Bringing basic research on early experience and stress neurobiology to bear on preventive interventions for neglected and maltreated children

Page created by Peggy Walters
 
CONTINUE READING
Development and Psychopathology 18 ~2006!, 651–677
Copyright © 2006 Cambridge University Press
Printed in the United States of America
DOI: 10.10170S0954579406060330

Bringing basic research on early experience
and stress neurobiology to bear on
preventive interventions for neglected
and maltreated children

MEGAN R. GUNNAR,a PHILIP A. FISHER,b and THE EARLY
EXPERIENCE, STRESS, AND PREVENTION NETWORK
a Institute
               of Child Development, University of Minnesota; and
b Oregon      Social Learning Center

Abstract
A major focus in developmental psychopathology is on understanding developmental mechanisms and, armed with
this information, intervening to improve children’s outcomes. Translational research attempts to bridge the distance
between understanding and intervention. In the collaborations that have formed the core of our research network on
early experience, stress, and prevention science, we have focused on translating basic research on early experiences
and stress neurobiology into preventive interventions for neglected and abused children. Our experiences in
attempting to move from bench to bedside have led us to recognize the many challenges that face translational
researchers. This review provides a brief synopsis of the animal model literature on early experience and stress
neurobiology from which we glean several key bridging issues. We then review what is currently known about the
impact of childhood neglect and abuse on stress neurobiology in human adults and children. Next, we describe how
this work has informed the evaluation of our preventive interventions with maltreated children. Finally, we discuss
several considerations that should facilitate a more complete integration of basic research on early experience and
stress neurobiology into preventive intervention strategies.

How do early care experiences shape develop-                accumulate lists of all the ways in which early
ment, and to what extent can later experiences              adversity may limit or impair children. Rather,
remediate the effects of early adverse care?                the objectives have been to better understand
These questions have tremendous importance                  developmental mechanisms of early adversity
for our understanding of developmental psy-                 and, armed with this information, to intervene
chopathology ~Cicchetti, 1989!. The goals of                to improve children’s outcomes. Unfortu-
research on early adversity have never been to              nately, there is a large distance between em-
                                                            barking on the first objective and achieving
This paper reflects the work of the Early Experience,       the second one. Bridging this distance is a
Stress and Prevention Science Network ~R21 MH65046!,        primary goal of translational research ~Strau-
whose members are Mary Dozier, Philip Fisher, Nathan        man & Merrill, 2004!.
Fox, Megan Gunnar, Seymour Levine, Charles Neal, Seth
                                                               In the collaborations that have formed
Pollak, Paul Plotsky, Mar Sanchez, and Delia Vazquez.
Preparation of this manuscript was supported by a Senior    the core of our Early Experiences, Stress,
Scientist Award ~K05 MH66208! to Megan Gunnar, and          and Prevention Science Research Network, we
by MH59780 and MH65046, NIMH, U.S. PHS; MH46690,            have focused on one facet of this translational
NIMH and ORMH, U.S. PHS; and DA17592, NIDA,                 challenge: applying basic research on the de-
NIH, U.S. PHS to Philip Fisher.
                                                            velopmental neurobiology of stress to the un-
    Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Megan
R. Gunnar, Institute of Child Development, 51 East River    derstanding of how early neglect and abuse
Road, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN; E-mail:     shapes vulnerability to emotional and behav-
gunnar@umn.edu.                                             ioral disorders and then using this information

                                                        651
652             M. R. Gunnar, P. A. Fisher, and The Early Experience, Stress, and Prevention Network

to inform preventive interventions for mal-        disorders ~e.g., Manly, Kim, Rogosch, & Cic-
treated children. Our experiences in attempt-      chetti, 2001!. Although some children who
ing to move from bench to bedside have led us      suffer maltreatment are resilient, becoming
to recognize the many challenges that face         competent adults despite the odds ~Kinard,
translational researchers. These challenges will   1998!, many others suffer from a variety of
need to be overcome if we are to adequately        clinical disorders in adulthood including
integrate basic neuroscience into developmen-      substance abuse and affective and personality
tal psychopathology and effective preventive       disorders ~e.g., Putnam, 2003!. Research ex-
treatments ~Cicchetti & Tucker, 1994!. In what     amining why some individuals are resilient
follows, we will describe the paths we have        has identified protective factors, but cannot
taken and some of the issues with which we         wholly explain resilience among maltreated
have grappled. We wish that we could con-          children ~e.g., Cicchetti & Rogosch, 1997; Ki-
clude by demonstrating that we have accom-         nard, 1998!. Similarly, efforts to determine
plished the task we set ourselves, fully moving    maltreatment factors associated with different
research on early experiences and stress neuro-    types of disorders has examined such charac-
biology into effective preventive interven-        teristics as the timing, dose, duration, and type
tions for children. However, at this stage, what   of maltreatment ~e.g., Manly et al., 2001!.
we have to offer is a progress report on where     These studies indicate that more severe, mul-
we are and the obstacles we and others have        tiple, and prolonged maltreatment results in
encountered thus far.                              increased symptoms; however, a coherent
    We begin this review by briefly summariz-      and consistent model of the differential ef-
ing the data on behavioral outcomes of ne-         fects of specific subtypes of maltreatment on
glected and abused children and the major          specific symptom clusters or mental health
psychological orientations that have been ap-      disorders does not appear to have emerged.
plied to explicate why these outcomes occur.       Interestingly, one challenge to identifying
Next, we very briefly synopsize the vast liter-    associations between the type and timing of
ature, spanning the last half-century, on early    maltreatment and disordered outcomes may
experience and stress neurobiology using ro-       be that outcomes vary over the course of de-
dent models. From this work we have distilled      velopment. Externalizing problems appear to
key issues that provide a framework for our        predominate during childhood, while sub-
translational efforts. We then review what is      stance abuse and affective disorders are ob-
currently known about the impact of child-         served in adulthood ~e.g., Tieman, van der
hood neglect and abuse on stress neurobiol-        Ende, & Verhulst, 2005!. The developmental
ogy in human adults and children. Based on         trajectories associated with these varied out-
similarities in primate and human develop-         comes are at present not understood, due in
ment, we extend this review to nonhuman            part to the complexity and cost of prospective
primate studies. Finally, we discuss how this      longitudinal designs.
basic work has informed the evaluation of our          Until recently, theoretical perspectives on
preventive interventions with neglected and        maltreatment have been solely psycho-
maltreated children, and considerations that       logical, reflecting two dominant lenses: the
may move the field forward to more fully in-       developmental–organizational perspective, and
tegrate an understanding of stress neurobiol-      the social learning theory perspective. From a
ogy into preventive intervention strategies.       developmental–organizational perspective,
                                                   maltreatment disturbs resolution of stage-
                                                   salient developmental issues. If the experi-
Early Neglect and Abuse: Increased Risk            ence occurs early in development, attachment
of Behavioral and Emotional Problems               relationships are likely to be disturbed ~Page,
                                                   1999!. Studies have revealed that exposures
Decades of research have provided unequivo-        to frightening or overwhelming behaviors from
cal evidence that childhood maltreatment in-       the caregiver are associated with the develop-
creases the risk of psychological and behavioral   ment of a disorganized0disoriented ~Type D!
Stress neurobiology and prevention science                                                       653

