CONVERGENT/DIVERGENT COGNITIVE STYLES AND MATHEMATICAL PROBLEM SOLVING
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
JOURNAL OF SCIENCE AND MATHEMATICS EDUCATION IN S.E. ASIA Vol. XXIV, No. 2 CONVERGENT/DIVERGENT COGNITIVE STYLES AND MATHEMATICAL PROBLEM SOLVING Hassan Alamolhodaei Ferdowsi University of Mashhad Mashhad, Iran Students’ approaches to mathematical problem solving vary greatly. In particular, variations appear for those approaches which require conceptual understanding and visual ways of solutions. The main objective of the current study is to compare students’ performance with different cognitive styles (Convergent vs. Divergent) upon mathematical pictorial problem solving. A sample of 93 third year undergraduate mathematics students were tested according to Hudson’s test together with one mathematics examination done. Results obtained support the hypothesis that students with divergent cognitive styles show higher performance than convergent ones in pictorial problems. The implications of these results on teaching and setting problems emphasize that pictorial problems and the cognitive predictor variable (Convergent/ Divergent) could be challenging and a rather distinctive factor for students. INTRODUCTION In recent years the study of cognitive styles has become a broad stream in cognitive psychology and mathematics education. Individuals display their own personal cognitive styles which is a major attribute in what makes an individual to respond to various situations (Anastasi, 1996). According to Messick (1976), cognitive styles are information processing habits representing the learners typical style of perceiving, thinking, problem solving and remembering. In fact, each individual has his/her own styles for collecting and organizing information into knowledge which can be of benefit (Cross, 1976). 102
JOURNAL OF SCIENCE AND MATHEMATICS EDUCATION IN S.E. ASIA Vol. XXIV, No. 2 A large body of researches suggest that students with different cognitive styles approach processing of information and problem solving in different ways. Moreover, a close relationship between cognitive style and learning style has been revealed (Witkin, Moore, Goodenough & Cox, 1977; Witkin & Goodenough, 1981; Messick, 1976; Kogan, 1976; Johnstone & A1- Naeme, 1991). Styles, regardless of their types, are different from ability which some believe to be a characteristic of intelligence. Whereas ability refers more to the content of cognition, cognitive styles help one predict how information is processed by each individual (Messick, 1976; Kogan, 1976; Witkin et al., 1977; Witkin & Goodenough, 1981). A widely used dimension of cognitive style in education is the Convergence/Divergence style, which specifies an individual’s mode of perceiving, thinking, problem solving and visualizing. CONVERGENT/DIVERGENT LEARNING STYLES A convergent (Con) learner is one who tends to look for unique methods and unique solutions. Such thinkers are noted for creativity or lateral thinking. A divergent (Div) learner is characterised by lateral thinking, creativity and capacity to see new combinations of ideas and to examine the possibilities of more than one way of doing things, leading to several outcomes (Hudson, 1966, 1968; Guilford, 1959, 1978). For example, in mathematics, if asked: what is the solution set of the equation x2 - 5x + 6 = 0, in the set of real numbers (IR)? The student should answer that the only solution in IR is the set {2, 3} with no other choice, or the one and only [2 x2 ] + sgn(x) value of lim is 2. It seems that these examples are typical x→∞ x2 + x problems requiring convergent thinking. In fact many of mathematical tasks require a convergent style. Although, a variety of responses to stimuli is the unique feature of divergent thinking, this does not mean that such a way of thinking has no positive role in the process of reaching a unique conclusion. Hudson (1966) suggested that convergers are naturally attracted toward one end of the spectrum and divergers to the other end. He also rejected the belief of many psychologists that divergent thinkers are potentially creative and 103
JOURNAL OF SCIENCE AND MATHEMATICS EDUCATION IN S.E. ASIA Vol. XXIV, No. 2 convergent thinkers are potentially uncreative. In addition, the convergence/divergence dimension is a measure of bias, not the level of ability. CONVERGENT/DIVERGENT STYLE AND ABSTRACT LEARNING The literature in the convergence/divergence cognitive styles (e.g., Hudson, 1966, 1968; Guilford, 1967 and Messick, 1976) suggests that convergent thinkers prefer formal materials and logical arguments. They may be superior in performance to divergent thinkers on tasks which are well structured and demand logical ability, while divergent thinkers presumably are better in the more opened tasks than convergent thinkers. The convergers enjoy precision and logical conclusions, whereas the divergers’ views are restrictive. Guilford (1967) suggested that generating logical necessities is the critical feature of convergers, whereas