LOTTERY "STRATEGIES": MONETIZING PLAYERS' BEHAVIORAL BIASES - RAMAN KACHURKA MICHAŁ WIKTOR KRAWCZYK No. 29/2020 (335) - Warsaw 2020 - Faculty of ...

 
CONTINUE READING
LOTTERY "STRATEGIES": MONETIZING PLAYERS' BEHAVIORAL BIASES - RAMAN KACHURKA MICHAŁ WIKTOR KRAWCZYK No. 29/2020 (335) - Warsaw 2020 - Faculty of ...
UNIVERSITY OF WARSAW
                   FACULTY OF ECONOMIC SCIENCES

           Working Papers
              No. 29/2020 (335)

        LOTTERY "STRATEGIES":
MONETIZING PLAYERS' BEHAVIORAL BIASES

            R AMAN K ACHURKA
        M ICHAŁ W IKTOR K RAWCZYK

                Warsaw 2020
WORKING PAPERS 29/2020 (335)

Lottery "strategies": monetizing players' behavioral biases

Raman Kachurka*, Michał Wiktor Krawczyk*

Faculty of Economic Sciences, University of Warsaw
* Corresponding authors: raman.kachurka@outlook.com mkrawczyk@wne.uw.edu.pl

Abstract: The popularity of lotteries around the world is puzzling. In this paper, we study one
factor, which might contribute to this phenomenon, namely lottery “strategies” that could
allegedly improve players’ odds. In an online survey of lottery players we find that such strategies
are popular and their use is related to more frequent lottery play and a number of personality traits
and beliefs about gambling. Systematically searching for websites and books, we amass the
largest dataset of lottery strategies in existence. We subsequently analyze their descriptions,
categorize them, and investigate how they exploit their target audience’s behavioral biases,
including the illusion of control, authority bias, magical thinking, the illusion of correlation,
gambler’s fallacy, hot hand fallacy, representativeness heuristic, availability heuristic, and regret
aversion. We find that the strategies maintain gamblers’ (false) beliefs about the possibility of
controlling lottery results. This exploratory work contributes to a deeper understanding of
(problem) gambling and lays the foundation for the design of experiments testing how the specific
features of different strategies may interact with beliefs and trigger (excessive) lottery play.

Keywords: decision      making under risk, lottery strategy, illusion of control

JEL codes: C91,    D01, D81, D83, D91

Acknowledgements: We  gratefully acknowledge the support of the National Science Centre,
grant 2016/21/B/HS4/00688 and Konstanty Kalinowski Scholarship Program II.

Working Papers contain preliminary research results. Please consider this when citing the paper. Please contact the
authors to give comments or to obtain revised version. Any mistakes and the views expressed herein are solely those
of the authors
Kachurka, R. and Krawczyk, M. /WORKING PAPERS 29/2020 (335)                          1

1 Introduction

Given their low expected return (typically less than 50% of the money invested), the popularity
of state lotteries around the world is puzzling. One factor that might contribute to explaining
the puzzle is the use of strategies. By these, we mean instances, in which external sources (for
example, authors of books) advise lottery consumer which practices to follow when playing:
which numbers to choose, how many tickets to buy, when or where to buy them, or what actions
should precede or accompany playing.

           We focus our attention on the strategies promoted as a way to increase either the chances
of winning or the winning amount. Most of them make explicit recommendations concerning
the general betting strategy or concrete combinations of numbers to bet on. Strategies
effectively encourage extensive lottery play because they convey the message that the players
may be in control, turning the game of chance into a game of skill. Strategies often rely on the
“analysis” of past drawings, magic formulas, or combinatorics (lottery wheeling1). Strategies
may thus promote unsound beliefs, resulting in unrealistic expectations, painful
disappointments, and suboptimal financial planning.

           They may also encourage users to spend a lot of time on completely futile attempts to
predict the unpredictable – the lottery outcomes – instead of more satisfactory or productive
activities. They may promote the usage of mystery winning formulas or convince them of
conspiracy theories or a secret way to win, undermining the belief that the lottery is random
and fair, which could be a fertile ground for other conspiracy theories. These negative
consequences are likely to fall disproportionately upon the most vulnerable populaces, for
example, those with less education and those prone to pathological gambling.

           Existing literature shows that lottery strategies cannot be profitable. Lottery drawing
machines are regularly checked for possible biases and independent scholarly studies confirm
that they do not show any (Chen et al., 2014; Farrell et al., 2000). The only feat that the players
may achieve (and only in the parimutuel lotteries) is to slightly reduce their expected losses by
picking relatively unpopular combinations, which translates into a lower probability of having
to share the jackpot with others. In particular, Chen et al. (2014) and Oyeleke and Otekunrin

1
    Appendix A contains description of lottery strategies and other specific, lottery-related, terms.
Kachurka, R. and Krawczyk, M. /WORKING PAPERS 29/2020 (335)                      2

(2014) found that betting on “cold” numbers (that have not been drawn for a while) brings a bit
more than betting on truly random numbers.

        If strategies cannot bring money, why would players want to use them? While
insufficient research has been devoted to the issue, we may expect reasons largely analogous to
those of playing the lottery in the first place. Thus, strategies may appeal to players’ dreams of
winning and getting rich in an instant. They may strengthen the belief that the game is not purely
random, thus appealing to the illusion of control (J. Langer, 1975). Ejova et al. (2010) propose
a useful distinction here between primary control (direct control over the outcome of
a stochastic process) and secondary control (ability to detect lucky moments). Naturally, neither
type of control is possible with lotteries.

        Studies exploring the cognitive psychology of lottery play (M Griffiths & Wood, 2001;
Rogers, 1998) propose that the decisions of many lottery players are influenced by their
irrational beliefs (R Ladouceur et al., 1995; Robert Ladouceur & Walker, 1996; Toneatto et al.,
1997; N. E. Turner et al., 2005)– for example, magical thinking or superstition. Such irrational
beliefs could be a way of dealing with the uncontrollable surrounding environment (Rudski,
2004; Vyse, 2013). We may expect strategies to feed into such erroneous beliefs.

        One specific, popular misconception is that sequences such as {1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6} are
generally (incorrectly) considered less likely to be drawn (Hardoon et al., 2001; Krawczyk &
Rachubik, 2018; Robert Ladouceur & Walker, 1996). This could be explained by the
representativeness heuristic (Kahneman & Tversky, 1972), under which even a small sample is
expected to show the features of the random process, which has generated it. Thus, a “truly
random” combination is expected to include, for example, some low numbers and some high
numbers. Another popular bias is gambler’s fallacy (C. T. Clotfelter & Cook, 1993), in which
subsequent drawings are perceived as not independent, so that numbers may be “due” and so
supposedly more likely to show up if they have not been drawn recently.