attachment, characterized by freezing and dis-      may affect brain development and heighten
sociative behavior and heightened risk for          risk of psychological disorders ~see for re-
numerous poor outcomes in childhood and be-         views, Bremner & Vermetten, 2001; De Bel-
yond ~Lyons-Ruth, 2003!.                            lis, 2005; Heim, Owen, Plotsky, & Nemeroff,
    Through the lens of social learning theory,     1997; Shea, Walsh, Macmillan, & Steiner,
learning principles account for behaviors ex-       2005; Teicher, Andersen, Polcarri, Anderson,
hibited by neglected and abused children ~Reid      & Navalta, 2002!. Neurobiological models may
& Kavanagh, 1985!. Behaviors on the part of         hold keys to mechanisms through which psy-
the child that are less likely to elicit abusive    chosocial interventions operate and to expla-
treatment may be adaptive in the context of         nations of individual differences in response
abuse, but when generalized to other settings       to treatment. These neurobiological models are
may disrupt the child’s relationships ~e.g., with   largely based on animal studies of the effects
teachers, peers, and so on!. Indeed, poor peer      of early life adversity on stress neurobiology
relations associated with a tendency to attribute   and brain development. The largest body of
hostile intent to others has been noted for mal-    such early experience research has been con-
treated children ~Price & Glad, 2003!. Mal-         ducted on rodents, notably the rat.
adaptive social behavior may lead children into         Over the past 50 years, rodent early expe-
antisocial peer groups, increasing the risk of      rience studies have shown that early parental
conduct disorders, and substance abuse ~Pat-        care profoundly influences brain develop-
terson, DeBaryshe, & Ramsey, 1989!. Mal-            ment, regulates gene expression, and shapes
treated children have been consistently shown       the neural systems that in humans are in-
to be at high risk for externalizing behavior,      volved in vulnerability to affective disorders
conduct problems and substance abuse ~Ege-          in response to later stressful life events ~e.g.,
land, Yates, Appleyard, & van Dulmen, 2002!.        Levine, 2005a!. Recent studies also indicate
    To date, preventive interventions for mal-      that interventions in the postinfancy period
treated children also have been based almost        may help ameliorate some, but not all, of the
exclusively on the two psychological perspec-       impacts of early inadequate parental care ~Fran-
tives describe above. Early interventions based     cis, Diorio, Plotsky, & Meaney, 2002!. From
on developmental–organizational perspec-            the beginning, this rodent research was con-
tives have focused on attachment, attempting        ducted to inform the understanding of human
to increase parental sensitivity, and responsive-   development and psychopathology. Below we
ness to foster more secure parent–child rela-       review the rodent literature and arrive at a
tionships and alter the child’s inner working       core list of critical bridging themes. Thorough
models ~Cicchetti, 2005; Dozier, 2003!. Inter-      understanding of these themes, however, re-
ventions based on social learning theory have       quires a brief description of the neurobiology
attempted to avert child behavior problems by       of stress.
training parents to use clear, consistent, non-
hostile guidance and discipline techniques
                                                    The Neurobiology of Stress
~Fisher, Burraston, & Pears, 2005!. The effec-
tiveness of these psychosocial interventions is     Stress neurobiology is organized at three lev-
typically evaluated at the group level; how-        els ~see Figure 1!: a corticolimbic level that or-
ever, there are always differences in effective-    chestrates responses to anticipated threat, a
ness at the level of the individual. These          hypothalamic–brainstem level that coordinates
psychosocial models often fall short of fully       central and peripheral responses in response to
explicating why some individuals respond and        corticolimbic input ~and also in response to less
others do not.                                      processed input regarding threats to homeosta-
                                                    sis!, and a neural to adrenal gland level that ef-
                                                    fects increases in stress-sensitive hormones
Early Experience and Stress Neurobiology
                                                    ~glucocorticoids and epinephrine; Herman &
Recently, researchers have turned to neurobi-       Cullinan, 1997!. Corticotropin-releasing fac-
ological models to explain how maltreatment         tor ~CRF! is a neuroactive peptide that oper-
654

      Figure 1. The three levels of neurobiological organization of the stress system that are responsive to psychological stressors. The corticolimbic level of
      organization involves the anterior cingulate ~ACC! and orbital frontal cortex ~OFC!, which relay information to subcortitical structures involved in the stress
      response. The ACC and OFC are reciprocally interconnected with each other and with the amygdala, which has connections with the hippocampus and BNST.
      The hypothalamic–brainstem level of organization involve the hippocampus and brainstem structures such as the locus coeruleus, which releases NE to brain
      areas involved in alerting. The BNST provides pathways into the PVN of the hypothalamus, which produces corticotrophin-releasing hormone ~CRH! and
      arginine vasopressin ~AVP!, whereas the hippocampus and regions in the medial frontal cortex ~e.g., ACC! maintain feedback control on the paraventricular
      nucleus ~PVN!. Considering the neural to adrenal level of analysis, nuclei in the lateral hypothalamus activate highly interconnected nuclei in the brainstem,
      including the parabrachial nuclei, that regulate the sympathetic ~NE and epinephrine, EPI! and parasympathetic ~acetylcholine, Ach! nervous systems via
      pathways traveling through the spinal cord to preganglionic nuclei or to target organs ~e.g., the adrenal medulla!. The production of CRH and AVP by the PVN
      regulates activity of the HPA axis and the production of glucocorticoids ~GCs! as depicted more fully in Figure 2. Adapted from Gunnar and Davis ~2003!.
Stress neurobiology and prevention science                                                     655