generating the possibilities from the given information is the characteristic of divergers. In addition, Hudson (1966, 1968) found that being highly imaginative is a striking feature of divergent thinking learners. He also suggested that convergent learners like to keep emotions apart from studies and that divergent ones prefer studies involving emotions. CONVERGENT/DIVERGENT AND SCIENCE EDUCATION Support for the suggestion that science students are biased towards convergent thinking and that arts students towards divergent thinking may be found in several studies (e.g., Guilford, Hoepfner & Peterson 1965; Hudson, 1966, 1968; Mackay and Cameron, 1968; Field & Pool, 1970; Richards and Bolton, 1971; Sally & Bostack, 1979; Webster & Walker, 1981; and Runco, 1986). It has been suggested by Hudson (1966, 1968) that convergent pupils tend to specialize in the sciences and classics, but divergent pupils in the arts, history and modern languages. He also found that between three and four times as many convergers do mathematics, physics and chemistry for every one tending to go into the arts. Results cited by Field and Pool (1970) indicated that although the majority of science specialists entering university were convergent thinkers, it is 104
JOURNAL OF SCIENCE AND MATHEMATICS EDUCATION IN S.E. ASIA Vol. XXIV, No. 2 mainly the divergent thinkers among them who finally achieved better results. These researchers found that there was a relationship between students’ choice of faculty (arts or science) and their convergent/divergent learning style in agreement with Hudson’s finding (1966) in this domain. Al-Naeme (1991) suggested the important role of convergence/divergence style in tackling the mini-project problems in chemistry, with the superiority of divergent thinking over convergent thinking in such tasks. An important question could be raised at this point. Is there any discipline in which students could cope equally well with a convergent or divergent bias? Orton (1992) suggested that, biology, geography and economics are subjects which do not fall into just dimension of divergent thinking or convergent thinking. It seems that only a minority of learners may cope well with convergent and divergent styles at the same time. CONVERGENT/DIVERGENT STYLES AND MATHEMATICS PROBLEM SOLVING Guilford et al. (1965) suggested a positive correlation between divergent thinking and learning mathematics. On the other hand, Kempa and McGough (1977) found that students with an interest in art (divergers) tend to prefer the verbal communication mode in learning mathematics, whereas students’ mathematical biases are found to be strongly associated with performance in the symbolic communication mode and anti-performance for the verbal mode. It may be reasonable to note that the nature of mathematical tasks indicate that students should cope well with convergent and divergent thinking in the problem solving situations. In fact, at the beginning of a solution they need to think openly and converge step by step to the necessary solution. It was also found in a research work that learning various aspects of calculus tasks demands different dimensions of cognitive style on the part of learners. For instance, divergent thinkers favour pictorial thinking, curve interpretation and calculus word problems (Alamolhodaei, 1996). This means that divergers achieved high results than convergers in such questions tasks in calculus. The most recent studies in mathematics teaching and problem solving view pictorial thinking as positive factors, around which to build 105
JOURNAL OF SCIENCE AND MATHEMATICS EDUCATION IN S.E. ASIA Vol. XXIV, No. 2 instructions and learning (Moses, 1982; Mundy, 1987; Vinner & Dreyfus 1989; Vinner, 1982, 1989; Resnick, 1989; Presmeg, 1986; Leinhardt, Zaslavsky & Stein, 1990; Dreyfus, 1992; Moore, 1994; Campbell, Collis & Watson, 1995; and Alamolhodaei, 1996). Students’ misconceptions about functions can be traced logically to pictorial meaning (Leinhardt, Zaslavsky, & Stein, 1990). Research into maths education shows that students generally are very weak visualizers in calculus course materials, which in turn lead to a lack of meaning in the formalities of mathematical analysis (Tall, 1991; Alamolhodaei. 1996). The nonvisual way of teaching has the effect of leading students who are visual thinkers to believe that success in mathematics learning and problem solving depends on rote memorisation of routine rules and formulas. (Presmeg, 1986a). VISUAL/NON VISUAL WAYS OF SOLVING PROBLEMS Visual thinking is a way of thinking and can be viewed as a non-analytic and non-algorithmic mode (Moses, 1982). Mathematical visual thinkers are people who prefer to use visual ways of solutions in mathematical tasks which may be solved by both pictorial and non-pictorial methods (Presmeg, 1986b). As Campbell et. al., (1995) suggested, the most effective mathematics learning style involved the use of visual thinking together with an emphasis on abstraction and generalisation. This dual emphasis could