Screen-time influences children's mental imagery performance - Eliant.eu
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
Received: 7 August 2019 | Revised: 12 March 2020 | Accepted: 17 April 2020 DOI: 10.1111/desc.12978 PAPER Screen-time influences children's mental imagery performance Sebastian P. Suggate | Philipp Martzog Department of Educational Sciences, University of Regensburg, Regensburg, Abstract Germany Mental imagery is a foundational human faculty that depends on active image con- Correspondence struction and sensorimotor experiences. However, children now spend a significant Sebastian P. Suggate, Department of proportion of their day engaged with screen-media, which (a) provide them with Educational Science, Universitaetsstr. 31, 93040 Regensburg, Germany. ready-made mental images, and (b) constitute a sensory narrowing whereby input Email: sebastian.suggate@ur.de is typically focused on the visual and auditory modalities. Accordingly, we test the Funding information idea that screen-time influences the development of children's mental imagery with a Software - AG Stiftung, Grant/Award focus on mental image generation and inspection from the visual and haptic domains. Number: ER-P 11657 In a longitudinal cross-lagged panel design, children (n = 266) aged between 3 and 9 years were tested at two points in time, 10 months apart. Measures of screen-time and mental imagery were employed, alongside a host of control variables including working memory, vocabulary, demographics, device ownership, and age of exposure to screen-media. Findings indicate a statistically significant path from screen-time at time 1 to mental imagery at time 2, above and beyond the influence of the control variables. These unique findings are discussed in terms of the influence of screen- time on mental imagery. KEYWORDS cognitive development, electronic media, mental imagery, mental simulation, screen-media, screen-time 1 | I NTRO D U C TI O N hallmark features of screen-time—almost regardless of whether the device is a television, smartphone, or computer—are, firstly, its degree of passivity regarding its provision of mental images and, secondly, TV provides the viewer with ready-made visual im- its sensory narrowing. Beginning with passivity, the images provided ages and thus does not provide viewers with practice by screens can generally be described as “ready-made” in that they in generating their own visual images. are provided directly via the screen media. Accordingly, it could be (Valkenburg & van der Voort, 1994, p. 317) surmised that they may not require active image construction, other- wise typical in mental life such as when reading text (i.e. the reduction Children spend a significant proportion of their time operat- hypothesis, see Valkenburg & van der Voort, 1994). Second, during ing, viewing, and engaging with screen devices such as televisions, screen-time sensory input is typically dominated by two modalities, computers, game consoles, tablets and smartphones—sometimes namely the visual and auditory, presumably somewhat to the detri- in excess of 4 hr/day (Gingold, Simon, & Schoendorf, 2014; Hinkley, ment of others (e.g. tactile, proprioceptive, visceroceptive, and even Salmon, Okely, Crawford, & Hesketh, 2012; Rideout, 2017). Two olfactive and gustative). This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivs License, which permits use and distribution in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited, the use is non-commercial and no modifications or adaptations are made. © 2020 The Authors. Developmental Science published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd Developmental Science. 2020;00:e12978. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/desc | 1 of 13 https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12978
2 of 13 | SUGGATE and MARTZOG It is now indisputable that sensory experience provides human cognition with not only input, but impetus for its development Research Highlights (Lewkowicz, 2000), with mental imagery and thought depending • Mental imagery lies at the heart of mental life and re- on activation of sensory areas extending beyond the visual and au- quires both active image generation and a broad range ditory modalities (Connell & Lynott, 2012; James, 2010; Martzog & of sensorimotor experiences. Suggate, 2019; Wellsby & Pexman, 2014). Although previous work has • Screen media provide children with ready-made and considered passivity during screen-time (Valkenburg & van der Voort, visually dominated mental images, hence may reduce 1994), research has neglected the question of whether screen-time's multimodal mental imagery. narrower sensory input affects an important aspect of cognitive de- • Using a longitudinal cross-lagged design with 266 chil- velopment, namely, mental imagery. However, it is an open question as dren we tested the effect of screen-time on mental im- to whether screen-time suppresses mental imagery (i.e. reduction hy- agery, controlling for a host of variables. pothesis) or potentially stimulates imagery by training rapid processing • Greater screen-time linked to reduced mental imagery in of images (i.e. the stimulation hypothesis). To address this tantalizing children. question, we use a longitudinal cross-lagged design to examine the ef- fect of screen-time on children's mental imagery. the home environment, on features of cognitive development. The 1.1 | Active and passive screen-media second research direction relates to work seeking to enhance chil- dren's learning and development by using media. A number of studies have investigated links between screen-time and aspects of cognitive, academic, and child development (Allchorne, Cooper, & Simpson, 2017). Screen-time here is defined as time spent 1.2.1 | General cognitive development viewing content displayed and projected from active and passive screen-media, namely, those media that present visual information on Despite attention-grabbing headlines such as “screentime is mak- two dimensional displays. This encompasses traditional media (e.g. tel- ing kids, moody, crazy and lazy” (Dunckley, 2015), research actually evision and Personal Computers) and new media, such as smartphones often lacks consistency of findings and often concrete theoreti- and game-consoles. The defining feature of these media is that they cal mechanisms linking screen-time to specific outcomes, particu- convey sensory experiences primarily via the visual, but also auditory larly in the case of mental imagery. Turning to findings, some have senses, with only minor stimulation of other sensory modalities. linked screen-time and eye-problems (Rosenfield, 2011), obesity Importantly for the current paper, the advent of active screen-me- through impoverished physical movement (Fitzpatrick, Pagani, & dia, such as touch-screens and media requiring direct interaction in Barnett, 2012; Walsh et al., 2018), blue light exposure and sleep de- shaping the course of subsequent media content (e.g. game-con- ficiencies (Dworak, Schierl, Bruns, & Strüder, 2007), and academic soles), requires careful consideration in comparison with more pas- achievement through reduced time for formal learning (Beentjes sive media (i.e. television viewing). First, these active screen-media & van der Voort, 1988; Hancox, Milne, & Poulton, 2005; Weis & generally require the input of participants in shaping the course of Cerankosky, 2010). the images provided by the screens. For example, the course taken Turning specifically to cognitive development, findings are in role-play computer games depends on active input, as does mixed. Beginning with general developmental indicators, some re- word-processing, taking photos, chatting, and so forth. Second, the search indicates a small detrimental effect of excessive screen-time haptic and fine-motor system is also active in delivering this input on achieving developmental milestones (Madigan, Browne, Racine, (e.g. pressing