ates at the first two levels to coordinate behav-       The second CRF pathway involves CRF-
ioral, emotional, autonomic, and endocrine fac-     producing neurons in the central nucleus of
ets of stress and defensive responding              the amygdala ~CeA; Van Bockstaele, Colago,
~Heinrichs & Koob, 2004!. Two loosely cou-          & Valentino, 1998!. The CeA receives infor-
pled CRF pathways are involved. One, termed         mation through multiple pathways, many of
the hypothalamic CRF pathway, involves CRF-         which involve cortical systems that support
producing neurons in the paraventricular nu-        the integration of past and present experi-
clei of the hypothalamus ~PVN !, which              ences, thereby allowing the anticipation of
regulates activity of the hypothalamic–             threat ~see Figure 1!. Psychosocial stressors
pituitary–adrenocortical ~HPA! axis ~see Fig-       operate through these pathways converging
ure 2!. CRF secreted from these neurons travels     on the CeA to orchestrate behavioral, auto-
through a small blood connection to the ante-       nomic, and neuroendocrine reactions in antici-
rior pituitary where it, along with other co-       pation of threat ~Heinrichs & Koob, 2004!.
secretagogues ~e.g., arginine vasopressin,          Efferent CeA–CRF pathways project indi-
AVP!, stimulates the production and release         rectly to PVN–CRF neurons through the bed
of adrenocorticotropic hormone ~ACTH!.              nucleus of the stria terminalis ~BNST! to stim-
ACTH released into general circulation stim-        ulate the HPA axis, and directly to the locus
ulates cells in cortex of the adrenal glands to     coeruleus to stimulate release of norepineph-
produce and release glucocorticoids.                rine ~NE! into the terminal fields of the as-
   Glucocorticoids ~predominantly cortisol in       cending noradrenergic system, supporting
primates and corticosterone in rodents! are         cognitive arousal and focusing, and behav-
steroid hormones that affect almost every or-       ioral and emotional components of fight0fight0
gan and tissue of the body ~Sapolsky, Romero,       freeze responses ~Van Bockstaele et al., 1998!.
& Munck, 2000!. In the brain, glucocorticoids       Bidirectional connections of the amygdala and
operate through two types of receptors with         regions in the medial prefrontal cortex ~mPFC;
distinct functions ~de Kloet, Vreugdenhil, Oitzl,   orbital frontal cortex or OFC and anterior cin-
& Joels, 1998!. At basal levels glucocorti-         gulate cortex or ACC! support the modulation
coids operate predominantly through Type I          of behavior and cognition in relation to expec-
or mineralocorticoid receptors to maintain the      tations of rewards and punishments. These cor-
sensitivity of neurons to their neurotransmit-      ticolimbic pathways influences the balance
ters and maintain the capacity of the brain to      between responding based on rapid, habitual,
respond to conditions that potentially threaten     emotionally charged modes of acting and more
the organism’s viability. At elevated or stress     nuanced, considered, and dispassionate modes
levels, glucocorticoids operate through Type II     ~Sullivan & Gratton, 2002!. Notably, chronic
or glucocorticoid receptors ~GR! to counter-        intravenous infusions of glucocorticoids tend
act the impact of other stress processes, sculpt    to upregulate CeA–CRF activity, biasing func-
neural systems to retain information about          tioning toward rapid, emotion-charged fight0
threats to well-being, and return the organism      flight0freeze responses, while at the same time
to prestress levels of functioning. GR-mediated     downregulating PVN–CRF, resulting in nor-
neurochemical events, if well timed, acute,         mal to hyporesponsiveness of the HPA axis
and contained, support organism via-                ~Schulkin, McEwen, & Gold, 1994!. In addi-
bility; however, these events are potentially       tion, particularly during development, CRF
damaging if too frequent or too prolonged           and glucocorticoids interact with other neuro-
~Sapolsky et al., 2000!. Multiple mechanisms        peptides ~e.g., oxytocin! and neurotransmitter
operate in response to elevated glucocorti-         systems ~e.g., serotonin and dopamine!, result-
coids to suppress or downregulate the HPA           ing in widespread influences of frequent stress
system including negative feedback mecha-           level activation of the CeA–CRF and PVN–
nisms that regulate acute responses and geno-       CRF systems on emotional and cognitive de-
mic alterations at various levels of the system     velopment ~Roceri et al., 2004!.
that affect the responsiveness following pro-           Genetic variability appears to modify these
longed or frequent activation.                      effects. As only one example, individuals car-
656              M. R. Gunnar, P. A. Fisher, and The Early Experience, Stress, and Prevention Network

      Figure 2. The brain structures that are central to the activation and inhibition of the HPA endocrine
      stress response: ~A! the main brain areas that participate in the regulation of the HPA axis and ~B! a
      schematic representation of the HPA endocrine stress response initiated by the release of CRH0AVP
      from the medial parvocellular region of the paraventricular nucleus ~mpPVN! in the hypothalamus.
      Glucocorticoids inhibit this system acting at the level of the pituitary, hypothalamus, and hippocampus.
      GABA, gamma aminobutyric acid; CRH, corticotropin-releasing hormone. From “Stress Neurobiology
      and Developmental Psychopathology,” by M. Gunnar and D. M. Vazquez. In D. Cicchetti and D. Cohen
      ~Eds.!, Developmental Psychopathology: Developmental Neuroscience ~2nd ed., Vol. 2!, 2006, New
      York: Wiley. Copyright 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Reprinted with permission.

rying at least one short allele of the serotonin           show larger cortisol responses to psycho-
transporter gene polymorphism exhibit larger               social stressors ~Barr et al., 2004; Sanchez,
amygdala responses to threat stimuli ~Hariri               Noble, et al., 2005!. They are also at higher
et al., 2002!. In addition, these individuals              risk for depression following early experi-
Stress neurobiology and prevention science                                                         657

