Australasian Mathematical Psychology Conference 2019 - MELBOURNE, AUSTRALIA
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
Welcome Welcome to Melbourne and welcome to the 2019 Australasian Mathemati- cal Psychology Conference. The conference is hosted this year by the Mel- bourne School of Psychological Sciences at the University of Melbourne, with generous support from the Complex Human Data Hub within the School. In addition to the main conference programme, the schedule and ab- stracts of which are included in this document, we also have a conference dinner (to be held at the Lincoln Hotel) and the traditional soccer game. De- tails about the events and venues can be found in this document (see p. vii) and on the website at http://mathpsy.ch/venues. We would like to give a special thank you to the student volunteers, with- out whom we wouldnʼt be able to have a conference at all! We hope that you have a fantastic time during the conference. Please let us know if we can help you with anything during your time here. Simon Dennis, Daniel Little, Adam Osth, and Simon Lilburn AMPC 2019 Organising Committee We acknowledge the Traditional Owners of the land that this conference is held on, the Wurundjeri peoples of the Kulin nation, and pay our respects to Elders past and present. iii
Code of conduct All attendees, speakers, sponsors and volunteers at our conference are re- quired to agree with the following code of conduct. Our conference is ded- icated to providing a harassment-free conference experience for everyone, regardless of gender, gender identity and expression, age, sexual orienta- tion, disability, physical appearance, body size, race, ethnicity, religion (or lack thereof), or technology choices. We do not tolerate harassment of con- ference participants in any form. Sexual language and imagery is not appro- priate for any conference venue, including talks, workshops, parties, Twitter and other online media. If you have any concerns during your time at the conference, please con- tact one of the organisers. v
Venue The primary location of the conference is the Melbourne Business School, located at 200 Leicester St. just south of the main Parkville campus of the University of Melbourne. This is very easily accessible by tram: all north- bound tram routes from either Flinders St. railway station or Melbourne Central railway station run through the Melbourne CBD (up Swanston St.) and pass by the University of Melbourneʼs main tram stop, which is within easy walking distance of the Business School (the preceding stop at Lincoln Sq. is even closer to the conference venue). The soccer game will be held on the Melbourne University Cricket Grounds, which are a short walk north of the conference venue up Swanston St. and along Tin Alley (into the main campus). This also passes by the Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences. The conference dinner is also located within walking distance of the con- ference venue, at The Lincoln, on the corner of Queensberry and Cardigan streets. These locations are all marked on the accompanying map. vii
viii
Important information Wi-Fi password The conference will have free wi-fi access for all attendees. The SSID of the conference wi-fi is MBS Visitor. When prompted in the portal upon open- ing a web browser, enter the following details: Username cmbs Password 3449 Please let use know if you have any connectivity issues, and we will do our best to provide technical support. Nearby lunch venues The conference is primarily located in the Melbourne Business School, near to the main Parkville campus of the University of Melbourne, on Leicester St. The conference venue is close to several very good places to eat break- fast, lunch, or dinner. In addition to Lygon St., there are a number of very good lunch venues located within walking distance. Here are a few of our favourites: Kaprica Terrific pizza that surpasses anything1 on the traditional Italian dining strip of Lygon St. Cosy, bustling, but not too cramped or crowded. Larger groups may be split, however. Although it has vegan and gluten free options, better vegan and GF options are available slightly farther afield (Shakahari, across Lygon St. on Faraday St., is a very good op- tion, although you may be pressed for time in the lunch break). Lo- cated south of Lincoln Square. Seven Seeds A coffee roaster located across University Square and slightly south (on Berkeley St.) with fare and fit-out straight from Melbourne café central casting, although with the distinction of being one of the original movers on the scene. The coffee is well regarded, particularly the blends served with milk, although many prefer that of the also nearby branch of Everyday Coffee (on Queensberry St.), which is a strictly coffee-only affair. Seven Seeds is particularly good as a place to catch breakfast on the way in. 1 Even the lauded DOC Pizza on Drummond St. ix
x Don Tojo Japanese bento and donburi served with a speed that will make you start wondering whether maybe Bem was on to something. An undergraduate favourite for decades, with a small menu of modest but nourishing options, all for a price that has withstood the tides of inflation. Nasi Lemak House Inexpensive Malaysian hawker food in a small restau- rant. The laksa is good, but the titular nasi lemak is better. Norsiahʼs Kitchen—across Swanston St. from the conference venue—is another option for Malaysian (and Indonesian) food, but with a slightly more no-frills (read: cash only), meat-heavy approach. Soccer match A storied tradition of the AMPC is the inclusion of a sporting event. Often this is cricket but, by request, and following the waning nobility of the Aus- tralian test cricket team, this year we will host a soccer match. Participation in the soccer match is by no means required, or even encouraged, but spec- tators will be treated to a display of majesty and sporting prowess. The talks will end early on the Friday to allow commencement of the game on the Melbourne University cricket pitch (marked on the map) at 5:00 pm. The match will last for one hour, allowing sufficient time for con- valescence before the commencement of the conference dinner at The Lin- coln at 7:30 pm. The institution affiliated with the fairest player of the match, in keeping with tradition, will be awarded the AMPC Ashes. These ashes honour the staid seat of rationality embodied by mathematical psychology.