        Strategies are also likely to affect players' feelings, including positive anticipatory
emotions, such as hope, related to the lottery (Kocher et al., 2014). One way to model such
a tendency is in terms of overweighting small probabilities of extreme events, as in prospect
theory (Tversky & Kahneman, 1992) in which, unlike in the normative expected utility theory,
decision weights given to different monetary outcomes may deviate from their factual
probabilities. Interestingly, it is not only the probability of winning but the expectation of the
Kachurka, R. and Krawczyk, M. /WORKING PAPERS 29/2020 (335)                       3

level of happiness after hitting the jackpot that is overly optimistic (Brickman et al., 1978;
Loewenstein & Schkade, 1999).

       To the extent that strategies appeal to hope and the illusion of control, they are likely
to be particularly attractive to     more vulnerable populaces – poorer and less educated
individuals. Indeed, they are more in need of extra cash and the feeling of control over their
financial situation. If that is happening, it would contribute to the tendency of these groups to
disproportionately likely to gamble, also gamble pathologically (Brown et al., 1992; C.
Clotfelter & Cook, 1999; C. T. Clotfelter & Cook, 1989).

       Behavioral tendencies observed in decision making under risk and uncertainty may also
explain why people may continue to use strategies. For example, many players bet on the same
combination of numbers in each drawing (Suetens et al., 2016). One explanation refers to regret
aversion – players tend to avoid the anticipatory regret that could ensue once their regular
numbers are drawn but they have not purchased a ticket (Wolfson & Briggs, 2002). This notion
is consistent with the observation that people are reluctant to exchange a lottery ticket for
another ticket (Bar-Hillel & Neter, 1996; Krawczyk & Rachubik, 2018).

       Risk seeking in losses (Tversky & Kahneman, 1992) could motivate a player to keep
using a strategy due to the existing losses associated with strategy purchase or usage. Such
a practice of “chasing losses” is an important sign of problem gambling. Due to illusory
correlation (M Griffiths & Wood, 2001), the player could continue to use a strategy due to the
false impression that it indeed increased their winning rate. “Near miss” or “near win” could
create an illusion that the player almost guessed the winning numbers and should thus keep
using the strategy (Rogers, 1998).

       Similarly, persuasion employed by the sellers of lottery strategies is likely to contribute
to their use. Among Cialdini’s principles of influence (2007), three seem particularly important,
namely authority, consistency, and scarcity. Indeed, as casual observation suggests, strategy
vendors often strive to establish themselves as experts, provide a spotless portfolio of
testimonials, and suggest that their “limited-time offer” is a very special occasion indeed.

       It is also worth checking if the willingness to use strategies or, more generally, special
practices when playing, depends on the influence of other people (Mark Griffiths & Barnes,
2008), who could also use strategies or practices. Interestingly, playing in a group could be an
aspect of a strategy by itself. Indeed, playing in a lottery syndicate, in which players
Kachurka, R. and Krawczyk, M. /WORKING PAPERS 29/2020 (335)                       4

proportionately share the costs of tickets and the winnings is a popular practice (Rogers &
Webley, 2001).

       To the best of our knowledge, the only closely related previous study (investigating
lottery strategies) is that by Turner (2003). These authors located a large number of sources,
mostly books (1157 items in total) offering advice on gambling. However, they investigated
only a small sample of them (18 books and 4 webpages), of which four books and one webpage
contained lottery strategies. The work is rather exploratory and descriptive. Their key
observation is that a large number of gambling strategies are available on the market and most
of them contain a mix of accurate and inaccurate, misleading information about gambling.

       In our Study 1 we built upon this analysis, modifying it on several dimensions. We
focused on sources solely devoted to lottery strategies, rather than considered all sources on
gambling. We examined in detail a much larger number of sources, namely 89 books and 83
websites. We did not merely inspect the sources; we also rated them on several features. As
a result, we were able to assess the prevalence of implicit references to various constructs
proposed in the previous behavioral literature of decision making under risk and uncertainty
such as the illusion of control and regret aversion, among others (see Appendix A). Finally, we
were able to perform a quantitative analysis, including factor analysis, identifying key
dimensions that span the variability observed in the data.

       It should be emphasized that Study 1 represents an exploratory work in a vast and as of
yet poorly scrutinized research area. This is associated with certain limitations. In particular,
we initially planned to measure the popularity of various (online) sources of lottery strategies
and relate their popularity to strategy-specific features. We faced extreme practical difficulties
in this enterprise. Roughly, we only have the measure of the popularity of books and some
webpages promoting lottery syndicates. This approach could tell us very little about the demand
side of the market: we could tell what strategies are offered but not much about who and why
consumes them.

       To address these important issues, we supplemented our ethnographic work with an
online survey of lottery players (Study 2). We inquired about their use of strategies from books
and webpages and motivations to use them. We also asked about their practices when playing.
We identified demographic, personality, and cognitive correlates of these choices.
Kachurka, R. and Krawczyk, M. /WORKING PAPERS 29/2020 (335)                             5

2 Study 1: analysis of available strategy materials

2.1 Research goals and methodology
Our hypothesis was that lottery strategies exploit cognitive biases, heuristics or the irrational
beliefs of players about lottery play. Additionally, we wanted to check whether popular and
unpopular lottery strategies appeal to different cognitive biases and heuristics. We gathered
lottery strategies from books (available on Amazon.com) and webpages. We also attempted to
measure the popularity of each source. Then we classified the obtained data, describing the
main features of each source.

2.1.1 Data collection

Webpages with lottery strategies were identified using Google Search, Alexa.com,
keywordtool.io, and Google Keyword Planner. We were not aware of an existing database
detailing lottery strategy websites. Thus, we constructed it ourselves using the snowball
method, starting from the sample of webpages and extending it to include similar pages.

           First, we performed an initial Google search using lottery strategy related keywords, for
example, “lottery strategy”, “lottery system”, and “how to choose lottery numbers”. We
performed every search using a new virtual machine and the United States IP and English
language settings to avoid search engine bias. In this way we have come up with an initial list
of webpages. At this point, we had both webpages with and without lottery strategies. Then,
using the Alexa.com service provided by Amazon, we collected a list of webpages visited by
the same audience2, as those of our initial lists that did contain strategies. We repeated this
exercise until the last (fifth, as it turned out) iteration increased the number of unique webpages
on our list by less than 1%.

