THE SELF-CONCEPT OF YOUNG PEOPLE WITH SPINA BIFIDA: A POPULATION-BASED STUDY
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
THE SELF-CONCEPT OF YOUNG PEOPLE WITH SPINA BIFIDA: A POPULATION-BASED STUDY P. L . Appleton P. E. Miinchom N. C.Ellis C.E. Elliott V. Boll P. Jones This paper reports a study of self-concept However, samples have tended to be small in young people with spina .bifida. (Campbell ec al. 1977, Spaulding and Previous studies are briefly described, Morgan 1986, Lavigne et a/. 1988); hospital followed by an account of the relevance to out-patient clinic lists have been the physical disability of findings in the predominant source of samples; there developmental psychology of self-concept. have been 10 selection biases in some Interventions by professionals for studies (Campbell er a/. 1977, Spaulding physically disabled young people will need and Morgan 1986, Murch and Cohen increasingly to take account of young 1989); and theoretical models of self- people’s views of themselves, and their concept development and disability have wishes and plans for the future not been employed (Harper 1991). Thus, (Department of Health 1989). It follows for instance, while it is known that able- that the scientific study of self-concept bodied teenagers are concerned with their and self-worth in those with a physical own physical appearance and that this disability is an essential component of the factor is linked very closely to their global knowledge that professionals should be self-worth (Harter 1986), no study has using for designing multi-agency inter- identified the exact association between ventions. Young people’s overall sense of feelings about the body and feelings about self-worth, their motivation to develop the self-as-a-whole of young people with and change, the value they place on spina bifida. Until recently one problem various aspects of personal functioning, has been the lack of appropriate measuring and their sense of social identity, are all instruments. factors which are central to an under- It is now known that self-concept can standing of how best to intervene to help be broken down into dimensions (or young people with physical disabilities ‘domains’), and that children and teen- (Thomas et al. 1989) and how to provide agers can accurately assess what they feel more appropriate developmental oppor- about themselves in relation to these differ- tunities (Rutter 1987). ent domains (Harter 1986, Marsh 1989). To date, the reported studies of global We would expect that children with self-worth (or self-esteem) in spina bifida spina bifida, like able-bodied children, subjects find mean differences between would have individual areas of difficulty spina bifida and control samples to be and individual areas of success in the marginal (Campbell el at. 1977) or non- various domains of self-conceut. In existent (Spaulding and Morgan 1986). general it would be expected that, because
of difficulties with mobility, continence importance reduced. and intellectual performance, young Harter (1986) and Crocker and Major people with spina bifida would evaluate (1989) summarised evidence that actively themselves relatively poorly in the domains reducing the importance of domains in of athletic, physical and scholastic self- which people perceive themselves to be v QI concept. Harter (1985a, 1986, 1990) used less able, i.e. ‘discounting’, optimises Q‘ the term ‘perceived competence’ to refer global self-worth. By placing less impor- to the evaluation the young person makes tance on (or discounting) a particular of performance in each domain. I t would aspect of self-development, performance be useful to know whether perceived com- failures in an identified area are no longer petence in these key arcas is indeed lower a threat to self-worth. However, this among physically disabled than among cognitive process could result in decreased able-bodied young people. motivation for change and development However, the evaluation placed on (Crocker and Major 1989). experience or performance in a particular One of the most robust findings in the domain of self-concept will depend on literature of self-esteem is the correlation how important that area is to the indi- between a person’s evaluation of their vidual personally: not being good at physical appearance and their global self- mathematics may not affect your self- worth. Harter (1986) showed that self- esteem if it is not important to you rated physical appearance, as one domain (Harter 1986). of self-concept, correlates more highly I t is essential, therefore, that self- (0.6 to 0.8) with global self-worth than concept instruments incorporate a method does any other domain. There is also of ascertaining the importance that a evidence that this correlation is higher for child places on each domain, as well as a girls (Harter 1986). So how do young self-rated measure of competence. I f people with a physical disability cope with children feel competent in a domain the socio-cultural emphasis, especially for which is important to them, then self- adolescent girls, on the value of physical worth is likely t o be boosted. On the other appearance? hand, competence may be irrelevant for From early adolescence onwards, able- self-worth if the domain is not important bodied girls are at greater risk for low or personally valued. Using instruments self-esteem and depression than boys developed by Harter ( 1 9854, and Renick (Harter 19850, Petersen et al. 1991). and Harter (1988), it is possible to Studies of self-concept in those with a measure both competence and importance physical disability have rarely examined for each domain. The relation between gender, perhaps because of sample-size these two measures can then be examined. problems. It may be a salient variable, Following William James’ notion of the acting as an effect modifier, protecting or ratio of pretension (aspiration or impor- making the disabled child more vulner- tance) to success (competence), Harter able. For instance, being female and being calculated a ‘discrepancy’ score (com- adolescent (as distinct from being in petence minus importance) to convey the middle childhood) might render the size and direction of disparity for each physically disabled young person more domain. Harter (1986) demonstrated a susceptible to socio-cultural norms con- linear relationship between discrepancies cerning the importance of physical and global self-worth, negative domain appearance. discrepancies being associated with lower One cognitive process that could be global self-worth. protective of self-worth is the young ’ How would a child with several areas of person’s choice of a model for comparison difficulty cope with a range of potentially (Renick and Harter 1989). In a study by stressful discrepancies between competence Harter (1986), there was evidence to and importance? If the child feels less suggest that mentally disabled children competent than peers, but experiences chose to compare themselves primarily similar aspirations, there is a risk of low with other mentally disabled children. self-worth. In order to regulate this, Global self-worth is also governed by competence could be overestimated or the extent to which a young person feels 199