1 9:00 AM 9:05 AM 9:00 AM. Welcome to AMPC 2019 9:10 AM 9:15 AM Session 9:10 AM. Smith: Modelling the speed and accuracy of continuous outcome colour decisions: 9:20 AM Metric and categorical effects (p. 56) 9:25 AM 9:30 AM 9:35 AM 9:30 AM. Brown: The role of passing time in decision-making (p. 11) 9:40 AM 9:45 AM 9:40 AM. Ratcliff : Revisiting collapsing boundaries (p. 50) 9:50 AM 9:55 AM 10:00 AM 10:05 AM 10:00 AM. Hawkins: Time-varying cognitive models of decision making (p. 24) 10:10 AM 10:15 AM 10:10 AM. Lerche: The diffusion model provides new insights into the field of motivational psychol- 10:20 AM ogy (p. 35) 10:25 AM 10:30 AM 10:30 AM. Eidels: The cost of errors: Confusion analysis and the mental representation of numer- 10:35 AM als (p. 18) 10:40 AM 10:45 AM 10:50 AM Morning tea (provided) 10:55 AM 11:00 AM 11:05 AM 11:10 AM Session 11:05 AM. Heathcote: Control failures in Simon and Flanker Tasks (p. 26) 11:15 AM 11:20 AM 11:25 AM 11:30 AM 11:25 AM. Thorpe: Converting continuous tracking data to response time distributions (p. 59) 11:35 AM 11:40 AM 11:35 AM. Osth: The extralist feature effect in recognition memory: Re-evaluating constraints on 11:45 AM global matching models (p. 46) 11:50 AM 11:55 AM 12:00 PM 11:55 AM. Ballard: The dynamics of decision making during goal pursuit (p. 9) 12:05 PM 12:10 PM 12:15 PM Break for lunch Thursday 14/2 (AM)
2 13:15 PM 13:20 PM Return from lunch 13:25 PM 13:30 PM Session 13:25 PM. McCormick: Using the rank order task to estimate discriminability in eyewitness identifi- 13:35 PM cation (p. 42) 13:40 PM 13:45 PM 13:45 PM. Palada: Accumulating evidence about evidence accumulation models in applied con- 13:50 PM texts (p. 47) 13:55 PM 14:00 PM 13:55 PM. Hotaling: New insights into decisions from experience: Using cognitive models to 14:05 PM understand how value information, outcome order, and salience drive risk taking (p. 27) 14:10 PM 14:15 PM 14:20 PM 14:15 PM. Cavallaro: Consumer choices under time pressure (p. 12) 14:25 PM 14:30 PM 14:25 PM. Cooper: Discriminating shoppers: Applications of SFT to consumer choice (p. 15) 14:35 PM 14:40 PM 14:45 PM 14:50 PM 14:45 PM. Lin: Response times and the exploration-exploitation trade-off (p. 38) 14:55 PM 15:00 PM 14:55 PM. Robins: Causality in social network research (p. 52) 15:05 PM 15:10 PM 15:15 PM 15:20 PM 15:25 PM Afternoon tea (provided) 15:30 PM 15:35 PM 15:40 PM 15:45 PM Session 15:40 PM. Howe: Evidence for a general conformity mechanism: People follow norms even when 15:50 PM they come from the outgroup (p. 28) 15:55 PM 16:00 PM 16:00 PM. Andreotta: Analysing social media data: A mixed-methods framework combining 16:05 PM computational and qualitative text analysis (p. 8) 16:10 PM 16:15 PM 16:10 PM. Kashima: Taking an intentional stance in joint action: How can we explain cross- 16:20 PM cultural variability? (p. 30) 16:25 PM 16:30 PM 16:35 PM 16:30 PM. Cavve: Differentiating social preference in hypothetical distributive decisions (p. 13) 16:40 PM 16:45 PM 16:40 PM. Dennis: Privacy versus open science (p. 16) 16:50 PM 16:55 PM 17:00 PM Thursday 14/2 (PM)
3 9:00 AM 9:05 AM Session 9:00 AM. Perfors: Why do echo chambers form? The role of trust, population heterogeneity, and 9:10 AM objective truth (p. 48) 9:15 AM 9:20 AM 9:25 AM 9:20 AM. Kemp: Season naming and the local environment (p. 31) 9:30 AM 9:35 AM 9:40 AM 9:45 AM 9:40 AM. Shou: Exploring group decision making under ambiguity and risk (p. 55) 9:50 AM 9:55 AM 10:00 AM 10:00 AM. Kuhne: Knowledge is prior: Using past model fits to develpp informative priors in 10:05 AM model selection. (p. 34) 10:10 AM 10:15 AM 10:10 AM. Yim: Semantic integration of novel words through syntagmatic and paradigmatic associ- 10:20 AM ations (p. 65) 10:25 AM 10:30 AM 10:30 AM. Mason: Distraction and delay: Memory and evaluation of temporal sequences of 10:35 AM events (p. 41) 10:40 AM 10:45 AM 10:50 AM Morning tea (provided) 10:55 AM 11:00 AM 11:05 AM 11:10 AM Session 11:05 AM. Newell: Mathematical formalization of Construal Level Theory regarding risk prefer- 11:15 AM ences at different psychological distances (p. 44) 11:20 AM 11:25 AM 11:30 AM 11:25 AM. Goh: The value of predictive information in decision-making under uncertainty (p. 23) 11:35 AM 11:40 AM 11:35 AM. Dunn: Models of risky choice: A signed difference analysis (p. 17) 11:45 AM 11:50 AM 11:55 AM 12:00 PM 11:55 AM. Farrell: Updating judgement contexts with extreme stimuli (p. 19) 12:05 PM 12:10 PM 12:05 PM. Reynolds: I Don’t Know... How to model this. (p. 51) 12:15 PM Break for lunch Friday 15/2 (AM)
4 13:15 PM 13:20 PM Return from lunch 13:25 PM 13:30 PM Session 13:25 PM. Sewell: The speed-accuracy tradeoff in probabilistic categorization: Selective influ- 13:35 PM ence? (p. 53) 13:40 PM 13:45 PM 13:45 PM. Turner: Changing our minds about change-of-mind models: Existing models cannot 13:50 PM account for effects of absolute evidence magnitude (p. 60) 13:55 PM 14:00 PM 13:55 PM. Hayes: The diversity effect in inductive reasoning depends on sampling assump- 14:05 PM tions (p. 25) 14:10 PM 14:15 PM 14:20 PM 14:15 PM. Ransom: When memories fade do sampling effects linger? (p. 49) 14:25 PM 14:30 PM 14:25 PM. Garrett: Estimating multiple item sets: Harder than you think! (p. 22) 14:35 PM 14:40 PM 14:35 PM. Smithson: A new approach to compositional data analysis (p. 57) 14:45 PM 14:50 PM 14:55 PM 14:55 PM. Kennedy: Unrepresentative samples and the quest for generality: Ideas from survey 15:00 PM statistics (p. 32) 15:05 PM 15:05 PM. Martinie: Using crowd meta-knowledge to identify expertise in the single-question 15:10 PM domain (p. 40) 15:15 PM 15:20 PM 15:25 PM Afternoon tea (provided) 15:30 PM 15:35 PM 15:40 PM 15:45 PM 15:40 PM. Lilburn: The integration of stimulus information in visual short-term memory (p. 37) Session 15:50 PM 15:50 PM. Taylor: Examination of doubly stochastic processes in a neural model of visual working 15:55 PM memory. (p. 58) 16:00 PM 16:00 PM. Marris: Modelling human perceptual learning with pre-trained deep convolutional 16:05 PM neural networks (p. 39) 16:10 PM 16:10 PM. Xie: Sequential testimony is as good as independent testimony in judgments under 16:15 PM uncertainty (p. 64) 16:20 PM 16:25 PM 16:20 PM. Blaha: Storyline visualizations for eye tracking data (p. 10) 16:30 PM 16:35 PM Friday 15/2 (PM) The end of talks will be followed at 5:00 PM by soccer, with the conference dinner at 7:30 PM in the Main Dining Room of The Lincoln.
