The position of authenticity within extant models of personality.

Page created by Ricardo Crawford
 
CONTINUE READING
The position of authenticity within extant models of
personality.
MALTBY, John, WOOD, Alex M., DAY, Liza and PINTO, Diana
Available from Sheffield Hallam University Research Archive (SHURA) at:
http://shura.shu.ac.uk/6053/

This document is the author deposited version. You are advised to consult the
publisher's version if you wish to cite from it.
Published version
MALTBY, John, WOOD, Alex M., DAY, Liza and PINTO, Diana (2012). The position
of authenticity within extant models of personality. Personality and Individual
Differences, 52 (3), 269-273.

Repository use policy
Copyright © and Moral Rights for the papers on this site are retained by the
individual authors and/or other copyright owners. Users may download and/or print
one copy of any article(s) in SHURA to facilitate their private study or for non-
commercial research. You may not engage in further distribution of the material or
use it for any profit-making activities or any commercial gain.

                  Sheffield Hallam University Research Archive
                               http://shura.shu.ac.uk
The position of authenticity within extant models of personality                     1

         RUNNING HEAD: The position of authenticity within extant models of personality.

              The position of authenticity within extant models of personality.

                                       John Maltby*,

                       School of Psychology, University of Leicester,

                                    Leicester, LE1 9HN.

                                       Alex M. Wood,

                 School of Psychological Sciences, University of Manchester,

                                   Manchester M13 9PL.

                                          Liz Day.

             Faculty of Development and Society, Sheffield Hallam University,

                                     Sheffield, S10 2BP.

                                        Diana Pinto,

                       School of Psychology, University of Leicester,

                                    Leicester, LE1 9HN.

* Corresponding Author: Tel: +44 (0)116 229 7198: Fax: +44 (0)116 229 7196. Email:

jm148@le.ac.uk
The position of authenticity within extant models of personality   2
The position of authenticity within extant models of personality                           3

                                          Abstract

The aim of the current study was to explore where Authenticity, derived from the humanistic

tradition of psychology, was positioned within a number of extant models of personality.

Exploratory and Confirmatory factor analysis of data from four samples (total N=1,286)

suggested that Authenticity can be considered as loading on the Honesty-Humility factor of

personality. These findings are discussed in terms of the wider theoretical overlaps between

honesty-humility and psychological functioning as emphasised by the humanistic tradition of

psychology

Keywords: Authenticity; Honesty-Humility; Five-Factor; Biological Models of Personality.
The position of authenticity within extant models of personality                                 4

Introduction

Within the humanistic tradition of psychology authenticity is seen as the core aspect of

human functioning (Horney, 1951; Rogers, 1959). Authenticity comprises three aspects: (1) a

lack of self-alienation, involving a consistent sense of identity which is consistent with all

deep down beliefs, actual feeling, inherent tendencies, and objective reality; (2) authentic

living, in line with this identity, and (3) not accepting external influence, when it goes against

personal beliefs (Wood, Linley, Maltby, Baliousis, & Joseph, 2008). Authenticity occurs

when the three are in alignment. Therefore a person is authentic when they have a fully

consistent identity in line with reality (low self-alienation), behave consistently with this

(high authentic living), and don’t allow external influences to either distort self-perception or

prevent the authentic behavioral expression of one’s self (low accepting external influences).

Clinically, distress is caused when inauthenticity comes to conscious awareness (Joseph &

Linley, 2005). The Authenticity Scale (Wood et al., 2008) measures conscious awareness of

authenticity, with the self-alienation, accepting external influence, and authentic living

subscales measuring stable ways of functioning, demonstrating trait like properties evidenced

by consistency between scores over time (e.g. r=.79 to .84 over 2 weeks and r=.78 to .81 over

4 weeks).

       Given the central role of authenticity in the Humanistic school in psychology, its

place within wider extant personality models has been little considered. Studies examining

the tripartite model of authenticity shows appropriate but distinct variance from the six main

personality traits; the five factor model of personality (Costa & McCrae, 1992), comprising

extraversion, neuroticism, agreeableness, conscientiousness, and openness, and the sixth

factor of personality, ‘Honesty-Humility’ (Lee & Ashton, 2008). These traits account for a

small but significant 11%–13% of the variance in authenticity (Wood et al., 2008). Further,

Gray’s (1987) and Cloninger’s (Cloninger, Svrakic, & Przybeck, 1993) models of personality

account for between 5.8% and 18% of the variance in authenticity (Pinto, Maltby, & Wood,
The position of authenticity within extant models of personality                              5

2011). However, little is known as to where authenticity can be exactly positioned in regards

to extant models of personality, or whether, authenticity, similarly to other variables, such as

religiosity, conservatism, or risk taking (Paunonen & Jackson, 2000), are positioned outside

extant models of personality.

