DIGITAL FINGERPRINTS OF COGNITIVE REFLECTION - PSYARXIV

Page created by Rita Castillo
 
CONTINUE READING
DIGITAL FINGERPRINTS OF COGNITIVE REFLECTION - PSYARXIV
1       Digital Fingerprints of Cognitive Reflection
 2        Mohsen Mosleh1, Gordon Pennycook2, Antonio A. Arechar1, and David G. Rand1,3,4
 3
 4   1
       Sloan School of Management, Massachusetts Institute of Technology; 2Hill/Levene Schools of Business,
 5   University of Regina; 3Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology;
 6   4
       Institute for Data, Systems, and Society, Massachusetts Institute of Technology
 7
 8
 9                                     Corresponding authors: mmosleh@mit.edu
10                           This working paper has not yet been peer-reviewed.
11                                         Posted April 14 2020
12
13         Social media is playing an increasingly large role in everyday life. Thus, it is of both
14   scientific and practical interest to understand behavior on social media platforms.
15   Furthermore, social media provides a unique window for social scientists to deepen our
16   understanding of the human mind. Here we investigate the relationship between
17   individual differences in cognitive reflection and behavior on Twitter in a sample of N =
18   1,953 users recruited via Prolific Academic. In doing so, we differentiate between two
19   competing accounts of human information processing: an “intuitionist” account whereby
20   reflection plays little role in daily life, and a “reflectionist” account whereby reflection
21   (and, in particular, overriding intuitive responses) does play an important role. We found
22   that people who score higher on the Cognitive Reflection Test (CRT) – a widely used
23   measure of reflective thinking – were more discerning in their social media use: They
24   followed more selectively, shared news content from more reliable sources, and tweeted
25   about weightier subjects (e.g. politics). Furthermore, a network analysis indicated that
26   the phenomenon of echo chambers, in which discourse is more likely with like-minded
27   others, is not limited to politics: we observe “cognitive echo chambers” in which people
28   low on cognitive reflection tend to follow the same set of accounts. Our results help to
29   illuminate the drivers of behavior on social media platforms, and challenge intuitionist
30   notions that reflective thinking is unimportant for everyday judgment and decision-
31   making.
32
33
34          Social media has become a dominant force in modern life – it is a major channel for
35   social interactions, political communications, and commercial marketing. Social media can
36   have both positive and negative impacts. For example, on the positive side, user-generated
37   content on social media has facilitated social connection by helping friends and relatives who
38   are separated by distance stay abreast of what is happening in each other’s lives1,2, and by
39   helping to connect strangers that have similar interests3. Social media has also helped to spread
40   invaluable information about topics such as awareness of diseases and philanthropic causes
41   (e.g., the ALS ice bucket challenge4), helped people in need generate resources (e.g.,
42   crowdfunding for medical bills5), and quickly disseminated information during disasters (e.g.,
43   Facebook’s “marked safe” tool6). However, social media also allows the spread of
44   misinformation and scams7-10, may facilitate the emergence of echo chambers and political
45   polarization11-13, and could be a host for interference and automated propaganda bots14-18.
46          Given the substantial importance of social media in people’s lives, and the wide range of
47   content available therein, it is therefore of scientific and practical interest to understand how
48   people interact with social media, and what influences their decisions to share various types of
49   content and follow different accounts/pages. Prior work in this vein has explored the

                                                        1
50   relationship between social media use and various personality and demographic measures, such
51   as the “Big-Five” personality traits19-22, the “Dark Triad” personality traits23,24,
52   partisanship25,26, age25,26, and gender27.
53           Here we add to this literature by contributing an account of basic information processing
54   that is capable of explaining social media engagement across a wide range of content. We do
55   so using the lens of cognitive science. Furthermore, by assessing how people behave on social
56   media, we help to litigate an ongoing debate within the cognitive science literature between
57   two competing accounts of the cognitive factors that determine people’s beliefs and behaviors.
58   This debate is grounded in dual-process theories, which distinguish reflective or analytic
59   thought from the intuitive responses that emerge autonomously (and often quickly) when an
60   individual is faced with a triggering stimulus28-31. One of the key implications of this distinction
61   is that analytic thinking (unlike intuitive processing) is, to some extent, discretionary – that is,
62   people may or may not engage in deliberation at any given time, and this tendency to deliberate
63   varies across individuals29,32. Consider the following question: “If you’re running a race and
64   you pass the person in second place, what place are you in?”33 The answer that intuitively
65   comes to mind for many people is ‘first place’, but this is not the correct answer (if you pass
66   the person in second place, you are in second place). Correctly answering problems such as
67   this therefore typically requires most individuals to stop and think analytically, over-riding an
68   intuitive response that, at first blush, seems correct34-37. This individual difference is often
69   referred to as cognitive style. Here we will investigate how cognitive style relates to behavior
70   on Twitter. To do so, we will measure cognitive style using the Cognitive Reflection Test
71   (CRT) 34 – a set of questions with intuitively compelling but incorrect answers (such as the
72   example above) that is widely used in behavioral economics and cognitive psychology to
73   measure the propensity to engage in analytic thinking (and that does not strongly correlate with
74   personality, e.g., ‘Big Five’38,39).
75           Although there appears to be general agreement surrounding the theoretical utility of
76   dual-process theory (although for alternative perspectives, see40,41), there is a great deal of
77   disagreement about the relative roles of intuition and reflection in people’s everyday lives. It
78   has been famously argued that humans are like an “emotional dog with a rational tail”42 – that
79   our capacity to reflect is underused in such a way that its primary function is merely to justify
80   our intuitive judgments43. Similarly, it has been argued that the main function of human
81   reasoning is argumentation rather than truth-seeking44,45.
82           Unlike in the example CRT question given above, where analytic thinking is integral to
83   correcting our (sometimes inaccurate) intuitive impulses (see ref 31), this “intuitionist”
84   perspective implies that, although people may sometimes override intuitive responses on
85   idiosyncratic word problems administered in social science studies, the real-world function of
86   analytic thinking is to merely justify and reinforce the beliefs and behaviors that we have
87   learned culturally. Relatedly, it has been argued that human capacity to reflect actually reduces
88   accuracy by driving polarization around ideological issues46,47 – thus, by this account,
89   reasoning is not simply unable to direct our beliefs and behaviors beyond intuition; in some
90   cases, it actively makes us less reasonable.
91            However, there is also a growing literature that demonstrates positive everyday
92   consequences of analytic thinking29. This “reflectionist” perspective48 argues that thinking
93   analytically actually does have a meaningful impact on our beliefs and behaviors, and typically
94   does so in a manner that increases the accuracy of our beliefs. Evidence for this account comes
95   from lab studies where analytic cognitive style (e.g., higher score on the CRT) is associated
96   with a wide range of social phenomena, such as religious disbelief49-51, paranormal disbelief52-
     54
97      , rejection of conspiracist claims55, increased acceptance of science56,57, and rejection of
98   pseudo-profound bullshit58. More reflective individuals are also less likely to offload their
99   thinking to the internet via search engines59. Particularly relevant to social media, recent work