5 9:00 AM 9:05 AM 9:10 AM 9:15 AM 9:20 AM 9:20 AM. Ngo: The effect of stimulus presentation time on response and stimulus bias: A Session 9:25 AM diffusion-model based analysis (p. 45) 9:30 AM 9:30 AM. Moneer: The effect of feature separation on processing architecture and implications for 9:35 AM models of visual attention (p. 43) 9:40 AM 9:45 AM 9:40 AM. Ferdinand: The coevolution of artifacts and ideas: An inference-based model of cultural 9:50 AM evolution (p. 20) 9:55 AM 10:00 AM 10:05 AM 10:00 AM. Zhou: Decision-making in source memory (p. 66) 10:10 AM 10:15 AM 10:10 AM. Fox: Does source memory exist for unrecognized items? (p. 21) 10:20 AM 10:25 AM 10:20 AM. Shabahang: An associative theory of semantic composition (p. 54) 10:30 AM 10:30 AM. Kocsis: The relationship between memory and judgment: Do source memory errors 10:35 AM influence retrospective evaluation? (p. 33) 10:40 AM 10:45 AM 10:50 AM Morning tea (provided) 10:55 AM 11:00 AM 11:05 AM 11:10 AM Session 11:05 AM. Vanunu: Rarity vs. extremity and the effect of task complexity in decisions from expe- 11:15 AM rience. (p. 61) 11:20 AM 11:25 AM 11:30 AM 11:25 AM. Walsh: Re-contextualisation of harmonic power for measuring local complexity (Good 11:35 AM wholesome local fun with fractals and eFourier) (p. 62) 11:40 AM 11:45 AM 11:50 AM 11:45 AM. Liew: Distinguishing new categories from not-old categories (p. 36) 11:55 AM 12:00 PM 11:55 AM. Innes: Flying blind: Does adding information really help? (p. 29) 12:05 PM 12:05 PM. Waters: Comparative analysis of search task performance in 2D and 3D environ- 12:10 PM ments (p. 63) 12:15 PM 12:20 PM 12:15 PM. CoE: Overview of the proposal for the ARC Centre of Excellence for Computational 12:25 PM Behavioural Science (p. 14) 12:30 PM 12:35 PM 12:40 PM Conference conclusion and general meeting Saturday 16/2
Abstracts Note: The presenting author will be typeset in boldface type. 7
8 Analysing social media data: A mixed-methods framework combin- ing computational and qualitative text analysis Matthew Andreotta School of Psychological Science, University of Western Australia Robertus Nugroho Data61, CSIRO Soegijapranata Catholic University Mark Hurlstone School of Psychological Science, University of Western Australia Fabio Boschetti Ocean & Atmosphere, CSIRO Simon Farrell School of Psychological Science, University of Western Australia Iain Walker School of Psychology and Counselling, University of Canberra Cecile Paris Data61, CSIRO To qualitative researchers, social media offers a novel opportunity to har- vest a massive and diverse range of content, without the need for intrusive or intensive data collection procedures. However, performing a qualitative analysis across a large social media data set is cumbersome and impracti- cal. Instead, researchers often extract a subset of content to analyse, but a framework to facilitate this process is currently lacking. We present a four- phased framework for improving this extraction process, which blends the capacities of data science techniques to project large data sets into smaller spaces, with the capabilities of qualitative analysis to address research ques- tions. We applied the framework to 201,506 Australian tweets on climate change from 2016. Through combining Non-Negative Matrix inter-joint Fac- torisation (Nugroho, Zhao, Yang, Paris, & Nepal, 2017) and Topic Alignment (Chuang et al., 2015) algorithms with the qualitative techniques of Thematic Analysis, we derived five overarching topics of climate change commentary. Our approach is useful for researchers seeking to perform qualitative analy- ses of social media, or researchers wanting to supplement their quantitative models with a qualitative analysis of broader social context and meaning. A preprint of this work is available at https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/bynz4 Chuang, J., Roberts, M. E., Stewart, B. M., Weiss, R., Tingley, D., Grimmer, J., & Heer, J. (2015). TopicCheck: Interactive alignment for assessing topic model stability. In Proceedings of the Con- ference of the North American Chapter of the Association for Computational Linguistics - Human Lan- guage Technologies (pp. 175–184). Denver, Colorado: Association for Computational Linguistics. https://doi.org/10.3115/v1/N15-1018 Nugroho, R., Zhao, W., Yang, J., Paris, C., & Nepal, S. (2017). Using time-sensitive interactions to improve topic derivation in Twitter. World Wide Web, 20(1), 61–87. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11280- 016-0417-x
9 The dynamics of decision making during goal pursuit Tim Ballard Psychology, University of Queensland Andrew Neal University of Queensland Simon Farrell University of Western Australia Andrew Heathcote University of Tasmania Goal pursuit can be thought as a series of interdependent decisions made in an attempt to progress towards a performance target. Whilst much is known about the intra-decision dynamics of single, one-shot decisions, far less is known about how this process changes over time as people get closer to achieving their goal and/or as a deadline looms. For example, people may respond to a looming deadline by either increasing the amount of effort they apply or by changing strategy. We have developed an extended version of the linear ballistic accumulator model that accounts for the effects that the dynamics of goal pursuit exert on the decision process. In this talk, I de- scribe a series of recent studies that test this model. In each study, partic- ipants performed a random dot motion discrimination task in which they gained one point for correct responses and lost one point for incorrect re- sponses. Their objective was to achieve a certain number of points within a certain timeframe (e.g., at least 30 points in 40 seconds). Preliminary results suggest that decision thresholds were highly sensitive to deadline, such that people prioritised speed over accuracy more strongly as the time remaining to achieve the goal decreased. The decision process was also sensitive to the amount of progress that remained before the goal was achieved, the diffi- culty of the decision, the incentive for goal achievement, and whether the goal was represented as an approach goal or an avoidance goal. These find- ings illustrate the sensitivity of decision making to the higher order goals of the individual, and provides an initial step towards a formal theory of how these higher level dynamics play out.
