; MSCL = Medical Symptom Checklist; POMS = Profile of Mood States; SCL-90-R = Symptom Checklist-90 Revised; GSI = General Severity Index; Anx = anxiety; Dep = depression; BDI = Beck Depression Inventory; BAI = Beck Anxiety Inven- tory; FSS = Fear Survey Schedule; Inv = inventory; Autobiog = autobiographical; VAS = visual analog scale; Strat = strategies; Q = questionnaire; fibro = fibromyalgia; SOSI = Symptoms of Stress Inventory; SF-36 = Short Form 36; INSPIRIT = Index of Core Spiritual Experiences; STAI = State-Trait Anxiety Inventory; DSI = Daily Stress Inventory; NS = no significant difference between posttest and follow-up. a Includes 51 patients from Kabat-Zinn (1982). b Includes 21 patients from Part 1 of this study. c Includes patients from Kabat-Zinn (1982) and Kabat-Zinn et al. (1985). d Subset of patients in Teasdale et al. (2000). e Insufficient data to calculate. f Subset of patients in Speca et al. (2000). 133
study that reported follow-up data. Mean posttreatment Axis I Disorders. Kabat-Zinn et al. (1992) examined a and follow-up effect sizes are presented in the final two sample of 22 patients with generalized anxiety and panic columns of Table 1. disorders, and found significant improvements in several No studies of DBT, ACT, or RP were included, be- measures of anxiety and depression, both at posttreatment cause none were found that examined the mindfulness and at 3-month follow-up. A no-treatment control group component independently of the behavior change strate- was not included. Miller, Fletcher, and Kabat-Zinn (1995) gies also included in these treatment approaches. Thus, reported a 3-year follow-up of the same participants and although empirical studies support the efficacy of these found that treatment gains had been maintained. treatments (Curry, Marlatt, Gordon, & Baer, 1988; Ito, Kristeller and Hallett (1999) examined the effects of Donovan, & Hall, 1988; Koons et al., 2001; Linehan, Arm- MBSR on binge eating disorder. In a pre-post design with strong, Suarez, Allmon, & Heard, 1991; Linehan, Heard, & no control group, 18 female patients showed statistically Armstrong, 1993; Linehan, Tutek, Heard, & Armstrong, significant improvements in several measures of eating and 1994; Strosahl, Hayes, Bergan, & Romano, 1998; Zettle & mood. Raines, 1989), the relative contribution of mindfulness Teasdale et al. (2000) examined the effects of MBCT on training to these treatment effects has not been investi- rates of depressive relapse in a large sample of patients gated. In contrast, studies of MBCT were included, be- whose major depressive disorder (MDD) had remitted af- cause mindfulness training appears to be the central focus ter treatment with medication. All participants had dis- of this approach, although some cognitive techniques have continued their medications at least 12 weeks before the been incorporated. study began. Patients were randomly assigned to either MBCT (8-week manualized group treatment) or TAU and General Findings then followed for 1 year. For patients with three or more Chronic Pain. Four studies have examined the effects of previous depressive episodes, results showed much lower MBSR on patients with chronic pain. The first study relapse rates for MBCT patients (37% of patients relapsed) (Kabat-Zinn, 1982) describes pre-post data for 51 patients. than for the TAU group (66% of patients relapsed) during The second study (Kabat-Zinn, Lipworth, & Burney, the 1-year follow-up period. However, relapse rates for 1985), has two parts. Part 1 presents pre-post data for a the MBCT and TAU groups did not differ for patients with sample of 90 patients, including the 51 patients from only one or two previous episodes. Kabat-Zinn (1982). In Part 2, 21 of these 90 patients are Using a subset of the participants from Teasdale et al. compared to 21 other pain patients who had received TAU (2000), J. M. G. Williams et al. (2000) found that those in the pain clinic but had not participated in MBSR. Parts who had completed MBCT produced fewer general and 1 and 2 of this study are entered separately in Table 1. The more specific memories when asked to recall events from third study (Kabat-Zinn, Lipworth, Burney, & Sellers, their pasts in response to cue words. The authors speculate 1987) is an extensive series of follow-up evaluations of that mindfulness training may modify the overgeneral chronic pain patients who had completed MBSR over the autobiographical memory believed to be characteristic of preceding several years, including patients in the previous individuals with depression (Kuyken & Brewin, 1995). two studies. Thus, the first four entries in Table 1 are de- rived from three published articles with overlapping par- Other Medical Disorders. Two studies have investigated ticipant samples. Finally, Randolph, Caldera, Tacone, and effects of MBSR on fibromyalgia. Both reported improve- Greak (1999) investigated the effects of MBSR in an in- ments in a variety of symptoms. In a study of psoriasis pa- dependent sample of 78 chronic pain patients. tients, Kabat-Zinn et al. (1998) found that patients who In general, findings for chronic pain patients show sta- listened to mindfulness audiotapes during individual light- tistically significant improvements in ratings of pain, other therapy sessions showed quicker clearing of their skin medical symptoms, and general psychological symptoms. (Mdn = 65 days) than did patients who received light ther- Many of these changes were maintained at follow-up eval- apy alone (Mdn = 97 days). Speca, Carlson, Goodey, and uations. Most of these comparisons used pre-post designs Angen (2000) examined the effects of MBSR in a group with no control group. of cancer patients and reported significant reductions in CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY: SCIENCE AND PRACTICE • V10 N2, SUMMER 2003 134
