Affective systems of Norwegian mathematics student/teachers concerning 'unusual' problem solving.
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
Affective systems of Norwegian mathematics student/teachers concerning 'unusual' problem solving. Citation for published version (APA): Pepin, B. (2012). Affective systems of Norwegian mathematics student/teachers concerning 'unusual' problem solving. In Proceedings of ICME 12 : the 12th International Congress on Mathematics Education, 8-12 July 2012, Seoul, South Korea [TSG27-16] Document status and date: Published: 01/01/2012 Document Version: Publisher’s PDF, also known as Version of Record (includes final page, issue and volume numbers) Please check the document version of this publication: • A submitted manuscript is the version of the article upon submission and before peer-review. There can be important differences between the submitted version and the official published version of record. People interested in the research are advised to contact the author for the final version of the publication, or visit the DOI to the publisher's website. • The final author version and the galley proof are versions of the publication after peer review. • The final published version features the final layout of the paper including the volume, issue and page numbers. Link to publication General rights Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the public portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. • Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the public portal for the purpose of private study or research. • You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain • You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the public portal. If the publication is distributed under the terms of Article 25fa of the Dutch Copyright Act, indicated by the “Taverne” license above, please follow below link for the End User Agreement: www.tue.nl/taverne Take down policy If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us at: openaccess@tue.nl providing details and we will investigate your claim. Download date: 20. Sep. 2020
12th International Congress on Mathematical Education Topic Study Group 27 8 July – 15 July, 2012, COEX, Seoul, Korea AFFECTIVE SYSTEMS OF NORWEGIAN MATHEMATICS STUDENT/TEACHERS CONCERNING ‘UNUSUAL’ PROBLEM SOLVING Birgit Pepin Sør-Trøndelag University College, Trondheim, Norway The research reported in this paper draws on a small intervention study on teachers’ problem solving beliefs. Three groups of teachers, each at different stages of their educational and professional development, were asked to solve a carefully designed mathematical ‘unusual’ problem, and write down their reactions at different stages of solving the problem. The results provide (1) insights into teachers’ affective systems linked to their engagement in the task; and (2) how their problem solving affective systems are likely to be influenced by ‘emotional knowledge’ and skills. It is argued that affective systems in relation to mathematical problem solving activities (in particular ‘unusual problems’) can be regarded as an interplay between emotions, experiences in actions, and engagement structures. Key words: affective systems- teachers- unusual problem-solving BACKGROUND A number of studies in mathematics education have indicated that teachers’ beliefs about mathematics and its teaching and learning play a significant role in shaping their patterns of behaviour in the classroom (Peressini, Borko, Romagnano, Knuth & Willis 2004; Fang 1996; Thompson 1992; Ernest 1989). These beliefs make up an important part of teachers’ general knowledge through which teachers perceive, process and act upon information in the classroom and in other contexts. They can be embodied, amongst others, in the teacher’s theories about a particular subject area’s learning and teaching (Fang 1996), or about particular practices, such as problem solving. The literature is clear that learning is situated, that is, how a person learns a particular set of knowledge and skills, and the situation in which the person learns, are a fundamental part of what is learnt (Greeno, Collins & Resnick 1996). A situated perspective on teacher learning suggests that knowledge and beliefs, the practices they influence, and the influences themselves, are inseparable from the situations in which they are embedded (Putnam & Borko 2000; Borko & Putnam 1996). For example, Ernest (1988) is in no doubt that there is a general socialising influence across particular contexts in a school mathematics department, and this departmental influence often overrides a teacher’s individual beliefs. Talking about beliefs and ‘engagement structures’ (with which beliefs are intertwined), Goldin, Epstein, Schorr & Warner (2011) argue that when students work on conceptually challenging, and different, mathematics, they have to engage in non-routine thinking where “mathematical meanings matter more than procedures” (p.547), or to problem-solve where an 5803