10 Storyline visualizations for eye tracking data Leslie Blaha Airman Systems Directorate, Air Force Research Laboratory Dustin Arendt Pacific Northwest National Laboratory Tim Balint TU Delft Joe Houpt Wright State University In this talk, Iʼll review work on data driven clustering and visualizing sets of eye tracking data to capture patterns in dynamic tasks. Storyline visu- alization is a technique that captures the spatiotemporal characteristics of individual entities and simultaneously illustrates emerging group behaviors. We developed a storyline visualization leveraging dynamic time warping to parse and cluster eye tracking sequences. Visualization of the results cap- tures the similarities and differences across a group of observers perform- ing a common task. We applied our storyline approach to gaze patterns of people watching dynamic movie clips. We use these to illustrate variations in the spatio-temporal patterns of observers as captured by different data encoding techniques. We illustrate that storylines further aid in the identi- fication of modal patterns and noteworthy individual differences within a corpus of eye tracking data.
11 The role of passing time in decision-making Scott Brown School of Psychology, University of Newcastle Nathan Evans University of Amsterdam Guy Hawkins University of Newcastle Theories of perceptual decision-making have been dominated by the idea that evidence accumulates in favour of different alternatives until some fixed threshold amount is reached, which triggers a decision. Recent the- ories have suggested that these thresholds may not be fixed during each decision, but change as time passes. These collapsing thresholds can im- prove performance in particular decision environments, but reviews of data from typical decision-making paradigms have failed to support collapsing thresholds. We designed three experiments to test collapsing threshold as- sumptions in decision environments specifically tailored to make them opti- mal. An emphasis on decision speed encouraged the adoption of collapsing thresholds – most strongly through the use of response deadlines, but also through instruction to a lesser extent – but setting an explicit goal of reward rate optimality through both instructions and task design did not.
12 Consumer choices under time pressure Jon-Paul Cavallaro University of Newcastle Guy Hawkins University of Newcastle Scott Brown University of Newcastle Hypothetical consumer choice scenarios provide insight into a consumerʼs decision-making process when purchasing products or services. The con- sumerʼs choices elicited in these scenarios are assumed to indicate the con- sumerʼs subjective value or utility of a product or service. One technique used to represent hypothetical consumer scenarios is the discrete choice experiment (DCE). DCEs are a quantitative technique used to capture con- sumer preferences for multi-attribute products or services. Historically, DCEs account for choices only. We have extended on DCE research by in- cluding a response time measure and a time pressure manipulation to evalu- ate the effect of decision time on the utility inferred from consumersʼ choices. This extension is motivated by findings from the speeded decision-making literature that tells us of the importance of decision time and the impact that time pressure has on choice-related model parameters. Across four hypo- thetical choice scenarios, we found that the time available to make multi- attribute decisions impacts the utility that is inferred from those decisions. The utilities inferred from multi-attribute decisions are inherently tied to the time taken to make those decisions, which has not been widely acknowl- edged in the DCE literature.
13 Differentiating social preference in hypothetical distributive deci- sions Blake Cavve School of Psychology, University of Western Australia Mark Hurlstone School of Psychological Science, University of Western Australia Simon Farrell School of Psychological Science, University of Western Australia Neoclassical economic theory assumes that decision making is primarily driven by rational material self-interest. A number of more recent psycho- logical and economic models challenge this assumption, highlighting in- stead the role of social context in judgement and decision making. Poten- tial manifestations of social preference span several forms of equality or fairness, to various forms of competitive status-based concerns (the most prominent being Brown et al., 2008). Such motivations are assumed to un- derlie financial choices including support for taxation regimes. Even seem- ingly similar social preferences generate different implications regarding distributive and re-distributive decisions of individuals. In order to differentiate preferences reflecting concern for material self- interest, equality and competitive status in re-distributive decisions, Social Utility functions (Loewenstein et al., 1989) were elicited in several (hypothet- ical) decision making domains (e.g., income, vacation time, attractiveness). Preferences regarding hypothetical allocations varied by domain, and sub- stantial discrete individual differences were observed. Overall, Bayesian model selection indicates prominent fairness-based preferences for re- source distribution, consistent with Inequality Aversion (Fehr & Schmidt, 1999). Brown, G. D. A., Gardner, J., Oswald, A. J., & Qian, J. (2008). Does Wage Rank Affect Employeesʼ Well-being? Industrial Relations, 47(3), 355–389. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-232X.2008.00525.x Fehr, E., & Schmidt, K. (1999). A Theory of Fairness, Competition, and Cooperation. The Quar- terly Journal of Economics, 114(3), 817–868. Loewenstein, G., Thompson, L., & Bazerman, M. (1989). Social utility and decision making in interpersonal contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(3), 426–441. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022- 3514.57.3.426
14 Overview of the proposal for the ARC Centre of Excellence for Com- putational Behavioural Science Yoshihisa Kashima Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, University of Melbourne Simon Dennis Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, University of Melbourne Amy Perfors Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, University of Melbourne Thanks to the participation of many members of the mathematical psychol- ogy community, we have submitted an invited proposal for the Centre of Excellence for Computational Behavioural Science. In this discussion, we will present our motivation for the proposal, provide an overview of the pro- posed Centre activities including research, capacity building, and outreach, and seek continuing participation and support of the mathematical psychol- ogy community. Discussions will surround the lessons learned from our preparation of the bid, and the opportunities and potential risks of CoE bids in general.