       The first context is the six main factors of personality. Authenticity may load on more

than one of these factors. For example, authenticity is intrinsically related to positive well-

being (Wood, et al, 2008). Given the prominence of neuroticism and extraversion in

predicting affect across the life-span respectively (Wilson & Gullone, 1999), either of these

two personality factors may encompass authenticity. However, authenticity seems most

comparable to honesty-humility, which comprises adjectives such as truthfulness, positive

values, honesty and sincerity (Ashton & Lee, 2007). Similarly, the literature emphasises

authenticity as comprising the promotion of positive experiences, truthfulness, uniqueness,

and positive growth (Rogers, 1965). Therefore, honesty-humility seems most similar to

encompass authenticity. However, Wood et al. (2008) found correlations no larger than r=.11

between the tripartite model of authenticity and the Honesty/Humility subscale of the

HEXACO Personality Inventory (Lee & Ashton, 2004). This finding seems surprising given

the identified overlaps in descriptions between the constructs. Therefore the relationship

between the tripartite model of authenticity and honesty/humility requires further

consideration.

       The second context is to include other extant personality models. These include the

biological models of personality; Eysenck’s (1990) model of personality (neuroticism,

extraversion and psychoticism); Gray’s (1987) reinforcement sensitivity theory (behavioural

approach and inhibition systems); and Cloninger’s (Cloninger, et al. 1993) model of

temperaments (novelty seeking, harm avoidance, reward dependence and persistence) and

character domains (self-directedness, cooperativeness and self-transcendence). These

personality models can be positioned largely within the six factor personality framework
The position of authenticity within extant models of personality                                6

(Revelle, 1997). For example, Eysenck’s model maps directly onto the five factor model with

psychoticism as better represented by low agreeableness and low conscientiousness. Gray’s

behavioral approach and Cloninger’s novelty seeking are thought to mirror extraversion, and

Gray’s behavioral inhibition and Cloninger’s harm avoidance mirror neuroticism (Revelle,

1997; Stewart, Ebmeier, & Deary, 2004). However, Pinto et al. (2011) have noted theoretical

and empirical overlaps between Horney’s (1951) descriptions of low authenticity emerging

from neurosis and leading to inhibited behaviours reminiscent of Gray’s inhibitory system or

Cloninger’s harm avoidance, and therefore the consideration of biological models of

personality is important in this consideration.

       Furthermore, any deliberation should include Self-Determination Theory (Deci &

Ryan, 2002) that makes the distinction between autonomy, controlled and impersonal

motivations. Autonomy, controlled and impersonal motivations of Self-Determination Theory

seem to be largely independent of the five factor model, though control and impersonal

motivations overlap with agreeableness and neuroticism respectively (Olesen, Thomsen,

Schnieber & Tønnesvang, 2010). However, autonomy places an emphasis on demonstrating

intrinsic and truthful (albeit by being consistent), needs, values, and motivations and

therefore may reflect authenticity.

       To properly consider whether authenticity is positioned within or outside existing

personality theory and research, it is necessary to consider the position of the tripartite model

of authenticity within a range of extant personality theories. Using the biological and self-

determination theories alongside the six factors derived from the lexical hypothesis would be

useful to ensure factors were optimally operationalized. The aim of the current study was to

explore whether authenticity was positioned within the six factors emerging from the lexical

hypothesis, biological and self-determination theories of personality.

                                            Method
The position of authenticity within extant models of personality                           7

Samples

       Sample 1

Seven hundred and eight respondents (332 males, 376 females) were sampled from a series of

community groups and workplaces in the Midlands and Yorkshire region of the United

Kingdom. Ages ranged from 18 to 70 years (M=33.54, SD=9.9). Participants were

predominantly White (57.6%), with 17.5 % of respondents Asian and 16.7% Black.

       Sample 2

One hundred and seventy nine respondents (60 males, 119 females) were sampled from a

midlands university undergraduate student population in the United Kingdom. Ages ranged

from 18 to 24 years (M=19.55, SD=1.2). Participants were predominantly White (69.8%),

with 18.4 % of respondents Asian and 8.4% Black.