                                                      2
100   finds that people who perform better on the CRT are less likely to believe blatantly false “fake
101   news” stories60,61 and they self-report a lower likelihood of sharing such content on social
102   media61, as well as reporting less trust in unreliable fake news or hyper-partisan news sources62.
103   Taken together, this body of previous evidence supporting the reflectionist account suggests
104   that people who perform better on the CRT may differ systematically in their social media
105   behavior from people who perform worse – and in particular, that higher CRT performers may
106   be more discerning (i.e., less likely to follow and share epistemically questionable or facile
107   content).
108           Crucially, however, the full body of this research is based on self-reported beliefs and
109   behaviors in survey studies. This is a substantial limitation because the debate between the
110   intuitionist and reflectionist perspectives comes down to the outcomes or consequences of the
111   capacity and disposition to engage in reflective reasoning in the context of daily life. The
112   intuitionist perspective dictates that analytic thinking is not particularly important or effective
113   outside of artificial laboratory settings, and therefore that differences in analytic thinking
114   should not have a meaningful impact on everyday beliefs and behaviors. The reflectionist
115   perspective, conversely, dictates that analytic thinking is crucial for dictating everyday
116   behaviors outside the lab. Here, we investigate the relationship between analytic thinking and
117   naturally occurring behavior on social media, with the goal of distinguishing between these
118   broad accounts of information processing.
119           To do so, we use a hybrid lab-field set-up to investigate such differences by linking
120   survey data to actual behavior on Twitter. We recruited a panel of participants (N = 1,953; 55%
121   female, Mage = 32, 61% UK residents, 37% US residents), who completed the CRT and
122   provided their Twitter username. We then used the Twitter API to pull public information from
123   the users’ profiles on Twitter, allowing us to investigate the relationship between a user’s CRT
124   score and three main dimensions of their “digital fingerprint”: basic characteristics of their
125   profile, the accounts they follow, and the contents of their tweets.
126           In the main text, we report zero-order relationships between measures of interest and z-
127   scored CRT score. Except when otherwise noted, all results continue to hold when including
128   age, gender, ethnicity, US residency, education level (categorical), income, and political
129   ideology as controls; and when accounting for multiple comparisons using either the
130   Bonferroni-Holm correction63 or maintaining a 5% false discovery rate using the Benjamini
131   Hochberg procedure64 (see SI for models with controls and corrected p-values).
132
133   Results
134   Profile characteristics
135           We begin by examining the relationship between CRT and basic profile features:
136   number of accounts followed, number of followers, total number of tweets, number of tweets
137   in the past two weeks, number of favorited tweets, lists, and number of days on Twitter (N =
138   1,900, all users that provided Twitter screen name and completed CRT). As each of these
139   quantities is a count variable, we use negative binomial regression to predict each quantity,
140   taking CRT as the independent variable. We find that higher CRT subjects follow significantly
141   fewer other accounts (IRR =.844, p0.10 for all; see SI
147   Section 1 for full regression tables).
148
149   Accounts followed

                                                      3
150           Following up on the observation that higher CRT participants followed significantly
151   fewer accounts, we next examine which accounts are followed by lower versus higher CRT
152   participants – that is, we examine how CRT relates to which types of content users consume
153   on Twitter (according to Grinberg et al. 2019, the accounts one follows form a good proxy for
154   the content one is exposed to).
155

156
157   Figure 1 Co-follower network. Nodes represent Twitter accounts followed by at least 30 users in our dataset and edges are
158   weighted based on the number of followers in common. The intensity of color of each node shows the average CRT score of
159   its followers (darker = higher CRT). Nodes are positions using directed-force layout on the weighted network. Visualization
160   depicts two distinct communities: one where the accounts are followed mostly by low CRT users and one with higher (mixed
161   level) of CRT users.

162           We begin by assessing structural differences between the accounts followed by the
163   users given their CRT. To do so, we construct the co-follower network: each node in the
164   network represents a Twitter account that is followed by at least 30 participants in our dataset
165   (1,129 nodes; results are robust to using thresholds other than 30, see SI Section 2), and the
166   edge between two given nodes is weighted by the number of participants in our dataset that
167   follow both nodes (Figure 1). Community detection analysis65 on the co-follower network
168   reveals two distinct clusters of accounts (Figure 1). Table 1 shows the top accounts in each
169   cluster. The clusters differ substantially in the cognitive style of their followers, with the
170   average CRT score of followers of the accounts in one cluster being 2.0 standard deviations
171   larger than the other cluster (Cohen’s d = 2.0; cluster 1: mean CRT = 0.428, SD = 0.030,
172   fraction of nodes = 0.60; cluster 2: mean CRT = 0.529, SD = 0.069, fraction of nodes = 0.40).
173   Furthermore, the average CRT of the followers of a given account is a highly significant
174   predictor of which community that account belongs to, with a one standard deviation increase
175   in followers’ average CRT score being associated with a more than 16-fold increase in the odds
176   of an account being in the higher CRT cluster (logistic regression, OR = 16.790, p
184   Table 1. Top accounts in each cluster within the co-follower networks. For each cluster, the table shows representative
185   accounts with a large number of followers along with the mean and standard deviation of CRT score of their followers

                               Cluster 1                                                    Cluster 2

                                            Followers’ mean                                              Followers’ mean
           Account          Followers                                  Account         Followers
                                              CRT score                                                    CRT score

         barackobama            316                0.542                 aldiuk            108                 0.474
          stephenfry            243                0.574               sainsburys          96                  0.457
         bbcbreaking            226                0.541               poundland           93                  0.456
        realdonaldtrump         193                0.525              argos_online         90                  0.462
          jk_rowling            151                0.541               Bmstores            89                  0.449
        Theellenshow            144                0.536              Lovewilko            88                  0.433
          Amazonuk              138                0.572               Morrisons           87                  0.473
         Rickygervais           138                 0.52              Nextofficial         87                  0.435
             Nasa               135                0.643               Superdrug           85                  0.395
            Twitter             133                0.509                 Asda              82                  0.400
186
187   Contents of tweets
188           Finally, we shift from the accounts users follow (and thus the content they consume) to
189   the content users create and/or distribute themselves: their tweets (1,537 subjects had accessible
190   public tweets on their timeline, generating a total of 2,402,082 tweets).
191           First, we investigate the quality of the information shared by users. To do so, we focus
192   on tweets or retweets containing links to one of 60 news websites whose trustworthiness was
193   rated by professional fact-checkers in previous work62; these news sites span a wide range of
194   information quality, from entirely fabricated “fake news” sites to hyper-partisan sites that
195   present misleading coverage of events that did actually occur to reputable mainstream news
196   sources. For our analysis, we extracted and unshortened all URLs in all tweets, and collected
197   any tweets containing links to one of the 60 sites (733 users tweeted at least one link from one
198   of these sites, with 11,342 tweets in total).
199           We then perform a linear regression predicting the trustworthiness of the tweeted news
200   source (in [0-1]) based on the CRT score of the user who shared the link, with robust standard
201   errors clustered on user. Doing so finds a positive correlation between CRT score and
202   trustworthiness of shared news sources (b = 0.106, p =0.011; Figure 2). For example, higher
203   CRT users were more likely to retweet links to the BBC (OR = 1.229, p
209   analysis becomes non-significant when controlling for political ideology and/or education.]
210   See SI Section 3 for statistical details.
211
212
                                                         1                                                                                                                NYTimes
                                                                                                               WashPo
                                                     0.9                                                                                                                           BBC
                                                                                                               CNN
                                                     0.8
                          Fact-checker trust score

                                                                          MSNBC
                                                                                                                                       WSJ
                                                                                                  ABCNews                                                       LATimes
                                                     0.7
                                                     0.6                                                        CBSNews
                                                                                                                                       USAToday
                                                     0.5
                                                                         FoxNews
                                                     0.4                                                        DailyMail                                  HuffPo                  Yahoo

                                                     0.3
                                                                                 NYDailyNews               DailyKos
                                                     0.2                                                                Breitbart
                                                     0.1                                                                          RawStory

                                                         0
                                                          0.35                        0.4           0.45                    0.5                       0.55                          0.6
                                                                                              Tweeters' average CRT score
213
214   Figure 2. Trustworthiness of shared news sources vs average CRT of score of users. Each dot represents an outlet shared
215   by users in our sample on Twitter. The size of the dots represents the number of observations. For clarity, we show outlets
216   which have been shared at least 50 times by the users.