15 Discriminating shoppers: Applications of SFT to consumer choice Gavin Cooper School of Psychology, University of Newcastle Guy Hawkins University of Newcastle Consumers regularly make multi-alternative, multi-attribute decisions about products and services. The possible decision strategies used by consumers that have been proposed in the literature have been problematic to discrim- inate between in data. These same decision strategies often share higher level features between them that match the mental architectures that Sys- tems Factorial Technology (SFT) has been developed to discriminate be- tween. These higher level features include when a consumer stops process- ing information and makes a decision (the stopping rule) and whether infor- mation is processed in serial or parallel. We have aimed to categorise pre- viously proposed decision strategies by the architecture and stopping rule they assume, creating sets of strategies that can be ruled in or out through the use of the methods of SFT, which we support with data from a novel ex- perimental task. This extension of SFT into the field of consumer choice presents new opportunities in the discrimination between alternative hy- potheses of decision strategies.
16 Privacy versus open science Simon Dennis Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, University of Melbourne Paul Garrett University of Newcastle Hyungwook Yim University of Tasmania Jihun Hamm Ohio State University Adam Osth University of Melbourne Vishnu Sreekumar National Institutes of Health Ben Stone University of Melbourne Pervasive internet and sensor technologies promise to revolutionize psy- chological science. However, the data collected using these technologies is often very personal - indeed the value of the data is often directly related to how personal it is. At the same time, driven by the replication crisis, there is a sustained push to publish data to open repositories. These movements are in fundamental conflict. In this paper, we propose a way to navigate this issue. We argue that there are significant advantages to be gained by ceding the ownership of data to the participants who generate it. Then we provide desiderata for a privacy preserving platform. In particular, we suggest that researchers should use an interface to perform experiments and run analy- ses rather than observing the stimuli themselves. We argue that this method not only improves privacy, but will also encourage greater compliance with good research practices than is possible with open repositories.
17 Models of risky choice: A signed difference analysis John Dunn School of Psychological Science, University of Western Australia Edith Cowan University Li-Lin Rao CAS Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences Signed difference analysis is a methodology that is used to derive a set of or- dinal predictions from a mathematical model (Dunn & James, 2003; Dunn & Kalish, 2018). It generalizes state-trace analysis to models with more than one latent variable (Dunn & Kalish, 2018) where each of two or more depen- dent variables is an arbitrary monotonic function of a specified function of the latent variables. This is a property of many models of risky choice in which the probability of choosing option A over option B is an unknown monotonic function of a model-specific function of the subjective utilities of the two options (and potentially additional parameters). We consider two models of risky choice – the fixed utility model (e.g., cumulative prospect theory) and the random subjective expected utility (RSEU) model proposed by Busemeyer and Townsend (1993). The main difference between the two models is that the predictions of the fixed utility model depend only on the difference between the utilities of the two options while those of the RSEU model also the utilities are modified by a term representing their subjective variance. We derive critical predictions from each of these models and test them against data from two experiments. Busemeyer, J. R. & Townsend, J. T. (1993). Decision field theory: A dynamic-cognitive approach to decision making in an uncertain environment. Psychological Review, 100(3), 432–459. Dunn, J. C. & Anderson, L. M. (2018). Signed difference analysis: Testing for structure under monotonicity. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 85(3), 36–54. Dunn, J. C. & James, R. N. (2003). Signed difference analysis: Theory and application. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 47(4), 389–416. Dunn, J. C. & Kalish, M. L. (2018). State-trace analysis. Springer.
18 The cost of errors: Confusion analysis and the mental representa- tion of numerals Ami Eidels University of Newcastle Murray Bennett University of Newcastle Paul Garrett University of Newcastle People express quantities via numbers, using a remarkably small set of only ten basic units – digits. Confusing digits could be costly, and not all confu- sions are equal; confusing a price tag of 2 dollars with 9 dollars (or 2 million vs 9 million, for a more dramatic effect), is naturally more costly than con- fusing 2 with 3. Confusion patterns are intimately related to the distances between mental representations, which are hypothetical internal symbols said to stand for, or represent, ʻrealʼ external stimuli. The distance between the mental representations of two digits could be determined by their nu- merical distance. Alternatively, it could be driven by visual similarity (or by some other properties). We investigated the mental representations of familiar and unfamiliar numerals (4 sets: Arabic, Chinese, Thai, and non- symbolic dots) in a set of identification experiments, using Multi Dimen- sional Scaling and Cluster Analysis. We controlled for undesired effects of response bias using Luceʼs choice model.
19 Updating judgement contexts with extreme stimuli Simon Farrell School of Psychological Science, University of Western Australia Greta Fastrich University of Reading A number of theories assume that objects are not judged in isolation, but are compared to other objects. In many experiments the context is the ob- jects seen in the experiment; for example, if judging the size of squares, the judgement context would be the set of all (or some of) the squares seen so far in the experiment. We ask what happens when an extreme stimulus is only occasionally presented. Does it enter into the judgement context, or is it effectively discounted? Across two experiments involving magni- tude judgements on squares and numbers, we find little effect of the out- lier on following judgements. Nonetheless, we show that people used the experiment context to form their judgements, by showing sensitivity to the skew of the distributions. Fitting two models of context-based judgement— Parducciʼs range-frequency theory, and Haubensakʼs consistency model— suggests the combined effects of overall context and individual items is chal- lenging.