       Sample 3

One hundred and ninety two respondents (79 males, 113 females) were sampled from six

large employers in the Yorkshire region of the United Kingdom. Ages ranged from 18 to 60

years (M=38.50, SD=10.4). Participants were predominantly White (60.2%), with 17.2 % of

respondents Asian and 13.0% Black.

       Sample 4

Two hundred and seven respondents (86 males, 121 females) were sampled from a number of

community and religious groups in the Yorkshire region of the United Kingdom with the aim

of obtaining an ethnically diverse sample. Ages ranged from 18 to 68 years (M=38.2,

SD=12.7). Participants were predominantly White (37.2%) with 30.9 % of respondents Asian

and 30.0% Black.

Measures

       Authenticity was measured using the Authenticity Scale (Wood et al, 2008)

comprising three four-item subscales; Authentic Living (e.g.“I am true to myself in most

situations”), Accepting External Influence (e.g.“I usually do what other people tell me to do”)
The position of authenticity within extant models of personality                              8

and Self-Alienation (e.g.“I feel out of touch with the ‘real me’"). Responses are scored on a

7-point scale, anchored to 1(Disagree Strongly) to 7(Agree Strongly). Internal reliabilities

range from .70 to .86 and test-retest reliabilities range between .78 and .91 (Wood et al.,

2008). The subscales demonstrate validity via expected relationships with positive emotional

and social functioning (Wood et al., 2008).

       The Five Factor Model of Personality was assessed via the 50-item International

Personality Item Pool (IPIP) measures of extraversion (e.g.“Start conversations”),

neuroticism (e.g.“Get upset easily”) , agreeableness (e.g.“Feel others' emotions”),

conscientiousness (e.g.“Like order”) and openness (e.g.“Have excellent ideas”; Goldberg et

al., 2006). Responses are scored on five-point scales, anchored to 1(Very Inaccurate) and

5(Very Accurate).The scales show internal reliability statistics of between .77 and .86 and

correlations with the NEO Personality Inventory (Costa & McCrae, 1992) of between .85 and

.92 (Goldberg et al., 2006).

       The Honesty-Humility factor was assessed via the four 8-item IPIP Facet Scales

(Sincerity, e.g.“Don't pretend to be more than I am”; Fairness, e.g.“Try to follow the rules”;

Greed Avoidance, e.g.“Don't strive for elegance in my appearance” and Modesty e.g.“Am

just an ordinary person”) for the HEXACO Personality Inventory (IPIP-HEXACO; Goldberg

et al., 2006; Ashton, Lee, & Goldberg, 2007). Responses are scored on a five-point scale,

anchored to 1(Very Inaccurate) and 5(Very Accurate) and the subscales scores were added

together to form an overall scores for Honesty-Humility. The IPIP–HEXACO scales internal-

consistency reliabilities range from .73 to .88, and convergent correlations of between .76 and

.98 with the original HEXACO facet scales (Ashton et al., 2007).

       Cloninger’s model of personality was assessed by using the IPIP-TCI Scales

(Goldberg et al., 2006). Two hundred and eighty nine items from the IPIP are used to

measure four temperament domains; Novelty-Seeking (e.g.“Break rules”), Harm-Avoidance

(e.g.“Avoid dangerous situations”), Reward Dependence (e.g.”Follow directions”) and
The position of authenticity within extant models of personality                               9

Persistence (e.g.“Meet challenges”) and three character domains; Self-Directedness (e.g.”

Know what I want”), Cooperativeness (e.g.” Reassure others”) and Self-Transcendence

(e.g.”Radiate joy”). Responses are scored on 5-point scales, anchored to 1(Very Inaccurate)

and 5(Very Accurate). The scales show internal reliability statistics of between .66 and .86

and appropriate correlations with the Temperament and Character Inventory (Cloninger et

al.,1993; Goldberg et al., 2006).