217
                                                         CRT and Topics on Tweets
218        Next, we examine the topics people tweeted about using Structural Topic Modeling66, a
219   general framework for topic modeling with document-level covariate information. For each
220   user, we merged all tweets from the timeline as a document              and
                                                              peopl, trump, amp,         used
                                                                                  will, say,          thecan, user
                                                                                                   Topic 1:
                                                                                             just, like,               CRTone,score
                                                                                                              realdonaldtrump,     vote, now, as the

221   covariate for the topic1 modeling. Running the model for different number of topics (k = 5-10),
                                                                             make, get, think, right, want, need, know, year

222   we found that two particular topics are consistently correlated              with high       Topic 2:versus low CRT users:
                                                                   win, follow, amp, enter, competit, chanc, giveaway, end, retweet,
223   A topic involving
                 2        politics (e.g., “people”, “vote”, “trump”, “brexit”) time                   was positively correlated
                                                               freebiefriday, winner, just, give, simpli, prize, tampc, voucher, away, day,

224   with CRT and a topic involving “get rich quick” schemes (e.g., “win”, “enter”, “chance”
                                                                                                   Topic 3:
225   “giveaway”, “prize”)
                       3      was negatively correlated with  win, CRT.          Figure
                                                                   giveaway, just,                  3 shows
                                                                                   enter, earn, free,                  thesponsor,
                                                                                                         chanc, check, gift,
                                                                              amp, get, prize, want, cash, mile, love, point
                                                                                                                               difference
                                                                                                                                   card, via,     in
226   topic prevalence for each topic against the users’ CRT score for a 7-topic model (our results
227   are robust to the
                     4   choice of the number of topics; see via,
                                                               SIyoutub,
                                                                     Section            4). Topic 4:
                                                                           new, thank, video, amp, like, game, can, music, get, make, art,

228                                                                                                                                    use, will, check, today, write, post, follow

229                                                      CRT and Topics on Tweets
                                                              5                                                                                           Topic 5:
                                                                                                                        just, like, get, one, dont, love, know, can, time, now, think, day, peopl,
                                                                                                                                          want, make, good, got, realli, look, thing
                                                                                                                        Topic1. peopl, will, amp, trump, say, can, just,
                                                      6                                                                 vote, like, now, one,Topic          get,1:
                                                                                                                     peopl, trump, amp, will, say, just, Topic    6:need,
                                                                                                                                                           like, can,
                                                                                                                                                                             right, make, think,
                                                                                                                                                                      realdonaldtrump, one, vote, now,
                                                                                                                        time,
                                                                                                                      day, thank,brexit,
                                                                                                                                  love,
                                                                                                                                    make,   want,
                                                                                                                                        get,get,       know
                                                                                                                                             just,think,
                                                                                                                                                    today,  amp,
                                                                                                                                                         right,   now,need,
                                                                                                                                                                want,   look,know,
                                                                                                                                                                              will, one,
                                                                                                                                                                                     yearnew, can, time,
                                                                  1                                                                       good, pleas, see, week, great, work

                                                     7                                                                                                     Topic 2:
                                                                                                                         Topic2.
                                                                                                                          win, follow,follow,
                                                                                                                                       amp, enter,  amp,     enter,
                                                                                                                                                      competit,
                                                                                                                                                           Topic chanc,
                                                                                                                                                                  7:
                                                                                                                                                                        competit,
                                                                                                                                                                          giveaway, end, chanc,
                                                                                                                                                                                           retweet,
                      2
                                                                                                                         giveaway,
                                                                                                                      freebiefriday,
                                                                                                                     game,  new, amp,     end,
                                                                                                                                     winner,
                                                                                                                                        play,  get,retweet,
                                                                                                                                              just, give,  simpli,
                                                                                                                                                    will, win,     winner,
                                                                                                                                                                   prize,
                                                                                                                                                               team,            give,
                                                                                                                                                                      just,tampc,        simpli,
                                                                                                                                                                                  voucher,
                                                                                                                                                                             watch,         away,
                                                                                                                                                                                    fan, good, one,day,
                                                                                                                                                                                                    now,
                                                                                                                                                             time
                                                                                                                                           season, can, like, time, look, last
                                                                                                                         freebiefriday, prize, like, just, day, tampc, good,
                                                                                                                         away
                                                                                                                                                        Topic 3:
              -0.2               -0.13                        0.0               0.1         0.2     0.3
                                                                                                                     win, giveaway, just, enter, earn, free, chanc, check, gift, sponsor, card, via,
                                                                                                                                    amp, get, prize, want, cash, mile, love, point
                                          Topic prevalence by CRT score
                                                     CRT_ACC
230
231    Figure 3 Difference
                       4   in topic proportion against CRT score of users.via,Topic    1 related to politicalTopic 4:
                                                                                                                         engagement is positively
                                                                               youtub, new, thank, video, amp, like, game, can, music, get, make, art,
232   correlated with CRT score and topic 2 involving “get rich quick” schemes is negatively             correlated
                                                                                           use, will, check,               withfollow
                                                                                                             today, write, post,   CRT score.

233                                                           5                                                                                           Topic 5:
                                                                                                                        just, like, get, one, dont, love, know, can, time, now, think, day, peopl,

234           Finally, we examined the language used in the tweets at the level of individual words.                                      want, make, good, got, realli, look, thing

235   This final analysis
                       6
                          aims mostly to validate the standard interpretation   of6: the CRT as capturing
                                                                            Topic
                                                                                                                     day, thank, love, get, just, today, amp, now, look, will, one, new, can, time,
                                                                                                                                         good, pleas, see, week, great, work

                                                     7
                                                                                                                                                       Topic 7:
                                                                                                                     game, new, amp, play, get, will, win, team, just, watch, fan, good, one, now,
                                                                                                           6                            season, can, like, time, look, last

              -0.2               -0.1                         0.0               0.1         0.2     0.3