be a beneficial help for students reducing the limitations associated with one way of visualization or abstraction (Alamolhodaei, 1996). Vinner (1989) noted that graphical interpretations and the graphical consideration have a crucial role in understanding the course material in algebra and calculus. Therefore, the visual interpretations of algebraic notions should be taught as well as their applications in proofs and problem solving. The visual considerations explain algebraic moves which, otherwise, look artificial and arbitrary. Presmeg cited Moses’ definition of the visual way of mathematics problem solving that included solutions involving constructions, diagrams, drawings, tables, charts or graphs, whether written down or in the students’ mind. 106
JOURNAL OF SCIENCE AND MATHEMATICS EDUCATION IN S.E. ASIA Vol. XXIV, No. 2 However, as noted by Dreyfus (1992), students’ reluctance to use visual reasoning and pictorial considerations are documented widely in the literature. Many students, especially at the secondary school level, tend to regard mathematical thinking as being largely verbal in nature. This view is due partly to the highly algebraic form in which mathematical work is typically expressed and students often fail to develop the visual, nonverbal, component of their mathematical thinking abilities (Shear, 1985). Many students are unable to recognize a healthy match between their visual thinking and the answers they reach through mathematical manipulation (Mundy & Lauten, 1994). It was found that calculus students do not find it easy to use graph sketching and curve interpretations to recognize that a function has limits or one-sided limits, ( lim F (x) or lim +F (x) & lim -F (x) ) whether it is con- x→a x→a x→a tinuous at a point or on an interval, or is differential on that interval (Alamolhodaei, 1996). Pictorial and geometrical interpretation of some main concepts and theorems of calculus such as Rolle’s theorem, the mean value theorem, multivariable calculus (for example, evaluation of double and triple integrals) can be important for better understanding. As Eisenberg and Dreyfus (1986) noted, such materials are highly visual in nature and hence students fail to handle the relevant pictorial transformations into analytical thinking. Many studies confirmed that students prefer functions expressed in terms of algebraic formula rather than in other kinds of representations such as pictorial form (for example, Vinner & Dreyfus, 1989; Leinhardt, et. al., 1990; Mundy & Lauten 1994 and Alamolhodaei, 1996). However, Mundy and Lauten (1994) suggested that learning about functions can be promoted through the connections between functions and their graphs. In addition, Ervynck (1981) suggested that the use of graphical representations may help to overcome the inherent difficulty of passing from a visual image to a formal definition of the limit concept. Eisenberg and Dreyfus (1986) noted that, in calculus, spatial visualization is commonly used for explaining the main concepts, the derivative and the integral. Theorems which are not easy to be proved algebraically will often be easy to understand and prove geometrically (i.e. visually) and vice versa (Shear, 1985). Graphical work 107
JOURNAL OF SCIENCE AND MATHEMATICS EDUCATION IN S.E. ASIA Vol. XXIV, No. 2 has also a great importance on developing concepts of rate of change (Orton, 1983). THE PRESENT STUDY The main aim of the current study was to identify students’ difficulties associated with mathematics pictorial problems. As Moore (1994) suggested, one of the major sources of students’ difficulties in math proof is their poor pictorial understanding of the concepts. The focus of this research was to provide a profile of learners’ performance with different cognitive styles (convergent/divergent) in tackling pictorial problems. Thus, the main question addressed in this study is to find how different mathematical behaviour of students would be with (convergent/divergent) learning styles working on mathematics tasks and that if there is any interaction between students styles (convergent/ divergent) and their performance in pictorial tasks. It seemed to the author, as a main hypothesis, that divergent students would be expected to show higher results than the convergent ones in pictorial mathematics problems and curve interpretation. In other words the mean scores of (divergent) students could be higher than (convergent) students working on mathematical tasks comprising pictorial problems. In fact, such questions tend to make the task rather distinctive and challenging on the part of students. METHOD Sample This study was conducted on third year undergraduate mathematics students at Ferdowsi University of Mashhad in the north east of Iran. An attempt was made to select a sample of 93 students, while they were doing the maths courses as part of higher education requirements. They were mainly female students. 108