ences of abuse and neglect ~Caspi et al., 2003;      However, comparing brain development across
Kaufman et al., 2004!.                               mammalian species is difficult. Although the
                                                     general pattern of brain development is prob-
Key Translational Issues                             ably comparable across mammals, specifics
                                                     likely vary and specifics are required to trans-
Models derived from rodent studies of ad-            late mechanisms of early experience effects.
verse early experience can be employed to               For example, methylation of the GR gene
provide a biological level of explanation            expressed in the hippocampus appears to be an
for the above noted association in children          important mechanism through which early ex-
between early maltreatment and later height-         periences in the rodent influence later stress
ened risk for emotional and behavioral disor-        reactivity and vulnerability ~for review, see,
ders. At the most general level, the rodent          Meaney & Szyf, 2005!. At birth, GR genes in
studies show that disturbances in early care         the rodent hippocampus are heavily methyl-
exert a pervasive and lasting impact on the          ated. This means that they are not available for
two neural pathways described above, and that        transcription. The GR gene in the brain tran-
alterations in these systems has potential to        scribes the proteins needed to produce the GR,
compromise subsequent development ~Meaney            which, in turn, mediates many components of
& Szyf, 2005; Sanchez, Ladd, & Plotsky,              the stress response, including negative feed-
2001!. However, if we are to move beyond             back regulation of the HPA axis in response to
these broad generalities to inform specific in-      psychosocial stressors. Maternal care in the in-
tervention strategies, we need to determine          fant rodent determines patterns of GR gene de-
whether and how particular elements of the           methylation with higher levels of maternal
rodent early experience models apply to hu-          stimulation ~i.e., licking and grooming! being
man development. This, in turn, requires at-         associated with more demethylation. Accord-
tention to the critical details of the rodent        ingly, the offspring of high licking and groom-
findings. As we discuss in this section, we          ing mothers have more operational GR genes
have identified four dimensions of the rodent        in the brain and regulate stress more effec-
models that appear critical to building the trans-   tively. These demethylations of the GR gene
lational bridge:                                     occur most prominently during the first week
1. developmental timing of adversity,                of life in the rat pup, consistent with evidence
2. the presence ~or absence! of a relative stress    that manipulations that decrease maternal care
   hyporesponsive period ~SHRP! during hu-           have more profound effects if they are imposed
   man development,                                  beginning in the first rather than the second post-
3. parental or caregiver mediation of any            natal week. Manipulations in the later peri-
   SHRP, and                                         pubertal period that reduce many of these early
4. the impact of caregiving that mediates the        experiences effects do not affect GR methyl-
   SHRP on the development of corticolim-            ation, suggesting that this early experience ef-
   bic stress response organization.                 fect is more or less permanent ~Francis et al.,
                                                     2002!. GR methylation can be affected in adult-
                                                     hood in the rat through pharmacological ma-
Timing
                                                     nipulations ~Weaver et al., 2005!. Importantly,
The effects of experiences on brain develop-         pharmacological manipulations that demethyl-
ment depend on the maturity of the brain when        ate the GR gene also result in changes in other
the events are experienced. Timing is critical.      aspects of stress neurobiology, suggesting that
Rat pups are born young relative to human            GR methylation is not only relatively perma-
infants; that is, birth occurs in the rodent when    nent but also fairly critical to the evidence that
the brain is much less mature than the brain of      early experiences in the rodent have lifelong
the full-term human infant. Indeed, the first        consequences for adult stress vulnerability and
week of the rodent’s life are often equated          resilience.
with development of the human infant during             Translating the results of rodent GR gene
the last trimester of gestation ~Dobbing, 1981!.     studies requires that we know when in human
658             M. R. Gunnar, P. A. Fisher, and The Early Experience, Stress, and Prevention Network

development adult levels of central GR methyl-      are observed in the first 2 weeks of the pup’s
ation are determined. As far as we know, this       life. This roughly corresponds to a develop-
information simply is not available for hu-         mental epoch in rodents that has been referred
mans or any other primate. Indeed, our level        to as the relative stress hyporesponsive period
of knowledge of human developmental neuro-          ~SHRP!. During the SHRP, the rat pup’s HPA
biology is sufficiently limited that in many        axis ~the adrenal cortex in particular! shows
cases, even when basic studies provide a tar-       very little reactivity when challenged by a va-
get for early experience effects, that knowl-       riety of stressors ~Sapolsky & Meaney, 1986!.
edge does not help us pin point comparable          The SHRP may have evolved to protect the
periods in the development of stress neurobi-       rapidly developing brain from the impact of
ology in humans. Nonetheless, given the im-         elevated glucocorticoids. Indeed, there is con-
portance of the first postnatal week in rodents     siderable evidence that although basal activity
and its rough comparability to the last trimes-     of the HPA axis is necessary for normal devel-
ter of gestation in humans, it is prudent to        opment of the central nervous system, ele-
broaden our early experience-translational win-     vated levels of glucocorticoids and CRH during
dow to include the prenatal period.                 this period in the rodent result in significant
    Certainly there is abundant evidence that       apoptosis ~cell death! and alter the develop-
many children who suffer adverse care during        ment of brain regions that play critical roles in
postnatal development often are the products        learning, memory, and stress resilience ~Sapol-
of high risk or stressed pregnancies ~Thomp-        sky & Meaney, 1986!. In building a transla-
son et al., 1994!. There is increasing evidence     tional bridge, therefore, we need to know
that maternal stress and anxiety during preg-       whether there is a period in human develop-
nancy are associated with lower birth weight        ment that is functionally comparable to the
infants, in part through increased maternal HPA     SHRP in the rodent. That is, is there a period
activity, upregulation of placental CRH pro-        during human development when it is diffi-
duction, with resulting decreases in birth weight   cult to produce elevations in cortisol to stress-
and length of gestation ~Wadhwa, 2005!. Lower       ors, and if so, when? Disturbances in care
birth weight is a known risk factor for poor de-    during this time would be hypothesized to have
velopmental outcomes, many of which over-           the greatest impact on the development of stress
lap with problems observed for neglected and        neurobiology.
abused children ~e.g., Indredavikm, Vik, Hey-           Evidence is accumulating that in human
erdahl, Kulseng, & Brubakk, 2005!. There is         children there may be a roughly comparable
also increasing evidence that lower birth weight    period that emerges in infancy and extends
is associated in adults with increased risk of      throughout most of childhood ~see for review,
metabolic syndrome ~i.e., high blood pressure,      Gunnar, 2003!. At birth, cortisol elevates
high cholesterol, abdominal fat, type II diabe-     readily to a wide variety of stressors from
tes! and that elevated and poorly regulated glu-    noninvasive stressors like a physical examina-
cocorticoids may mediate this risk ~Phillips        tion to invasive stressors such as a heel lance.
et al., 2000!. Thus, as we proceed with our work    The HPA system appears to remain highly re-
on postnatal maltreatment, we should keep in        sponsive for several months postbirth, exhib-
mind that although the rodent findings appear       iting elevations to physical examinations and
promising in explaining the impact of post-         to childhood inoculations. However, over the
natal neglect and abuse in human children, some     course of the first year it becomes increas-
of the mechanisms might actually translate more     ingly difficult to produce elevations in corti-
directly to the impact of adversity on prenatal     sol to acute stressors, including physical
human development.                                  examinations, brief separations ~e.g., 3–5 min!,
                                                    inoculations, approach by strangers, and other
                                                    events that are capable of eliciting increases
Relative stress hyporesponsive period
                                                    in heart rate and behavioral distress.
In the rodent, the most profound effects pro-           Although it seems fairly clear that the hu-
duced by systematically altering parental care      man stress hyporesponsive period emerges
Stress neurobiology and prevention science                                                        659

