AMITRIPTYLINE- AND MIANSERIN-INDUCED CHANGES IN ACQUISITION OF PAIRED-ASSOCIATION LEARNING-TASK

Page created by Edith Leonard
 
CONTINUE READING
Br. J. clin. Pharmac. (1978),5, 149-153

AMITRIPTYLINE- AND MIANSERIN-INDUCED
CHANGES IN ACQUISITION OF
PAIRED-ASSOCIATION LEARNING-TASK
R. LIUEQUIST, T. SEPPALA & M.J. MAlTILA
Departments of Pharmacology and Psychology, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland

  I The double-blind study on twenty healthy students was an attempt at assessing the effects of 2-
  week's treatment with amitriptyline (25 mg three times a day) and mianserin (10 mg three times a day),
  each alone or separatively inbibed with alcohol (0.5 g/kg) on the immediate memory and on the ac-
  quisition of a paired-association learning-task.
  2 Amitriptyline impaired both the short-term memory-span and acquisition, and alcohol potentiated
  these effects. The action of mianserin did not deviate significantly from that of the placebo, and it also
  failed to interact with alcohol.
  3 It is concluded that the decrement in learning capacity, that occurs after the 2-week's treatment
  with therapeutic doses of amitriptyline, reflects changes in both the intrinsic and the regulatory
  mechanisms of learning.

Introduction
Previous results from this laboratory (Liljequist,          1976; Ghose, Coppen & Turner, 1976; Coppen,
Linnoila & Mattila, 1974) indicate that low                 Cupta, Montgomery, Ghose, Bailey, Burns & De
therapeutic doses of nortriptyline, given three times a     Ridder, 1976).
day to healthy human subjects, impaired learning,
probably by interfering with the acquisition processes.
In these conditions another tricyclic antidepressant       Methods
chlorimipramine, proved ineffective and even an-
tagonized the alcohol-induced impairment. Of these          Subjects and treatments
antidepressants, chlorimipramine affected more
peripheral sympathetic nerve terminals, as                 Twenty healthy subjects, fourteen males and six
demonstrated by the tyramine pressor test (Seppila,        females, aged 21.1 + 1.4 years (mean + s.d.) and
Linnoila, Elonen & Mattila, 1975). This effect is          weighing 67.9 + 9.9 kg, served as paid volunteers.
probably due to the chlordesimipramine formed from         None of the subjects were regularly on drugs nor had
the parent drug during prolonged treatment (Nagy,          a history of psychiatric illness. The participants were
1974).                                                     informed of the objects and the procedure of the ex-
   In addition to their affecting the availability of      periment and consented to volunteer the experiment.
cyclic monoamines on the synapses, antidepressants            Mianserin hydrochloride (10 mg) (Tolvong,
also have an antimuscarinic effect. This might interfere   Bolvidorg, Organon), amitriptyline hydrochloride
with cognitive processes, as demonstrated with             (25 mg) (Tryptizols, Merck, Sharp & Dohme), or
atropine and scopolamine (Whitehouse, 1964;                lactose placebo were administered in identical tablets
Buresova, Bures, Bochanecky & Weiss, 1964;                 three times a day, double-blind cross-over for 2 weeks
Bochdanecky & Jarvik, 1967). For this reason we            each. A one week's drug-free period was allowed
have compared the effects of amitriptyline and             between the treatments. The sequence of treatments
mianserin on those variables which represent acquisi-      was ordered according to the Latin square design.
tion stages in learning processes. Mianserin is a              When the subjects came to the testing sessions, they
tetracyclic compound with clinical antidepressant           ingested an alcoholic or nonalcoholic bitter drink, the
action (Murphy, 1975; Kafoe & Leonard, 1973).               dose of ethyl alcohol being 0.5 g/kg body weight. The
Unlike amitriptyline, it does not block the amine           drinks were ingested chilled, the drinking time was 10
uptake in vivo experiments, and it also lacks an-           min. The test began 30 min after the ingestion of
ticholinergic effects (Raiteri, Angelini & Bertollini,      drugs.
150     R. LILJEQUIST, T. SEPPALA & M.J. MATTILA