                                                                      CRT_ACC
236   the tendency to use an analytic cognitive style29. To do so, we employed the Linguistic Inquiry
237   Word Count (LIWC; a psychologically validated set of word dictionaries67) approach to test
238   how CRT scores related to the probability of a user’s tweets containing words related to various
239   LIWC categories. Specifically, if people who do well on the CRT are more likely to engage in
240   thinking (insight) to override (inhibit) their intuitive (often emotional) responses, then we
241   would expect positive correlations between CRT and the use of insight and inhibition words,
242   and negative correlations between CRT and the use of positive and negative emotion words.
243   Secondarily, we investigated the relationship between CRT and several other word categories.
244   We explored the relationship between CRT and the use of words related to morality, as previous
245   work has shown that CRT is associated with different moral values68,69, judgments70, and
246   behaviors71-73, but has not examined the relationship between CRT and engagement with
247   morality more generally. We also looked at the relationship between CRT and use of words
248   related to politics as prior work has found a link between CRT and political engagement74,
249   using the dictionary of words suggested by75. [We also planned to investigate the link between
250   CRT and religious words, based on prior work linking CRT to reduced belief in God76, but
251   found that use of religious words was not associated with belief in God; therefore we would
252   not expect a relationship with CRT.]
253
                                                                                         Insight                                                Inhibition                                                        Positive Emotion

                                                          0.16     p
266   CRT correlation, Table 2 shows the five words with the largest difference in frequency between
267   low and high CRT subjects (using median split).
268           We analyzed 1,787,197 tweets/retweets written in English language from 1,560 users
269   in our dataset whose timeline was accessible and had tweeted in English. As predicted based
270   on the conceptualization of CRT as measuring deliberativeness, we find that users with higher
271   CRT scores are more likely to use words associated with insight (OR = 1.135, p
308           One line of prior work which the current results bear on has to do with media truth
309   discernment. Past work has shown that people who are more analytic and reflective are better
310   at identifying true versus false news headlines, regardless of whether the headlines align with
311   their ideology (e.g., ref 60,61). However, these studies have relied entirely on survey
312   experiments, where participant responses may be driven by experimenter demand effects or
313   expressive responding. Additionally, in these experiments, participants judge a comparatively
314   small set of headlines (pre-selected by the experimenters to be balanced on partisanship and
315   veracity). Thus, these prior results may be idiosyncratic to the specific headlines (or approach
316   for selecting headlines) used in designing the survey. Furthermore, these studies have focused
317   on contrasting true headlines with blatantly false headlines (which may be comparatively rare
318   outside the laboratory25,26, rather than articles which are misleading but not entirely false (e.g.,
319   hyper-partisan biased reporting of events that actually occurred62). Thus, the results may not
320   generalize to the kinds of misinformation more typically encountered online. Finally, these
321   studies have focused on judgments of accuracy, rather than sharing decisions. Thus, whether
322   these previously documented associations extended to actual sharing in the naturally occurring
323   social media environment is an open question – particularly given that the social media context
324   may be more likely to active a political identity (as opposed to accuracy or truth) focus78,79.
325   Yet, despite these numerous reasons to think that prior findings may not generalize outside the
326   survey context, we do indeed find that participants who perform better on the CRT share news
327   from higher quality news sources. This observation substantially extends prior support for a
328   positive role of reasoning in news media truth discernment.
329           Our results are also relevant in similar ways for prior work regarding the role of
330   cognitive sophistication in political engagement. Prior evidence using survey experiments
331   suggests that people who are more cognitively sophisticated (e.g., higher CRT, more educated,
332   higher political knowledge) show higher rates of engagement with politics74,80. However, it has
333   also been suggested that this relationship may be the result of social desirability bias, such that
334   more cognitively sophisticated people simply over-report political engagement to please the
335   experimenter81,82. Our results, however, suggest that more reflective people are indeed actually
336   more engaged with politics on social media. This supports the inference that analytic thinking
337   is associated with increased political engagement.
338           More broadly, cognitive reflection has been associated with lower gullibility – that is,
339   less acceptance of a large range of epistemically suspect beliefs (such as conspiracy theories,
340   paranormal claims, etc. – see 29 for a review), including decreased susceptibility to pseudo-
341   profound bullshit58. Again, however, these findings are rooted in survey evidence and not real-
342   world behavior, and could reflect socially desirable responding. Here we find that low CRT is
343   associated with increased following of and tweeting about money-making scams and get-rich-
344   quick schemes. This supports the conclusion that more intuitive people are indeed more
345   gullible.
346           One of the most intriguing results that we uncovered was the clustering of accounts
347   followed by lower versus higher CRT participants. In particular, there was a cluster of accounts
348   that were predominantly followed by low CRT participants. This observation is particularly
349   interesting in the context of the extremely extensive discussion of partisan echo chambers, in
350   which supporters of the same party are much more likely to interact with co-partisans11,12,16.
351   Our network analysis indicates that the phenomenon of echo chambers is not limited to politics:
352   the cognitive echo chambers we observe have potentially profound implications for how
353   information flows through social media. Furthermore, it is likely that cognitive echo chambers
354   are not confined to social media – future work should investigate this phenomenon more
355   broadly.

                                                       9
356           There are, of course, important limitations of the present work. Most notably, we were
357   only able to consider the Twitter activity of a tiny subset of all users on the platform. Thus, it
358   is important for future work to examine how our results generalize to other sets of users – and
359   in particular, to users who did not opt in to a survey experiment. One potential approach that
360   may be fruitful in this endeavor is training a machine learning model to estimate users’ CRT
361   scores based on their social media activity. Relatedly, it will be important to test how the results
362   generalize to other social media platforms (e.g. Facebook, LinkedIn), and to users from non-
363   Western cultures. Future work should also examine how the results obtained here generalize to
364   other measures of cognitive sophistication beyond the CRT.
365          In sum, here we have shed light on social media behavior using the lens of cognitive
366   science. We have provided evidence that one’s extent of analytic thinking predicts a wide range
367   of social media behaviors. These results meaningfully extend prior survey studies,
368   demonstrating that analytic thinking plays an important role outside the laboratory.
369
370   Methods
371            Participants provided informed consent, and our studies were approved by the Yale
372   Human Subjects Committee1, IRB Protocol # 2000022539.
373            Participants. We recruited participants via Prolific83, a subject pool for online
374   experiments that consists of mostly UK- and US-based individuals. Participants completed the
375   survey on June 15-20, 2018. Twitter IDs were provided by participants at the beginning of the
376   study. However, some participants entered obviously fake Twitter IDs – for example, the
377   accounts of celebrities. To screen out such accounts, we excluded accounts with follower
378   counts above the 95th percentile in our dataset. We had complete data and usable Twitter IDs
379   for 1,953 users (55% female, Mage = 32, 61% UK residents, 37% US residents).
380            Survey materials and procedure. Participants first completed a news discernment
381   task. For this, they were given the following instructions: “You will be presented with a series
382   of news headlines from 2016-2017 (20 in total). We are interested in whether you think the
383   headlines are accurate or not. That is, do you think that they are accurately describing
384   something that actually happened. Note: The images may take a moment to load.” They were
385   then presented with a set of 20 politically neutral headlines in a random order and, for each
386   headline, they were asked the following question61,74: “To the best of your knowledge, how
387   accurate is the claim in the above headline?” (response options: Not at all accurate, Not very
388   accurate, Somewhat accurate, Very accurate). The headlines were either false (“fake”) or true
389   (“real”). The false headlines were verified as untrue by Snopes.com, whereas the true headlines
390   were all obtained from reputable mainstream news outlets. The mainstream headlines were
391   selected to be relatively surprising. The full set of headlines can be found here:
392   https://osf.io/guk3m/.
393            Participants then completed a demographics questionnaire that included education,
394   English fluency, social and economic political ideology (as separate questions), ethnicity,
395   belief in God, religious affiliation, class, and income. Next, participants were given the 7-item
396   CRT61, which consists of a reworded version of the original 3-item CRT34 and a 4-item non-
397   numeric CRT33. Finally, participants were asked if they responded randomly, searched online
398   for the headlines, and/or skipped through the headlines at the start of the study.
399            Twitter Data. We then used the Twitter API to retrieve users’ public information,
400   including general profile information (total number of tweets, accounts followed, followers,
401   etc.), the content of their last 3,200 tweets (capped by the Twitter API limit), and the list of