       Gray’s model of personality was measured via the 24-item BIS/BAS Scales (Carver

& White, 1994). The BIS scale assesses anxiety sensitivity to events (e.g.“Criticism or

scolding hurts me quite a bit”). The BAS scale is subdivided into Drive (e.g.” I go out of my

way to get things I want”), Fun-Seeking (e.g.” I crave excitement and new sensations”) and

Reward Responsiveness (e.g.” It would excite me to win a contest”).. Responses are anchored

to 1(Very true to me) to 4(Very false for me) format. Internal reliabilities for the scales range

from .77 for the BIS, and .89 for Reward Responsiveness, .85 for Drive and .80 for Fun

Seeking (Cooper, Gomez, & Aucote, 2007). Validity for the scale is demonstrated via

corresponding performances on reward and punishment tasks (Brenner, Beauchaine, &

Sylvers, 2005).

       The dimensions of Self-Determination Theory were assessed by the General

Causality Orientation Scale (GCOS; Deci & Ryan, 1985). The scale comprises 17 vignettes

which describe social or achievement orientated situations (e.g. a scenario where the

individual is asked to lead a work project), each with three reactions. Then, on a 7-point

scale, from 1(Very unlikely) to 7(Very likely), respondents indicate the extent to which their

reaction is autonomous (e.g.“seek participation from others”), controlled (e.g.“take charge in

making decisions”), and impersonal (e.g.“follow precedent”). Internal reliability (>.75) and

test-retest (>.74) coefficients are reported for the three scales (Deci & Ryan, 1985). The

validity of the scales is demonstrated via the autonomy orientation being positively related to

self-esteem and self-actualization, the controlled orientation being related to public self-
The position of authenticity within extant models of personality                               10

consciousness, and the impersonal orientation being related to poorer well-being (Deci &

Ryan, 1985; Hodgins, et al., 1996).

       The administration order of the measures was counterbalanced within each sample.

Due to the length of the questionnaire, participants were allowed as much time as required to

complete the questionnaire and return it at a later date if required.

                                             Results

Exploratory Factor Analysis

       The first step of the analysis was to determine the factor structure of the data using the

data from sample 1 (n=708). The 23 scales were subjected to Maximum Likelihood Analysis

(Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin Measure of Sampling Adequacy=.815; Bartlett's Test of Sphericity,

x2=5261.15, df=253, p
The position of authenticity within extant models of personality                               11

variables. This suggestins an optimal 6 factor solution with the first six factors accounting for

19.62%, 13.57%, 9.97%, 7.61%, 5.54%, and 5.18% of the variance respectively.

        These factors were then subjected to oblique (promax) rotation with delta set to 0. An

oblique rotation was used as the factors were expected to be correlated. Table 1 presents the

rotated matrices (noting the inclusion of internal reliability statistics for each scale).

                                [INSERT TABLE 1 ABOUT HERE]

        For interpretation purposes, the criteria of .32 was used for a minimum loading of an

item, because this equates to 10% overlapping variance with other items on the factor

(Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). In this solution, the first factor to emerge would be the sixth

factor of personality with the IPIP honesty-humility subscales loading on this factor.

Moreover, the authenticity subscales load on this factor, with accepting external influence

and self-alienation loading negatively on the factor, and autonomous orientation loading

positively, and the controlled orientation loading negatively on this factor. The second factor

is an Extraversion factor with extraversion loading alongside the three behavioural activation

system subscales and novelty seeking. The third factor is an Agreeableness factor, with

agreeableness, loading alongside reward dependence and cooperativeness. The fourth factor

is a Neuroticism factor, with neuroticism loading alongside behavioural inhibition and harm

avoidance. The fifth factor is a Conscientiousness factor with conscientiousness loading

alongside persistence and self-direction. The final factor is an Openness factor, with openness

to experience loading alongside self-transcendence. Finally, amotivation orientation derived

from Social Determination Theory failed to load above .32 on any of the six factors, but

loaded the most highest on the neuroticism factor.

        The correlations between the factors, using Cohen’s (1992) effect size criteria

(.1=small, .3=medium, .5=large) to assess the degree of association, were generally small (r<

.25) with three exceptions. The Honesty-Humility factor shared a positive correlation of a
The position of authenticity within extant models of personality                               12

medium effect size with the agreeableness (r=.36) and conscientiousness (r=.31) factors and

a negative correlation of a medium effect size with the neuroticism factor (r=-.36).

Confirmatory Factor Analysis

       The factor structure suggested by the EFA was tested with multigroup confirmatory

factor analysis (CFA), using Samples 2, 3, and 4, and performed using covariance structural

equation modeling with the Analysis of Moment Structure (AMOS) software. In contrast to

conventional CFA, multigroup CFA additionally tests whether the factor structure is invariant

across samples and demographic groups (Byrne, 2004). Thus, the multigroup approach

provides several replications of the CFA, as well as testing the generalizability of the

findings.