              8                                                            10
        0)
        01)   7                                                      Cn    8
                                                                     c
        a)                                                           0
              6                                                      4)
                                                                     C_    61-
        .O
              5                                                      Ca) 4
                                                                     0)
        0                                                            01)
              4                                                            2
        C.
        0)    3                                                             0
        :5                                                                       P   AT M A        <    <
        0)                                                                                         +    +
        o     2 -
        ~0
        E     1                                             Figure 2 Effects of amitriptyline (AT), mianserin
        z     QL
                                                            (M), alcohol (A) and placebo (P) on paired associate
                     P AT M A < <                           learning. Means + s.d. of the number of presentation
                                         +    +             times of the paired association learning task refers to
                                                            memory acquisition. Data are based on the analysis
                                                            of nineteen subjects. Statistical differences were
 Figure 1 Effects of amitriptyline (AT), mianserin          calculated by using Student's paired t-test
 (M), alcohol (A) and placebo (P) on immediate              (**P
EFFECT OF AMITRIPTYLINE AND MIANSERIN ON LEARNING                      151

              28
                                                                   Impairment
              26                                            N
                                                                 60-
              24
                                                            0
              22
                                                            U)
                                                            -    40F
        co    20
        0n                                                  C)
                                                            ._

              18                                            0)   20F
        0
        al)
       4-
              16
                                                            C
       E      14                                            Cz
                                                            0.    QL
       .0                                                   -D
       E                                                                AT AT M M A
152     R. LIUEQUIST, T. SEPPALA & M.J. MATTILA

Subjective evaluation                                       properties than did amitripyline, contraindicates the
                                                            role of general sedative effects. Vigilance, coordination,
Drowsiness was subjectively reported by fourteen sub-       and reaction capacity appeared unchanged during the
jects when being on mianserin and by ten subjects           learning tests (Seppila, 1977).
when being on amitriptyline and by one subject after            If it is accepted, that amitriptyline and alcohol, at
placebo. Eight subjects reported dryness of the mouth        the time of rising blood alcohol concentration, in-
after amitriptyline and none after mianserin or              terfere with the process of consolidation, it would be
placebo.                                                     relevant to find the loci of action.
                                                                The mechanism of short-term memory has been
                                                             proposed as reflecting a synaptic transmission efficacy
                                                            (Eccles, 1964), whereas the formation of memory-
Discussion                                                   trace more specifically depends on the cholinergic
                                                             mechanisms of the limbic system, and the connections
The present study demonstrates that amitriptyline in         to the cortex and reticular activating system
therapeutic doses impairs memory functions, whereas         (Il'yuchenok, 1965). Thus, part of the effects of