      1
          Data was collected when the researchers were with Yale University.

                                                             10
402   accounts followed by each user in our dataset. We linked the survey responses with Twitter
403   data for our subsequent analysis.
404           For word-level analysis, we removed punctuation and lower-case words then cross-
405   referenced all words in each tweet with the patterns in each word dictionary. We then flagged
406   the tweet against all categories that had at least one pattern matched.
407           To create the co-follower network, we first constructed a bipartite graph representing
408   all users in our study and all accounts they followed on Twitter. We then created the associated
409   weighted mono-partite graph of the accounts that have at least K followers from our subject
410   pool. Each account is represented by the aggregated demographic characteristics of its
411   followers (e.g., fraction female, fraction US resident, fraction white, average age).
412
413
414           Acknowledgments
415   The authors gratefully acknowledge funding from the Templeton World Charity Foundation
416   (grant number TWCF 0350), the Ethics and Governance of Artificial Intelligence Initiative of
417   the Miami Foundation, the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada. The
418   authors are very appreciative of helpful comments from Dean Eckles, Ziv Epstein, Adam Bear,
419   and Cameron Martel.

                                                    11
420   References
421   1     Coyle, C. L. & Vaughn, H. Social networking: Communication revolution or evolution?
422         Bell Labs Technical Journal 13, 13-17 (2008).
423   2     Wellman, B. Computer networks as social networks. Science 293, 2031-2034 (2001).
424   3     Boyd, D. M. & Ellison, N. B. Social network sites: Definition, history, and scholarship.
425         Journal of computer-mediated Communication 13, 210-230 (2007).
426   4     Vaidya, M. (Nature Publishing Group, 2014).
427   5     Chandler, R. GoFundMe sees boom in medically-related fundraising campaigns. WHO
428         TV March 14 (2015).
429   6     Sa, B. P., Chen, W. & Kodama, T. (Google Patents, 2017).
430   7     Del Vicario, M. et al. The spreading of misinformation online. Proceedings of the
431         National Academy of Sciences 113, 554-559 (2016).
432   8     Lazer, D. M. et al. The science of fake news. Science 359, 1094-1096 (2018).
433   9     Vosoughi, S., Roy, D. & Aral, S. The spread of true and false news online. Science 359,
434         1146-1151 (2018).
435   10    Pennycook, G. et al. (PsyArXiv, 2019).
436   11    Barberá, P., Jost, J. T., Nagler, J., Tucker, J. A. & Bonneau, R. Tweeting from left to
437         right: Is online political communication more than an echo chamber? Psychological
438         science 26, 1531-1542 (2015).
439   12    Garimella, V. R. K. & Weber, I. in Eleventh International AAAI Conference on Web
440         and Social Media.
441   13    Brady, W. J. & Crockett, M. How effective is online outrage. Trends in cognitive
442         sciences 23, 79 (2019).
443   14    Woolley, S. C. Automating power: Social bot interference in global politics. First
444         Monday 21 (2016).
445   15    Badawy, A., Ferrara, E. & Lerman, K. in 2018 IEEE/ACM International Conference
446         on Advances in Social Networks Analysis and Mining (ASONAM). 258-265 (IEEE).
447   16    Stewart, A. J. et al. Information gerrymandering and undemocratic decisions. Nature
448         573, 117-121, doi:10.1038/s41586-019-1507-6 (2019).
449   17    Aral, S. & Eckles, D. Protecting elections from social media manipulation. Science 365,
450         858-861 (2019).
451   18    Crockett, M. J. Moral outrage in the digital age. Nature Human Behaviour 1, 769-771,
452         doi:10.1038/s41562-017-0213-3 (2017).
453   19    Matz, S. C., Kosinski, M., Nave, G. & Stillwell, D. J. Psychological targeting as an
454         effective approach to digital mass persuasion. Proceedings of the national academy of
455         sciences 114, 12714-12719 (2017).
456   20    Correa, T., Hinsley, A. W. & De Zuniga, H. G. Who interacts on the Web?: The
457         intersection of users’ personality and social media use. Computers in human behavior
458         26, 247-253 (2010).
459   21    Back, M. D. et al. Facebook profiles reflect actual personality, not self-idealization.
460         Psychological science 21, 372-374 (2010).
461   22    Golbeck, J., Robles, C. & Turner, K. in CHI'11 extended abstracts on human factors in
462         computing systems. 253-262 (ACM).
463   23    Preotiuc-Pietro, D., Carpenter, J., Giorgi, S. & Ungar, L. in Proceedings of the 25th
464         ACM international on conference on information and knowledge management. 761-
465         770 (ACM).
466   24    Sumner, C., Byers, A., Boochever, R. & Park, G. J. in 2012 11th International
467         Conference on Machine Learning and Applications. 386-393 (IEEE).