buttons), although in a comparatively impoverished Mori, & Tough, 2019). Some studies also find that language develop- form due to the sensory homogeneity of touch screens or keys (e.g. ment in infancy is negatively affected by screen-time (Chonchaiya Hipp et al., 2017). As discussed below, such active screen-media may & Pruksananonda, 2008; Zimmerman, Christakis, & Meltzoff, 2007), activate mental imagery in a different way to passive screen-media others that young children do not acquire new words from screen (i.e. television). For the purpose of the current paper, we exclude media (Krcmar, Grela, & Lin, 2007; Robb, Richert, & Wartella, 2009), new media, such as 3D interactive technologies, because these are while others still demonstrate that educational content can impart not in widespread use and little research exists on these. vocabulary (Rice, Huston, Truglio, & Writhgt, 1990) and narrative skill (Linebarger & Piotrowski, 2009; Linebarger & Vaala, 2010; Linebarger, 2005). However, an understanding as to why screen- 1.2 | Screen-time and cognitive development time might differentially affect language development is incomplete, although studies suggest that transferring from virtual to real worlds At a general level, research on the effect of screen-time on cognitive can be difficult for infants (Hipp et al., 2017). development includes two sets of studies. The first group concerns Executive functions have been examined more extensively. broad effects of screen-time, usually for entertainment purposes in One set of findings suggests that screen-time, particularly in the
SUGGATE and MARTZOG | 3 of 13 form of interactive video games, can enhance cognitive control in argue in the next section, research has perhaps overlooked one key adults (Anguera et al., 2013; Powers, Brooks, Aldrich, Palladino, & feature of screen-media: such media present children with rapidly Alfieri, 2013). Other studies find that the multitasking and rapid changing pre-made sensory images that are typically specific to the changes inherent in screen-time negatively affect executive func- visual and auditory modalities alone. This might in turn influence the tions in both adults (Ophir, Nass, & Wagner, 2009) and children mental simulation of external events (i.e. mental imagery). (Lillard & Peterson, 2011; Nathanson, Aladé, Sharp, Rasmussen, & Christy, 2014). Furthermore, screen-time has been linked to in- creased symptomology associated with attention-deficit hyperac- 1.3 | Mental imagery tivity disorder (Nikkelen, Valkenburg, Huizinga, & Bushman, 2014). However, further confusing the picture, a cross-sectional study Visual imagery has been described as seeing with the mind's eye from China found that screen-time positively linked to preschool (Kosslyn, 1994) and the close cousin thereof, namely mental imagery, children's executive functions (Yang, Chen, Wang, & Zhu, 2017). can be understood as simulation or internal re-creation of percep- Another study using a large Australian sample found that media tual experience (Barsalou, 1999). Mental imagery can be conceived exposure at age 2 years, but not age 4, negatively related to later of as containing four stages, image generation, maintenance, scan- self-regulation (Cliff, Howard, Radesky, McNeill, & Vella, 2018). ning, and transformation (Kosslyn, Margolis, Barrett, Goldknopf, & Daly, 1990). Developmental effects also exist, with children being relatively poorer at generating, scanning, manipulating, or transform- 1.2.2 | Enhancing learning through screen-time ing images (Kosslyn et al., 1990). In addition, studies have shown that sensory and motor systems underlie mental imagery (e.g., Martzog & On the other hand, a raft of approaches and studies demonstrate Suggate, 2019), as has been found in other domains such as memory, that, depending on age and content, children and adults can success- language, and thought (Barsalou, 2008). fully learn from screen-media (Barr & Linebarger, 2017). Generally, Indeed, various theoretical approaches argue that sensory and these approaches seek to capitalize on and enhance learning pro- sensorimotor experiences form the basis of mental imagery and cog- cesses via a number of techniques, sources, and strategies (Troseth, nition. For instance, in embodied cognition theories, cognitive pro- Strouse, Flores, Stuckelman, & Russo Johnson, 2020). When the cesses have been described as resulting from an internal simulation goals are clear and the program is well-designed, even passive of underlying actions and perceptions (Barsalou, 2008; Glenberg media (i.e. television) can enhance school readiness, problem solv- & Gallese, 2012; Glenberg et al., 2008). According to perceptual ing, and learning (see Kirkorian, Wartella, & Anderson, 2008). At a symbols theory, Barsalou (1999) characterizes simulations as “the theoretical level, well-designed programs could invoke established top-down activation of sensory-motor areas” (p. 641). Re-enacted learning principles, such as social learning, capturing and sustain- perceptual experiences appear to bear close similarities to the ex- ing attention, encouraging mental model development, reinforcing, periences behind mental imagery (Kosslyn, 1994). Both simulation facilitating explorative learning and knowledge elaboration (Barr & theory (Jeannerod, 2001), and emulation theory of representation Linebarger, 2017; Hattie, 2012; Kirkorian et al., 2008). (Grush, 2004), make the claim that motor and visual images are anal- Specifically, prompts provided by interactive electronic books ogous to real-world physical and visual experiences because they can support learning (Strouse & Ganea, 2016, 2017), especially for make use of similar neural infrastructure as indicated by motor and low SES families (Troseth et al., 2020) and including a social model visual cortex activation during imagery (see Jeannerod, 2001 for a (e.g. a parent) as a co-viewer can further enhance gains (Strouse, review and Kosslyn, Ganis, & Thompson, 2001; Tomasino, Werner, Troseth, O'Doherty, & Saylor, 2018). Pertinent for the current line Weiss, & Fink, 2007). of inquiry, children can experience difficulty deriving three-dimen- sional information from two-dimensional media, which implicates both children's sensorimotor systems and, as discussed next, mental 1.4 | Links between screen-time and imagery in learning and cognitive development (Troseth, 2010). mental imagery Although not directly investigating links between screen-time 1.2.3 | Summary and mental imagery, as here defined, there have been studies on links between television and day-dreaming/creative imagination Indeed, taking research on screen-time and cognitive development (Valkenburg & Peter, 2013; Valkenburg & van der Voort, 1994, 1995). as a whole, we, along with others (e.g., Troseth, 2010), note that clear Consistent with the reduction hypothesis, studies have found that and plausible theoretical mechanisms need to be carefully tested children perform more poorly on measures of creative and divergent with ecologically valid designs amenable to causal interpretation, production after viewing a television versus hearing a radio pro- namely longitudinal cross-lagged panel designs (Kearney, 2017). gram (Valkenburg & Beentjes, 1997). Furthermore, in a study with a Effects appear to be context (Hirsh-Pasek et al., 2015) and develop- large sample of children and using a cross-lagged design, television mentally dependent (e.g., Barr & Linebarger, 2017). However, as we viewing affected both the content of day-dreaming and reduced its