           Of the resulting 632 webpages, 228 were found to indeed contain lottery strategies,
including 41 webpages promoting syndicate lottery play only. The latter represents a distinct
case, as they generally do not advise when and where to play or how to choose numbers – they
merely encourage playing in groups. Among the webpages excluded from the analysis were
those containing a review of lottery strategies, lottery rules, or online lotteries. Some of these
webpages were not classified as containing strategies but were still misleading - a webpage

2
    Specifically, we used Alexa’s Audience Overlap tool, see https://try.alexa.com/marketing-stack/audience-
overlap-tool for details.
Kachurka, R. and Krawczyk, M. /WORKING PAPERS 29/2020 (335)                     6

containing statistics about the frequent and infrequent numbers from recent drawings but
without explicit advice about how to use this information is a case in point.

       To verify the comprehensiveness of our list, we used the leading search engine, namely
Google. To come up with relevant queries, we used Alexa.com to define the keywords (n =
754) that people typically use to find the webpages with lottery strategies from our list. Using
the keywordtool.io service, we found keywords (n=2323) related to the aforementioned list of
keywords. Then, we took a sample of the keywords (n = 50) to check if someone searching for
them will indeed end up on a webpage from our list. We performed Google Search with each
of these 50 keywords, storing the first 10 results (as 95% of users do not look beyond the top
10 hits (Lorigo et al., 2006)). We confirmed that as much as 99% of the webpages found in this
way had already been on our list.

       Because a detailed analysis of each source is very time consuming, we only conducted
such an analysis for a random sample of one-third of them (Appendix A), resulting in 83
investigated webpages. Likewise, of the most popular e-books found in Amazon’s Humor &
Entertainment\Puzzles&Games\Gambling\Lotteries section we investigated all that we could
access, totaling 89. Despite various attempts, we did not find a good measure of webpage
popularity. In the case of books, we measured popularity based on their Amazon bookstore
popularity ranking provided by Amazon.com. For example, a book titled “Lotto Wheel Five To
Win” by Gail Howard ranked 2 in the Humor and Entertainment\Puzzles&Games\Gambling\
Lotteries section was the second best-selling book in this category.

2.1.2 Data classification

For each book, we collected the link to the book at Amazon, title, author’s name, year of
publication, price, and the Amazon bookstore popularity ranking. For each book, we collected
the link to the homepage and price. Some webpages offer either a web software available
directly in the browser, or downloadable software. In both cases, we collected the link to the
software (if any).

       Based on the previous literature, we defined twelve features that we call explicit
recommendations (or simply recommendations) because they tell the players how, when, or
where to choose lottery numbers – for example, that they should avoid recently drawn numbers.
We also defined ten other features that did not represent any specific advice but were still an
Kachurka, R. and Krawczyk, M. /WORKING PAPERS 29/2020 (335)                           7

important component of the strategy. Appeal to authority is one example in this category. All
the explicit recommendations and other features are described in Appendix A.

           Further data collection was performed by two independent raters.3 Each of them
carefully investigated each website and book in the sample. For every feature, she indicated
which books and websites contained this feature, if possible supplementing this categorization
with relevant quotes from the source. As a result for each source-feature combination we had
a rating of 0 if both rathers agreed the source did not have the feature, a rating of 1 if both
agreed it died and a rating of .5 if only one did.

           For webpages, we also collected information about the design, which could have an
impact on strategy popularity. We measured subjective webpage design score, ranging from
1 to 4, where 1 generally indicates an old-fashioned and/or overly flashy webpage that is hard
to read and navigate, whereas 4 indicates a modern, elegant, and user-friendly one. For books,
we collected information about book cover design. We also indicated the availability of graphs
and tables, as well as videos and screenshots (in case lottery software was a part of the offer).

           We calculated recommendations’ and other features’ indicators, as well as webpage and
book design scores, as the average score given by two separate reviewers. Of course, these were
to some extent subjective. Still, the inter-rater agreement was acceptable for all variables, and
high on average (see Appendix B for all the details).

2.2 Results
We divide the results section into two parts. Firstly, to give a general picture of the available
lottery systems, we ran a qualitative analysis of their features. We start with those that
frequently appeared and were roughly uniform across all types of strategies: an appeal to an
illusion of control, establishing an authority, or magical thinking. Then we describe the broad
themes identified: the analysis of the past, the elimination of “unlikely” combinations, lottery
wheeling, lottery syndicates, and others. They roughly correspond to different types of
strategies, although these themes are not mutually exclusive.

           Secondly, we present the results of the quantitative analysis. We report the prevalence
of all features using dummy variables to indicate whether the given feature is present in

3
    Average interrater agreement was 0.78 for books and 0.76 for webpages. For details – see Table B1.
Kachurka, R. and Krawczyk, M. /WORKING PAPERS 29/2020 (335)                                      8

a specific strategy. We subsequently show the results of factor analysis to find the main strategy
traits. Finally, we investigate the correlation of features with books’ popularity.

2.2.1 Qualitative analysis

Illusion of control

Perhaps the most important feature of a lottery strategy is that it appeals to the illusion of
control. What good is a strategy that leaves the user at the mercy of a lottery’s poor odds? Not
surprisingly, authors thus routinely try to create an impression that you can “fix the odds”, that
a lottery is a game of skill which may be profitable if played right. Many sources explicitly
state: “turn a game of chance into a game of skill” (B14). Interestingly, the word “control” itself
is often used – “you can actually control and improve your luck” (B2); “I'll prove to you that
the lottery absolutely is controlled” (B3).

           As a result, “each dollar that you wager in the lottery or lotto becomes a logical
investment designed to reap profits” (B4). Because the lottery is supposedly not about luck
anymore, once the right strategy is used, a guarantee to win may be given: “GUARANTEED
WIN! (…) your numbers are (…) combined into the correct combinations to give a specific win
guarantee”; “learn a technique that can guarantee you success more than 99% of other scratch
card players” (B5).

           Exactly how the winning guarantee comes about depends on the strategy. They may
emphasize skill and effort or speak of the “proven steps and strategies on how to join the elite
lottery winners’ circle by transforming a game of chance and luck into a game of skill; if you
do not develop your skill in choosing numbers … the odds of winning any lottery are quite
unfavorable” (B6).