                                                  12
468   25   Guess, A., Nagler, J. & Tucker, J. Less than you think: Prevalence and predictors of
469        fake news dissemination on Facebook. Science advances 5, eaau4586 (2019).
470   26   Grinberg, N., Joseph, K., Friedland, L., Swire-Thompson, B. & Lazer, D. Fake news
471        on twitter during the 2016 US Presidential election. Science 363, 374-378 (2019).
472   27   Muscanell, N. L. & Guadagno, R. E. Make new friends or keep the old: Gender and
473        personality differences in social networking use. Computers in Human Behavior 28,
474        107-112 (2012).
475   28   Evans, J. S. B. & Stanovich, K. E. Dual-process theories of higher cognition advancing
476        the debate. Perspectives on Psychological Science 8, 223-241 (2013).
477   29   Pennycook, G., Fugelsang, J. A. & Koehler, D. J. Everyday consequences of analytic
478        thinking. Current Directions in Psychological Science 24, 425-432 (2015).
479   30   Evans, A. M., Dillon, K. D. & Rand, D. G. Fast but not intuitive, slow but not reflective:
480        Decision conflict drives reaction times in social dilemmas. Journal of Experimental
481        Psychology: General 144, 951-966 (2015).
482   31   Kahneman, D. Thinking, Fast and Slow. (Farrar, Straus and Giroux, 2011).
483   32   Stanovich, K. E. & West, R. F. Advancing the rationality debate. Behavioral and brain
484        sciences 23, 701-717 (2000).
485   33   Thomson, K. S. & Oppenheimer, D. M. Investigating an alternate form of the cognitive
486        reflection test. Judgment and Decision making 11, 99 (2016).
487   34   Frederick, S. Cognitive Reflection and Decision Making. The Journal of Economic
488        Perspectives 19, 25-42 (2005).
489   35   Pennycook, G., Cheyne, J. A., Koehler, D. J. & Fugelsang, J. A. Is the cognitive
490        reflection test a measure of both reflection and intuition? Behavior Research Methods
491        48, 341-348 (2016).
492   36   Mata, A., Ferreira, M. B. & Sherman, S. J. The metacognitive advantage of deliberative
493        thinkers: A dual-process perspective on overconfidence. Journal of personality and
494        social psychology 105, 353 (2013).
495   37   Toplak, M. E., West, R. F. & Stanovich, K. E. The Cognitive Reflection Test as a
496        predictor of performance on heuristics-and-biases tasks. Memory & cognition 39, 1275
497        (2011).
498   38   Juanchich, M., Dewberry, C., Sirota, M. & Narendran, S. Cognitive reflection predicts
499        real-life decision outcomes, but not over and above personality and decision-making
500        styles. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 29, 52-59 (2016).
501   39   Yılmaz, O. & Sarıbay, S. A. An attempt to clarify the link between cognitive style and
502        political ideology: A non-western replication and extension. (2016).
503   40   Kruglanski, A. W. & Gigerenzer, G. Intuitive and deliberate judgments are based on
504        common principles. Psychological review 118, 97 (2011).
505   41   Keren, G. A tale of two systems: A scientific advance or a theoretical stone soup?
506        Commentary on Evans & Stanovich (2013). Perspectives on Psychological Science 8,
507        257-262 (2013).
508   42   Haidt, J. in Psychological Review Vol. 108          814-834 (American Psychological
509        Association, 2001).
510   43   Haidt, J. The Righteous Mind: Why Good People Are Divided by Politics and Religion.
511        (Pantheon Books, 2012).
512   44   Mercier, H. The argumentative theory: Predictions and empirical evidence. Trends in
513        Cognitive Sciences 20, 689-700 (2016).
514   45   Mercier, H. & Sperber, D. Why do humans reason? Arguments for an argumentative
515        theory. (2011).
516   46   Kahan, D. Making climate-science communication evidence-based. Culture, politics
517        and climate change: How information shapes our common future, 203-220 (2013).

                                                  13
518   47   Kahan, D. M. et al. The polarizing impact of science literacy and numeracy on
519        perceived climate change risks. Nature climate change 2, 732 (2012).
520   48   Pennycook, G. The new reflectionism in cognitive psychology: Why reason matters.
521        (Routledge, 2018).
522   49   Shenhav, A., Rand, D. G. & Greene, J. D. Divine intuition: Cognitive style influences
523        belief in God. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 141, 423 (2012).
524   50   Pennycook, G., Cheyne, J. A., Seli, P., Koehler, D. J. & Fugelsang, J. A. Analytic
525        cognitive style predicts religious and paranormal belief. Cognition 123, 335-346,
526        doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.03.003 (2012).
527   51   Gervais, W. M. & Norenzayan, A. Analytic Thinking Promotes Religious Disbelief.
528        Science 336, 493-496, doi:10.1126/science.1215647 (2012).
529   52   Svedholm, A. M. & Lindeman, M. The separate roles of the reflective mind and
530        involuntary inhibitory control in gatekeeping paranormal beliefs and the underlying
531        intuitive confusions. British Journal of Psychology 104, 303-319 (2013).
532   53   Bouvet, R. & Bonnefon, J.-F. Non-reflective thinkers are predisposed to attribute
533        supernatural causation to uncanny experiences. Personality and Social Psychology
534        Bulletin 41, 955-961 (2015).
535   54   Cheyne, J. A. & Pennycook, G. Sleep paralysis postepisode distress: Modeling potential
536        effects of episode characteristics, general psychological distress, beliefs, and cognitive
537        style. Clinical Psychological Science 1, 135-148 (2013).
538   55   Swami, V., Voracek, M., Stieger, S., Tran, U. S. & Furnham, A. Analytic thinking
539        reduces belief in conspiracy theories. Cognition 133, 572-585 (2014).
540   56   Gervais, W. M. Override the controversy: Analytic thinking predicts endorsement of
541        evolution. Cognition 142, 312-321 (2015).
542   57   Pennycook, G., Cheyne, J. A., Koehler, D. & Fugelsang, J. A. On the belief that beliefs
543        should change according to evidence: Implications for conspiratorial, moral,
544        paranormal, political, religious, and science beliefs. (2019).
545   58   Pennycook, G., Cheyne, J. A., Barr, N., Koehler, D. J. & Fugelsang, J. A. On the
546        reception and detection of pseudo-profound bullshit. Judgment and Decision making
547        (2015).
548   59   Barr, N., Pennycook, G., Stolz, J. A. & Fugelsang, J. A. The brain in your pocket:
549        Evidence that Smartphones are used to supplant thinking. Computers in Human
550        Behavior 48, 473-480 (2015).
551   60   Bago, B., Rand, D. G. & Pennycook, G. Fake news, fast and slow: Deliberation reduces
552        belief in false (but not true) news headlines. Journal of experimental psychology:
553        general (2020).
554   61   Pennycook, G. & Rand, D. G. Lazy, not biased: Susceptibility to partisan fake news is
555        better explained by lack of reasoning than by motivated reasoning. Cognition 188, 39-
556        50 (2019).
557   62   Pennycook, G. & Rand, D. G. Fighting misinformation on social media using
558        crowdsourced judgments of news source quality. Proceedings of the National Academy
559        of Sciences 116, 2521-2526 (2019).
560   63   Holm, S. A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scandinavian journal
561        of statistics, 65-70 (1979).
562   64   Benjamini, Y. & Hochberg, Y. Controlling the False Discovery Rate: A Practical and
563        Powerful Approach to Multiple Testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series
564        B (Methodological) 57, 289-300 (1995).
565   65   Blondel, V. D., Guillaume, J.-L., Lambiotte, R. & Lefebvre, E. Fast unfolding of
566        communities in large networks. Journal of statistical mechanics: theory and experiment
567        2008, P10008 (2008).