Pepin apparently ‘impossible situation’ necessitates the construction of new representations and/or thinking. This is likely to be true for teacher beliefs when they work on non-standard mathematical problems. Putting teachers in such affective/social interactions, they respond differently (than to standard questions which they are used to in their classrooms) and it is likely to bring out a wider and perhaps more complex spectrum of beliefs during the ‘in-the-moment’ engagement. Thus, the aim of the study is to explore in which ways students and teachers (work with and) reflect on their work with an ‘unusual mathematical problem’, and in which ways this may provide insights into their affective systems. THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK Beliefs have been defined in different ways. Schoenfeld (1985) perceives mathematical beliefs as “the set of understandings about mathematics that establish the psychological context within which individuals do mathematics” (p.5). In a wider context Aguirre & Speer (2000) contend that beliefs are “conceptions, personal ideologies, world views and values that shape practice and orient knowledge” (p.328). The important point in all these definitions, and relevant for this study, is that beliefs are linked to actions and practice in mathematics. However, and connecting to more recent work on attitude (e.g. Di Martino & Zan, 2011), the construct of attitude “is shaped within the context of social psychology as orientation to behave in a certain way” (p.473). Recent work in the field refers to a tripartite model where attitude has a cognitive, an affective, and a behavioural component (ibid). The literature is clear about the “deep interplay between beliefs and emotions” (e.g. Di Martino & Zan, 2011, p.473). They quote McLeod (1992) who contends that each construct (beliefs, emotions, attitude) has a cognitive and an emotional component (though with a varying degree of importance): “Beliefs, attitudes, and emotions ... vary in the level of intensity of the affects that they describe, increasing in intensity from ‘cold’ beliefs about mathematics to ‘cool’ attitudes related to liking or disliking mathematics to ‘hot’ emotional reactions to the frustration of solving non-routine problems. Beliefs, attitudes, and emotions also differ in the degree to which cognition plays a role in the response, and in the time they take to develop.” (p.578) Encompassing these, Philippou & Christou (2002) coined the term ‘affective systems’ which includes emotions and feelings, as well as attitudes, beliefs and conceptions. Beliefs in particular are conceived as “the personal appraisal, judgements, and views that constitute one’s subjective knowledge about self and the environment”(p.212). There have been several studies (e.g. Philippou & Christou 1998, Swars, Smith & Smith 2009) where researchers have developed interventions designed to impact positively on students’ problem-solving beliefs, and these studies identified particular features that appeared to have succeeded in doing that. These studies often base their thinking on the idea that belief systems are composed of ‘episodically’-stored material and that such episodic memory is organised “in terms of personal experiences, episodes or events” (Nespor 1987, p.320, in Stylianides & Stylianides 2011); thus, it is believed, providing students with ‘influential episodes’ may shape their interactions with subsequent events. 5804
Pepin Previous studies which have used interventions such as the BHP (e.g. Stylianides & Stylianides 2011) have based their thinking on Schoenfeld’s ideas on beliefs with respect to problem solving. According to Schoenfeld (1985) a person’s beliefs about mathematics “can determine how [s/he] chooses to approach a problem ... and how long and hard [s/he] will work on it” (p.45). Schoenfeld’s work is clearly relevant here, as a person’s persistence with as well as the ways of approaching a problem seem to be at stake when working with an unusual problem. For example, many students believe that mathematical problems have to contain numbers and formulas, and they are hesitant to work on problems that do not fit their expectations of a mathematical problem, and thus they may give up earlier if they cannot see how to progress (e.g. Callejo & Vila 2009). THE STUDY This study is based on a design experiment (e.g. Cobb, Cofrey, di Sessa, Lehrer & Schauble, 2003) the author conducted in a master and teacher professional development course, with three different groups of teachers and student teachers: (1) a group of experienced teachers (between 3 and 20 years of experience) participating in a professional development course (18 teachers); (2) a group of student teachers (0-5 years of teaching experience) at the beginning of their master study (year 4, at beginning of first semester) (11 