Establishing authority

Why should the reader believe that the strategy will help her “fix the odds”, turning a lottery
into a game of skill? Establishing authority is a key element. One way to achieve this involves
using (pseudo) scientific terminology, promising that the promoted software uses “neural

4
    Each book and webpage has its code, which starts with the letter B or W, respectively. Exact book title or webpage
link could be found by its code in the “Reference list of cited book and webpages”, at the end or the work
Kachurka, R. and Krawczyk, M. /WORKING PAPERS 29/2020 (335)                         9

networks” and “advanced combination[al] logic” (W1). Occasionally, this delivers highly
dubious statements such as “negative emotions vibrate on a negative frequency” (B7). Many
authors also make their language sufficiently vague to prevent the verification of their
recommendations. Additionally, the mechanics of the strategy are rarely disclosed: “once you
have the winning numbers for at least seven previous drawings (...) plug them into the simple
formula… [which is] based on [a] lot of complex mathematics. But I've done all the hard work
for you” (W2).

       Another narrative strategy involves providing pseudo-arguments that mix fact and
fiction or draw unwarranted conclusions. For example, a book by Michael A. Muse, whose first
seven chapters contain no words at all, consisting of just one number each, assures in the
concluding chapter that “if you play these numbers, in any combination, every time that you
buy a lottery ticket, the statistical probability that you will win increases with each ticket that
you buy, no matter which lottery or size” (B8). While this claim cannot be refuted, the
recommendation to play these rather than some other numbers is at best worthless. At worst,
the advice is harmful – if others follow it, the jackpot is more likely to be split. Similarly, Gail
Howards argues that strategies work, because the fraction of strategy users is higher among
lottery winners than among all players (B9). Of course, even if true, this pseudo-argument
overlooks the possibility that strategy users on average purchase more tickets than other players.

       Another way to establish credibility, several books and websites are signed by “doctors”
and “professors”. Some of these people, for example, Dr Iliya Bluskov and Dr Scott Brown,
appear to be or have been genuine university faculty with PhDs. By contrast, we could not
locate Professor William R. Foster, for example. References to a “vast study” by a “Belgian
science man” (W3) and the “conclusions of a major class project at a well-known University”
(W4) are completely elusive. Likewise, if a strategy was created by “an MIT Professor” and it
“took 27 years to develop” (W5), why were they not willing to sign their magnum opus?

       Regardless of alleged academic credentials, many books and websites relate a long
personal story of how the developer was looking for a way to win a lottery and although
frustrated with their initial attempts, persevered and perfected their strategy. “I built my own
strategies through countless hours of studying the odds and investigating how and why people
are able to win” (B10). Another page reports that the findings of “[a] ten-year experiment [in
which] dreams have been painstakingly analyzed” (B11).
Kachurka, R. and Krawczyk, M. /WORKING PAPERS 29/2020 (335)                      10

       These stories are often accompanied by warnings against other allegedly misleading,
ineffective, or expensive strategies that the author encountered during their search for the
ultimate lotto strategy. For example, “95% of other so-called lottery strategies being sold out
there are Not Tested or Proven to give you wins, they are just cheap useless software or made-
up strategies by some person then hyped up to sound great, with a high price tag” (W6). Raising
suspicion towards others is noteworthy given that creating a general impression that the
business is corrupted may undermine their trustworthiness. This is particularly true given that
these claims are essentially never substantiated, so the reader may have a hard time guessing
why she should only be suspicious towards these other authors.

       Two main strategies are used at this stage to remedy any doubts about a strategy’s
credibility. First, the claims made by the strategy stating that they are “the most reliable” or
“the only” are made and often “backed” by shiny images of all sorts of seemingly made-up
certificates of a “trusted seller”, “number one”, or guaranteeing “100% satisfaction”. Second,
many websites provide numerous testimonials. Of course, it is difficult for a typical user to
verify whether “Charles from [the] United States” (W7) won, let alone whether this happened
because he had been using the strategy in question.

Magical thinking

Moving now to the first of the four broad themes showing up in some strategies only. Many
strategies derive the feeling of control from various forms of magical thinking. The sources
speak of the “laws of attraction”, which vaguely suggests that a strong desire to win suffices to
somehow exert at least partial control over the outcomes. The readers are told that “in your
mind you have the power to make your thoughts a reality” (B12). This is because “thoughts are
charged with energy, especially when triggered by emotion (…) a strong desire for a goal that
is charged with positive energy, attracts a positive response, especially when every effort is
made to attain that goal”. These strategies often include warnings that a “barrier of negativity
[must be eliminated] from the fabric of [their] belief strategy” (B2).

       Numerous sources recommend exercises to “focus on opportunities”, “attract good
luck” “visualize winning”, “manifest your millions” and “use your vibrational reach to enhance
your lottery winnings” (B13). Occasionally, these sources deliver factually wrong claims, for
example, that “the actual chance of you winning the lottery is 50-50” (W8). Often, they
resemble religious or spiritual texts. They recommend prayers and meditation and assert
Kachurka, R. and Krawczyk, M. /WORKING PAPERS 29/2020 (335)                        11

a Belief in Just World. Lottery prizes are portrayed as a reward for good deeds or a promise to
put them to good use after winning – “in order to attract great wealth you need to give”;
“[returning a lost ring] will engage forces in the universe to create equilibrium by returning to
me incredible fortunes at some point in the future” (B2).

          Often, references to the ancient art of interpreting dreams are made. Many sources teach
which lottery numbers are associated with special dates, items, people, events, and feelings
experienced in dreams. Some take the magical link beyond dreams: “every experience,
encounter, dream, tragedy, event, action or reaction produces its own unique single digit
number” (B11). Some sources try to link magic with science or technology, asserting that “it
has been proven that our process works out a magic for our members” (W9). Others are
promising the “psychic ability to correctly predict the winning lotto numbers through our new
lottery software breakthrough” (W10), and that “your numbers are magically (mathematically)
combined”, recommending “scientific prayer to the universal intelligence of your subconscious
mind” (B14).

Analysis of previous draws

The second major theme we analyze, the strategies focusing on the analysis of the past, usually
try to distinguish themselves from the magic: “we achieved this not by hocus pocus ideas,
mysterious patterns, (…) but by thorough and professional mathematical analysis” (W11).
These sources speak of “hot” and “cold” numbers, “trends”, “patterns”, and “cycles”, and
encourage users to analyze the “past five” draws or “games out”.

          This type of strategy usually comes with lottery software to assist the player in storing
and analyzing past results. Often, the program is what the user is paying for. The typical
functionalities include downloading past results, checking players’ numbers against them to
find out whether they won, implementing strategy recommendations, and even printing selected
numbers directly onto a lottery ticket. All these could significantly simplify and speed up
gambling, making intense lottery play (and consequently, greater losses) possible with less
effort.