JOURNAL OF SCIENCE AND MATHEMATICS EDUCATION IN S.E. ASIA Vol. XXIV, No. 2 Research instruments and procedures Cognitive style measure: This research was based on the Hudson’s (1966) original study in this dimension of learning style. The researcher used a version of convergent/divergent tests that have been designed and applied by Al-Naeme (1991) and Alamolhodaei (1996). The test comprised six short tests for which a limited time for completion of each test was allowed. Students were required to write as many answers as possible for every question they were given. One mark was given for every single correct response (Hudson, 1966). The highest possible score that could be gained in these six tests was 130. Under this situation, a normal distribution of performance was obtained. A slice of one quarter of a standard deviation on either side of the mean scores was classified as “Intermediate” and excluded from the hypotheses testing in this study to obtain two contrasting groups (convergent/divergent). The quantity of the mean score +0.25 SD was regarded as a crucial point between moving from convergent thinking style into divergent thinking one or vice versa. Therefore, moving up from the mean score +0.25 SD of sample population was classified as divergers, while moving down from mean score -0.25 SD was grouped as convergers. Table 1 represents statistical information of tests carried out to evaluate the (convergent/divergent) scores obtained by students while Table 2 shows the number of students in each one of the styles (convergent/intermediate/divergent). Table 1 Statistical information of (Convergent/Divergent) tests Group Mean Score SD Maximum Score Minimum Score N=93 54.53 11.30 32.00 92.00 Table 2 The distribution of cognitive styles over the sample Group Convergent Intermediate Divergent N=93 48.6% 22.58% 36.55% 109
JOURNAL OF SCIENCE AND MATHEMATICS EDUCATION IN S.E. ASIA Vol. XXIV, No. 2 Math task: The effectiveness of the convergent/divergent style should be investigated by the students’ performance in pictorial mathematical problem solving. Thus a question task with ten pictorial and curve interpretation problems was designed (see Appendix). Students were asked to explain the reasons for their choices in the answer sheets. The maximum score for this task was 20. In addition, students took part in this exam without any previous readiness for doing such pictorial mathematical tasks. Scores obtained by all three groups of cognitive styles (convergent/ intermediate/divergent) represent a normal distribution. DATA ANALYSIS The data analysis procedure was mainly done by using the mean score. A parametric statistical test (One-way ANOVA) was used in order to find out whether the differences in students’ performance in mathematical problem solving activities are statistically significant or insignificant. Results are given at the .05 level. The reliability coefficient (Cronbach’s α) for the (convergent/divergent) tests was estimated to be 0.65. RESULTS In order to examine the hypothesis of this study the performance of students with thinking styles (Convergent/Divergent) in the math exam had to be investigated. The mean scores and standard deviation (SD) in this assessment related to (convergent/intermediate/divergent) learning styles in the sample are set out in Table 3. According to one-way ANOVA, on mean scores of the math exam, a significant difference was found in performance among three groups of styles (F = 4.23, P < .05). Figure 1 represents the superiority of students with different styles, (convergent vs. divergent) based on their mean scores in the pictorial math exam. Table 3 Mean scores and SD in the math exam Group Convergent (N=38) Intermediate (N=21) Divergent (N=34) Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Math exam 9.63 3.1 9.76 3.01 11.75 3.6 110
JOURNAL OF SCIENCE AND MATHEMATICS EDUCATION IN S.E. ASIA Vol. XXIV, No. 2 Math (Mean) 15- 11.76 10- 9.63 5- Con Div Figure 1. The students’ achievements with different styles in the math exam DISCUSSION According to the mean scores of students with convergent/divergent cognitive styles in Table 3, the divergent learners performed and achieved higher results than convergent learners in pictorial questions. The difference between mean scores in groups of students (convergent vs. divergent) was found to be significant owing to p < .05. Therefore, this finding could support the hypothesis of this study. It seems from the results attained that mathematical pictorial problems could be a rather distinctive and challenging task on the part of learners. Instances of misconception found in their answers to the questions proposed in the math exam, may be regarded as a support to this. Particularly questions No. 2, 4 & 5 represent this occuring (see, Appendix). Table 3 indicates that, for students with divergent cognitive style the mean scores in math task is 11.75, while for convergent ones it is 9.63. This means that being divergent in thinking style could be more beneficial than being convergent in tackling pictorial and curve interpreting problems. Moreover, this result supports the previous research findings indications (e.g., Alamolhodaei, 1996) that convergent students experience more troubles when handling the complexity of curve interpretation and pictorial problems even in school and higher education. 111