20 The coevolution of artifacts and ideas: An inference-based model of cultural evolution Vanessa Ferdinand Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, The University of Melbourne Learning is rarely, if ever, an unbiased process. As cultural artifacts repli- cate by being passed from individual to individual, among social groups, and across generations, the cognitive biases involved in the perception, pro- cessing, and production of these artifacts can operate as selection pressures on them, causing certain forms to increase in number at the expense of oth- ers. Here, I will discuss the similarities between replicator dynamics (a gen- eral model of evolution) and Bayesian inference (a general model of learn- ing) and utilize their mathematical equivalence to specify a model where cultural artifacts and learnersʼ hypotheses about those artifacts co-evolve. Culture is a special evolutionary system that is composed of two types of replicators: public structures in the world, such as artifacts and behaviors, and private structures in the mind, such as brain states or hypotheses (Sper- ber, 1996). The most interesting part of this model is the interpretation of fitness for both types of replicators. The fitness of public replicators is given by their likelihood under the population of hypotheses in learnersʼ minds, and the fitness of private replicators is dictated by their likelihood under the population of artifacts in their environment. Both of these replicators can place constraints on one another as culture evolves and drive the system to unexpected places when fitness values are asymmetric.
21 Does source memory exist for unrecognized items? Julian Fox University of Melbourne Adam Osth University of Melbourne Source memory is memory for the context in which information is pre- sented. Most models of source memory predict that it is not possible to re- trieve source information from items that are unrecognized. For example, multinomial processing-tree models (e.g., Batchelder & Riefer, 1990) and the bivariate signal detection model of Hautus et al. (2008) predict that when an item is unrecognized, source retrieval is not performed and a guess re- sponse is elicited. Empirically, there have been mixed results regarding the possibility of source discrimination for unrecognized items. Studies that presented recognition and source judgments for the same item in immedi- ate succession (i.e., a non-blocked design) revealed chance-level source ac- curacy for unrecognized items, while studies that presented an initial block of recognition judgments, followed by a block of source judgments (i.e., a blocked design), revealed slightly above-chance source accuracy for unrec- ognized items. A potential explanation for the discrepancy is that source discrimination is possible for unrecognized items, but that when a negative recognition judgment is made immediately prior to the source judgment, as is the case in non-blocked designs, participants are dissuaded from attempt- ing effortful source retrieval. The present study assessed source memory for unrecognized items in three conditions: non-blocked, blocked, and ʻre- verse blockedʼ (where the block of source judgments preceded the recogni- tion block). It was found that accuracy was significantly above chance in the blocked and reverse blocked conditions, but consistently at chance in the non-blocked condition. These results suggest that source discrimination is above chance for unrecognized items, but that blocked designs are needed to elucidate the effect as non-blocked designs lead to source guessing.
22 Estimating multiple item sets: Harder than you think! Paul Garrett Psychology, The University of Newcastle Zachary Howard The University of Newcastle Joe Houpt Wright State University David Landy Indiana University Ami Eidels The University of Newcastle Like many species, humans can perform non-verbal estimates of quantity through our innate approximate number system. However, the cognitive mechanisms that govern how we compare these estimates are not well un- derstood. Little research has addressed how the human estimation-system evaluates multiple quantities, and fewer studies have considered the cost to cognitive workload when undertaking these tasks. Here, we apply the math- ematical tools of Systems Factorial Technology to a comparative estimation task. Across a series of experiments, we assess whether quantities, repre- sented by red and blue discs, are estimated simultaneously (in parallel) or sequentially (in serial), and under what restrictions to cognitive workload. Our findings reveal that two item-sets may be estimated simultaneously through a parallel estimation system, under severe restrictions to cognitive workload capacity. These restrictions are not due to the estimation process. The results can be extended to comparisons made with the subitizing range.
23 The value of predictive information in decision-making under uncer- tainty Ariel Goh School of Psychological Sciences, Monash Institute of Cognitive and Clinical Neurosciences Daniel Bennett Princeton University Stefan Bode The University of Melbourne Trevor T-J Chong Monash University Humans exhibit a drive towards acquiring information. Notably, studies of humans and non-human animals suggest that information is processed by similar neural circuits that underlie reward valuation. This project inves- tigated how humans value information that predicts, but does not change, the outcome of an upcoming event (non-instrumental information). We con- ducted two experiments to examine the physical effort costs individuals are willing to incur for such information. Effort was operationalised as amounts of force applied to a hand-held force-sensitive dynamometer. In the first experiment, the amount of information available was held constant, and participants chose between exerting higher effort levels to obtain predic- tive information about a lottery outcome, versus exerting minimum effort and foregoing such information. Results showed that participants willingly exerted effort to obtain the information, but this effect declined as effort costs increased. In Experiment 2, we manipulated the amount of informa- tion provided at the start of each trial, and thus the amount of uncertainty participants experienced. Results showed that participants invested more effort for information when prior uncertainty was high (i.e., when the out- come was ambiguous) compared to when it was low (i.e., when the outcome was predictable). Bayesian model comparison using the Watanabe-Akaike Information Criterion revealed that subjective valuation of information was best modelled as a function of both effort costs and the magnitude of avail- able information, where information was quantified as the degree to which information reduced residual uncertainty about the outcome. Model com- parison also suggested that participantsʼ uncertainty was best modelled by the Rényi entropy of beliefs (a generalisation of Shannon entropy). Overall, these results suggest that informationʼs intrinsic value is based on its capac- ity to reduce uncertainty, and that this valuation is reflected in a willingness to trade off effort for information. This work helps explain the bias humans exhibit toward information acquisition, even when this is sub-optimal or inefficient.