          Computer-assisted scientific analyses of previous drawings seem to be a logical
approach to selecting numbers. After all, that is what intelligent people do before making an
important financial decision: they analyze the data. Several sources reinforce this way of
thinking, advertising a “Lottery Statistical Analysis tool that uses past lottery results to give you
Kachurka, R. and Krawczyk, M. /WORKING PAPERS 29/2020 (335)                        12

the best possible information for selecting and playing your lottery numbers” (W12) and talking
about “intelligently choosing lottery numbers based on the data” (B15). It may be intuitively
difficult to accept that the analysis of data is useless in this context.

        More specifically, there are two main ways in which past drawings can be taken into
account. First, several sources recommend betting on numbers that have not been drawn much
recently. It is argued “that the same lottery number combination that won the jackpot or the
grand prize will win again in the future… had never been a possibility” (B6). Likewise, “when
picking lottery numbers you should NEVER pick the numbers that were drawn last week,
because there is no chance they will be drawn this week” (B16). By a natural extension, the
users are recommended to bet on numbers that have not been drawn for a while (“due” or “cold”
numbers). The belief that they are more likely to show up this time is related to several concepts
discussed in the literature: the negative recency effect, the gambler’s fallacy, and the
representativeness heuristic.

        The opposite strategy involves betting on numbers that are “hot” because they have been
drawn recently. The authors recommend “get[ting] relevant data about a particular lottery and
then choos[ing] your numbers as per the highest frequently drawn numbers” (B17). It is claimed
that “each lottery will have its own unique set of unlucky numbers” (B10). Such claims are
often validated by showing small and likely purposefully picked samples with no statistical
analysis. While it may seem suggestive that one number has been drawn five times in a given
sample while another has not been drawn once, in fact, such an occurrence arising randomly
under a uniform distribution is not that unlikely. Again, the concept of representativeness
explains why such “evidence” is persuasive – there is a natural tendency to expect a very
uniform distribution even in a small sample so that observing actual, unequal frequencies of
numbers leads to the suspicion that the underlying probabilities are not identical.

        Some of the sources point to specific reasons for this. These “explanations” range from
quite general, simple, and sometimes vague, like “balls have different weight” (B3), “balls in
many lotto picking machines at times do not thoroughly mix” (W13); through to more elaborate
ones using technical language – “the drag coefficient of each ball is different, therefore patterns
will develop that can be tracked” (B18). All these explanations (conspiracy theories) could
appeal to the need to be unique – being chosen to obtain secret knowledge that others have no
access to (Lantian et al., 2017) – and a need for control (Byford, 2011; Newheiser et al., 2011).
Kachurka, R. and Krawczyk, M. /WORKING PAPERS 29/2020 (335)                        13

       Although betting on numbers that have been frequently drawn recently and betting on
numbers that have not been drawn recently seem contradictory, they are often combined in the
same source. They may be reconciled by using different time horizons: it is intuitively appealing
to bet on the numbers that seem to be drawn often in the long run but have not been drawn for
a while. Alternatively, one strategy may be applied to individual balls, another to combinations.
In any case, the naturally contradictory nature of being “hot” and being “cold” might help to
avoid the negative verification of the strategy. If only it is sufficiently vague as to when one
should go for the “hot” and when for the “due”, whatever shows up may be ex post interpreted
as “confirming” the strategy.

Elimination of “unlikely” combinations

The third major theme that can be found in several strategies involves the promise of improving
the odds by excluding some combinations, regardless of whether they had been recently drawn.
Interestingly, just like the authors of the strategies based on the analysis of the past sometimes
want to emphasize that they have nothing to do with magical thinking, strategies in this category
may be positioned as distinct from those based on the past: “winslips is a fail-safe number
reduction strategy based on PURE mathematics, certainly NOT a pointless prediction website
based on past history results” (W14).

       A typical example in this group is Gail Howard’s concept of “most typical ranges”
(W8). It is based on a pseudo-argument: because it is very rare that the sum of the drawn
numbers is very low or very high, these combinations should be avoided. Of course, these
combinations have the same probability of being drawn as any other combination. Analogously,
many authors recommend avoiding betting on odd numbers only or even numbers only, instead
advocating for “balanced” combinations.

Lottery wheeling

Finally, the last major group of strategies involves wheeling. Based on combinatorics, these
strategies involve a conditional guarantee of success. For example, if a player is lucky enough
to select 10 numbers in a 6/49 lotto such that five of them turn out to be winning numbers,
a wheeling system may guarantee that she ends up with at least four winning numbers in one
of her lines (which are the appropriately chosen six-element subsets of the set of 10). In most
cases, the strategy requires the purchase of many tickets, which might make them particularly
interesting to syndicates. In the case of individual players, this might induce overspending.
Kachurka, R. and Krawczyk, M. /WORKING PAPERS 29/2020 (335)                      14

       Most sources focusing on wheeling alone provide relatively reliable information. Then
again, it should be clear that wheeling seems particularly attractive when the player is able to
determine a possibly narrow set (for example, a set of 10 in the example above) of “likely”
numbers, which is not possible. It is thus not surprising to see lottery wheeling combined with
more misleading concepts. For example, Gail Howard proposes “balanced wheels”, based on
the fallacious idea that combinations of all very low or all very high numbers are particularly
unlikely.

Other strategies

Syndicate play represents a distinct case. Webpages promoting syndicates often give no explicit
advice on how to choose numbers, but promise to take care of all the difficulties associated with
number picking and prize sharing. Besides, syndicates tend to win many prizes due to the large
number of tickets they buy (which of course means their clients lose even more money).
Reporting only winners, syndicate organizers appeal to availability heuristics and create the
illusion of control, attracting more new players.

       To complete the qualitative analysis, it should be noted that quite a few of the sources
do give some reasonable (even if not sophisticated) advice: that one should not bet more than
one can afford to lose; that losing a ticket means losing money; that one should double-check
for possible winnings; and that in pari-mutuel games it is better to bet on unpopular
combinations (although it is not necessarily easy to identify them). It seems, however, that in
some cases these sensible recommendations are mostly used to lend credibility to the whole
strategy (including its useless or misleading components).