the effects of mianserin do not differ from those of the     amitriptyline are mediated through inhibition in the
placebo. The exact nature of the impairment of the          cholinergic brain structures, and through a block in
cognitive functions remains unanswered until after a        the corresponding components of the reticular forma-
more systematic study of memory processes.                  tion. Consequentally, the cortex is de-afferented, and
   During mianserin and placebo treatment, the short-       the spatial and temporal activity between the nerve
term memory storage was within the normal                   cells is diminished (Il'yuchenok, 1975). This might
range, i.e. approximately seven items. After                manifest as a deterioration of short-term memory
amitriptyline, performance corresponded lower limits        span.
to be accepted as normal. Alcohol alone significantly           Small, single doses of ethanol are (possibly) accom-
reduced the short-term memory span. The results of          panied by changes in central catecholamine
the present study, concerning the effects of relatively     metabolism (Corrodi, Fuxe & Hokfelt, 1966; Carlsson
small, acute doses of alcohol, tally well with previous     & Lindqvist, 1973). There is evidence that drugs
studies (Liljequist et al., 1974). When alcohol and         which block catecholamine synthesis (a-methyltrosine)
amitriptyline were administered simultaneously, the         or storage (reserpine) may impair learning by secon-
short-term memory test indicated momentary impair-          dary rather than by primary actions (Izquierdo, 1975).
ment in general intelligence (Wechsler, 1944).              Therefore, the mechanisms of the effects of alcohol on
Since the experimental population consisted of              memory are difficult to explain.
students, we expected their verbal learning capacity to        The different actions of amitriptyline and mianserin
be high or normal. Therefore, amitriptyline and alcohol     on memory are possible to explain, except by
can impair sequential verbal learning, and if they are      cholinergical properties, also by their action on brain
acting at the same time, also other cognitive functions     amine metabolism. Mianserin possibly increases the
are affected.                                               turnover of noradrenaline and, to a lesser degree, of
   The performance factors often interfere with the         dopamine, thus increasing the availability of
learning capacity. The experimental procedure used in       noradrenaline to the receptors (Kafoe & Leonard,
this study excludes the possibility, that the impaired      1973). In contrast, amitriptyline reduces the turnover
learning capacity during amitriptyline and alcohol          of both noradrenaline and serotonin by inhibiting the
treatment resulted from inadequate attention during         re-uptake of these amines (Kafoe & Leonard, 1973).
acquisition, since the subjects had to repeat the           Perhaps due to this property of mianserin, it even fails
response syllable to reach the learning criterion.          to induce depression in the stable reticular activating
Nevertheless, the learning scores which measure the         system, as demonstrated by Vargaftig, Coignet, de
acquisition stage (Atkinson & Shiffrey, 1968), were         Vos, Grijsen & Bonta ( 1971) in rabbits.
significantly impaired. Since the indication of learning       Nortriptyline, which is structurally related to
required success in recalling, there is the possibility,    amitriptyline, caused significant but weaker impair-
that the only stage affected was the retrieval from         ment than amitriptyline under similar experimental
immediate memory. However, since the other tests            conditions. Chlorimipramine and mianserin behaved
(Seppala, 1977) were adequately performed, this             in a similar manner, except that a slight alcohol-
possibility is not probable.                                antagonizing effect was found with chlorimipramine
   If it is accepted that the formation of the long-term    which in a 2-week study showed a strong action to
memory traces was impaired, it is still unclear, as to      adrenergic nerve function (Liljequist et al., 1974).
whether this reflects specific memory, or properties of a   Therefore, the interpretation done in that study, i.e. the
more general sedative effect of the drugs used. The         change in noradrenergic tone is an essential property
observation that mianserin in spontaneously reported        for memory functions, is supported by the results of
questionnaires appeared to have more sedative               the present study.
EFFECT OF AMITRIPTYLINE AND MIANSERIN ON LEARNING                         153