gradually over the first year, it is not as clear   children when they are in full-day out-of-
how long it extends. Recent studies suggest         home child care may provide some insight
that by puberty laboratory stressor tasks pro-      ~for review, see Gunnar & Donzella, 2002!.
duce elevations in cortisol with increasing re-     Although child care is not equivalent to the
sponse levels observed over the pubertal            separation paradigms examined in the animal
transition ~reviewed in Gunnar & Vazquez,           studies, child care often involves a reduction
2006!. Similar to the rodent, where corticoste-     in individualized care. Comparable to the ro-
rone levels rise as the animal emerges out of       dent findings, cortisol levels are not elevated
the SHRP, a rise in basal levels of cortisol has    in the first few hours of the child care day;
been reported in human children around this         however, by late afternoon, levels are higher
point in development ~see for review, Gunnar        than are noted at home on nonchild care days.
& Vazquez, 2006!. An increase in basal activ-       Increases over home levels are largest for tod-
ity of the axis and a corresponding increase in     dlers, but are still significant in cross-sectional
HPA reactivity to stressors around the puber-       studies until children are 5 or 6 years old.
tal transition would be consistent with pu-            Similar to the rodent findings, children with
berty marking the close of a relative stress        child care providers who engage them with
hyporesponsive period in human develop-             focused attention and responsive stimulation
ment. If the HPA stress hyporesponsive period       do not exhibit these elevated levels. Further
demarcates the period when stress neurobiol-        evidence for caregiver mediation of the hu-
ogy is open to be shaped by experience, then        man functional equivalent of the SHRP comes
in humans that period may extend throughout         from studies of attachment in toddlers during
childhood.                                          a fear-eliciting stress paradigm ~Nachmias,
                                                    Gunnar, Mangelsdorf, Parritz, & Buss, 1996!.
                                                    Toddlers in secure attachment relationships
Caregiving Mediation
                                                    show no elevations in cortisol, whereas tod-
In the rodent, the evidence is fairly conclusive    dlers in insecure attachment relationships ex-
that the SHRP is maintained by maternal care.       hibit significant cortisol elevations to events
Specifically, maternal licking and grooming         that produce fearful behavior. If patterns of
and milk into the gut are the stimuli that buffer   caregiving are important in maintaining the
the rat pup’s HPA axis ~Rosenfeld, Suchecki,        relative hyporesponsiveness of the stress sys-
& Levine, 1992!. If these stimuli are removed       tem early in development, then identifying the
for a number of hours, the buffering mecha-         ingredients of stress buffering caregiving will
nism is disturbed and large increases in corti-     be necessary to translate the basic research to
costerone, ACTH, and CRF can be observed.           preventive interventions. Consistent with Do-
If there is a functionally equivalent SHRP in       zier’s Attachment and Biobehavioral Catch-up
humans, are there comparable social stimuli         intervention ~Dozier, Peloso, Sepulveda, et al.,
that help to maintain the HPA system in its         in press!, we have examined whether the
relatively buffered state? In rodents, the role     caregiver’s sensitivity and responsiveness af-
of maternal stimulation in maintaining the          fects the development of stress neurobiology.
SHRP was identified by removing maternal            Parents who were low in sensitivity and re-
stimulation for periods of time and then, for       sponsiveness during medical exams when their
some pups, replacing elements of maternal care      children were 2, 4, and 6 months of age had
during separation. Brief periods ~up to several     children who as toddlers exhibited larger cor-
hours! of separation do not induce elevations       tisol responses to childhood immunizations
in corticosterone or ACTH in rat pups, but          ~Gunnar, Broderson, Nachmias, Buss, & Rig-
after 6- to 12-hr elevations are observed. Ar-      atuso, 1996!. Recently, Hane and Fox ~in press!
tificially providing licking and grooming and       extended these findings to include measures
milk maintain the SHRP ~Suchecki, Rosen-            of frontal EEG asymmetry. Right frontal EEG
feld, & Levine, 1993!. Needless to say, there       asymmetry is associated with withdrawal emo-
are challenges in examining parallel phenom-        tions ~e.g., fear, sadness! and risk for anxiety
ena in humans; however, cortisol levels for         and depression ~Davidson, 2002!. Hane and
660             M. R. Gunnar, P. A. Fisher, and The Early Experience, Stress, and Prevention Network

Fox ~in press! found that mothers who were          and prolonged glucocorticoid infusions re-
low responsive had infants who exhibited more       model the dendrites of the mPFC ~Brown, Hen-
right frontal EEG asymmetry and were also           ning, & Wellman, 2005!. As noted earlier, the
more fearful, whereas the infants of high re-       mPFC plays a significant role in regulating
sponsive mothers exhibited a left frontal EEG       behavioral, endocrine, and autonomic re-
pattern and were more bold. Studies with rhe-       sponses to stressors ~Sullivan & Gratton, 2002!.
sus monkeys have demonstrated that greater              Relatively few studies of maltreated chil-
right frontal EEG patterns are not only asso-       dren have employed neurocognitive tests that
ciated with greater behavioral fearfulness, but     can identify specific neurological deficits ~Pol-
also with higher cortisol reactions to psycho-      lak, 2005!. General cognitive impairments have
social challenge ~Kalin, Larson, Shelton, &         been associated with neglect, with abused chil-
Davidson, 1998!. Taken together, these find-        dren sometimes performing more compe-
ings suggest that in early childhood, at least,     tently than neglected children ~Pears & Fisher,
caregiver sensitivity and responsiveness may        2005!. Among abused children with posttrau-
play the role that maternal licking and groom-      matic stress disorder ~PTSD!, both general def-
ing in rodents does to maintain a relatively        icits and deficits in executive functions have
buffered or hyporesponsive neuroendocrine           been noted ~De Bellis, 2005!. These deficits
stress system.                                      are similar to ones reported for nonhuman pri-
                                                    mates and rats reared under conditions of so-
                                                    cial isolation ~see for review, Sanchez et al.,
Corticolimbic level of stress organization
                                                    2001!. Social, as opposed to stimulus, depri-
Although early experience research in rats fo-      vation may underlie these effects, as only so-
cused initially on development of the HPA           cial deprivation in rats has been shown to
axis ~Levine, 2005b!, recently there has also       influence functions associated with the mPFC
been interest in the impact of early experi-        ~Schrijver, Pallier, Brown, & Wurbel, 2004!.
ences on development of the mPFC and stress-        Notably, few studies of maltreated children or
mediating corticolimbic circuits. Repeated          nonhuman primates have observed problems
separations in rats that result in increased vul-   in memory processes associated with hippo-
nerability to stress also affect the develop-       campal functioning, problems frequently noted
ment of the mPFC. Rat pups exposed to               in rodent studies of maternal deprivation and
disturbances in maternal care exhibit lower         attributed to disturbances in regulation of the
levels of neurotrophins that support neural plas-   HPA axis. Of particular note for abused and
ticity, with decreases most pronounced in the       neglected children is evidence that early ad-
prefrontal cortex ~PFC; Roceri et al., 2004!.       verse care increases the risk of inattention and
Maternally deprived pups also exhibit defi-         overactivity ~Kreppner, O’Connor, & Rutter,
cient attention, particularly problems with set     2001!. These problems are associated with dis-
shifting tasks that are dependent on the mPFC       turbances in frontostriatal circuitry ~Casey
~Lovic & Fleming, 2004!. In addition, they          et al., 1997!. Problems on tasks subserved by
display alterations in the responsiveness of        frontostriatal circuits are noted for children
mesocortical dopamine neurons to stress and         neglected early in life even after, through im-
psychostimulants ~Brake, Zhang, Diorio,             provements in care, the children’s general cog-
Meaney, & Gratton, 2004!. As with work on           nitive functioning has returned to the normal
the HPA axis, these effects appear to be asso-      range ~Bruce, Tarullo, & Gunnar, 2005!. Im-
ciated with maternal behavior, particularly ma-     aging studies also tend to support impacts on
ternal licking and grooming. The co-occurrence      the development of the PFC in maltreated chil-
of problems in mPFC function and stress re-         dren. In addition to overall reductions in brain
sponsiveness likely reflect interactions be-        volume, reduced white matter in the PFC and
tween neuroendocrine stress systems and             corpus collosum have been noted ~see for re-
frontal functioning throughout development.         view, De Bellis, 2005!. In at least one study,
The PFC is also a target of glucocorticoids,        particularly marked disturbances were re-
and there is evidence that both chronic stress      ported in the mPFC. Imaging studies of so-
Stress neurobiology and prevention science                                                                    661