2.2.2 Quantitative analysis

In total, we have reviewed 89 books and 83 webpages – see Table 1 and Table 2, respectively,
for descriptive statistics and Appendix A for the full list. We report the values for books and
webpages separately because they are different in many ways. For example, because books tend
to be longer, whereas websites may offer more visuals and interactive elements; they may also
appeal to different audiences. Perhaps more importantly, as strategy books seem to be gradually
getting replaced by websites and online videos. It is interesting to see what changes in the form
and content are likely to come with this transition. Appendix A also contains a detailed
explanation of variables.
Kachurka, R. and Krawczyk, M. /WORKING PAPERS 29/2020 (335)                        15

Table 1 Descriptive statistics: books

                  Variable              Obs      Mean      Std.Dev.     Min          Max

          price                         88       7.12       21.23       0.99     199.99

          year                          89       2014        5.18      1987       2020

          number_of_pages               77      86.35      110.00          4         673

          review_score                  80       3.40        0.89          1          5

          number_of_reviews             80      44.63      102.55          1         645

          ranking                       72      94.15      108.49          1         429

          design_score                  89         2         0.67          1          4

As can be seen in Table 1, the books in our sample are not so recent. We generally judged them
to be poorly designed (2 design points out of 4). It is interesting to note that these books had on
average as many as 44 Amazon reviews, giving them a surprisingly high mean of 3.4 stars out
of 5. Of course, some of the reviews may have been paid for or simply made up. Amazon has
an indicator for reviews made by buyers (“Amazon Verified Purchase” review). We did not
differentiate between “Amazon Verified Purchase” reviews and other reviews due to the
significant manual effort needed to collect this information.

Table 2 Descriptive statistics: webpages

                    Variable        Obs       Mean      Std.Dev.      Min      Max

             price                   34       21.83       19.88        0        97

             software                83        0.81       0.40         0         1

             nmbr_of_products        83        3.65       6.64         1        37

             design_score            83        2.51       0.83         1         4
Kachurka, R. and Krawczyk, M. /WORKING PAPERS 29/2020 (335)                      16

The descriptive stats for webpages are reported in Table 2. The systems offered via webpages
sometimes come in many versions, in which case we considered the cheapest one. One outlier
with lottery software price of $729.99 (W15) was disregarded in the calculation of the mean.
Webpages show a smaller range of prices, with strategies typically being considerably more
expensive than books (p < .001)5. 11 webpages offer a subscription service, for example,
delivering lucky numbers weekly for a fee. Webpages tend to be worse designed than books (p
= .002).

Table 3 The prevalence of explicit recommendations in books and webpages
                                                                                 One-sided
                                                        Books    Webpages
                Explicit recommendation                                        Fisher’s exact
                                                        (n=89)    (n=83)
                                                                               test (p-value)
                magic                                   38%*       32%              0.149

                special_meaning_nmbrs                     9%       9%               1.000

                secret_knowledge                         29%       22%              0.458

                based_on_past                            52%       61%              0.370

                high_freq                                22%       29%              0.039

                low_freq                                 24%       23%              1.000

                wheeling                                 24%       41%              0.039

                syndicate_promotion                      11%       13%              0.543

                has_syndicate                              -       7%                 -

                bet_on_the_same_nmbrs                     6%       1%               0.060

                bet_on_unpopular_numbers                  8%       29%              0.498

                bet_on_rnd_nmbrs                          2%       0%               0.001

                other_useful_advice                       8%       30%              0.000

                *numbers add up to more than 100% because categories are not mutually exclusive

5
    Here and thereafter we use one-sided Fisher’s exact test
Kachurka, R. and Krawczyk, M. /WORKING PAPERS 29/2020 (335)                      17

The books and webpages are similar in terms of their prevalence of explicit recommendations
(Table 3), except that more websites (30%) than books (8%) offer some (p
Kachurka, R. and Krawczyk, M. /WORKING PAPERS 29/2020 (335)                         18

The books and webpages are also similar in terms of their prevalence of other features (Table
4), except that a promise of money back or a trial offer is more common in webpages than in
books – in 22% of cases versus 9%, respectively (p=.017). This is not surprising, because 81%
of webpages offer either downloadable or online web software, which could be tested by the
user and blocked by the strategy author after a test period. We did not verify if the promise to
pay back was credible.

        The most frequent feature is an explicit promise of gaining control over the lottery, often
supported by testimonials, personal authority, usage of a (pseudo-) scientific terminology,
tables and graphs, with no significant difference between books and webpages.

        To comprehensively uncover the main dimensions underlying data variability, we
conduct a factor analysis. Table B2 shows the results for the explicit recommendations and
Table B3 for other features. Overall, the factor analysis is largely consistent with the qualitative
analysis, indicating universally important features, such as the appeal to authority and specific
themes, analysis of the past, superstition, lottery syndicate promotion, and lottery wheeling.
The promise of control was not associated with any specific factor – it was common across
different types of strategies.

        Concerning book popularity, we run a series of regression models to discover their
correlates. They showed only moderate goodness of fit and very little robust findings, beyond
that better-designed books tend to sell better. This analysis is available upon request.

3 Study 2: online survey

To gain insight into the popularity of strategies and their users’ motivations and characteristics,
we conducted an online survey. The participants were recruited using advertisements on several
Internet forums for lottery players, within the subject pool of our laboratory of experimental
economics, as well as using Amazon mTurk virtual labor market. Only individuals who
reported playing state lotteries – at least occasionally – were invited to respond to the survey.
In total, we had 342 participants, of which 42.4% were female. About 73.4% resided in the US,
the rest coming from a number of different countries. The mean age was 37.3, with a standard
deviation of 14.0 years. Nearly 13.5% declared playing the lottery daily, 30.1% a few times per
week, 36.3% a few times per month, and 20.2% a few times per year.
Kachurka, R. and Krawczyk, M. /WORKING PAPERS 29/2020 (335)                      19

 3.1 Survey questions
         The survey was displayed on four screens, with a median filling time of 10 minutes. The
 entire questionnaire can be found in Appendix A. The most important questions referred to the
 knowledge and use of strategies available as books or websites. Participants were asked how
 often they played and whether they used “quickpicks” (and why/why not). They were also asked
 about several practices they followed when playing the lottery that were identified as important
 in our Study 1, such as using numerology or analysis of past lottery outcomes, regardless of
 how they were inspired to do so and beliefs about the lottery. Finally, we collected demographic
 data, including religiosity attitudes (Koenig & Büssing, 2010), administered the Big Five
 personality scale TIPI (Gosling et al., 2003), elicited beliefs about games of chance (Wood &
 Clapham, 2005) and risk attitudes in general and in financial matters (Dohmen et al., 2011).

 3.2 Results
 As explored before, a pre-condition of using a typical published strategy is that one chooses
 lottery numbers on their own, instead of relying on the randomization device provided by the
 lottery operator (quickpicks). As shown in Table 5, a large fraction of our responders does that,
 at least some of the time.