4 of 13 | SUGGATE and MARTZOG occurrence (Valkenburg & van der Voort, 1995). On the other hand, time at time 1, and screen time at time 2 from mental imagery at time consistent with the stimulation hypothesis, rapid processing of 1, while accounting for control variables. A pattern consistent with screen-images might stimulate the perceptual system (Dye, Green, the unidirectional causal operation of screen-time on mental imag- & Bavelier, 2009), perhaps explaining why some work has found indi- ery would be found if the diagonal pathway from screen-time (t1) to cations that video-gaming can support information processing (Dye mental imagery (t2) were significant, but the converse pathway were et al., 2009; Powers et al., 2013). not, indicating unidirectional influences as opposed to bidirectional association (Kearney, 2017). Furthermore, the design permitted us to control for a host of the- 1.5 | The current study oretically important control variables beyond parental demographic data and including working memory and vocabulary. The latter two As outlined and defined here, two features define mental im- are key covariates because working memory is intimately related to agery. First, mental imagery constitutes activity in the form of executive functions, which, along with vocabulary, have been found image generation, maintenance, scanning, and transformation to relate to screen-time usage. Additionally, we used a novel men- (Kosslyn et al., 1990). Second, mental imagery depends on broader tal imagery measure, namely a mental comparison task, designed to sensorimotor simulations and experiences (Barsalou, 1999; specifically target the sensorimotor foundations of mental imagery Kosslyn, 1994). Two functional properties of screen media bear (Martzog & Suggate, 2019), that generates response accuracy and close examination. response latency. First, screen-media provide images that presumably somewhat In accordance with the reduction hypothesis, we expected that circumvent the effortful construction processes required during mental imagery performance (accuracy) would be lower in children mental imagery, which has been called the reduction hypothesis exposed to more screen-time because they have less experience (Valkenburg & van der Voort, 1994). Conceivably, various screen-me- with the active creation of their own mental images. Theoretically, it dia might differentially result in a reduction in mental imagery, for is conceivable that screen-media train rapid processing of mental im- instance if during screen-time children anticipate, or reflect on, con- ages that have been provided by screens, perhaps leading to greater tent, then some mental imagery might be employed. Furthermore, if mental imagery processing speeds for familiar images. Additionally, actions are to be planned and executed via screen-media, it is likely previous work has found that screen-time increases children's impul- that mental imagery of subsequent actions would be stimulated sivity (Lillard & Peterson, 2011; Nikkelen et al., 2014) and process- more so with active than passive media. Accordingly, it might be ex- ing speed (Dye et al., 2009). Accordingly, we tentatively expected pected that screen-media generally would reduce the active gener- screen-time to result in faster response latencies, consistent with ation of mental images, but that active screen-media might have less the stimulation hypothesis (Valkenburg & van der Voort, 1994). of a detrimental effect on imagery compared to passive media. Finally, we tested whether passive and active screen-media differ- Second, as mentioned, screen-time represents a sensory narrow- entially relate to mental imagery, reasoning that the added activity ing, in that two modalities (i.e. the visual and auditory) are likely stim- (i.e. planning and executing actions) inherent in active screen-media ulated while other broader sensorimotor experiences (e.g. motor, means that active screen-time may not relate negatively to mental haptic, proprioceptive) are suppressed. Again, it may be important imagery. to distinguish between passive and active screen-media, in that the latter involve some direct motor action (Galetzka, 2017). However, given the homogeneity of motor action when interacting with flat 2 | M E TH O D screens or analogous buttons—which by nature vary little in terms of haptic or proprioceptive feedback—screen time likely results in com- 2.1 | Participants paratively impoverished sensorimotor experiences otherwise to be expected in childhood (e.g. outdoor play, block games). Indeed, men- Participants in this study were 266 children (51.1% girls) aged be- tal imagery, which is itself a fundamental building block of thought tween 35 and 120 months (M = 75.26, SD = 20.05) at the first time and cognition (Barsalou, 1999; Kosslyn, Ganis, & Thompson, 2003), point, attending either preschools (n = 141) or primary schools depends on both broader sensorimotor experience and active image (n = 125) in a small city in Germany. Thirty-two percent of children generation. Thus, it would appear pressing to investigate the effect had at least one parent born in a foreign country and 26.3% spoke that children's screen-time experiences have on their mental imag- a language other than German at home. Aside from German, there ery performance. However, to date, no study has directly investi- was no clear majority amongst the home languages spoken, with gated this. these being a mixture of Eastern European and Asiatic languages. Accordingly, we conducted a longitudinal cross-lagged panel Additionally, 56.4% of the families had at least one parent with a study measuring 266 preschool and primary school children's mental University degree or equivalent. The national average for individual imagery and screen-time use at two points in time, 10 months apart. adults (and hence not directly comparable) in a similar age range to Crucially, our use of a cross-lagged panel design has the key advan- the parents is 29% for this generation (Federal Bureau of Statistics, tage that mental imagery at time 2 can be predicted from screen 2017), indicating that the sample was likely more highly educated.
SUGGATE and MARTZOG | 5 of 13 2.2 | Measures smartphone, game-console), giving a theoretically possible score range from 4 to 32. Demographic data were collected via a parent questionnaire. Parents were asked about languages spoken at home, educational background, home country, screen-time usage, device ownership, 2.2.2 | Mental imagery and first contact with media. We employed a mental imagery task based on previously used men- tal size comparisons tasks (Moyer, 1973; Paivio, 1975), that has been 2.2.1 | Screen-media questionnaire recently utilized and further developed (Martzog & Suggate, 2019). Pertinent to the task was that children needed to rely on information A parent questionnaire was used to measure children's screen- derived from the mental images themselves, not declarative knowl- time and media usage. Given notorious difficulties in measuring edge about the images. Children were asked to imagine two specific screen-time, in part due to information bias and social desirability, objects, and then asked to make a judgment as to which from the tar- we optimized our method over the course of several pilot stud- get and distractor item was better encapsulated by a sensory feature ies. At a theoretical level, measures involving diaries have been (i.e. “which is shinier, [a] trumpet or [a] violin?”). Note that the ques- recommended in preference to questionnaires because these are tion was thus phrased, such that the stimuli appear at the end so that thought to provide more accurate estimates (Reinsch, Ennemoser, processing can only begin after presentation. Also, in German, the & Schneider, 1999). However, one key disadvantage with diaries indefinite article “a” was not grammatically necessary in the ques- is low-compliance. To address these issues, we opted for a diary- tion sentence, thus reducing memory load between presentation of questionnaire format in which parents were asked about their chil- the two target stimuli. The invoked modalities were determined by dren's screen time activities at different points in the day. Thus, two conditions, firstly the question contained an adjective pertain- during the working week, we asked about usage before school/ ing to the modality (e.g. “shiny”) and secondly, the target words had preschool, in the afternoon, and in the evening, and then on the a sensory feature that was prominent in the corresponding modality weekend. Additionally, we asked about the amount of time spent (e.g. “trumpet”). Although the original task contained stimuli pertain- on various devices, including televisions, computers, tablets, play- ing to the haptic, visual, and visual-haptic modalities, analyses found consoles, and smartphones. Thereby, we responded to previous that the task was best conceptualized as a general imagery measure work calling for a focus on more modern media in addition to tele- (Martzog & Suggate, 2019). During task development, Martzog and vision (Valkenburg & Peter, 2013). Because of our hypotheses that Suggate (2019) accounted for diverse lexical features (e.g. length, screen-time affects imagery via sensory-narrowing, we also asked syllabic structure, frequency, imageability, manipulability, sensory parents how old children were when they first began using the ratings). various appliances to determine the effect of long-term exposure. Response accuracy and latency were both recorded by the ex- Finally, we also included questions asking about the ownership of perimenter using response keys on a laptop. In total there were 34 electronic media appliances. items, each containing a distractor and a target and children were Accordingly, our data provided three scores: (a) device owner- asked to respond as soon as they knew the answer without, how- ship, (b) daily exposure time, and (c) age at which exposure began. ever, emphasizing speed in order to avoid hectic and erratic re- Screen-time was rated on a 6-point Likert scale for each medium (no sponses. Response accuracy and response time was recorded and screen-time,
6 of 13 | SUGGATE and MARTZOG 2.2.4 | Vocabulary TA B L E 1 Descriptive statistics for control variables, screen- time, and mental imagery Children's vocabulary was assessed using the vocabulary test at time Descriptive statistics 1 from the Kaufmann ABC (Kaufman & Kaufman, 2015). In this task, Variables M SD N Min Max children are shown pictures and are required to name the object in the pictures. One point was awarded for each correct item and there Control variables was a discontinue rule after 4 consecutive errors, and a basal item Age (months) 75.26 20.05 259 35 120 was established after three correct responses. The maximum num- Vocabulary 19.02 5.28 255 4.00 37.00 ber of points possible was 39 and the internal consistency of the Working 2.44 1.28 248 0 4.00 vocabulary test was estimated at α = 0.89. memory Screen exposure 23.18 3.90 255 11.00 32.00 (age) 2.3 | Procedure Device 5.68 1.173 255 1.00 7.00 ownership Children were tested twice on the screen-time, imagery, and some Time 1 variables of the control variables, on average 9.81 (SD = 1.33) months apart, Mental imagery 26.86 5.36 254 2.00 34.00 (acc) once in the academic year of 2017–2018 and again in 2018–2019, in Mental imagery 2,589 1,068 254 745 10,023 their educational institutions. Data were drawn from a larger longi- speed (ms) tudinal study in progress. All tasks were administered individually by Screen-time 1.87 1.43 237 0.00 9.14 trained research assistants and the second author according to test Time 2 variables instructions. Parents completed questionnaires, at two time points parallel to data collection, providing information on their children's Screen-time 1.52 1.11 197 0.00 5.93 screen-time and demographic data. For preschool children, between Mental imagery 25.62 4.97 250 2.00 32.00 (acc) two and three testing sessions were required, each of approximately Mental imagery 2,327 1,494 250 487 13,105 20 min, so as to not overtax concentration spans. Approval from the speed (ms) Ministry of Education was obtained prior to conducting the study and written consent was provided by the parents of participating children, followed by the latter's verbal assent. Finally, although in our mental imagery task we did not directly ask children to respond as quickly and accurately as possible, the data represent a double challenge in that response time is not inde- 2.4 | Data analyses pendent of response accuracy. Accordingly, we treated these two variables separately, reasoning that response speed—regardless of Data analyses consisted of first screening the data for anomalies accuracy—provided one source of information about how partici- (skew and kurtosis) and calculating descriptive statistics. We win- pants approached the task (i.e. the stimulation hypothesis) and that sorized the data by capping outliers on mental imagery reaction time response accuracy to another (i.e. the reduction hypothesis). to the three standard deviations above the item level mean. Next, we conducted correlation analyses to explore relations between the exogenous and endogenous variables central to the cross-lagged 3 | R E S U LT S panel design and path modeling. Path models allowed us to conduct regression analysis (Kline, 2011) testing for cross-lagged effects con- 3.1 | Descriptive statistics sistent with the causal operation of screen-time on mental imagery (Reinders, 2006), controlling for the influences of a host of varia- In Table 1 the descriptive statistics for scores on the screen-time, bles (Byrne, 2010; Kline, 2011). Path models were calculated using mental imagery, and control variables are presented. Inspection of AMOS v. 23 (Arbuckle, 2014) with missing values being given full skew and kurtosis statistics suggested that data were normally dis- consideration through full maximum likelihood estimation. Screen- tributed, however, response latency to the imagery items appeared time and mental imagery were modeled as manifest variables to fa- to be right skewed (skewedness in the vicinity of 2.50). Of the im- cilitate model convergence and the control variables (presented in agery data, 4.5% was missing at time 1 and 6.0% at time 2, with the Table 2) were added as predictors with paths onto both time 1 and corresponding percentages for the screen-time data being 10.9% time 2 screen-time and mental imagery. Control variables included and 25.94%. Next correlation coefficients were calculated for the parent education, ethnic status, device ownership, age of exposure variables in Table 1, which are presented in Table 2. Trends indicate to screen-media, vocabulary, working memory, chronological age, that screen-time correlated negatively with accuracy, as did re- and, to control for variations in testing procedure, the number of sponse speed on the imagery task. Vocabulary and working memory months between time 1 and time 2. positively predicted mental imagery and were generally negatively
SUGGATE and MARTZOG | 7 of 13 TA B L E 2 Product-moment and partial correlation coefficients between screen-time, control, and mental imagery variables 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 1 Vocabulary — 0.38* 0.08 0.28* −0.12 0.55* −0.25* −0.21* 0.36* −0.19* 2 Working memory 0.61* — 0.08 0.19* −0.14* 0.34* −0.15* −0.02 0.23* −0.02 3 Screen exposure 0.05 0.04 — −0.13* −0.36* 0.13* 0.01 −0.27* 0.14* −0.05 (age) 4 Device ownership 0.30* 0.22* −0.13* — 0.06 0.17* 0.01 −0.06 0.22* −0.04 5 Screen-time t1 0.02 0.05 −0.35* 0.08 — −0.05 0.01 0.64* −0.21* −0.02 6 Mental imagery 0.71* 0.63* 0.08 0.20* 0.10 — −0.26* −0.12 0.35* −0.20* t1 (acc) 7 Mental imagery −0.34* −0.29* 0.01 −0.02 −0.04 −0.36* — −0.01 −0.12 0.27* speed t1 (ms) 8 Screen-time t2 −0.04 0.15* −0.27* −0.03 0.66* 0.07 −0.07 — −0.17* −0.04 9 Mental imagery 0.54* 0.51* 0.10 0.25* −0.06 0.58* −0.24* −0.01 — −0.03 t2 (acc) 10 Mental imagery −0.34* −0.26* −0.04 −0.08 −0.09 −0.37* 0.33* −0.13 −0.21* — speed t2 (ms) 11 Age t1 0.55* 0.69* −0.03 0.12 0.21* 0.65* −0.26* 0.24* 0.53* −0.35* Note: Correlations above the diagonal have age partialled out, t1 = time 1, t2 = time 2. *p < .05. Screen- res. res. media (capital) Age- Mental imagery Mental imagery exposure (t1) β = .22* (t2) screen-time Chronologi cal age Ethnic β = -.16* status Time between t1 and t2 β = .00 Parental education Vocabulary Screen-time (t1) β = .59* Screen-time (t2) Working memory res. res. FIGURE 1 Structural equation model depicting cross-lagged panel design testing links between screen-time and mental imagery