mood disturbance and stress levels. Carlson, Ursuliak, effect sizes in this calculation, the effect sizes obtained from Goodey, Angen, and Speca (2001) reported that these Kabat-Zinn (1982) and Parts 1 and 2 of Kabat-Zinn et al. changes were maintained at 6-month follow-up. (1985) first were averaged, because these comparisons have overlapping participant samples. Similarly, the mean effect Mixed Clinical Populations. Kutz et al. (1985) studied a sizes obtained for Teasdale et al. (2000) and J. M. G. sample of long-term psychodynamic therapy patients with Williams et al. (2000) were averaged, because these two diagnoses including anxiety and obsessive neuroses, and studies also have overlapping participant samples. After narcissistic and borderline personality disorders. They these preliminary calculations, 15 independent posttreat- completed a 10-week MBSR program while continuing ment mean effect sizes, each from a separate sample, were with their individual psychotherapy and showed statisti- available for analysis. Their mean was 0.74 (SD = 0.39). cally significant improvements in a variety of self- and When each of these 15 effect sizes was weighted by sample therapist-rated symptoms. Roth and Creasor (1997) stud- size, overall mean effect size was 0.59. ied outpatients from a low-income, primarily Latino pop- ulation attending an inner city health clinic and showed Mean Effect Size at Follow-Up statistically significant improvements on several measures of Follow-up data were reported less often. Effect sizes at medical and psychological functioning. Reibel, Greeson, follow-up ranged from 0.08 to 1.35. Before an overall Brainard, and Rosenzweig (2001) studied medical patients mean of these effect sizes was calculated, mean effect sizes with a variety of medical and psychiatric diagnoses and obtained from studies with overlapping participant samples found significant improvements in medical and psycho- were averaged. The overall mean of these independent logical symptoms. None of these studies used control follow-up effect sizes was 0.59 (SD = 0.41). groups. Cohen (1977) has described effect sizes of d = 0.2, d = 0.5, and d = 0.8 as small, medium, and large, respectively. Nonclinical Populations. Massion, Teas, Hebert, Wert- Thus, on the average, the literature reviewed here suggests heimer, and Kabat-Zinn (1995) analyzed urine levels of a that mindfulness-based interventions have yielded at least melatonin metabolite in two groups of women. Levels medium-sized effects, with some effect sizes falling within were significantly higher in women previously trained in the large range. Many of the effect sizes calculated for these MBSR who continued to meditate regularly than in wo- studies are probably conservative, because several studies men who had never been trained and did not meditate. did not present means, standard deviations, or t values, The authors cite previous findings suggesting that mela- making it necessary to calculate d from the p value. In many tonin level may be related to immune function (Bartsch et cases exact p values were not reported. Instead, for ex- al., 1992;Guerrero & Reiter, 1992), and suggest that mind- ample, a p value between .01 and .05 might have been re- fulness meditation may influence health status through its ported as .05, which was then used to compute d. Larger effects on melatonin. Astin (1997) and Shapiro et al. (1998) ps yield smaller ds. In addition, when findings were re- studied student populations who completed group MBSR, ported only as nonsignificant, effect sizes of zero were reporting significant effects on psychological symptoms, recorded. If means, SDs, or t values had been reported in empathy ratings, and spiritual experiences. Both of these these cases, the calculated effect size might have been larger studies used waiting-list control groups. Williams, Kolar, than zero. Reger, and Pearson (2001) studied community volunteers who completed MBSR to reduce their stress levels, re- Relationships Between Mean Effect Size at Posttreatment and porting significant improvements in medical and psycho- Study Characteristics logical symptoms. Relationships between mean effect sizes at posttreatment and selected methodological variables can be seen in Table Mean Effect Size at Posttreatment 2. (Follow-up effect sizes are not included in this table.) Posttreatment effect sizes ranged from 0.15 to 1.65. An The small number of studies available and the noninde- overall mean of these effect sizes, collapsed across studies, pendence of some of the effect sizes make statistical analy- was calculated. In order to include only independent mean ses of these differences impractical. Thus, these findings MINDFULNESS TRAINING • BAER 135