 Table 5 Responders using quickpicks and playing with own numbers

                                                                    Share of responders saying
                                             Share of all
                                N                                quickpics are available (n = 322)
                                        responders (n = 342)
                                                                   in the lottery usually played

Quickpicks unavailable in
                                36             10.1%                             -
the lottery usually played

Quickpics available and…       322             89.9%                          100.0%

… never used                    39             10.9%                          12.1%

… occasionally used             53             14.8%                          16.5%

… usually used                  94             26.3%                          29.2%

… always used                  136             38.0%                          42.2%
Kachurka, R. and Krawczyk, M. /WORKING PAPERS 29/2020 (335)                       20

 Those who always use quickpicks, when asked about their pros and cons, most commonly
 mentioned that they save time (31% - percentages calculated from expert-coded open-ended
 responses, see Appendix A). Of those who at least occasionally play with their numbers, the
 most common explanation, provided by 30% of responders, emphasized that it allows them to
 play with their favorite/lucky/meaningful numbers, not necessarily implying that they believed
 it to increase their probability of winning. As one of them put it, “(…) number has a significance
 sometimes. They may come from a dream, or a life situation, or a simple preference.”
 Interestingly and in keeping with the results of Study 1, as many as 21% spontaneously said
 that playing with their numbers gave them more control: “I feel like I have more control and
 it's more personal.”

          Choosing numbers on one’s own does not imply following any specific published
 strategy. Judging from their free-form responses, about 21% would just use personally
 important dates (like the birthday of a loved one) and 22% other personally meaningful or lucky
 numbers. About 18% would choose randomly and 9% go with a hunch, with just 8% performing
 some kind of analysis of the history of past drawings, as often encouraged by published
 strategies. As shown in Table 6, it is indeed only a sizeable minority of responders that recall
 a book or a website containing a strategy; of these who do, most follow it to some extent at
 least.

 Table 6 Responders recalling and using published strategies

                                             % of all responders     % of responders who recall at
                                      N
                                                   (n = 342)          least one strategy (n = 128)

Never come across a strategy         199            60.9%                          X

Came across at least one
                                     128            39.1%                       100.0%
strategy and…

  … does not follow it at all          11            3.4%                         8.6%

  … follows it to a minor extent       77           23.6%                        60.2%

  … follows it to a large extent       30            9.2%                        23.4%

  … follows if fully                   10            3.1%                         7.8%
Kachurka, R. and Krawczyk, M. /WORKING PAPERS 29/2020 (335)                    21

  These data confirm our supposition that a non-trivial share of lottery players use published
  strategies at least some of the time. Unfortunately, in this case, the direct open-ended questions
  about the contents of these strategies and reasons for using (or not using) them did not yield
  much. Indeed, only a small minority of those recalling a strategy came up with its unique
  identifier (for example, website address or full book title), summarized in a meaningful way, or
  responded to our question as to why they followed it (or did not follow it). These isolated
  responses will thus not be analyzed here.

           We identified the correlates of following a strategy. Our key dependent variable was
  followstrategy_all, taking the value of 1 for those who never follow one, 2 for those who follow
  it only to a minor extent, 3 - to a large extent, and 4 - for those who fully follow the strategy6.
  Of course, in any case, the distances between the levels are not interpretable, so we revert to
  the ordered logit. Several model specifications are reported in Table 7. In model 1 we only
  include basic demographics. In model 2, we add the frequency of lottery play. In model 3 we
  further add questions about religious practice. In model 4 we add psychological traits. Model 5
  adds reported beliefs about lottery, model 6 adds questions about social aspects of playing and
  model 7 – risk attitudes.

  Table 7 Determinants of the tendency to follow a strategy: an ordered logistic regression

        Variable               (1)      (2)        (3)         (4)        (5)         (6)         (7)
source_mturk                  0.07     0.50       -0.09       -0.59      -1.00       -1.00       -0.93
age                           -0.04    -0.03      -0.03       -0.01      -0.05       -0.07       -0.06
age2                          0.00     0.00       0.00        0.00        0.00       0.00        0.00
woman                         -0.18    -0.02      -0.12       -0.10      -0.14       -0.19       -0.20

  6
      The construction of this variable may require some discussion. We merge together those who
  do not recall any published strategy with those who have seen one (or more) but disregard it,
  the bottom line being they are not following any strategy. Some justification for this
  categorization comes from the fact that some players may have never seen a strategy or have
  seen one but forgotten it precisely because they were not interested in strategies (and so they
  would not follow one even if they recalled it). Alternative modeling choices would have led to
  qualitatively similar results.
Kachurka, R. and Krawczyk, M. /WORKING PAPERS 29/2020 (335)              22

university                     0.12      0.02     -0.11     -0.14     -0.03        0.00      0.02
income                         0.14      0.17      0.12      0.14      0.13        0.09      0.08
USA                          1.46***    1.24**    1.43**    1.20**     0.97        0.93      0.90
India                          0.60      0.50      0.49      0.24      0.40        0.40      0.32
Poland                        -14.57    -13.60    -13.66    -14.24    -15.54       -15.01   -14.91
frequency                    0.48***    0.34**    0.31**    0.26*      0.22        0.21
religiosityprivate                       0.12      0.11      0.10      0.10        0.11
religiositydivine                        0.03      0.06     -0.02     -0.04        -0.06
religiositycarry                        0.28**    0.25**     0.12      0.13        0.13
extraversion                             0.12      0.10      0.11      0.11
agreeableness                            0.09     -0.01      0.01      0.01
conscientiousness                        -0.02     0.01      0.01      0.01
emotional_stability                      0.04      0.06      0.03      0.04
openness_to_experience                   -0.06    -0.10     -0.13     -0.14
bhigher                                            0.01      0.03      0.02
blower                                             0.10      0.11      0.09
bthesame                                          -0.02     -0.04     -0.05
blotteryfair                                      -0.07     -0.04     -0.04
blotteryskill                                    0.39***   0.45***   0.44***
bluckycoin                                         0.13      0.14      0.14
brituals                                          -0.02     -0.01      0.00
bgamblfallacy                                     -0.10     -0.11     -0.11
blotteryunfair                                     0.03      0.04      0.05
bjackpot                                           0.10      0.10      0.09
peerplaybuy                                       0.231*     0.23
peerplaychoice                                    -0.12     -0.14
peerplayresults                                   -0.19     -0.21
peerplaypeople                                     0.28      0.29
riskgeneral                                                  0.07
riskfinancial                                                0.02
cut1 _cons                     0.98     3.23**    3.78**    4.47**    3.99*        3.89      4.14*
cut2 _cons                    2.48*    4.78***   5.41***   6.12***    5.73**   5.66**       5.91**
cut3 _cons                   3.97***   6.29***   6.99***   7.70***   7.35***   7.32***      7.57***
N                              324       324       324       324       324          324      324

    *** p
Kachurka, R. and Krawczyk, M. /WORKING PAPERS 29/2020 (335)                   23

   We find a robust (and intuitive) link between the willingness to follow a strategy and the belief
   that the lottery is a game of skill. The effect of frequency of lottery play appears to be mediated
   by beliefs about the lottery, as it loses its significance as we add them.