teachers); and (3) a group of student teachers (0-5 years of teaching experience) during their first year of master study (year 4, 2nd semester) (13 teachers). The groups were chosen because of their difference, in particular in terms of their educational and professional background, that is number of years working with mathematics (in education) and as teachers in the classroom. The intervention, the Blonde Hair Problem (BHP), was chosen from the literature on the basis of its ‘usability’ and effectiveness in terms of providing insights into students’ beliefs regarding problem solving (e.g. Philippou & Christou, 1995; Stylianides & Stylianides, 2011). It was assumed that this would also be the case for student teachers and experienced teachers alike. (Please see Philippou & Christou (1995), or Stylianides & Stylianides (2011) for the description of the problem, the solution outline and its design features). In terms of implementation the problem was presented and ‘administered’ exactly the same way for the three groups. It was important to reassure the students/teachers that they were not assessed for solving the task, but that this activity was done in order to reflect on ‘beliefs’ (part of their course module): 1. The problem was presented on a handout and read out (but not discussed), without commenting on whether it was solvable or not. Student/teachers were asked to work individually and to write down (a) their initial reaction to the BHP; (b) in which ways it may differ from other problems they have met. Answers were subsequently collected. 2. Students were told that the problem was solvable and they were asked to solve the task, and they could discuss in small groups. 5805
Pepin 3. Once all groups had solved the task, individuals were asked to present their solution/s at the board. This initiated a discussion about the mathematics which was led by the instructor. 4. Students were asked to describe/write down their experiences of working on the BHP. 5. Students were asked to read and compare their responses to what they had written at the beginning. The responses of all students to all five tasks were collected for analysis, and a discussion was held about teacher beliefs (as part of the course). In terms of analysis student/teachers responses to all five tasks/questions offered data for examining the different facets of student/teacher beliefs when confronted with a ‘different’ problem. All responses were coded independently, and codes compared between student/teachers and questions. THE FINDINGS The findings (for this paper) can be categorised under two headings: (1) Initial engagement/interest and self- efficacy; and (2) The role of communication and group work/support. (1) In terms of initial engagement and interest, and self-efficacy to be able to solve the BHP, the two student groups differed quite considerably from the teacher group. Whereas most of the teachers in the teacher group wanted to solve the problem and they thought that they could do it (with some help or clues), the students reported interest, but were unsure whether the problem was solvable, and if so, whether they could solve it. Teachers used expressions like “I want to solve it”, or “interesting task, gives you the wish to solve it”, although they were not certain how to solve it. Another interesting feature of teachers’ reactions to the BHP was that they directly connected it to their classroom teaching and pupil learning: “Funny ... funnier than the usual context we have in textbooks.” (T2) “I think that pupils who think that they cannot do mathematics will give up after the 3rd sentence.” (T7) “Nice starting point to link maths and everyday.” (T1) “I had enough information after the first sentence. The next bit of information was just confusing. I don’t get it ... I guess that this ... [compares] to feelings my students have when solving these kinds of problems.” (T9) “I have tried these kinds of problems with my pupils. They really like them. ... it is important that students know that the problem is solvable.” (T5) On the other hand, at least half of the students were not sure whether the problem was solvable (‘not enough cues’), and all were uncertain whether they could solve it. None of them thought that they could solve it without help (and would give up if they were left to themselves). 5806