24 Time-varying cognitive models of decision making Guy Hawkins Psychology, University of Newcastle David Gunawan University of New South Wales Robert Kohn University of New South Wales Scott Brown University of New South Wales Almost all cognitive process models of decision making assume that the la- tent parameters driving performance are stationary across trials. This con- flicts with intuition, and data, that performance does not change with in- creasing exposure to a task. Here, we outline a flexible hierarchical Bayesian framework that allows for across-trial dynamics in the parameters of de- cision making models, and thus makes time-varying predictions for be- haviour. We demonstrate the approach with the Linear Ballistic Accumu- lator (LBA) model. We show that time-varying LBA models reliably recover the data-generating model in simulated data, and they are consistently se- lected over equivalently specified stationary LBA models. Furthermore, the time-varying LBA model provides a good account of across-trial dynamics observed in choice and response time data, and time-varying parameter esti- mates that provide insight into the dynamics of latent cognitive mechanisms driving observed decision behaviour.
25 The diversity effect in inductive reasoning depends on sampling as- sumptions Brett Hayes Psychology, University of New South Wales Danielle Navarro University of New South Wales Rachel G. Stephens University of New South Wales Keith Ransom University of Adelaide Natali Dilevski University of Sydney A key phenomenon in inductive reasoning is the diversity effect, whereby a novel property is more likely to be generalized when it is shared by an ev- idence sample composed of diverse instances than a sample composed of similar instances. We describe a Bayesian model and an experimental study that show that the diversity effect depends on a belief that samples of evi- dence were selected by a helpful agent (strong sampling). Inductive argu- ments with premises containing either diverse or non-diverse evidence sam- ples were presented under different sampling conditions, where instruc- tions and filler items indicated that the samples were selected intentionally (strong sampling) or randomly (weak sampling). A robust diversity effect was found under strong sampling but was attenuated under weak sampling. As predicted by our Bayesian model, the largest effect of sampling was on arguments with non-diverse evidence, where strong sampling led to more restricted generalization than weak sampling. These results show that the characteristics of evidence deemed relevant to an inductive reasoning prob- lem depend on beliefs about how the evidence was generated.
26 Control failures in Simon and Flanker Tasks Andrew Heathcote Psychology, University of Tasmania Dora Matzke University of Amsterdam We examine the complete failure of control within the Conflict LBA model, which explains conflict in Stroop, Simon and Flanker tasks in terms of prim- ing and the control deployed to counter the misleading effects of priming. A key concept for the model is that control is variable, sometimes under- compensating and sometimes overcompensating for priming. Within the context of the Conflict LBA model, we extended MacLeod and MacDon- aldʼs (2000) “inadvertent reading hypothesis” for the Stroop task, that oc- casional reading rather than colour naming responses explain some por- tion of incongruent errors and speeding in correct congruent responses, to Simon task data, collected by Forstman, van den Wildenberg and Rid- derinkhof (2008), and Flanker data, collected by White, Ratcliff and Starns (2011). Although the probability of complete failures to exercise any con- trol on some trials, causing participants to perform the wrong task (e.g., responding based on location in the Simon task or to the Flankers in the Flanker task), was relatively small, such failures were key to explaining the detailed shapes of conditional-accuracy functions. In the Flanker task, fail- ure probability was found to increase systematically as the proportion of incongruent trials in each block decreased. Estimations issues and the rela- tionships among the parameters of the extended Conflict LBA are discussed. Forstmann, B. U., van den Wildenberg, W. P., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2008). Neural mecha- nisms, temporal dynamics, and individual differences in interference control. Journal of Cogni- tive Neuroscience, 20(10), 1854–1865. MacLeod, C. M., & MacDonald, P. A. (2000). Interdimensional interference in the Stroop effect: Uncovering the cognitive and neural anatomy of attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(10), 383–391. White, C. N., Ratcliff, R., & Starns, J. J. (2011). Diffusion models of the flanker task: Discrete versus gradual attentional selection. Cognitive Psychology, 63(4), 210-238.
27 New insights into decisions from experience: Using cognitive mod- els to understand how value information, outcome order, and salience drive risk taking Jared M. Hotaling University of New South Wales Chris Donkin University of New South Wales Ben R. Newell University of New South Wales Andreas Jarvstad City, University of London Many real world decisions must be made on basis of experienced outcomes. However, little is known about the mechanism by which people make these decisions from experience. Much of the previous research has focused on contrasting these decisions with those based on described alternatives. Ob- servations of a reliable description-experience gap (D-E gap) led Hotaling, Jarvstad, Donkin, and Newell (under review) to conduct a series of studies investigating various factors influencing decisions from experience. Criti- cally, they found that the juncture at which value and probability informa- tion is provided has a fundamental effect on choice. They also found evi- dence for the impact of perceptual salience and outcome recency on choice. To better understand these results and their implications regarding the mechanisms underlying human decision making, we developed an exemplar- based cognitive model. It uses a noisy error-prone memory mechanism to explain how confusions between events give rise to various behavioral patterns. According to the model, each time an outcome is experienced, a record is laid down in memory. However, memory traces can be disturbed in several ways as new information enters the system. We tested several ver- sions of models within this basic framework, and found that one with mech- anisms for value-assignment confusions and risk bias provided the best ac- count. We discuss the implications of these findings on our understanding of the interplay between attention, memory, and choice, and the psychologi- cal underpinning of the description-experience gap.
28 Evidence for a general conformity mechanism: People follow norms even when they come from the outgroup Piers D. L. Howe Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, University of Melbourne Campbell Pryor Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, University of Melbourne Amy Perfors Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, University of Melbourne People are more likely to perform a particular action or hold a particular opinion when they know that other people have performed similar actions or have similar opinions, a phenomenon known as the descriptive norm ef- fect. There are a number of competing accounts of this phenomenon. Our previous work provided strong evidence against two of these accounts, the information and social sanctions account, and argued in favour of the ac- count proposed by self-categorization theory (Pryor, Perfors, Howe, 2019, Nature Human Behaviour, 3, 57-62). Self-categorization theory makes the intuitive prediction that people will actively avoid conforming to the norms of an outgroup in an effort to remain distinct from that outgroup. We tested this prediction in a series of experiments. By comparing competing Bayesian models, we showed that people conformed to descriptive norms even when they came from the outgroup. This result was replicated across multiple def- initions of ingroups and outgroups, including when the outgroup had oppos- ing social or political beliefs to the participant, and was robust with respect to our chosen priors. Additionally, we showed this effect for both meaning- ful and arbitrary norms, thereby ruling out a number of alternative expla- nations. These results suggest that a general desire to conform with others may outpower the common ingroup vs outgroup mentality. We make sug- gestions as to how this general conformity mechanism may operate.