8 of 13 | SUGGATE and MARTZOG TA B L E 3 Estimates for influence of control variables on screen-time and mental imagery from structural equation model in Figure 1 Screen-time t1 Mental imagery t1 Screen time t2 Mental imagery t2 Variable B SE β B SE β B SE β B SE Β Age (months) 0.02 0.01 0.24* 0.09 0.02 0.34* 0.01 0.01 0.14 0.06 0.02 0.23* Vocabulary −0.01 0.02 −0.08 0.40 0.06 0.39* −0.04 0.02 −0.20* 0.15 0.07 0.17* Working memory 0.00 0.10 −0.00 0.64 0.26 0.15* 0.11 0.08 0.12 0.36 0.30 0.09 Screen exposure −0.11 0.02 −0.31* 0.07 0.06 0.05 −0.03 0.02 −0.11 0.00 0.07 0.00 (age) Device ownership 0.06 0.07 0.05 0.17 0.20 0.04 −0.03 0.05 −0.03 0.48 0.22 0.11* Ethnic status 0.22 0.19 0.07 −0.45 0.50 −0.04 0.15 0.14 0.06 0.68 0.58 0.07 Parental −0.43 0.18 −0.15* 0.54 0.46 0.05 −0.01 0.13 −0.01 −0.01 0.53 0.00 education *p < .05. associated with screen-time. Age of media exposure was correlated as predictors in the model, and covarying the imagery residuals. The with screen-time and device ownership. model again showed good global fit, χ2/df = 1.54, CFI = 1.00, and RMSEA = 0.05, but the path of interest between screen-time and mental imagery response latency—although in the direction pre- 3.2 | Cross-lagged effect between screen-time and dicted by the stimulation hypothesis—was not statistically signifi- mental imagery cant, β = −0.05, p = .46, nor was the converse path from imagery to screen-time, β = −0.01, p = .91. In an alternative procedure, we (nat- We estimated two models to test links between screen-time and ural) log transformed the response latencies, which transformed the mental imagery, one each for mental imagery accuracy and mental skew and kurtosis statistics to near zero for these data; however, the imagery speed. In both instances, the control variables were speci- cross-lagged path from screen-time at time 1 to imagery response fied to predict the screen-time and mental imagery variables, which latency at time 2 was not significant, β = −0.06, p = .37, despite good contained cross-lagged paths. Beginning with accuracy, the model model fit. converged on the 9th iteration. A chi-square value estimating good- Finally, we examined the possibility of a speed accuracy trade-off ness of fit was not significant, χ2(2) = 1.34, p = .51, indicating good operating, whereby participants' response latencies were shorter at model fit. In addition to the chi-square statistic, other fit indices the expense of greater accuracy (Heitz, 2014). Product moment cor- are recommended, namely, that the CFI should be around or above relation coefficients indicated that accuracy correlated negatively CFI = 0.95, RMSEA around or below 0.05, and that χ2/df should not be with speed, r = −0.40 at time 1 and r = −0.21 at time 2, ps < .002, significant against a chi-square distribution (Byrne, 2010). The cur- thus suggesting the opposite of a speed-accuracy trade-off because 2 rent estimates indicated excellent model fit, χ /df = 0.67, CFI = 1.00, faster responders were more accurate. To control for developmen- and RMSEA = 0.000 (Byrne, 2010; Kline, 2011). As can be seen in tal influences, the partial correlation coefficients controlling for age Figure 1, accounting for the host of control variables screen-time between response latency and accuracy were calculated. At time 1, at time 1 negatively predicted mental imagery at time 2, whereas this was negative and significant, r(248) = −0.28, p < .001), indicat- the converse was not true. In Table 3, working memory, vocabulary, ing that greater accuracy was linked to greater speed, however, this and chronological age were significant predictors of mental imagery. correlation was not significant at time 2, r(240) = 0.00, p = .95. Thus, Both chronological age and age of exposure to screen media also although the data do not indicate a speed-accuracy trade-off, the predicted screen-time. To estimate the correlation between screen- previous model was re-run, this time including accuracy as a con- time and mental imagery accuracy at time 1, their residuals were trol variable, however, this did not alter the magnitude of the small, covaried. The corresponding correlation was not statistically signifi- negative, but nonsignificant path between screen-time and mental cant, r = 0.08, p = .23. imagery response latency. In a second path model, the same model was used with the ex- ception that response latency replaced response accuracy in the im- agery task. The model again showed good global fit, χ2/df = 0.64, 3.3 | Passive versus active screen-media and CFI = 1.00, and RMSEA = 0.000, but the path of interest, between mental imagery screen-time at time 1 and mental imagery response latency at time 2, was not statistically significant, β = −0.06, p = .35, nor was the To investigate links between active versus passive screen-media and converse path from imagery to screen-time, β = −0.01, p = .93. mental imagery we first examined descriptive statistics pertaining to Additionally, we attempted to partial out the influence of accuracy the daily engagement with the various media. These are presented from the mental imagery reaction time measurements by using these in Table 4. As can be seen in Table 4, television constituted the most
SUGGATE and MARTZOG | 9 of 13 heavily used screen-medium in this sample. The remaining media pathways without sacrificing ecological validity as in laboratory ex- were scarcely used and, given that they can all be classified as ac- periments (Kline, 2011). tive screen-media, were aggregated into a single measure (i.e. ac- Findings were clear in three regards. First, children who spent tive screen-media) for subsequent analysis. Next two structural path greater amounts of time using screen media showed statistically models were calculated, replicating those presented in Figure 1, significantly lower performances on mental imagery accuracy, with the exception that one was calculated for active and the other consistent with the reduction hypothesis (Valkenburg & van der for passive screen-time. As shown in Table 5, both models fitted Voort, 1994). Thus, our hypothesis that viewing screen-media, by the data well and, although the models are not depicted in full due virtue of their providing ready-made mental images that suppress to space constraints, they were highly similar to those in Figure 1. active image generation, receives initial support. Importantly, both types of screen-media at time 1 showed a similar, Second, we found virtually no difference in the negative cross statistically significant, cross-lagged link to mental imagery accuracy lagged-link between screen-time and mental imagery for media at time 2. classified as active versus passive. On the one hand, this finding is surprising because we expected that more active media would in- volve mental imagery abilities to a greater extent. However, our sup- 4 | D I S CU S S I O N position has not been verified by empirical evidence, such that it is plausible that even many more so-called active media types might We tested, for the first time, whether children's mental imagery still not involve much active imagery generation, perhaps especially abilities were affected by screen-time, the latter of which now in comparison to other typical childhood experiences (e.g. reading, constitutes a significant proportion of the mental activities that imaginative play). children engage in Gingold et al. (2014), Hinkley et al. (2012) and Third, contrary with the stimulation hypothesis, screen-time did Rideout (2017). Two features of screen-time that have scarcely been not relate to children's response latencies on the mental imagery investigated