                                                  14
568   66   Roberts, M. E. et al. Structural topic models for open-ended survey responses.
569        American Journal of Political Science 58, 1064-1082 (2014).
570   67   Pennebaker, J. W., Boyd, R. L., Jordan, K. & Blackburn, K. The development and
571        psychometric properties of LIWC2015. (2015).
572   68   Royzman, E. B., Landy, J. F. & Goodwin, G. P. Are good reasoners more incest-
573        friendly? Trait cognitive reflection predicts selective moralization in a sample of
574        American adults. Judgment and Decision Making 9, 176-190 (2014).
575   69   Pennycook, G., Cheyne, J. A., Barr, N., Koehler, D. J. & Fugelsang, J. A. The role of
576        analytic thinking in moral judgements and values. Thinking & Reasoning 20, 188-214
577        (2014).
578   70   Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M. & Cohen, J. D. An
579        fMRI Investigation of Emotional Engagement in Moral Judgment. Science 293, 2105-
580        2108 (2001).
581   71   Rand, D. G. Cooperation, fast and slow: Meta-analytic evidence for a theory of social
582        heuristics and self-interested deliberation. Psychological Science, Pre-print available at
583        SSRN: http://ssrn.com/abstract=2783800 (2016).
584   72   Köbis, N. C., Verschuere, B., Bereby-Meyer, Y., Rand, D. & Shalvi, S. Intuitive
585        Honesty Versus Dishonesty: Meta-Analytic Evidence. Perspectives on Psychological
586        Science, 1745691619851778 (2019).
587   73   Rand, D. G., Brescoll, V. L., Everett, J. A. C., Capraro, V. & Barcelo, H. Social
588        heuristics and social roles: Intuition favors altruism for women but not for men. Journal
589        of Experimental Psychology: General 145, 389-396 (2016).
590   74   Pennycook, G. & Rand, D. G. Cognitive reflection and the 2016 US Presidential
591        election. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 45, 224-239 (2019).
592   75   Preoţiuc-Pietro, D., Liu, Y., Hopkins, D. & Ungar, L. in Proceedings of the 55th Annual
593        Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics (Volume 1: Long Papers).
594        729-740.
595   76   Shenhav, A., Rand, D. G. & Greene, J. D. Divine intuition: Cognitive style influences
596        belief in God. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, doi:10.1037/a0025391
597        (2011).
598   77   Rand, D. G. et al. Social heuristics shape intuitive cooperation. Nat Commun 5,
599        doi:10.1038/ncomms4677 (2014).
600   78   Brady, W. J., Crockett, M. & Van Bavel, J. J. The MAD Model of Moral Contagion:
601        The role of motivation, attention and design in the spread of moralized content online.
602        (2019).
603   79   Van Bavel, J. J. & Pereira, A. The partisan brain: An Identity-based model of political
604        belief. Trends in cognitive sciences 22, 213-224 (2018).
605   80   Galston, W. A. Political knowledge, political engagement, and civic education. Annual
606        review of political science 4, 217-234 (2001).
607   81   Holbrook, A. L., Green, M. C. & Krosnick, J. A. Telephone versus face-to-face
608        interviewing of national probability samples with long questionnaires: Comparisons of
609        respondent satisficing and social desirability response bias. Public opinion quarterly
610        67, 79-125 (2003).
611   82   Enamorado, T. & Imai, K. Validating Self-reported Turnout by Linking Public Opinion
612        Surveys with Administrative Records. (2018).
613   83   Palan, S. & Schitter, C. Prolific. ac—A subject pool for online experiments. Journal of
614        Behavioral and Experimental Finance 17, 22-27 (2018).
615

                                                  15
Digital Fingerprints of Cognitive Reflection
                                                (Supplementary Material)

   Authors: Mohsen Mosleh1, Gordon Pennycook2, Antonio A. Arechar1, and David G. Rand1,3
           1
           Sloan School of Management, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, 2Hill/Levene Schools of Business,
           University of Regina, 3Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology

           1. Profile characteristics

        Table S1 Predicting profile characteristics taking users’ z-scored CRT score as independent variable using negative
   binominal regression. Model 1) no controls, Model 2) controlling for age, gender, and ethnicity, Model 3) controlling for age,
   gender, ethnicity, US residency, education, social/economic conservatism. pBH is corrected p-value using Bonferroni-Holms
   and pHolm using Benjamini Hochberg procedure.

                            Model 1                                  Model 2                                  Model 3

                 IRR                                  Coeff.                                     IRR
 Feature                   p       pBH      pHolm                    p       pBH      pHolm                  p        pBH      pHolm
                 (SE)                                  (SE)                                      (SE)

                 0.844                                 0.878                                    0.886
Followed                  0.000   0.000     0.000                0.002     0.013     0.013                 0.004     0.027     0.027
                (0.035)                               (0.036)                                  (0.037)
                 0.956                                 0.984                                    0.952
Followers                 0.375   0.454     1.000                0.745     0.852     1.000                 0.296     0.519     1.000
                (0.049)                               (0.048)                                  (0.045)
                 0.799                                 0.915                                    0.945
 Tweets                   0.029   0.073     0.172                0.297     0.416     0.892                 0.508     0.630     1.000
                (0.082)                               (0.078)                                  (0.081)
                 1.067                                 1.100                                    1.032
Favorites                 0.084   0.411     0.454                0.217     0.380     0.869                 0.686     0.686     1.000
                (0.084)                               (0.085)                                  (0.079)
                 0.902                                 1.017                                    0.953
 Listed                   0.352   0.454     1.000                0.852     0.852     1.000                 0.540     0.630     1.000
                (0.100)                               (0.092)                                  (0.076)
Tweets in
                 0.811                                 0.894                                    0.896
  last 2                  0.031   0.073     0.172                0.167     0.380     0.835                 0.235     0.519      1.00
                (0.079)                               (0.072)                                  (0.083)
  weeks
 Days on         1.008                                 1.018                                    1.017
                          0.454   0.454     1.000                0.103     0.359     0.615                 0.139     0.485     0.831
 Twitter        (0.011)                               (0.011)                                  (0.011)

                                                                 1
2. Accounts followed
    .
          Table S2. Characteristics of clusters within co-followers’ network for various threshold of number of followers (K).
    Regression results are generated by predicting which cluster the accounts belong to taking z-scored average CRT of followers
    as independent variable using logistic regression. Model 1) no controls, Model 2) controlling for age (average age of
    followers), gender (male fraction of followers), and ethnicity (white fraction of followers), Model 3) controlling for age,
    gender, ethnicity, US residency (US resident fraction of followers), education (college degree fraction of followers),
    social/economic conservatism (average conservatism of followers), and income (average income of followers). Across all
    values of the threshold for the number of followers, there exist one cluster with high average CRT score of followers and one
    with low average CRT score of followers. The average CRT of followers can significantly predict which cluster of the two
    clusters the accounts belongs to

Threshold     Cluster             CRT             Fraction of        Total       Model 1          Model 2              Model 3
   (K)                                             nodes in          nodes          OR               OR                    OR
                             Mean         SD
                                                  the cluster                      (SE)             (SE)                  (SE)
                                                                                0.056 ***         0.257***              0.451**
              Cluster 0      0.428      0.030         0.60           1129
                                                                                 (0.175)           (0.213)              (0.258)
   30
                                                                                17.902***         3.887***              2.117**
              Cluster 1      0.529      0.069         0.40           1129
                                                                                 (0.175)           (0.213)              (0.258)
   25                                                                           0.087***          0.315***              0.568**
              Cluster 0      0.429      0.032         0.64           1634
                                                                                 (0.124)           (0.157)              (0.186)
   25                                                                           11.457***         3.171***              1.760**
              Cluster 1      0.526      0.073         0.36           1634
                                                                                 (0.124)           (0.157)              (0.186)
                                                                                0.341***           0.797*                1.212
              Cluster 0      0.428      0.035          0.4           2477
                                                                                 (0.064)           (0.097)              (0.112)
                                                                                9.483***          3.033***             1.876***
   20         Cluster 1      0.526      0.077          0.3           2477
                                                                                 (0.094)           (0.118)              (0.145)
                                                                                0.422***          0.550***             0.492***
              Cluster 2      0.428      0.035          0.3           2477
                                                                                 (0.066)           (0.092)              (0.117)
                                                                                0.444***          0.556***             0.479***
              Cluster 0      0.424      0.040          0.3           3876
                                                                                 (0.053)           (0.072)              (0.086)
                                                                                0.647***            0.993                0.978
              Cluster 1      0.436      0.043          0.2           3876
                                                                                 (0.048)           (0.069)              (0.073)
   15
                                                                                6.369***          2.385***             1.624***
              Cluster 2      0.524      0.086          0.3           3876
                                                                                 (0.064)           (0.075)              (0.091)
                                                                                0.443***          0.788**                1.062
              Cluster 3      0.424      0.042          0.2           3876
                                                                                 (0.053)           (0.070)              (0.081)
                                                                                0.485***          0.600***             0.583***
              Cluster 0      0.422      0.051          0.2           6668
                                                                                 (0.039)           (0.052)              (0.058)
                                                                                0.642***            1.050               1.037*
              Cluster 1      0.434      0.051          0.2           6668
                                                                                 (0.035)           (0.048)              (0.051)
   10
                                                                                4.393***          1.807***              1.194**
              Cluster 2      0.522      0.098          0.4           6668
                                                                                 (0.041)           (0.050)              (0.061)
                                                                                0.534***          0.867**                1.100
              Cluster 3      0.426      0.055          0.2           6668
                                                                                 (0.037)           (0.047)              (0.054)
        *p
3. Quality of the information shared by users
Domain level