This study was supported by a grant from Sigrid Juselius       like to thank Organon B.V., for providing the drugs and
Foundation to Professor M.J. Mattila. The authors would        drinks and for financial support.

References
ATKINSON, R.C. & SHIFFREY, R.M. (1968). Human                     on the turnover of dopamine, noradrenalin and serotonin
   memory: A proposed system and its control processes.           in the rat brain. Arch. Int. Pharmacodyn, 206, 389.
   In The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, eds.          KANNENGIESSER, M.H., HUNT, P. ( RAYNAUD, J.P.
   Spence, K.W. & Spence J.T., 2, 89-195, New York:               (1973). An in vitro model for the study of psychotropic
   Academic Press.                                                drugs and as a criterion of antidepressant activity.
BISHOP. M.P., GUTHRIE, M.B. & HORNSBY, L.D.                       Biochem, Pharmac., 22, 73-84.
   (1969). Objective and subjective test in measuring          LILJEQUIST, R., LINNOILA, M. & MATTILA, M.J.
   responses to C.N.S. drugs in normal subjects. J. clin.         (1974). Effect of two week's treatment with
   Pharmac., 9, 305.                                              chlorimipramine and nortriptyline, alone or in combina-
BOCHDANECKY, Z. & JARVIK, M.E. (1962). Impair-                    tion with alcohol, on learning and memory in man.
   ment of one-trial passive avoidance learning in mice by        Psychopharmacologia (Berl), 39, 181-186.
   scopolamine, scopolamine methylbromide, and                 LILJEQUIST, R., LINNOILA, M., SAARIO, I.,
   physostigmine. Int. J. Neuropharmac., 6, 217-222.              SEPPALA, T. & MATTILA, MJ. (1975). Effect of two
BURESOVA, O., BURES, J., BOCHDANECKY, Z. &                        week's treatment with chlorpromazine, sulpiride, and
   ZEISS, T. (1964). Effect of atropine on learning, extinc-      bromazepam, alone or in combination with alcohol, on
   tion, retention, and retrieval in rats. Psychophar-            learning and memory in man. Psychopharmacologia, 44,
   macologia, 5, 255-263.                                         205-208.
CARLSSON, A. & LINDQVIST, M. (1973). Effect of                 MURPHY, J.E. (1975). A comparative clinical trial of Org.
   ethanol on the hydroxylation of tyrosine and tryptophan        GB 94 and imipramine in the treatment of depression in
   in rat brain in vivo. J. Pharm. Pharmac., 25, 437-440.         clinical practice. J. int. med. Res., 3, 251.
COPPEN, A., CUPTA, R., MONTGOMERY, S., GHOSE,                  NAGY, A. (1974). Imipraminets farmakokinetik. Scand.
   K., BAILEY, J., BURNS, B., & De RIDDER, JJ.                    Psycopharmac. Soc. Meeting, Copenhagen.
   (1976). Mianserin hydrochloride: A novel antidepres-        RAITERI, M., ANGELINI, F. & BERTOLLINI, A.
   sant. Br. J. Psychiat., 129, 342-345.                          (1976). Comparative study of the effects of mianserin, a
CORRODI, H., FUXE, K., & HOKFELT, T. (1966). The                  tetracyclic antidepressant, and of imipramine on uptake
   effect of ethanol on the activity of the catecholamine         and release of neurotransmitters in synaptosomes. J.
   neurons in the rat brain. J. Pharm. Pharmac., 18,              Pharm. Pharmac., 28,483-488.
   821-823.                                                    SEPPALA, T., LILJEQUIST, R., LINNOILA, M.,
ECCLES, J.C. (1964). The physiology of synapses. Berlin:         FREDRIKSSON-OVERO, K. & DORRITY, F. (1977).
  Springer-Verlag.                                               Correlations between serum ami- and nortriptyline con-
GHOSE, K., COPPEN, A. & TURNER, P. (1976).                       centrations and psychomotor performance. Acta Phar-
  Autonomic actions and interactions of Mianserin                mac. Tox. (in press).
  hydrochloride (Org. GB 94) and Amitriptyline in patients     SEPPALA, T., LINNOILA, M., ELONEN, E. &
  with depressive illness. Psychopharmacology, 49,                MATTILA, M.J. (1975). The effect of antidepressants
   201-204.                                                       and alcohol on psychomotor skills related to driving.
HAYS, W.I. (1963). Statistics for the psychologist. New           Clin. Pharmac. Ther., 17, 515-522.
  York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.                              SEPPALA, T. (1977). Psychomotor skills during acute and
HILGARD, E.R. (1951). Methods and procedures in the               two-week treatment with mianserin (Org. GB 94) and
   study of learning. In Handbook of experimental psy-            amitriptyline, and their combined effects with alcohol.
   chology, ed. Steven, S. S., pp. 517-567. New York: John        Ann. clin. Res., 9,66-72.
  Wiley & Sons.                                                VARGAFTIG, B.B., COIGNET, C.J., de VOS, C.J.,
IL'YUCHENOK, R.Y. (1975). Pharmacological and                     DRIJSEN, H. & BONTA, I.L. (1971). Mianserin
  neurochemical approaches to memory trace retrieval. In          hydrochloride: peripheral and central effects in relation
   Current developments in psychopharmacology, eds.               to antagonism against 5-hydroxytryptamine and tryp-
  Essman, W.B. & Valzelli, L., 1, 107-142.                        tamine. Eur. J. Pharmac., 16, 336-342.
IZQUIERDO, I. (1975). Pharmacological observations on          WHITEHOUSE, J.M. (1964). The effects of atropine on dis-
  the role of hippocampal and autonomic pharmacology in           crimination learning in the rat. J. Comp. Physiol.
  performance and learning. In Current developments in            Psychol., 57, 13-15.
  psychopharmacology, eds. Essman, W.B. & Valzelli, L.,        WECHSLER, D. (1944). Measurement of adult intelligence.
   1, 64-106.                                                     Baltimore: The Williams & Wilkins Co.
KAFOE, W.F. & LEONARD, B.E. (1973). The effect of a
  new tetracyclic antidepressant compound, Org. GB 94,                                         (Received March 7,1977)
You can also read