      Figure 3. The average amplitude ~ mV! of feedback negativity across correct and incorrect trials by
      group: means and standard errors. Adapted from Fisher et al. ~2006!. ERPs assessed at Cz to a flanker
      task for children in regular foster care ~RFC!, therapeutic foster care ~TFC!, and community compari-
      son ~CC! conditions. Therapeutic foster care was the Early Intervention Foster Care Program ~Fisher
      et al., 2005!.

cially deprived rhesus infants have reported              ner & Petersen, 1990!, and is a component of
similar findings, particularly with regard to             the mPFC network with extensive reciprocal
reduction in white matter volume in the fron-             connections to the amygdala and hippocam-
tal and parietal cortices and corpus collosum             pus and outflow pathways to the HPA and
~see for review, Sanchez et al., 2001!.                   sympathetic nervous system ~SNS!. Using a
    What is not clear is whether disturbances             flanker task, slowing of responses following
in mPFC functioning are produced by similar               an error and an early negative component in
mechanisms to those producing disturbances                the ERP in response to error feedback were
in behavioral, autonomic, and neuroendocrine              examined in children who had been randomly
responses to stressors. What is apparent from             assigned to treatment versus regular foster care.
the above studies is that translational research          They were compared to a similar group of
should incorporate neurocognitive assess-                 lower income nonmaltreated children. The in-
ments of specific prefrontal regions into as-             tervention involved supporting the foster
sessment protocols ~Pollak, 2005!. Consistent             parent’s ability to manage the behavior prob-
with this suggestion are data from a small                lems of their charges without hostility and in
pilot study using event-related potentials                ways that allowed supportive relationships to
~ERPs! to examine the impact of a foster care             be established between the foster parents and
intervention on activity of the ACC ~Fisher,              child. Children who had lived in treatment
Martin, Bruce, & Fox, 2006; see also Dozier’s             foster care, like the comparison children, ex-
Attachment and Biobehavioral Catch-up Inter-              hibited a significant error feedback-related neg-
vention Protocol, Dozier, Peloso, Sepulveda,              ativity in their ERPs ~see Figure 3!. This was
et al., in press!. The ACC plays a critical role          not noted among the regular foster care chil-
in effortful control of attention and action ~Pos-        dren. These data suggest that treatment foster
662             M. R. Gunnar, P. A. Fisher, and The Early Experience, Stress, and Prevention Network

care had helped normalize this aspect of mPFC       Association of Adverse Early Care With
functioning in these maltreated, foster care        Stress Responding in Adulthood
children.
                                                    The rodent model demonstrates impacts of
                                                    early adverse care on stress neurobiology as-
Summary                                             sessed in adulthood. If the rodent model is to
Four key bridging themes were identified: tim-      help guide intervention work, then adverse
ing, the SHRP, caregiver mediation of the           early care in humans should be associated with
SHRP, and impacts of caregiving on prefron-         heightened adult stress responding. However,
tal functioning and corticolimbic levels of         human studies are invariably difficult to inter-
stress organization. The timing of events rel-      pret because they must be based on experi-
ative to brain development in the rodent and        ments of nature and thus lack the rigor of the
human at birth suggests that the prenatal pe-       animal research. Therefore, as a bridge to hu-
riod in humans should be included as a focus        man development, we will first turn to nonhu-
of translational research on adversity and stress   man primate studies.
neurobiology. In addition, consideration of data
on relative stress hyporesponsiveness sug-          Nonhuman primate outcomes
gests that we should extend the lens beyond
infancy to include most if not all of childhood.    In nonhuman primate research ~see for re-
Evidence that the long-term effects of adverse      view, Levine, 2005b!, researchers have exam-
caregiving in the rodent reflect specific as-       ined a number of different adverse early life
pects of parental care that buffer the HPA axis     experiences, ranging from separating the in-
during early development encourages attempts        fant from the mother ~and sometimes also the
to identify elements of parental or caregiver       social group! to rearing the infant under con-
behavior that may serve similar buffering func-     ditions in which it receives little of the care
tions during human development. Currently,          typical of the species ~i.e., rearing on cloth
the best translation of these rodent findings       surrogates or only with other infant mon-
appears to be in measures of parental sensitiv-     keys!. Typically, outcome measures were ob-
ity and responsiveness; however, the lens on        tained when the animals were older, but still
parenting behavior may widen as more stud-          juveniles. These adverse early care experi-
ies are conducted that examine caregiving           ences have been shown to impact behavior,
and the regulation of stress neurobiology in        increasing fearfulness, reducing exploration
children. Finally, animal studies are begin-        of novel environments, and decreasing social
ning to note the impact of early parental care      status. Notably, relatively few consistent long-
on the development of prefrontal systems, in        term impacts on activity of the HPA axis have
addition to their earlier focus on limbic–          been reported. A few studies have shown long-
hypothalamic functions. These data are con-         term increases in the production of ACTH or
sistent with studies with maltreated children       cortisol; however, most studies have revealed
that have noted significant impacts on execu-       normal to blunted activity of the HPA system.
tive functions, and imaging studies that have          Moving to higher levels of the stress sys-
noted effects on development of the PFC. These      tem, however, we find more evidence of hy-
data emphasize the value of including neuro-        perresponsiveness following early adversity.
cognitive and imaging measures in preventive        Specifically, there is evidence of increased
intervention work on early experiences and          amygdala reactivity ~e.g., increased startle re-
stress in children ~see also Cicchetti, 1996;       sponses! several years after repeated, unpre-
Curtis & Cicchetti, 2003!. We turn now to a         dictable separations early in life ~Sanchez,
more focused examination of the extent to           Noble, et al., 2005!. Disturbances in maternal
which human studies provide evidence that           care in Bonnet macaques produces increased
early maltreatment has lasting impacts on the       CRF in spinal fluid, sensitization of the NE
neurobiology of stress, and in particular on        system, and behavioral sensitization to fear
activity of the HPA axis.                           stimuli, while resulting in normal to low cor-
Stress neurobiology and prevention science                                                        663