ies reported means, SDs, or t values, effect sizes for these Table 2. Mean effect size at posttreatment and methodological variables studies were calculated from p values. It is possible that Variable N Mean d SD mean effect size for chronic pain patients might have been larger if these studies had provided additional data. Research design Pre-post 8 0.71 0.44 Between group 10 0.69 0.34 Random assignment Clinical Significance of Findings (between group) The clinical significance of the changes reported in these Yes 7 0.75 0.34 No 3 0.55 0.38 studies is difficult to assess. Several studies reported only Type of control group raw scores on dependent measures, whereas others re- Wait list 5 0.74 0.44 TAU 4 0.55 0.20 ported percentage change in scores or the statistical signi- Participant population Chronic pain 4 0.37 0.24 ficance of the change in scores. In these cases the severity Other Axis Ia 4 0.96 0.47 of participants’ problems before treatment, or their prox- Medicalb 4 0.55 0.09 Nonclinicalc 4 0.92 0.44 imity to the normal range of functioning afterwards, can- Dependent measure Pain 17 0.31 0.30 not readily be determined. Other medical (self-rated)d 11 0.44 0.26 In order to assess the clinical significance of some of Anxiety 8 0.70 0.41 Depression 5 0.86 0.30 the findings reviewed here, reported raw scores for more Stress 2 0.63 0.02 frequently used dependent measures were converted to Global psychologicale 18 0.64 0.42 Objective medicalf 2 0.80 0.25 T-score equivalents or ranges of functioning, with use of Method of Calculating d Using Ms and SDs, or t 10 0.87 0.40 the instruments’ published manuals or profile sheets. For Using p 8 0.48 0.22 example, several studies reported pre- and posttreatment a Includes anxiety, depression, and binge eating. raw scores for the Global Severity Index (GSI) of the b Includes fibromyalgia, psoriasis, and cancer. Symptom Checklist 90-Revised (SCL-90-R) (Derogatis, c Includes students and nonclinical volunteers. d Includes fatigue and sleep ratings, and medical symptom checklist. 1983). These scores were converted to T scores (T-score e Includes POMS total mood disturbance, SCL-90-R GSI. equivalents for males and females were averaged) and then f Includes urine and skin analysis. averaged across studies. This procedure yielded a mean pretest T score for the GSI of 67, with a mean posttest T should be interpreted cautiously, as differences may not be score of 60. Because T scores have a mean of 50 and a SD significant. Mean effect sizes were similar for studies using of 10, this finding suggests that patients scored nearly 2 SDs pre-post and between-groups designs. Mean effect size was above the mean before treatment and 1 SD above the mean somewhat larger when participants had been randomly as- after treatment. Several studies using the GSI could not be signed to groups. Studies using waiting-list control groups included in this procedure because they did not report yielded slightly larger effect sizes than those using TAU. scores, instead reporting only percentage decrease in scores, When organized by type of participant, mean effect sizes or the statistical significance of the change in scores. appear somewhat larger for comparisons using nonclinical Similar procedures were followed for the Beck Depres- populations or patients with selected Axis I problems than sion Inventory (BDI) and the Beck Anxiety Inventory for those with chronic pain or medical problems. When (BAI) with use of ranges of functioning described in the organized by the type of dependent variable, mean effect manuals (Beck & Steer, 1987, 1993). On the BDI raw sizes ranged from 0.31 for pain measures to 0.86 for mea- scores of 0–9 are considered asymptomatic, whereas scores sures of depression. of 10–18 indicate mild to moderate depression. For two Finally, effect sizes derived from means and SDs or t val- studies reporting BDI scores, the mean pretreatment score ues were somewhat larger, on average (0.87) than those was 16.82 (mild-moderate), and the mean posttreatment derived from p values (0.48). This finding illustrates the im- score was 8.64 (asymptomatic). For two studies reporting portance of including means, standard deviations, and t BAI scores, the mean pretreatment score was 20.43 (mod- values in future research. Given the small number of avail- erate) and the mean posttreatment score was 7.94 (minimal able studies, examination of interactions between method to mild). of calculating d and other methodological variables is not These indications of clinical significance must be con- feasible. However, because none of the chronic pain stud- sidered tentative, because they are based on very few stud- CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY: SCIENCE AND PRACTICE • V10 N2, SUMMER 2003 136