      Table S3. Predicting quality score of the tweeted outlet taking users’ CRT score as independent variable using linear
regression cluster standard error by users. Model 1) no controls, Model 2) controlling for age, gender, and ethnicity, Model
3) controlling for age, gender, ethnicity, US residency, education, social/economic conservatism

                                     Model 1              Model 2              Model 3

                                   b                     b                    b
                                               p                    p                    p
                                  (SE)                 (SE)                 (SE)

                                 0.101                0.114                 0.094
                                            0.011                  0.001              0.003
                                (0.040)              (0.034)               (0.032)

Subject level

Table S4 Predicting average quality score tweets by the user’ CRT score as independent variable using linear regression.
Model 1) no controls, Model 2) controlling for age, gender, and ethnicity, Model 3) controlling for age, gender, ethnicity, US
residency, education, social/economic conservatism.

                                     Model 1              Model 2              Model 3

                                   OR                   OR                   OR
                                               p                    p                    p
                                  (SE)                 (SE)                 (SE)

                                 0.075                0.066                 0.035
                                            0.044                  0.09               0.518
                                (0.037)              (0.039)               (0.039)

                                                               3
4. Topic Modeling

We varied the number of topics in the structural topic modeling using the user CRT score as
the covariate. Across all number of topics, there is always one topic (related to political
engagement) that is positively correlated with CRT and one topic (related to “get rich quick”
schemes) that is negatively related to CRT.

     Table S5 Topic related to political engagement vs number of topics in the model.

  Number      Representative words                                                      Coefficient of estimating
  of topics                                                                             topic proportion using CRT

      k

      5       peopl, will, amp, can, just, like, say, trump, one, get, now, need,                0.071**
              vote, time, make, think, year, know, right, want
      6       peopl, will, amp, trump, just, say, can, like, vote, get, one, now,                0.080**
              need, make, think, right, time, know, want, year
      7       peopl, will, amp, trump, say, can, just, vote, like, now, one, get,                0.076**
              need, right, make, think, time, brexit, want, know
      8       peopl, like, just, trump, will, one, say, can, get, know, make,                     0.54**
              dont, think, realdonaldtrump, want, time, need, thing, right, amp
      9       peopl, will, amp, trump, say, vote, just, can, now, like, one, get,                0.064**
              need, brexit, think, realdonaldtrump, right, make, time, year
     10       peopl, will, amp, trump, say, vote, just, can, now, like, one, get,                0.064**
              need, brexit, think, right, realdonaldtrump, make, time, year
*p
5. Language use (word-level analysis)

      Tweet level

           Table S7 Predicting tweets word category using logistic regression taking z-scored users’ CRT score as independent
      variable clustering standard error by username. Model 1) no controls, Model 2) controlling for age, gender, and ethnicity,
      Model 3) controlling for age, gender, ethnicity, US residency, education, social/economic conservatism.

                              Model 1                                    Model 2                               Model 3

   Word            OR                                      OR                                      OR
                              p        pBH      pHolm                   p       pBH     pHolm                 p        pBH         pHolm
  category        (SE)                                    (SE)                                    (SE)

                 1.135                                    1.134                                   1.106
Insight                     0.000    0.000     0.000                   0.000   0.000   0.000                0.000     0.000        0.000
                (0.025)                                  (0.025)                                 (0.024)
                 1.105                                    1.111                                   1.101
Inhabitation                0.000    0.001     0.001                   0.000   0.001   0.001                0.001     0.002        0.004
                (0.027)                                  (0.029)                                 (0.028)
Positive         0.927                                    0.954                                   0.984
                            0.022    0.026     0.044                   0.145   0.169   0.288                0.539     0.593        1.000
emotion         (0.033)                                  (0.032)                                 (0.031)
Negative         1.139                                    1.135                                   1.115
                            0.000    0.000     0.000                   0.000   0.000   0.000                0.000     0.001        0.001
emotion         (0.031)                                  (0.032)                                 (0.030)
                 1.073                                    1.080                                   1.079
Morality                    0.000    0.000     0.001                   0.000   0.000   0.000                0.000     0.000        0.000
                (0.019)                                  (0.019)                                 (0.019)
                 1.172                                    1.156                                   1.143
Political                   0.005    0.008     0.016                   0.022   0.031   0.061                0.028     0.039        0.084
                (0.057)                                  (0.063)                                 (0.061)

      Subject level

            Table S8. Predicting fraction of tweets by a user in each word category using linear regression taking z-scored
      users’ CRT score as independent variable. Model 1) no controls, Model 2) controlling for age, gender, and ethnicity, Model
      3) controlling for age, gender, ethnicity, US residency, education, social/economic conservatism.

                              Model 1                                    Model 2                               Model 3

   Word            b                                        b                                        b
                               p       pBH      pHolm                    p      pBH     pHolm                  p        pBH        pHolm
  category
                  (SE)                                     (SE)                                    (SE)

                 0.107                                    0.101                                   0.099
Insight                     0.000     0.000     0.000                  0.000   0.000    0.000                0.000    0.000        0.001
                (0.025)                                  (0.025)                                 (0.026)

Inhabitation     0.104      0.000     0.000     0.000      0.108       0.000   0.000    0.000      0.102     0.000    0.000        0.000
                (0.025)                                  (0.026)                                 (0.026)
Positive         -0.054                                   -0.033                                  -0.014
                            0.031     0.031     0.040                  0.186   0.186    0.186                0.581    0.581        0.581
emotion         (0.025)                                  (0.025)                                 (0.025)
Negative         0.136                                     0.135                                   0.123
                            0.000     0.000     0.000                  0.000   0.000    0.000                0.000    0.000        0.000
emotion         (0.025)                                  (0.025)                                 (0.026)
                 0.070                                     0.078                                   0.076
Morality                    0.006     0.008     0.017                  0.002   0.003    0.007                0.003    0.005        0.010
                (0.025)                                  (0.025)                                 (0.026)
                 0.059                                     0.068                                   0.049
Political                   0.020     0.024     0.040                  0.007   0.008    0.014                0.054    0.064        0.107
                (0.025)                                  (0.025)                                 (0.025)

                                                                   5
You can also read