are, first, its sensory narrowing (i.e. dominance of the task, as we had posited based on previous work (Dye et al., 2009; auditory-visual modalities) and, second, its providing ready-made Lillard & Peterson, 2011; Nikkelen et al., 2014). Perhaps the dosage and often rapidly changing images which potentially suppress the of screen media here, which was less than in previous work, may active mental life (Valkenburg & van der Voort, 1994). We reasoned not have been sufficient to induce the greater impulsivity neces- that these two features of screen-time might lead to negative asso- sary to affect response latencies. In terms of our suggestion that ciations with mental imagery accuracy via the reduction hypothesis screen-media might train the perceptual system (Dye et al., 2009), and, conversely, a decrease in response latencies as predicted by the with hindsight, it could instead be argued that this likely only applies stimulation hypothesis. Furthermore, we reasoned that different (i.e. for certain kinds of games unlikely to have been systematically em- active vs. passive) screen-media might differentially affect mental ployed here—especially given that the dominant form of screen-time imagery. Finally, we tested these hypotheses using a longitudinal in this sample was television viewing. Accordingly, we tentatively cross-lagged panel design, which has the advantage of testing causal conclude that screen-time does not stimulate mental imagery per- formance when this requires mental comparisons of visual/haptic TA B L E 4 Estimated daily (hours) time spent with various media images. devices Alongside screen-time, vocabulary, working memory, and chrono- Descriptive statistics logical age were also significant predictors of mental imagery. At a con- ceptual level, our mental comparisons task required working memory Variables M SD N Min Max because the participants were required to compare mental images Time 1 to solve the task. Accordingly, we controlled in advance for work- Television 1.01 0.84 237 0.00 4.71 ing memory. Although it might be tempting to apply a similar line of Active media 0.17 0.20 237 0.00 1.14 reasoning to vocabulary's influence on mental imagery, we consider PC 0.06 0.20 237 0.00 1.64 it unlikely that children's vocabulary knowledge directly constrained Smartphone 0.18 0.45 237 0.00 4.57 task performance. Specifically, although it is true that children would Tablet 0.33 0.50 237 0.00 3.07 have to know the words in the mental comparisons task in order to Gaming console 0.10 0.32 237 0.00 2.14 be able to image them, stimuli were simple and hence could be ex- Time 2 pected to be familiar to the children (see Martzog & Suggate, 2019 for Television 0.76 0.67 197 0.00 3.93 stimuli and a discussion of this task). Instead, we suggest that children with larger vocabularies likely have richer perceptual representations Active media 0.20 0.25 195 0.00 1.30 in general (Connell & Lynott, 2016; Hargreaves, Pexman, Johnson, & PC 0.09 0.29 192 0.00 2.14 Zdrazilova, 2012; Suggate & Stoeger, 2017), which leads to greater Smartphone 0.21 0.41 192 0.00 2.36 imagery performance. Support for this idea also comes from the con- Tablet 0.32 0.48 194 0.00 3.29 tribution of age to mental imagery performance found here, which Gaming console 0.16 0.39 193 0.00 2.29 suggests that a more mature cognitive system relates to performance,
10 of 13 | SUGGATE and MARTZOG TA B L E 5 Model parameters comparing cross-lagged path for active versus passive screen-time (time 1) on mental imagery (time 2) Screen-time to Model df X2 p χ 2/df CFI RMSEA mental imagery (β) Active screen-media 2 0.10 0.95 0.05 1.00 0.00 −0.12* Passive screen-media 2 2.66 0.26 1.33 0.99 0.04 −0.11* *p < .05. extending beyond specific lexical level knowledge directly derived 4.2 | Limitations and future work from the imagery items. In the current study, we found that children spent, on aver- age, nearly 2 hr/day engaged in screen-media usage. This figure 4.1 | Theoretical and practical implications is consistent with previous work for this age group in Germany (Feierabend, Plankenhorn, & Rathgeb, 2017), but is still somewhat The current study adds to the rapidly growing body of research lower than that found in the United States, for example (Gingold looking at children's learning and development in relation to screen- et al., 2014), although more recent data from the United States also media (e.g., Herodotou, 2018). In terms of developmental work, the found a mean daily screen-media usage of 2 hr and 19 min. Reasons study's findings contribute to work suggesting that screen-time af- for this difference are speculative, but might be due to the region fects child development in a complex manner, with mental imagery in which the study was conducted, which has rural surroundings, now seemingly a factor to consider amongst others (see Barr & low levels of crime, and a culture in which unsupervised outdoor Linebarger, 2017). play is still encouraged. Presumably, conducting the study in sam- According to the current data, the, on average, 1 hr of televi- ples with greater levels of screen-time would result in greater as- sion viewing per day (ranging up to a maximum of 4 hr and 42 min) sociations with mental imagery due to reduced opportunities to across the course of nearly 10 months was enough to negatively engage in compensatory activities for the effects of screen-time. affect mental imagery accuracy at time 2. More surprisingly, the In a similar vein, in terms of statistical variance, such work might be corresponding time 1 engagement with active screen-media of especially fruitful in the United States where children spend about up to a maximum of 68 min/day—with a sample average of just twice as much time interacting with smart-phones (Rideout, 2017). 10 min/day—was enough to negatively predict time 2 mental im- Children's daily experiences appear to increasingly include agery. Turning these figures into total exposure across the course screen-time experiences, which may come at the expense of time of the study, across 10 months, it is likely that children spent, on engaged in activities that require greater levels of mental imagery average, over 300 hr watching television—with the heavy viewers (Ennemoser & Schneider, 2007; Weis & Cerankosky, 2010), such spending around 1,410 hr. In terms of the reduction hypothesis, as reading (Glenberg, Brown, & Levin, 2007) or imaginative play perhaps then it is not surprising that links between screen-time (Wallace & Russ, 2015). Accordingly, future work could expand on and mental imagery were found. Accordingly, mental imagery the current findings and test whether home reading activities, for seems to undergo continual development in the age of samples example, offset effects of screen-time and support mental imagery studied here and seems sensitive to reduced practice at the active development. In a similar vein, future work needs to examine the generation of mental images. As such, the current work is consis- sensorimotor consequences of screen-time. In the current paper, we tent with studies showing that mental processes and concepts are allude to a sensory narrowing during screen-time, in that the visual dependent on a rich array of sensorimotor information and pro- and auditory senses are stimulated whereas other sensorimotor mo- cesses (Connell & Lynott, 2016; Hargreaves et al., 2012; Suggate dalities may be neglected (i.e. proprioception, active motor control, & Stoeger, 2017). olfaction, gustation, haptics). Accordingly, future work should test The current study also extends previous work on media learn- sensorimotor development as a function of screen-time and time ing. Research has examined the conditions under which screen-me- engaged in sensorimotor activities, also studying neuroanatomical dia contribute to learning, among other factors, focusing on media changes underlying these skills (see Hutton, Dudley, Horowitz- content presentation, and children's developmental readiness Kraus, DeWitt, & Holland, 2019). (e.g., Barr & Linebarger, 2017). Although some work has examined In the current study, although we examined a host of control vari- the medium itself, for example by comparing reading from e-read- ables, recent work has discovered further factors that link to screen- ers versus books (Chang, Aeschbach, Duffy, & Czeisler, 2015), this time, such as self-regulation skills (Cliff et al., 2018) and factors lying study adds mental imagery to this already complicated picture behind familial and socioeconomic factors, such as stress. Although, (Barr & Linebarger, 2017). Conceivably, media might be tailored to our knowledge, work has not yet investigated whether these fac- to also encourage mental imagery, or in educational settings to tors link to mental imagery, the current findings need to be tempered be embedded in other activities that stimulate the sensorimotor by the fact that third variables may explain links found with screen- system, such as activities that involve outdoor experiences. time. Additionally, we utilized a single mental imagery measure