tisol levels ~e.g., Rosenblum et al., 2002!. Ex-      ports of childhood experiences, and often fail
treme disturbances in early care ~e.g., isolation     to control for current life stress ~see review by
rearing! have also been shown to produce sub-         Heim, Plotsky, & Nemeroff, 2004!. Nonethe-
tle changes in the primate hippocampus, which         less, these studies provide some useful guid-
may reduce the resilience of the hippocampus          ance. One of the clearest results from these
and increase the risk of hippocampal atrophy          studies is that the association between child-
in response to later insults ~Siegel et al., 1993!.   hood abuse and the neurobiology of stress in
    There is also increasing evidence that early      adulthood is a function of whether or not the
adverse care in primates influences the devel-        abuse is associated with adult psychopathol-
opment of the PFC. Thus, monkeys reared in            ogy. We will, thus, consider outcomes for adults
isolation exhibit problems with tasks that in-        with and without psychological disorders pur-
volve ventromedial regions of the PFC ~see            suant to their childhood maltreatment.
review, Sanchez et al., 2001!. As noted, me-             Turning first to individuals without psycho-
dial and ventral regions of the PFC and ACC           pathology, by definition, these individuals are
have rich bidirectional connections with the          resilient ~Kinard, 1998!. Across the various
amygdala, and appear to be involved in regu-          studies of resilient adults, we find evidence of
lating behavioral, autonomic, and neuro-              reduced activity of stress neurobiology. The
endocrine responses to psychosocial stressors         CRF challenge test produces elevations in
~Sullivan & Gratton, 2002!. Glucocorticoid            ACTH and cortisol. The magnitude of the
overexposure impairs these regions, poten-            ACTH response is inversely proportional to
tially reducing stress and emotion regulatory         the pituitary’s chronic or traitlike exposure to
competence. Overall, the nonhuman primate             CRF ~see for review, Heim et al., 2004!.
data are consistent with the idea that early          Chronic, high CRF exposure from the hypo-
adverse care will have its largest impact on          thalamic CRF system results in downregula-
systems that are still developing. The HPA            tion of CRF receptors in the pituitary. Resilient
axis is relatively mature at birth in most pri-       adult survivors of child abuse produce larger
mates, and thus may be relatively protected           ACTH responses in the CRF challenge test
from permanent changes introduced through             than do healthy adults with nonabusive child-
variations in experience ~e.g., Levine, 2005b!.       hood ~Heim et al., 2004!. This suggests an
In contrast, higher levels of the stress system       atypically low chronic CRF drive in these in-
~e.g., amygdala, mPFC! that mature for lon-           dividuals. Resilient adult survivors also pro-
ger periods after birth in primates may be more       duce larger ACTH responses to a psychosocial
significantly affected. These primate data sug-       stressor than do healthy adults with nonabu-
gest that in studying human postnatal expo-           sive childhood; however, their cortisol and car-
sure to adverse care we may need to focus on          diac responses are normal to blunted ~e.g.,
the extrahypothalamic CRF system, fearful,            Girdler et al., 2003!. This suggests reduced
anxious behavior orchestrated by circuits in-         sensitivity of the adrenal to ACTH. This has
volving the amygdala, and potentially the de-         been confirmed in ACTH challenge tests ~see
velopment of circuits in the PFC that are             for review, Heim et al., 2004!. Thus, individ-
involved in modulating, containing, and ter-          uals ~notably women! who experience sig-
minating fear and stress responses.                   nificant childhood maltreatment but do not
                                                      develop mental disorders show low neuroendo-
                                                      crine responsiveness to stressors, with evi-
Human adults
                                                      dence of traitlike low levels of hypothalamic
The animal models of early adverse care em-           CRF.
phasize deprivation or neglect. In contrast, all         Differences of opinion exist in whether to
of the adult human studies in this area have          view this low level of activity as a risk factor.
examined the sequelae of physical and sexual          Heim et al. ~2004! suggest that sensitization
abuse. Furthermore, most of these studies have        of the pituitary and counterregulation at the
methodological limitations in that they focus         level of the adrenal in these women may pre-
only on women, deal with retrospective re-            dispose them to hypersecrete CRF, result-
664             M. R. Gunnar, P. A. Fisher, and The Early Experience, Stress, and Prevention Network