JOURNAL OF SCIENCE AND MATHEMATICS EDUCATION IN S.E. ASIA Vol. XXIV, No. 2 CONCLUSIONS AND EDUCATIONAL IMPLICATIONS The present study showed a positive correlation between convergent/ divergent cognitive styles which are built upon individual differences and students’ mathematical performance in pictorial question tasks and could have implications for maths education. It was found that, one way of thinking (i.e. divergent) would enhance the achievement in pictorial and curve interpreting math tasks compared with another way (i.e. convergent). This means that divergent bias favour visual thinking and more visualization than convergent bias. It is fair to suggest that teaching styles and mathematical tasks should be planned to benefit both cognitive styles (convergent & divergent) of learners. As Dreyfus (1992) suggested, more than a balance in various forms of mathematics concepts, i.e., the integration of algebraic, verbal and visual thinking should be intended. Balance is to be an aim for integration and to achieve this, visual reasoning needs to be given parity along side algebraic and analytic reasoning if mathematics instructors wish to improve students’ understanding. However, it may be reasonable to note that the nature of many mathematical tasks indicate that students should cope well with convergent and divergent thinking in the problem solving situations. In fact, at the beginning of a solution they find they need to think openly and then converge step by step to the necessary answer. Textbooks and current teaching methods in mathematics in schools and higher institutions favour analytical and non-pictorial ways of thinking. The balance between them is not often valued in teaching and learning math by many teachers. Therefore, with respect to the curriculum, the results of the previous researches and the present study favour a parallel development of math that is visual in nature as well as it is analytical. Visual considerations and graphical interpretations have a crucial role in learning calculus. Moreover, teaching methods should match Convergent/Divergent learning styles. However, a pictorial approach to teaching and problem solving is not often valued by a lot of math educators. They are, unaware of the fact that meaningful learning in math and problem solving activities could be easier if both pictorial and analytical modes of 112
JOURNAL OF SCIENCE AND MATHEMATICS EDUCATION IN S.E. ASIA Vol. XXIV, No. 2 thinking are used. As a ground for future research the following questions need to be attempted. To what extent should a pictorial approach in math be taught to students? To what extent can pictorial considerations become a natural part of students’ mathematical thinking? The answers to these questions will undoubtedly be useful research areas for the future according to the present researcher. REFERENCES Alamolhodaei, H. (1996). A study in higher education calculus and students’ learning styles. Unpublished Ph.D. Thesis, University of Glasgow. Anastasi, A. (1996). Psychological testing. (7th Edition). New York: Macmillan. Al-Naeme, F. F. A. (1991). The influence of various learning styles on practical problem- solving in chemistry in scottish secondary schools. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Glasgow. Campbell, R. J. Collis, K. F. & Watson, J. M. (1995). Visual processing during mathematical problem solving. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 28, 177-194. Cross, K. L. (1976). Accent on learning. New York: Jossey-Bass Publishers. Dreyfus, T. (1992). Imagery and reasoning in mathematics and mathematics education. Selected Lectures from the 7th International Congress on Mathematics Education, 107-122, Quebec, Canada, Universite de Laval. Eisenberg. T. & Dreyfus, T. (1986). On visual versus analytical thinking in mathematics. Proceeding of the Tenth International Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, 153-158. London: IGPME. Ervynck, G. (1981). Conceptual difficulties for first year university students in the acquisition of the notion of limit of a function. Proceedings of the 5th International Conference of the Psychology of Mathematics Education, Grenoble, France, 330-333. Field, T. W. & Pool, M. E. (1970). Intellectual style and achievement of arts and science undergraduates. British Journal of Education, 40, 338-341. Guilford, J. P., Hoepfner, R. & Petersen, H. (1965). Predicting achievement in ninth-grade mathematics from measures of intellectual aptitude factors. Educational and Psychological Measurement, Vol. XXV, No. 30. Guilford, J. P. (1959). Three faces of intellect. American Psychologist, 14, 459-479. Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill. 113