       Three multigroup analyses were performed to test the fit of the six factor model, and

to respectively test whether the model was invariant across (a) sample, (b) gender, and (c)

ethnicity. Each multigroup analyses tested the same model. Six latent factors were specified,

as in Table 1. Each variable was specified as loading on only one factor. The six factors were

allowed to correlate, however the error variances of the observed variables were not allowed

to covary. This provides a highly conservative test of the six factor model, specifying that

each variable loaded on only the theoretically expected factor, and variables were only

correlated due to their shared loadings on this factor. Model fit was tested with the chi-

squared test of the difference between the implied and reproduced correlation matrices, the

standardized root-mean-square residual (SRMR), and the root mean square error of

approximation. As the chi squared test is sensitive to sample size, Hu and Bentler (1999)

recommend basing model fit assessments on the CFI and SRMR. CFA models are sensitive

to the number of variables in the model, and the high number of observed variables (23) in

the model, combined with no correlated errors, determined a priori that fit of the model could

never be very high. As such, relatively lenient values of SRMR and RMSEA ≤ .10 were

adopted as indicating acceptable fit, which are nevertheless conventionally considered
The position of authenticity within extant models of personality                               13

acceptable (Hu & Bentler, 1999), although perhaps at the upper limit of acceptability. For the

mulitigroup analysis, in the constrained model factor loadings (measurement weights) and the

correlations between factors (structural covariances) were fixed to be equal across the groups,

whilst in the unconstrained models they were allowed to vary. The test of the difference

between the constrained and unconstrained model was based on the difference in chi squared

fit between the models (itself chi squared distributed).

                                 [INSERT TABLE 2 ABOUT HERE]

       The loadings of each variable for each CFAs performed as part of the multigroup

analyses are presented in Table 2 (noting the inclusion of internal reliability statistics for each

scale). In most cases variables loaded highly on the latent variable. The table also shows a

remarkable amount of consistency in the size of the loadings across the three samples, three

broad ethnic groupings and both genders. Based simply on visual examination, it seems that

the model is invariant across these categories – although this assessment is tested statistically

through the multigroup CFA.

       The first multigroup analysis tested fit across sample. The constrained model

(2[df=722; n=578]=1297.44, RMSEA=.04 [90% CI =.03-.04], SRMR=.08) fit the data well,

to an equal degree as the unconstrained model ([df=648; n=578]=1226.56, RMSEA=.04

[90% CI =.04-.04], SRMR=.08; 2=70.88 df=74, p=.58), indicating that the factor

loadings were equal between the groups, and the measure was sample invariant. This is

significant, given the samples differed through sampling technique and composition (two

student groups, and one ethnically diverse occupational sample).

       As the model showed sample invariance, it is acceptable to combine the samples and

then create demographic groups (Byrne, 2004). In the next multigroup CFA, the samples

where then split according to gender (225 male, 353 female) and showed that again the

constrained model (2[df=469, n=578]=787.60, RMSEA=.03 [90% CI=.03-.04] , SRMR <

.10) fit the data well, to an equal degree as the unconstrained model (2[df=432,
The position of authenticity within extant models of personality                              14

n=578]=749.99, RMSEA=.04 [90% CI=.03-.04] , SRMR =.11; 2=37.61, df=37, p=.44),

suggesting the gender invariance of the measure.

       Finally, the samples were then split between three ethnic groups. Participants were

classed as either White (n=319), Asian (n=130), or Black (n=102). Finer grained comparisons

between ethnic groups (e.g., Indian or Chinese) were not possible due to an insufficient n for

CFA. The multigroup CFA again showed the constrained model (2[df=722,

n=551]=1249.75, RMSEA=.04 [90% CI=.03-.04], SRMR < .10) fit the data well, to an equal

degree as the unconstrained model (2[df=648, n=551]=1173.29, RMSEA=.04 [90% CI=.04-

.04], SRMR=.09; 2=76.37, df=74, p=.40), suggesting that the measure is invariant across

ethnic groups. The three multigroup CFAs showed the six factor model provided an

acceptable fit to the data, with the model being invariant across sample, gender, and ethnicity.