ies, some of which used uncontrolled pre-post designs. scores (r = .66) and in BDI scores (r = .59). Astin (1997), However, they suggest that mindfulness training, on aver- in a sample of college students, reported that participants age, may bring participants with mild to moderate psy- practiced meditation for an average of 30 min per day, 3.5 chological distress into or close to the normal range. days per week. Reported practice time and improvement on the GSI of the SCL-90-R were not significantly corre- Attrition, Adherence, and Maintenance of Mindfulness Practice lated. Reibel et al. (2001) reported that 90% of their mixed Thirteen studies reported both the number of individuals sample of medical patients practiced three times per week who agreed to participate in mindfulness training and the or more and 57% practiced nearly every day, most for 15– number who completed it. Program completion usually 30 min each time. was defined as attendance at a minimum number of ses- Four studies reported the extent to which participants sions, or was undefined. Percentage of enrolled partici- trained in mindfulness skills continued to practice these pants who completed treatment ranged from 60 to 97, skills after treatment had ended. In a series of follow-up with a mean of 85% (SD = 8.91). The lowest completion studies of former MBSR patients, Kabat-Zinn et al. (1987) rate (60%) was noted by Roth & Creasor (1997), who stud- noted that 75% of former patients reported that they still ied an inner city health clinic population. The highest com- practiced meditation (averaged across follow-up intervals pletion rate (97%) was reported by Shapiro et al. (1998), of 6–48 months). Of these patients, 43% meditated regu- whose participants were premedical and medical students. larly (≥ three times weekly, ≥ 15 min each time), whereas The most extensive analysis of program completion was 19% meditated sporadically (one or two times weekly, ≥ 15 provided by Kabat-Zinn and Chapman-Waldrop (1988), min each time, or ≥ three times weekly, ≤ 15 min each who reported completion rates for the MBSR program (at time), and 38% were classified as marginal meditators that time known as the Stress Reduction and Relaxation (< one time weekly for any length of time, or < three Program) at the University of Massachusetts Medical Cen- times weekly, < 15 min each time). Practice of yoga two ter. (This study is not included in Table 1, because it did or more times per week was reported by 31% of respon- not examine treatment effects.) During the 2-year period dents, and 49% reported using awareness of breathing in examined (1982–1984), 1,155 patients were referred to daily life often. the program, mostly by their physicians. Of these patients, Kabat-Zinn et al. (1992), at 3-month follow-up of 22 75% completed an intake interview, and 90% of those patients with anxiety disorders, found that 84% reported interviewed enrolled in the program. Of the 784 patients practicing meditation or yoga three or more times per who enrolled, 76% completed the program, whereas 15% week, for 15–45 min each time. Mindfulness of breathing dropped out after beginning and 9% never attended a ses- in daily life was practiced by 95% (77% often and 18% some- sion. Regression analyses showed that patients with stress- times). Miller et al (1995) contacted 18 of these patients for related problems (hypertension, anxiety, sleep disorders, a 3-year follow-up evaluation and reported that 10 (56%) etc.) were significantly more likely to complete the pro- still practiced meditation: 4 regularly, 3 sporadically, and 3 gram than those with chronic pain complaints (lower back, marginally (as defined above). Sixteen of 18 (89%) reported headache, etc.). Completers also had somewhat higher that they used awareness of breathing in daily life (4 often, pretreatment scores than noncompleters on the GSI and 11 sometimes, and 1 rarely). the Obsessive-Compulsive (OC) scales of the SCL-90-R. K. A. Williams et al. (2001), in a sample of community Within the chronic pain group, women were slightly more volunteers self-identified as “stressed out,” reported that at likely than men to complete the program. 3-month follow-up 81% of MBSR participants were prac- Only three studies reported the extent to which partic- ticing either meditation, yoga, or awareness of breathing in ipants completed their assigned home practice during the daily life. course of the mindfulness intervention. Kristeller and Hal- lett (1999), in a sample of women with binge eating disor- Patients’ Reactions to Treatment der, noted that participants reported engaging in a mean of In their follow-up study of former MBSR patients, Kabat- 15.82 hr of meditation (SD = 3.15) across the 6-week in- Zinn et al (1987) found that the majority of those who tervention program. Reported practice time was signifi- considered themselves improved since completing MBSR cantly correlated with improvements in Binge Eating Scale attributed 50–100% of their improvement to the MSBR MINDFULNESS TRAINING • BAER 137
You can also read