Pepin “They do not tell us whether it is solvable or not, and therefore ... must give up.” (S3) “It seems difficult because there isn’t enough hint of the problem. ... a fine balance in the task, and it ‘wakes’ the interest and a kind of urge to solve it.” (S8) “I found it very different from other problems I have met in school. The problem was presented in such a way that I felt it was possible for me to find the solution, that I had the same basis as the others in the room. Thinking back I found it fun and intriguing. It took a while working with the problem before I realized that I actually was using knowledge and skills I possess in my effort in solving the problem. At first it felt like I just was trying out different possible solutions, but that it soon was put in a system... Even though the solution was presented after I left, I certainly felt I had been brushing up my math skills." (S3) Students found the task ‘funny’, described it as ‘unmathematical’ and ‘tricky’ and that it did not seem ‘a ‘proper’ mathematics task’, in the sense that they did not find ‘well known procedures’ to solve it. This last notion (that no ‘formula’ was provided for solving the task) was quite prominent amongst students- they clearly wanted a ‘recipe’; that would have made them feel safe. It appeared that their self efficacy and confidence in problem solving was not as developed as that of the teachers. It was not clear whether this was due to teachers working with ‘known’ (school) mathematics every day, whereas students feel that they still have to learn/revise some of the mathematics for teaching, or whether it was the structure/type of the task that did not ‘fit into’ their perceptions of mathematical tasks. (2) Concerning the role of communication and group work/support, more students than teachers commented on the positive influence of communication/dialogue and group work as support for their working with the BHP and development of problem solving skills. “Through common discussions did I understand what the hints meant and thus could get to the right solution.” (S2) “I did not think that it was possible to solve the task, but when I started to discuss it together with the others, did I see quickly that it was possible.” (S7) “The collaborative work with peer students [was] positive, as [we could] exchange ideas and clear potential misunderstandings.” (S4) “We solved the problem together, with my colleagues. We ...” (T1) This can be seen in the light of the confidence with problem solving or self efficacy – they felt comfortable and ‘safe’ when working with their peers (see above), but perhaps also in view of their ‘normal’ working conditions: most teachers are ‘alone’ in teaching their classes, there is no one else in the classroom. It could be argued that this produces a kind of ‘self-dependency’. Professional development activities and discussions on particular mathematical tasks are rare. On the other hand students are used to group work and peer discussions in their university/college sessions. DISCUSSION OF RESULTS Self- belief systems, and these include self efficacy, are part of a person’s general beliefs system (Philippou & Christou, 2002), and numerous studies claim that confidence is one’s ability to undertake a certain action. In this study the students’ and teachers’ feelings of confidence depended on their experiences in connection to related and previous actions, i.e. 5807
Pepin whether they had solved or seen or experienced similar tasks before. It can be argued that whereas students’ experiences were likely to have been shaped by ‘formula and calculating’ approaches, the more experienced teachers’ experiences were more varied, which helped them to feel more confident about being able to ‘have a good go’ and perhaps succeed in solving the task. This was also expressed by, and connected to, their perceptions of whether the ‘unusual task’ was solvable: students wondered whether it was solvable, and many decided that it was unsolvable, because they appeared not to have the correct kinds of information. Most teachers, however, simply assumed that they should have a go before deciding otherwise, and they applied their skills in trying to solve it- a more practical approach, which appeared to provide resilience. Bandura (1997) highlights particular sources of self-efficacy, amongst them ‘mastery experiences’, and it appeared that teachers may have had more of those experiences. One of the advantages of construing affect as a representation system is that it “entails the recognition that affect is not simply an emotional state or an attitude, it is configured” (Goldin, 1998, p.214). This dual role could be traced in this ‘unusual’ problem solving situation, where for some students the confrontation with an ‘unusual’ problem might have signified ‘giving up’ or ‘I cannot do it’ (recalling an unsuccessful or uncomfortable situation), whilst for others, and in particular teachers, the ‘unfamiliarity’ led to new strategies (and perhaps the arousal of a challenging situation). However, when being able to work together on the problem in groups, all groups developed more positive engagement structures around the mathematical problem, and it was interesting that these were triggered by discussion. It can be argued that Bandura’s (1997) ‘social persuasion’ was a key element, linking to social behaviour when working with an ‘unusual problem’. Equally, it can be contended that communication and reflection with others is known to help to develop deeper mathematical understandings