       The correlations between the factors across the samples were generally low with some

notable exceptions. In terms of effect size across the three samples there was a negative

correlation of a large effect size between the Extraversion and Conscientiousness factors,

ranging from r=-.49 (Sample 1) to r=-.65 (sample 3). Among Samples 1 and 2, the Honest-

Humility factor shared a positive correlation of a medium effect size with the Agreeableness

(Sample 1, r=.37; Sample 2, r=.38) and Openness factors (Sample 1, r=.33; Sample 2, r=.43)

and a negative correlation of a medium effect size with the Neuroticism factor (Sample 1, r=-

.34; Sample 2, r=-.46). Additionally the Openness factor shared a positive correlation of a

medium effect size with the Agreeableness factor among sample 1 (r=.34) and 2 (r=.45) and

the Extraversion factor in sample 2 (r=.35).

                                          Discussion

From the Exploratory Factor Analyses of a number of personality measures a factor solution

emerges that reflects the six main personality factors; honesty-humility, extraversion,

agreeableness, neuroticism, conscientiousness and openness to experience, with authenticity

loading on the first of those factors. The subsequent Confirmatory Factor Analysis suggests
The position of authenticity within extant models of personality                             15

that this six factor model provided an acceptable fit to the data, and the model was invariant

across three samples, gender, and ethnicity. This finding suggests authenticity, a key concept

in Humanistic and Counselling psychology, is positioned within the six factors of personality.

This finding contrasts with Wood et al. (2008) who found no significant association between

authenticity and honesty-humility. However, the previous finding is based on one single

correlation among one sample. We present a finding replicated across Exploratory and

Confirmatory Factor Analysis, and within a multi-group CFA which shows the relationships

to be invariant across samples, ethnicity and gender. This suggests the current findings

provide a more robust consideration of the relationship.

       One aspect to consider from the current findings is the loadings of authenticity and

autonomy orientation alongside honesty-humility. Ashton and Lee (2007) have used an

evolutionary theoretical framework, specifically reciprocal altruism, to explain the concept of

honesty-humility. The current findings indicate that the Honesty-Humility factor may expand

in another theoretical direction regarding psychological functioning as emphasised by the

humanistic tradition of psychology.

       In summary, the study shows how authenticity is positioned within extant models of

personality, with all three aspects of the tripartite model of authenticity loading on the

honesty-humility factor of personality.
The position of authenticity within extant models of personality                          16

                                          References

Ashton, M.C., & Lee, K. (2007). Empirical, theoretical, and practical advantages of the

       HEXACO model of personality structure. Personality and Social Psychology Review,

       11, 150-166.

Ashton, M.C., Lee, K., & Goldberg, L.R. (2007). The IPIP-HEXACO scales: An alternative,

       public-domain measure of the personality constructs in the HEXACO model.

       Personality and Individual Differences, 42, 1515-1526.

Brenner, S.L. Beauchaine, T.P. & Sylvers, P.D. (2005). A comparison of

       psychophysiological and self-report measures of BAS and BIS activation.

       Psychophysiology, 42, 108-115.

Byrne, B.M. (2004). Testing for multigroup invariance using amos graphics: A road less

       traveled. Structural Equation Modeling, 11, 272-300.

Carver, C.S., & White, T.L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and

       affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS Scales.

       Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 319–333.

Cattell, R.B. (1966). The scree test for the number of factors. Multivariate Behavioral

       Research, 1, 140-161.

Cloninger, C.R., Svrakic, D.M., & Przybeck, T.R. (1993). A psychobiological model of

       temperament and character. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 975–990.

Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 155–159.

Cooper, A., Gomez, R., & Aucote, H. (2007). The Behavioural Inhibition system and

       Behavioural approach system (BIS/BAS) scales: Measurement and structural

       invariance across adults and adolescents. Personality and Individual Differences, 43,

       295-305.

Costa, Jr, P.T. & McCrae, R.R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and

       NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) professional manual. Psychological

       Assessment Resources, Odessa, FL.
The position of authenticity within extant models of personality                            17

Deci, E.L., & Ryan, R.M. (1985). The general causality orientations scale: Self-determination

       in personality. Journal of Research in Personality, 19, 109-134.

Deci, E.L., & Ryan, R.M. (2002). Self-determination research: Reflections and future

       directions. In E.L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self-determination

       research (pp. 431-441). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.

Eysenck, H.J. (1990). Biological dimensions of personality. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook

       of personality: Theory and research (pp. 244-276). New York: Guilford.