SUGGATE and MARTZOG | 11 of 13 including items targeting visual and haptic modalities. Subsequent A systematic review. Developmental Review, 44, 19–58. https://doi. org/10.1016/j.dr.2016.12.002 work should examine a broader repertoire of mental imagery, includ- Anguera, J. A., Boccanfuso, J., Rintoul, J. L., Al-Hashimi, O., Faraji, F., ing imagery pertaining to other sensory modalities and motor imag- Janowich, J., … Gazzaley, A. (2013). Video game training enhances ery (Borst, 2014). Given links between screen-time and executive cognitive control in older adults. Nature, 501(7465), 97–101. https:// functions (Lillard & Peterson, 2011), a more comprehensive battery doi.org/10.1038/nature12486 Arbuckle, J. L. (2014). Amos (Version 23.0) [Computer Program]. Chicago, extending beyond working memory might serve as an additional IL: IBM SPSS. control. Experimental work in which screen-time is manipulated, Barr, R., & Linebarger, D. N. (2017). Media exposure during infancy and controlling for media content and different levels of experience with early childhood. Cham, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing. screen media, could be used to compliment the more ecologically Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptions of perceptual symbols. Behavioral and valid longitudinal cross-lagged panel design employed here. Brain Sciences, 22, 637–660. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X9 9532147 Finally, our findings supported the reduction, but not the stim- Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, ulation, hypothesis. Concerning the latter, one might speculate that 59, 617–645. https://doi.org/10.1146/annur ev.psych.59.103006. a general increase in processing speed in the visual system from in- 093639 creased screen-time, with its often rapidly changing visual images, Beentjes, J. W. J., & van der Voort, T. H. A. (1988). Television's impact on children's reading skills: A review of research. Reading Research might cause a general processing advantage (e.g., Dye et al., 2009), Quarterly, 23, 389–413. https://doi.org/10.2307/747640 perhaps leading also to a tendency to respond rapidly due to greater Borst, G. (2014). Neural underpinnings of object mental imagery, spatial impulsivity (e.g., Lillard & Peterson, 2011). However, our analyses imagery, and motor imagery. In K. N. Ochsner & S. Kosslyn (Eds.), The at best only resulted in a small, albeit expectedly negative, pathway Oxford handbook of cognitive neuroscience, (Vol. 1, pp. 74–87). Core topics. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. between screen-time and reaction time on the imagery task. Byrne, B. M. (2010). Structural equation modeling with AMOS: Basic con- cepts, applications, and programming (2nd ed.). Multivariate applica- tions series. New York, NY: Routledge. 5 | CO N C LU S I O N Chang, A.-M., Aeschbach, D., Duffy, J. F., & Czeisler, C. A. (2015). Evening use of light-emitting eReaders negatively affects sleep, circadian tim- ing, and next-morning alertness. Proceedings of the National Academy The ability to draw forth mental images, inspecting and comparing of Sciences of the United States of America, 112, 1232–1237. https:// these, would appear to be a foundational human faculty lying at doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1418490112 the heart of cognitive functioning (Kosslyn et al., 2003). On the Chonchaiya, W., & Pruksananonda, C. (2008). Television viewing associ- ates with delayed language development. Acta Paediatrica, 97, 977– one hand, society is becoming increasingly technical and special- 982. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1651-2227.2008.00831.x ized, on the other hand, more dynamic and instable. Accordingly, Cliff, D. P., Howard, S. J., Radesky, J. S., McNeill, J., & Vella, S. A. (2018). education will need to ensure that children are creative and in- Early childhood media exposure and self-regulation: Bidirectional novative, alongside acquiring specialized skills. Mental imagery is longitudinal associations. Academic Pediatrics, 18, 813–819. https:// precisely an ability that can contribute to novel problem solving doi.org/10.1016/j.acap.2018.04.012 Connell, L., & Lynott, D. (2012). Strength of perceptual experience pre- and adaptive thinking as well as specialized skills, hence it would dicts word processing performance better than concreteness or im- seem wise to avoid compromising children's development in this ageability. Cognition, 125, 452–465. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogni regard. In this sense, the current findings that screen-time nega- tion.2012.07.010 tively affect mental imagery need to be actively replicated and Connell, L., & Lynott, D. (2016). Embodied semantic effects in visual word recognition. In M. H. Fischer & Y. Coello (Eds.), Conceptual pursued, with a focus on better understanding underlying mecha- and interactive embodiment (pp. 71–92). London, NewYork, NY: nisms, potentially leading to interventions to reduce screen-media Routledge. usage (Schmidt et al., 2012), and participation in educational ac- Dunckley, V. L. (2015). Screentime is making kids moody, crazy and lazy: tivities to foster mental imagery. 6 ways electronic screen time makes kids angry, depressed and un- motivated. Retrieved fromhttps://www.psychologytoday.com/intl/ blog/menta l -wealt h /20150 8/scree n time - is-makin g-kids-moody AC K N OW L E D G M E N T S -crazy-and-lazy This research was supported by a grant from the Software - AG Dworak, M., Schierl, T., Bruns, T., & Strüder, H. K. (2007). Impact of Stiftung awarded to the first author (grant number: ER-P 11657). We singular excessive computer game and television exposure on sleep patterns and memory performance of school-aged children. thank all participating children, parents, teachers, and schools and Pediatrics, 120, 978–985. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2007- the research assistants who helped with data collection. 0476 Dye, M. W. G., Green, C. S., & Bavelier, D. (2009). Increasing speed of pro- DATA AVA I L A B I L I T Y S TAT E M E N T cessing with action video games. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 321–326. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009. The data that support the findings of this study are available from 01660.x the corresponding author upon reasonable request. Endlich, D., Berger, N., Küspert, P., Lenhard, W., Marx, P., Weber, J., & Schneider, W. (2017). Würzburger Vorschultest: Erfassung schriftspra- REFERENCES chlicher und mathematischer (Vorläufer-)Fertigkeiten und sprachlicher Allchorne, K., Cooper, N. R., & Simpson, A. (2017). The relationship be- Kompetenzen im letzten Kindergartenjahr [Würzburger preschool test]. tween television exposure and children's cognition and behaviour: Göttingen: Hogrefe.