JOURNAL OF SCIENCE AND MATHEMATICS EDUCATION IN S.E. ASIA Vol. XXIV, No. 2 Guilford, J. P. (1978). Traits of creativity. In P. E. Vernon (Ed.) Creativity. Harmondsworth, Penguin Books. Hudson, L. (1966). Contrary imagination. London: Penguin Books. Hudson, L. (1968). Frames of mind. London: Methuen. Johnstone, A. H. & Al-Naeme, F. F. (1991). Room for scientific thought. International Journal of Science Education, 13(2), 187-192. Kempa R. F. & Mc Gough, J. M. (1977). A study of attitudes towards mathematics in relation to selected students characteristics. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 47, 296-304. Kogan, N. (1976). Individuality in learning. New York: Jossey-Bass Publishers. Leinhardt, G. Zaslavsky O. & Stein M. K. (1990). Functions, graphs and graphing: Tasks learning, and teaching. Review of Educational Research, 60(1), 1-64. Mackay, C. K. & Cameron. M. B. (1968). Cognitive bias in Scottish first year science and arts undergraduates. British Journal of Education Psychology, 38, 315-318. Messick, S. (1976). Individuality in learning. New York: Jossey-Bass Publishers. Moore, R. C. (1994). Making the transition to formal proof. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 27, 249-266. Moses, B. (1982). Visualisation: A different approach to problem-solving. School Science and Mathematics, 2, 141-147. Mundy, J. F. (1987). Spatial training for calculus students: Sex differences in achievement and in visualization ability. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 18(2), 126-140. Mundy, J. F. & Lauten, D. (1994). Learning about calculus learning. The Mathematics Teacher, 87(2), 151-121. Orton, A. (1983). Students’ understanding of integration. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 14, 1-18. Orton, A. (1992). Learning mathematics: Issues, theory and classroom practices, second edition. London: Casell Education Series. Presmeg, N. C. (1986a). Visualisation in high school mathematics. For the Learning of Mathematics, 6(3), 24-46. Presmeg, N. C. (1986b). Visualisation and mathematical giftedness. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 17, 297-311. Richards, P. N. & Bolton, N. (1971). Type of mathematics teaching, mathematical ability and divergent thinking in Junior school children. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 41, 32-37. 114
JOURNAL OF SCIENCE AND MATHEMATICS EDUCATION IN S.E. ASIA Vol. XXIV, No. 2 Resnick, L. B. (1989). Developing mathematical knowledge. American Psychologist, 44(2), 162-169. Runco, M. A. (1986). Divergent thinking and creative performance in gifted and non gifted children. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 46, 375-383. Sally, M. A. & Bostack, L. (1979). Convergent-divergent thinking and arts—science orientation. British Journal of Psychology, 70, 155-163. Shear, J. (1985). Visual thinking, algebraic thinking and a full unit-circle diagram. Mathematics Teacher, 78(7), 518-522. Tall, D. O. (1991). Intuition and rigour: The role of visualization in the calculus. In W. Zimmermann and S. Cunningham (Eds), Visualisation in teaching and learning mathematics. Washington: The Mathematical Association of America, 105-119. Vinner, S. (1982). Conflicts between definitions and intuitions: The case of tangent. In A. Vermandel (Ed.), Proceedings of the 6th International Conference on the Psychology of Mathematics Education (PME6), 24-28, Antwerp, Belgium. Vinner, S. (1989). The avoidance of visual considerations in calculus students. Focus on Learning Problem in Mathematics, 11(2), 149-156. Vinner, S. & Dreyfus, T. (1989). Images and definitions for the concept of function. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 20(4), 356-366. Webster, M A. & Walker, M. B. (1981). Divergent thinking in arts and science students: The effect of item content. British Journal of psychology, 72, 331-338. Witkin, H. A., Moore, C. A., Goodenough, D. R., & Cox. P. W. (1977). Field- dependent and field-dependent cognitive styles and their educational implications. Review of Educational Research, 47(1), 1-64. Witkin, H. A. & Goodenough, D. R. (1981). Cognitive styles: Essence and origins. New York: International Universities Press. 115
JOURNAL OF SCIENCE AND MATHEMATICS EDUCATION IN S.E. ASIA Vol. XXIV, No. 2 APPENDIX Some samples of exam questions are given below: 1. Let f: IR → IR be defined by f (x) = sin x - cos x. Determine a sketch of the graph of f around the point x = π 2 π π π π 2 2 2 2 2. For each of the following pairs of relations R1 and R2 on IR, sketch R1 R2 and .find its domain and range. x2 { } { R1 = (x, y): x2 + y2 < 25 and R2 = (x, y):y > 4 9 } y y=P(x) x 3. This is a sketch of the graph of a function y = P(x) which of the sketches below could be the graph of y = -P (x), y = P (-x) A B C y y y x x x 116
JOURNAL OF SCIENCE AND MATHEMATICS EDUCATION IN S.E. ASIA Vol. XXIV, No. 2 4. Each equation mathes one of the following graphs. Write down its matching equation. k y = kx2 y = kx2 + P y= y = qxk y=k x y = pqx x (p, q and k are constant). A Y B C Y Y X X X X X X D E F Y Y X X X X X X 5. Which of the following statement are true of the function f defined for -1< x
You can also read