Goldberg, L.R., Johnson, J.A., Eber, H.W., Hogan, R., Ashton, M.C., Cloninger, C.R., &

       Gough, H.C. (2006). The International Personality Item Pool and the future of public-

       domain personality measures. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 84-96.

Gray, J.A. (1987). The psychology of fear and stress (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge

       University Press.

Hodgins, H.S., Koestner, R., & Duncan, N. (1996). On the compatibility of autonomy and

       relatedness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 227-237.

Horn, J.L. (1965). A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor analysis.

       Psychometrika, 30, 179-185.

Horney, K. (1951). Neurosis and human growth. London: Routledge.

Hu, L.T., & Bentler, P.M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indices in covariance structure

       analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling,

       6, 1-55.

Joseph, S. & Linley, P. A. (2005). Positive Adjustment to Threatening Events: An

       Organismic Valuing Theory of Growth Through Adversity. Review of General

       Psychology, 9, 262-280.

Kaiser, H.F. (1960). The application of electronic computers to factor analysis. Educational

       and Psychological Measurement, 20, 141-151.
The position of authenticity within extant models of personality                            18

Ledesma, D.R. & Valero-Mora, P. (2007). Determining the number of factors to retain in

       EFA: an easy to use computer program for carrying out Parallel Analysis. Practical

       Assessment, Research and Evaluation, 12, 1-11.

Lee, K., & Ashton, M.C. (2004). Psychometric properties of the HEXACO personality

       inventory. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 39, 329–358.

Lee, K., & Ashton, M.C. (2008). The HEXACO personality factors in the indigenous

       personality lexicons of English and 11 other languages. Journal of Personality, 76,

       1001-1053.

Olesen,M.H., Thomsen, D.K., Schnieber, A. & Tonnesvang, J. (2010). Distinguishing

       general causality orientations from personality traits. Personality and Individual

       Differences, 48, 538-543.

Paunonen, S.V., & Jackson, D.N. (2000). What is beyond the Big Five? Plenty! Journal of

       Personality, 68, 821-835.

Pinto, D.G., Maltby, J. & Wood, A.M. (2011). Exploring the tripartite model of authenticity

       within Gray’s approach and inhibition systems and Cloninger’s bio-social model of

       personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 51, 194-197.

Revelle, W. (1997). Three fundamental dimensions of personality [WWW document].

       Retrieved from http://personality-project.org/perproj/theory/big3.table.html, 9th May

       2011.

Rogers, C.R. (1959). A theory of therapy, personality and interpersonal relationships, as

       developed in the client-centred framework. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: A study of a

       science (Vol. 3). New York: McGraw-Hill.

Rogers, C.R. (1965). Client-centered therapy: Its current practice, implication, and theory.

       Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Stewart, M.E., Ebmeier, K.P., & Deary, I.J. (2004). The structure of Cloninger's

   Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire in a British sample. Personality and Individual

   Differences, 36, 1403-1418.
The position of authenticity within extant models of personality                          19

Tabachnick, B.G. & Fidell, L.S. (2001). Using Multivariate Statistics (4th ed). Boston: Allyn

       and Bacon.

Wilson, K. & Gullone, E. (1999). The relationship between personality and affect over the

       lifespan. Personality and Individual Differences, 27, 1141-1156.

Wood, A.M., Linley, P.A., Maltby, J., Baliousis, M., & Joseph, S. (2008). The authentic

       personality: A theoretical and empirical conceptualization, and the development of

       the Authenticity Scale. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 55, 385-399.

Zwick, W.R., & Velicer, W.F. (1986). Comparison of five rules for determining the number

       of components to retain. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 432-442
The position of authenticity within extant models of personality                      20

                                           Tables

Table 1

Reliability Statistics and Maximum Likelihood Factor Analysis with Promax Rotation of all

the variables.