12 of 13 | SUGGATE and MARTZOG Ennemoser, M., & Schneider, W. (2007). Relations of television view- from the science of learning. Psychological Science in the Public ing and reading: Findings from a 4-year longitudinal study. Interest: A Journal of the American Psychological Society, 16, 3–34. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 349–368. https://doi. https://doi.org/10.1177/152910 0615569721 org/10.1037/0022-0663.99.2.349 Hutton, J. S., Dudley, J., Horowitz-Kraus, T., DeWitt, T., & Holland, Federal Bureau of Statistics [Statistisches Bundesamt]. (2017). S. K. (2019). Associations Between Screen-Based Media Use and Bildungsstand der Bevölkerung 2016 [Educationallevel of the pop- Brain White Matter Integrity in Preschool-Aged Children. JAMA ulation 2016] Retrieved from https://www.desta tis.de/DE/Publi Pediatrics, 174(1), e193869, https://doi.org/10.1001/jamap ediat kationen/Thematisch/BildungForschungKultur/Bildungsstand/Bildu rics.2019.3869 ngsstandBevoelkerung52100021670 04.pdf?blob=public ationFile James, K. H. (2010). Sensori-motor experience leads to changes in vi- Feierabend, S., Plankenhorn, T., & Rathgeb, T. (2017). Kindheit, Internet sual processing in the developing brain. Developmental Science, 13, Medien: Basisstudie zum Medienumgang 6–13-Jähriger in Deutschland. 279–288. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00883.x Stuttgart, Germany: Medienpädagogischer Forschungsverbund Jeannerod, M. (2001). Neural simulation of action: A unifying mechanism Südwest (LFK, LMK). for motor cognition. NeuroImage, 14(1 Pt 2), S103–S109. https://doi. Fitzpatrick, C., Pagani, L. S., & Barnett, T. A. (2012). Early childhood televi- org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0832 sion viewing predicts explosive leg strength and waist circumference Kaufman, A.S., & Kaufman, N.L. (2015). Kaufman assessment battery for by middle childhood. The International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition children–second edition (KABC-2). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe. and Physical Activity, 9, 87. https://doi.org/10.1186/1479-5868-9-87 Kearney, M. W. (2017). Cross-Lagged Panel Analysis. In M. Allen (Ed.), Galetzka, C. (2017). Commentary: Mobile and interactive media use by The SAGE encyclopedia of communication research methods. Thousand young children: The good, the bad, and the unknown. Frontiers in Oaks, CA, Boston, MA: SAGE Publications, Inc; Credo Reference. Psychology, 8, 461. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00461 Kirkorian, H. L., Wartella, E. A., & Anderson, D. R. (2008). Media and Gingold, J. A., Simon, A. E., & Schoendorf, K. C. (2014). Excess screen young children's learning. The Future of Children, 18, 39–61. https:// time in US children: Association with family rules and alternative ac- doi.org/10.1353/foc.0.0002 tivities. Clinical Pediatrics, 53, 41–50. https://doi.org/10.1177/00099 Kline, R. B. (2011). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. 22813498152 Methodology in the social sciences (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Glenberg, A., Brown, M., & Levin, J. (2007). Enhancing comprehension Press. Retrieved from http://site.ebrary.com/lib/acade miccomplet in small reading groups using a manipulation strategy. Contemporary etitles/home.action Educational Psychology, 32, 389–399. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Kosslyn, S. M. (1994). The resolution of the imagery debate. Cambridge, cedpsych.2006.03.00 UK: MIT Press. Glenberg, A. M., & Gallese, V. (2012). Action-based language: A theory Kosslyn, S. M., Ganis, G., & Thompson, W. L. (2001). Neural foundations of language acquisition, comprehension, and production. Cortex, 48, of imagery. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 635–642. https://doi. 905–922. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2011.04.010 org/10.1038/35090055 Glenberg, A. M., Sato, M., Cattaneo, L., Riggio, L., Palumbo, D., & Buccino, Kosslyn, S. M., Ganis, G., & Thompson, W. L. (2003). Mental imagery: G. (2008). Processing abstract language modulates motor system Against the nihilistic hypothesis. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 109– activity. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 905–919. 111. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00025-1 https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210701625550 Kosslyn, S. M., Margolis, J. A., Barrett, A. M., Goldknopf, E. J., & Daly, P. Grush, R. (2004). The emulation theory of representation: Motor control, F. (1990). Age differences in imagery abilities. Child Development, 61, imagery, and perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 377–396. 995. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130871 https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X04 000093 Krcmar, M., Grela, B., & Lin, K. (2007). Can toddlers learn vocabulary Hancox, R. J., Milne, B. J., & Poulton, R. (2005). Association of televi- from television? An experimental approach. Media Psychology, 10, sion viewing during childhood with poor educational achievement. 41–63. https://doi.org/10.1080/15213260701300931 Archives of Pediatric Adolescent Medicine, 159, 614–618. https://doi. Lewkowicz, D. J. (2000). The development of intersensory temporal org/10.1001/archpedi.159.7.614 perception: An epigenetic systems/limitations view. Psychological Hargreaves, I. S., Pexman, P. M., Johnson, J. C., & Zdrazilova, L. (2012). Bulletin, 126, 281–308. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.126. Richer concepts are better remembered: Number of features ef- 2.281 fects in free recall. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6. https://doi. Lillard, A. S., & Peterson, J. (2011). The immediate impact of different org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00073 types of television on young children's executive function. Pediatrics, Hattie, J. (2012). Visible learning for teachers: Maximizing impact on learn- 128, 644–649. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2010-1919 ing. London, UK; New York, NY: Routledge Taylor & Francis Group. Linebarger, D. L., & Piotrowski, J. T. (2009). TV as storyteller: How ex- Heitz, R. P. (2014). The speed-accuracy tradeoff: History, physiology, posure to television narratives impacts at-risk preschoolers' story methodology, and behavior. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 8, 150. https:// knowledge and narrative skills. British Journal of Developmental doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2014.00150 Psychology, 27, 47–69. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151008x4 00445 Herodotou, C. (2018). Young children and tablets: A systematic review Linebarger, D. L., & Vaala, S. E. (2010). Screen media and language de- of effects on learning and development. Journal of Computer Assisted velopment in infants and toddlers: An ecological perspective. Learning, 34, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcal.12220 Developmental Review, 30, 176–202. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Hinkley, T., Salmon, J., Okely, A. D., Crawford, D., & Hesketh, K. (2012). dr.2010.03.006 Preschoolers' physical activity, screen time, and compliance with Linebarger, D. L., & Walker, D. (2005). Infants' and toddlers' television recommendations. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 44, viewing and language outcomes. American Behavioral Scientist, 48, 458–465. https://doi.org/10.1249/MSS.0b013e318233763b 624–645. https://doi.org/10.1177/000276 4204271505 Hipp, D., Gerhardstein, P., Zimmermann, L., Moser, A., Taylor, G., & Barr, Madigan, S., Browne, D., Racine, N., Mori, C., & Tough, S. (2019). R. (2017). The dimensional divide: Learning from TV and touch- Association between screen time and children's performance on a screens during early childhood. In R. Barr & D. N. Linebarger (Eds.), developmental screening test. JAMA Pediatrics, 173(3), 244. https:// Media exposure during infancy and early childhood. Cham, Switzerland: doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2018.5056 Springer International Publishing. Martzog, P., & Suggate, S. (2019). Fine motor skills and mental imagery: Is Hirsh-Pasek, K., Zosh, J. M., Golinkoff, R. M., Gray, J. H., Robb, M. B., & it all in the mind? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 186, 59–72. Kaufman, J. (2015). Putting education in "educational" apps: Lessons https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2019.05.002
You can also read