29 Flying blind: Does adding information really help? Reilly Innes Psychology, University of Newcastle Zachary Howard University of Newcastle Alexander Thorpe University of Newcastle Ami Eidels University of Newcastle Scott Brown University of Newcastle In driving and avionics, as well as many other information rich environ- ments, the user interface is responsible for providing accessible and use- ful information without making the task more difficult. Adding information into a display is often viewed as a way to make a task easier and make use of emerging technology. However, designers often fail to account for the possible cost this may have on cognitive workload or task performance. In collaboration with Airbus & Hensoldt, we investigated the effects of new heads up display technology, which aimed to increase the amount of avail- able information to pilots. Using the detection response task (DRT), we pro- vide a measure of cognitive workload during a simulated helicopter flight. Thirteen pilots completed a 2x2 within-subjects experiment, where visual environment and level of information was manipulated. Participants re- sponse times to the DRT provide an index of cognitive workload, and this measure was analysed alongside flight performance. Results indicated that increased information improved flight performance. Furthermore, DRT re- sults indicated that cognitive workload was relatively unaffected by the level of symbology. This initial experiment is useful but requires a level of ques- tioning as to possible alternative explanations for results – which should be addressed in future studies.
30 Taking an intentional stance in joint action: How can we explain cross-cultural variability? Yoshihisa Kashima Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, University of Melbourne Michael Kirley University of Melbourne Yuan Sun RMIT Alex Stivala Swinburne University of Technology Simon Laham University of Melbourne Piers Howe University of Melbourne The concept of intention seems to be central in human sociality. Humans ascribe intentionality not only to other humans, but also to nonhuman be- ings and even inanimate objects to understand, explain, and predict their behaviours. This cognitive practice of taking an intentional stance seems ubiquitous. Yet, there is also some evidence of cross-cultural and historical variability in the extent to which people take an intentional stance. This is puzzling because intentional stance taking (IST) is regarded as a necessary aspect of joint action. If in fact humanityʼs success is largely due to our abil- ity to engage in joint actions to achieve a goal unattainable by individuals alone, and IST is necessary for engaging in a joint action, how can a society function without engaging in IST? We constructed a cultural evolutionary model to explain this apparent cross-cultural variability. We incorporate signalling to model intention-reading as integral to the stag hunt game as a game theoretic model of joint action, postulate different IST types that vary from a minimal level of mindreading to a heightened awareness and explicit consideration of other minds (hyper IST), and show by simulations the envi- ronmental circumstances in which different IST types become prevalent in a population. We show that minimal IST becomes a predominant cognitive practice when a community affords a limited opportunity to interact with strangers, whereas hyper IST becomes more predominant when society is highly open and mobile with many chances of interacting with strangers. Yet, if societal mobility is extremely high, hyper IST cannot sustain coopera- tive joint actions unless there is an institutional mechanism of social control to sanction against deception and defection. Implications of this research are discussed for theory of mind research, moral psychology, and other con- temporary research in psychology and cognitive science.
31 Season naming and the local environment Charles Kemp University of Melbourne Alice Gaby Monash University Terry Regier University of California, Berkeley Seasonal patterns vary dramatically around the world, and we explore the extent to which systems of season categories support efficient communica- tion about the local environment. Our analyses build on a domain-general information-theoretic model of categorization across languages, and we identify several qualitative predictions that emerge when this model is ap- plied to season naming, including the prediction that systems with odd num- bers of terms should be comparatively rare. We test the model quantitatively using a collection of season systems drawn from the linguistic and anthro- pological literature and data specifying temperature and precipitation in locations associated with these systems. The results include some successes for the model but also highlight some significant respects in which our ap- proach falls short of a complete account of season naming.
32 Unrepresentative samples and the quest for generality: Ideas from survey statistics Lauren Kennedy School of Social Work, Columbia University Andrew Gelman Columbia University Psychology has long been reliant on a variety of convenience samples. Al- though the source has changed with tools like Amazon Mechanical Turk, there is still no guarantee that AMT samples are representative of the gen- eral population on all relevant demographics. Convenience samples can be a threat to the generality of a study if the effect size differs between demo- graphic subsets and the sample differs in proportion to those subsets. Tools from survey statistics, a field which specializes in generalization from non- representative samples to a wider population, have not previously been ap- plicable due to the reliance on probability samples. In this talk we consider multilevel regression and poststratification (MRP), a technique that has proved useful for non-probability samples. We discuss the potential costs and benefits of incorporating this technique with psychological datasets.
33 The relationship between memory and judgment: Do source mem- ory errors influence retrospective evaluation? Marton Kocsis School of Psychological Science, The University of Western Australia Simon Farrell The University of Western Australia We often use summary judgments of our past experiences to inform future choices, with these evaluations argued to rely on what we can remember about each experience at time of choice. If we are retrospectively evaluat- ing multiple experiences from memory facilitate choice, do source mem- ory errors (e.g., misattributing events from one particular experience to an- other) influence our evaluation of a target experience? We presented par- ticipants with 3 interleaved affective word-lists (coded by colour) that on average were either positive or negatively valenced, and we post-cued one word-list as the target list for which recall and pleasantness ratings were captured. While people were able to successfully recall some items from the target list with minimal non-target intrusions and pleasantness ratings were consistent with mean valence of the target list, patterns of recall were inconsistent with the ability of the valence of target list items to predict rat- ings which appears to argue against evaluation being based on recalled tar- get list items. To further examine the relationship between memory and judgment, we compared models based of online evaluation that relies on the target-list items presented vs. retrieval-based evaluation based on target-list items recalled, and found that the retrieval-based model of evaluation was favoured by the data. One reconciliation of these seemingly contradictory results is that pleasantness ratings relied on the memory for evaluation of target list items which is retrieved independent of memory for the target list items themselves.
You can also read