                                                         Factors

                                          1       2      3       4      5      6

Five Factor Model

Neuroticism                     .79      .01    -.04    .01    .87     -.05   -.01

Extraversion                    .82      .07    .73     .18    .05     .24    -.21

Openness to Experience          .79     -.03    .29     -.08   .04     .19    .71

Agreeableness                   .84     -.01    -.15    .79    -.02    -.11   .07

Conscientiousness               .81     -.05    -.19    -.05   .02     .71    .08

Gray

BIS                             .77     -.03    -.01    .02    .50     .03    .02

BAS Fun Seeking                 .73      .06    .58     -.10   -.04    -.11   .18

BAS Drive                       .80      .01    .48     -.01   -.05    -.17   .12

BAS Reward Responsiveness       .76     -.02    .53     -.08   -.04    -.12   .11

Cloninger

Novelty Seeking                 .87     -.04    .58     -.03   .01     -.08   .18

Harm Avoidance                  .91      .05    -.04    -.03   .71     -.05   .04

Reward Dependence               .85     -.06    .16     .86    -.01    .05    -.06

Persistence                     .81     -.02    -.18    -.10   .01     .72    .11

Self Direction                  .83      .04    .05     -.05   -.13    .41    .06
The position of authenticity within extant models of personality                   21

Cooperativeness                 .82      .01    -.11    .70    .02   -.12   .09

Self-Transcendence              .75      .04    .11     .28    .02   .03    .58

Honesty-Humility

Honesty-Humility                .93      .52    -.14    .16    .09   .19    .11

Self-Determination Theory

Autonomous                      .81      .67    .07    -.02   -.14   .01    .03

Controlled                      .78     -.36    .28    -.05    .11   -.07   -.04

Impersonal                      .79     -.24    .06    -.06    .28   -.08   .06

Authenticity

Authentic Living                .71      .79    -.05    .04   -.01   -.08   .05

External Influence              .73     -.86    -.04    .11   -.06   .04    .02

Self Alienation                 .74     -.85    -.12    .06   -.07   .03    .11
Authenticity …22

Table 2

Reliability Statistics and Factor Loadings From Three Multi-group Factor Analyses

  Latent Factor               Variable              Sample              Sample             Gender                 Ethnicity

                                                  S1   S2    S3     S1      S2      S3     Men Women         Asian Black White

Honesty-Humility   Self Alienation                .73 .76 .73       -.80 -.80       -.79   -.82    -.81       -.83      -.86      -.76

                   External Influence             .75 .78 .77       -.79 -.80       -.80   -.82    -.77       -.80      -.84      -.78

                   Authentic Living               .71 .74 .73        .74    .75     .77    .75         .72     .70       .81       .75

                   Honesty-Humility               .90 .91 .88        .58    .60     .53    .59         .62     .58       .67       .56

                   Controlled                     .77 .75 .74       -.38 -.35       -.53   -.41    -.43       -.39      -.48      -.43

                   Autonomous                     .70 .79 .72        .62    .63     .73    .69         .66     .62       .74       .67

Neuroticism        Neuroticism                    .72 .75 .71        .90    .91     .91    .88         .88     .89       .88       .90

                   Harm Avoidance                 .90 .88 .85        .72    .71     .72    .68         .75     .74       .77       .69

                   BIS                            .74 .81 .73        .57    .60     .57    .48         .60     .46       .67       .60

                   Impersonal                     .70 .76 .77        .41    .38     .29    .29         .42     .39       .38       .33

Extraversion       Extraversion                   .73 .80 .70        .29    .48     .37    .28         .36     .28       .44       .40

                   BAS Fun-Seeking                .75 .74 .72        .68    .66     .69    .66         .63     .66       .68       .67
Authenticity …23

                  BAS Drive                .80 .74 .77    .60   .64   .55   .65   .61    .58    .67       .61

                  BAS Reward Responsiveness .78 .79 .71   .70   .68   .63   .59   .63    .56    .70       .69

                  Novelty Seeking          .84 .75 .79    .56   .68   .68   .64   .67    .59    .67       .67

Agreeableness     Reward Dependence        .77 .77 .82    .75   .78   .71   .73   .69    .66    .77       .78

                  Cooperativeness          .88 .85 .79    .63   .60   .67   .59   .45    .57    .69       .59

                  Agreeableness            .76 .73 .77    .78   .78   .72   .69   .76    .85    .69       .75

Conscientiousness Conscientiousness        .73 .72 .78    .74   .79   .77   .72   .78    .82    .68       .78

                  Self Direction           .79 .82 .83    .36   .44   .41   .23   .40    .44    .45       .35

                  Persistence              .70 .78 .75    .88   .84   .82   .81   .81    .79    .91       .84

Openness          Openness                 .76 .82 .74    .69   .61   .18   .79   .76   1.00    .49       .44

                  Self-Transcendence       .81 .76 .81    .82   .68 1.00    .58   .71    .24   1.00      1.00
You can also read