ing in depression and CRF receptor down-             of the sensitivity of the adrenal. Here, com-
regulation if they experience significant life       pared to adults with depression without
stressors in adulthood. On the other hand, it        childhood abuse who tend to escape from dexa-
is possible that low CRF and HPA reactivity          methasone suppression, depressed adults with
may have preceded rather than followed their         histories of child abuse tend ~like adults with
early life exposures, protecting these individ-      PTSD! to supersuppress in response to the
uals from developing emotional disorders in          low-dose dexamethasone test ~see for review,
response to their adverse childhood experi-          Heim et al., 2004!. However, this supersup-
ences. Future studies involving longitudi-           pression may reflect unmeasured PTSD in
nal work may be able to sort out these two           these depressed, abused women ~Rinne et al.,
alternatives.                                        2002!. Overall, studies using pharmacological
    Depression and PTSD are two of the se-           challenges suggest that childhood abuse plus
quelae of childhood maltreatment that have           either major depression or PSTD in adulthood
been explored most frequently in studies of          is associated with supersuppression of the HPA
stress neurobiology. PTSD and depression ex-         axis combined with hyperactivity of central
amined without reference to childhood abuse          CRF.
appear to share hyperactivity of the central             The results of studies using pharmacologi-
CRF system at hypothalamic and0or extra-             cal challenge tests do not mirror results ob-
hypothalamic levels ~Bremner et al., 1997;           tained when psychosocial stressors are used.
Heim et al., 2004!. Chronic CRF drive on the         Rather than the blunted ACTH response ob-
pituitary in both depression and PTSD leads          served in CRF challenge tests, for both child-
to counterregulatory downregulation at the           hood abuse survivors with major depression
level of the pituitary, resulting in blunted ACTH    ~Heim et al., 2000! and those with PTSD
responses to CRF challenge tests. However,           ~Bremner et al., 1997!, hyperresponsiveness
these disorders differ in the sensitivity of feed-   of ACTH and cortisol have been noted. The
back regulation of the HPA axis. Depression          dissociation between the hyporesponsiveness
among adults is associated with reduced sen-         when the pituitary is pharmacologically chal-
sitivity of negative feedback mechanisms, re-        lenged with CRF, and hyperresponsiveness
sulting in larger cortisol responses to stressors,   when psychosocial challenges are imposed
higher basal levels especially late in the day,      clearly implies upregulation of the corticolim-
and reduced suppression of the axis in re-           bic stress and emotion circuits in response to
sponse to dexamethasone ~Heim et al., 2004!.         childhood abuse. As with the resilient adult
In contrast, PTSD is associated with increased       survivors, we have a chicken and egg prob-
sensitivity of negative feedback mechanisms,         lem. We do not know whether these findings
resulting in blunted cortisol responses to stress-   reflect the effects of child abuse or explain
ors, lower basal cortisol levels especially late     why these individuals developed affective pa-
in the day, and enhanced suppression of the          thology in response to their early maltreat-
axis in response to dexamethasone ~Yehuda,           ment. Again, longitudinal work with maltreated
2000!. The question is whether affective dis-        children is needed to untangle the direction of
orders pursuant to childhood maltreatment fol-       effects.
low these neuroendocrine patterns.
    First, considering depressed women with a
                                                     Developmental Studies of Stress
history of childhood abuse, their patterns of
                                                     and Maltreatment
ACTH and cortisol responses to CRF chal-
lenge is comparable to those observed in             Alterations in stress neurobiology observed
depression and PTSD without early abuse              under conditions of adverse care may reflect
~Bremner et al., 1997; Heim et al., 2004!. Spe-      transient adjustments that will remit to normal
cifically, blunted ACTH and normal to blunted        functioning once the child’s care improves.
cortisol responses have been noted. This find-       Transient adaptations that permit mainte-
ing is consistent with chronic CRF drive.            nance of viability in response to adverse care
ACTH stimulation tests permit examination            reflect allostasis or the maintenance of stabil-
Stress neurobiology and prevention science                                                     665

ity ~homeostasis! through change ~McEwen,          hypocortisolism is a likely consequence of
2000!. Even when these allostatic adjust-          chronic stress ~Friese, Hesse, Hellhammer, &
ments remit under conditions of improved care,     Hellhammer, 2005!. This view is consistent
the processes of neural development they in-       with mounting evidence that children living
fluenced may impact the child’s subsequent         under conditions of maltreatment tend to ex-
development. This can be considered a devel-       hibit low early a.m. levels of cortisol and a
opmental version of the costs associated with      relatively low pattern of cortisol production
allostasis ~McEwen, 2000!. In designing pre-       over the day ~Gunnar & Vazquez, 2001!. In
ventive intervention research, thus, we need       our research network, we have observed low
to consider both the patterning of stress re-      early a.m. cortisol levels among young chil-
sponding observed for children during peri-        dren living in an orphanage in Russia ~re-
ods of adverse care as well as patterns and        viewed in Gunnar & Vazquez, 2001!, toddlers
sequelae noted for maltreated children follow-     within a month of adoption from Russian and
ing improvements in their care.                    Chinese orphanages ~Bruce, Kroupina, Parker,
    As in the adult work, more information is      & Gunnar, 2000!, and both infants and pre-
available on children who have been physi-         schoolers removed from their homes and
cally and sexually abused than on children         placed in foster care ~Dozier, Pelsos, Gordon,
who have been neglected but not abused—            et al., in press; Fisher, 2005!. Among the chil-
although many abused children also suffer ne-      dren recently removed from conditions of ne-
glect ~De Bellis, 2005!. In addition, we have      glect and abuse, between 35 and 40% exhibit
very few studies of children’s responses to        these abnormally low a.m. levels ~see Fig-
psychosocial stressors and even fewer studies      ure 4!. One of us ~Fisher! has examined the
that have used pharmacological probes. Most        abuse histories that differentiate preschool-
of our information on children, therefore,         aged children with low a.m. levels assessed
comes from measures of ambulatory cortisol         within a month of foster placement. Neither
assessed at various points in the day at home      physical nor sexual abuse predicted this pat-
or when the children are in group care set-        tern; instead the best was “failure to provide,”
tings. Although some of the studies have spe-      a measure of neglectful care. This finding is
cifically sampled children with depression or      highly consist with evidence of low a.m. cor-
PTSD, others have selected children based          tisol levels among institutionalized infants
solely on their maltreatment histories. Over-      and toddlers, children whose care has been
all, the study of stress neurobiology in mal-      described as “institutional neglect.”
treated children is a relatively new area, and        As noted, it seems likely that these low a.m.
thus much is unknown or uncertain. Despite         levels reflect a transient downregulation of
this, several themes appear to be emerging         the axis in response to frequent or chronic
that may provide some guidance in preventive       CRF drive and elevated glucocorticoids. Abuse
intervention designs.                              as well as neglect might have this effect. Two
                                                   studies with rhesus infants also tend to sup-
                                                   port this argument. Sanchez and colleagues
Low early a.m. cortisol may reflect                ~McCormack, Maestripieri, Plotsky, & Sán-
ongoing neglect and abuse                          chez, 2003!, have studied the infants of rhesus
                                                   mothers who spontaneously abuse their off-
As described above, counterregulatory mech-        spring. Abuse is most frequent in the first 2
anisms result in downregulation of hypotha-        months of life, followed by rejection and ne-
lamic CRF in response to frequent elevations       glect in subsequent months. During the first
in glucocorticoids ~Makino, Gold, & Schulkin,      month of life, early a.m. cortisol levels are
1994! and downregulation of CRF receptors          elevated in these infants relative to infants of
in the pituitary in response to chronic CRF        supportive mothers; however, beginning in the
drive ~see for review, Heim et al., 2004!. Thus,   second month a.m. levels are suppressed be-
although hyperresponsiveness of the axis is        low those of typically reared rhesus infants. A
often viewed as synonymous with acute stress,      similar pattern has been noted for rhesus in-
You can also read