   We now move on to the specific practices our responders employ when playing, no matter if
   they come from a published strategy or not. All of them are fairly common, with the median
   response being “sometimes” or “often” for each of them, see Table B4 in Appendix B. We also
   find these practices to be highly correlated. For concise reporting, we thus run a factor analysis,
   see Table B5 in Appendix B. As evidenced by parallel analysis (see Figure 3 in Appendix B),
   a single variable represents a disproportionate part of these variables’ variance. We thus use
   this aggregate factor, interpretable as a general tendency to use various practices when playing
   the lottery, as a dependent variable in our analysis. We run ordinary least squares regression
   using several specifications analogous to those reported previously in Table 7, see Table 8.
   Regressions modeling individual lottery practices are available upon request - they tend to yield
   qualitatively similar results.

   Table 8 Determinants of the general tendency to follow various practices when playing

Variable                        (1)       (2)       (3)         (4)         (5)        (6)         (7)
source_mturk                 0.94***   1.13***    0.89***     0.58***    0.39***      0.24*      0.24*
age                            -0.02     -0.02     -0.01       0.00        -0.01      0.00        0.00
age2                           0.00      0.00       0.00       0.00        0.00       0.00        0.00
woman                         -0.036    0.020      -0.035     -0.048      -0.072     -0.12*      -0.11*
university                    0.21**     0.14       0.06       0.01        -0.03      0.00        0.01
income                         0.03      0.05       0.01       0.03        0.03       0.03        0.03
USA                           0.44**     0.09       0.11      0.318*       0.20       -0.04       0.24
India                          0.39      0.13       0.09       0.24        0.23     (omitted)    0.293
other_country                  -0.08     -0.27     -0.28       0.04        -0.02     -0.24*       0.04
frequency                    0.19***   0.15***    0.12***     0.09**       0.06*      0.05*
knowstrategies               0.36***   0.25***     0.16**      0.05        0.06       0.06
religiosityprivate                     0.08***     0.07**     0.05**       0.03       0.03
religiositydivine                        0.02       0.04       0.01        -0.02      -0.02
religiositycarry                       0.13***    0.10***      0.03        0.03       0.03
extraversion                           0.040**     0.012       0.01        0.02
agreeableness                          0.07***     0.04**     0.03**      0.03**
conscientiousness                      -0.07***   -0.04**     -0.04**     -0.04**
emotional_stability                      -0.01     -0.01       0.00        -0.01
Kachurka, R. and Krawczyk, M. /WORKING PAPERS 29/2020 (335)                      24

openness_to_experience                  -0.01       -0.01       -0.02      -0.02
bhigher                                             0.01        0.00        0.00
blower                                              0.03        0.03        0.03
bthesame                                            0.01        0.00        0.00
blotteryfair                                        0.02        0.02        0.02
blotteryskill                                      0.09***    0.07***     0.07***
bluckycoin                                          0.02        0.01        0.01
brituals                                           0.05**      0.04*       0.04*
bgamblfallacy                                       0.03        0.02        0.02
blotteryunfair                                     0.06***     0.04**     0.04**
bjackpot                                            0.01        0.01        0.02
peerplaybuy                                        0.10***    0.10***
peerplaychoice                                     0.09***    0.09***
peerplayresults                                     0.01        0.01
peerplaypeople                                      0.02        0.02
riskgeneral                                                     -0.02
riskfinancial                                                   0.02
_cons                      -0.90*      -1.70***   -2.04***    -2.12***   -2.47***    -2.27***   -2.54***
N                               324         324        324         324         324        324        324
r2_a                            0.28       0.37        0.48       0.56        0.66       0.70       0.70

    *** p
Kachurka, R. and Krawczyk, M. /WORKING PAPERS 29/2020 (335)                       25

       We did not succeed in obtaining high-quality data on the popularity of specific
strategies, especially those available via webpages. Thus, we could not reliably pin down the
number of lottery strategy buyers, although the sheer volume of strategies suggests that they
find a lot of customers. This is also clearly confirmed by our survey results; using them we can
cautiously estimate that about one-third of those who play the lottery at least occasionally (thus
about one-sixth of the adult US population) have encountered a published strategy and follows
some of its elements at least some of the time. Our results also suggest that those who play
more, are more likely to use strategies, although it is impossible to establish causality (which
could plausibly go either way).

       We found that lottery strategies try to create an illusion of control over lotteries by
making extensive use of various cognitive biases and heuristics, including the illusion of
correlation, authority bias, gambler’s fallacy, hot hand fallacy, and magical thinking. It is
interesting to note how many strategies try to establish authority using scientific terminology,
formulas, and calculations. Of course, the bad news is that probability theory is largely
misrepresented or not taken seriously. The chances are, though, that most readers will not notice
sloppiness and internal inconsistencies. While reasonable recommendations can occasionally
be found, perhaps to boost reliability, they do not change the wider picture.

       In most cases, errors, manipulations, contradictions, and non-sequiturs can be easily
found even without expert knowledge of probability and statistics. Lottery players are likely to
stick to their motivated beliefs, though. What could be more enjoyable than to believe that you
are destined to win a fortune one day? Thus, the authors of lottery strategies, by creating an
illusion of control, just make it easier to believe in what people want to believe. These
observations are corroborated by our survey results, showing that the need for control is a key
motivation to choose numbers on one’s own (rather than rely on operator’s “quick picks”) and
that the fundamentally wrong belief that lottery is a game of skill is correlated to following
a strategy and to following various dubious lottery playing practices, whatever their source may
be.

       Unfortunately, there are populaces that are particularly vulnerable to false promises to
gain control over lottery results. For many poorer and less educated people, lotteries become
a source of dream and hope. They spend their time and money on this activity not only because
of entertainment but also to escape (or at least have a chance to escape) from poverty.
Unfortunately, in almost all cases lottery play makes poor people even poorer.
You can also read