when working on problems. However, it is argued in this case whether students/teachers had to work alone or in a group on an ‘unusual problem’ influenced how they construed the situation. It was the combination of ‘alone’ and ‘unusual’ that were likely to evoke negative ‘desires’ and consequently negative engagement structures for activation. Thus, ‘alone’ (no communication with others, and thus likely failure) and ‘unusual’ mathematical problem links to ‘negative engagement’. In other words, in this case emotions and particular mathematical experiences lead to engagement (or not): Emotions/affective system + in-the-moment mathematical experiences/in action - > engagement structures The question remains whether the arrow also works in the other direction, i.e. whether engagement structures can lead to/provoke mathematical experiences/unusual problems and emotions. Leaning on previous research (e.g. Philippou & Christou, 2002; Di Martino & Zan, 2011) it can be argued that affective systems in relation to mathematical problem solving activities (in particular ‘unusual problems’) can be regarded as an interplay between emotions, experiences in actions, and engagement structures. 5808
Pepin References Aguirre, J. & Speer, N. M. (2000).Examining the relationship between beliefs and goals in teacher practice. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 18(3): 327-356. Bandura, A. (1997) Self-efficacy: the exercise of control, New York: Freeman. Borko, H. & Putnam, R. (1996) Learning to teach (p. 673-708). In Berliner, D. & Calfee, R. (eds) Handbook of Educational Psychology, New York: MacMillan. Callejo, M.L. & Vila, A. (2009) Approach to mathematical problem-solving and students’ belief systems. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 72: 111-126. Cobb, P., Cofrey, J., di Sessa, A., Lehrer, R. & Schauble, L. (2003) Design experiments in educational research. Educational Researcher, 32(1): 9-13. Di Martino, P. & Zan, R. (2011) Attitude towards mathematics: a bridge between beliefs and emotions. ZDM Mathematics Education, 43: 471-82. Ernest, P. (1988) The impact of beliefs on the teaching of mathematics.Paper presented at ICME VI, Budapest, Hungary. Ernest, P. (1989) The knolwedge, beliefs and attitudes of the mathematics teaxcher: a model. In Journal of Education for Teaching, 15(1): 13-33. Fang, Z. (1996) A review of research on teacher beliefs and practices. Educational Researcher, 38 (1): 47-65. Goldin, G.A. (1998) Representational systems, learning, and problem solving in mathematics. Journal of Mathematical Behaviour, 17 (2): 137-65. Goldin, G.A., Epstein, Y.M., Schorr, R. & Warner, L.B. (2011) beliefs and engagement structures: behind the affective dimension of mathematical learning. ZDM Mathematics Education, 43: 547-60. Greeno, J.G., Collins, A.M. & Resnick, L.B. (1996) Cognition and learning(p.15-46). In Berliner, D. & Calfee, R. (eds.) Handbook of Educational Psychology. New York: MacMillan. McLeod, D.B. (1992) Research on affect in mathematics education: a reconceptualisation. (p.575-96) In D. Grouws (ed.) Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning.New York:, NY: McMillan. Nespor, J. (1987) The role of beliefs in the practice of teaching. Journal of Curriculum Studies, 19: 317-28. Peressini, D., Borko, H., Romagnano, L. Knuth, E. & Willis, C. (2004) A conceptual framework for elarning to teach secondary mathematics: a situative perspective. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 56:67-96. Philippou, G. & Christou, C. (1995) Teaching mathematics. Athens: Dardanos. Philippou, G. & Christou, C. (1998) The effects of a preparatory mathematics programme in changing prospective teachers’ attitudes towards mathematics. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 35: 189-206. Philippou, G. & Christou, C. (2002) A study of the mathematics teaching efficacy beliefs of primary teachers. In G.C. Leder, E. Pehkonen & G. Toerner (eds.) Beliefs: a hidden variable in mathematics education?. Dordrecht: Kluwer. Putnam, R.T. & Borko, H. (2000) What do new ideas of knowledge and thinking have to say about research on teacher learning? Educational Researcher, 29(1): 4-15. 5809
Pepin Schoenfeld, A.H. (1985) Mathematical problem solving. Orlando: Academic Press. Stylianides, G. J., & Stylianides, A. J. (2011). An intervention of students’ problem-solving beliefs. In Proceedings of the 7th Congress of the European Society for Research in Mathematics Education. Rzeszów, Poland. Swars, S,L., Smith, S.Z., Smith, M.E. et al. (2009) A longitudinal study of effects of a developmental teacher preparation programme on elementary prospective teachers’ mathematics beliefs. Journal of Mathematics Teacher Education, 12: 47-66. Thompson, A.G. (1992) Teachers’ beliefs and conceptions: a synthesis of research (p.127-146). In D.A. Grouws (ed.) Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning.New York:, NY: McMillan. 5810
You can also read