supported and accepted by parents, worth. It was'hypothesised that, while the teachers and peers (particularly class- relationship between discrepancies and mates). For able-bodied children, these global self-worth would, in general, hold associations are independent of the across groups, the physical appearance associations between domain-specific self- discrepancy would be less strongly evaluations and global self-worth (Harter associated with global self-worth in young 1986). For children with congenital/ people with spina bifida. 0 TJ acquired limb deficiencies, low classmate ( 5 ) To explore whether young people with L .- m support is a significant predictor of low SB are more likely to compare themselves 0 .-C self-worth and depressive symptomatology with other physically disabled young a vl (Varni et al. 1992). people or with able-bodied young people This study tested a number of theor- when assessing their own competencies in etical predictions, all of which are self-concept domains. -a w relevant to the design of interventions. In (6) To study the impact of the choice of 8 a doing so, we attempted to overcome some social comparison group on self-rated tli C of the methodological problems of competence and global self-worth among 7 previous studies. The objectives and young people with SB. It was hypothesised > hypotheses were as follows. that comparing oneself with other L 0 ( I ) To examine the multidimensional physically disabled young people would structure of self-concept in young people be associated with higher competence with spina bifida (SB) and to compare it scores than when comparing oneself with r: with that of able-bodied (AH) young AH peers. vl people who have no known learning (7) To study young people's perceived difficulties. social support as a function of group, age (2) To study and compare young people's and gender; to investigate the association self-rated competence in each of nine self- between social support and global self- concept domains and in global self-worth, worth. and to examine the effects of age and gender on the above measures. In line Method with the preceding discussion, it was For the purposes of this study, spina hypothesised that there would be group bifida was defined as open or closed (AB>SB), gender (M>F), and age (Y>O) myeiomeningocele, with or without main effects for social acceptance, athletic associated hydrocephalus, with or competence, physical appearance and without ventricular drainage. All patients global self-worth. We expected older girls had measurable functional impairment of with spina bifida to have the very lowest locomotion, continence, intellect or scores in these domains and in overall physical parameters associated with the self-worth. spina bifida. They were aged between (3) To study the personal importance or seven years and 18 years 1 1 months. The value which young people place on each diagnosis was confirmed in all cases with self-concept domain, comparing groups, the young person's own medical advisers and examining the effect of age and and/or by review of the medical notes. gender. In order to examine the possibility Case notes were obtained in order to of discounting, it was hypothesised that clarify points in the history and on cases group would be a main effect (SB < AB) in not seen clinically. all academic, physical appearance and athletic domains, with the exception of Subjects . older girls with spina bifida, among Recruitment for the study was from the whom the importance of physical appear- adjoining health authority catchment ance and athletic competence would be at areas of Clwyd, Wirral, Chester and the same mean levels as AB. Crewe. In order to get as complete a (4) To study the discrepancies between sample as possible, extensive investigations importance and self-rated competence in were made with consultant paediatricians, each domain by group, age and gender, clinical medical officers, the Association and to investigate the degree to. which for Spina Bifida and Hydrocephalus 200 these discrepancies determine global self- (ASBAH), and the Family Fund (a UK
TABLE I - n N Demographic characteristics of study sample 03 0' Conlrols Spina bi/ida Rejusals (N= 79) (N= 79) ( N = 17) Age (yrsmkhhs) Mean 13:) 13:7 12:ll SD 47 38 21 Gender Male 38 38 7 Female 41 41 10 Education/work status Mainstream 63 49 7 Special school 0 17 8 2 Residential college 0 4 0 Mainstream co,llege 9 1 1 College (SND) 0 3 0 Not in education 7 5 I 'SND= special needs department. national register of families with a the medical interview, and three further disabled child claiming a specific benefit). subjects did not receive the medical All eligible subjects were approached in interview for administrative reasons, but the first three areas from which ascer- in all these cases the medical records tainment data were available; in the final enabled a disability severity score area, subjects were approached on the (Wallander e l a/. 1989) t o be calculated. basis of random selection to complete our The remaining 70 received detailed sample. A total of 104 subjects were clinical assessment. approached. Medical assessments were performed by Seventeen families indicated that they five assessors (P.E.M.,G.c., v.K., R.P.,R.B.), did not wish to participate in the research, in a clinical setting rather than at home. and a further eight children were excluded If there was doubt about details in the due to either apparent severe cognitive history, these details were verified from impairment or severe family distress. This medical notes. Generally, parental recall left 79 cases in the sample. proved accurate. For those nine patients For each subject with spina bifida we not examined clinically, as much infor- obtained a comparison subject, matched mation as possible was collated from their for age (plus or minus six months), gender, hospital and community medical records. classroom and housing neighbourhood. The medical assessment comprised a Comparison subjects were selected from detailed medical history, questions on the same mainstream classroom as that aspects of physical ability and indepen- attended by the student with spina bifida dence (including continence and or, in the case of older subjects, from the mobility), a full physical examination and same/similar workplace, college or em- neurological assessment, and vision and ployment training scheme. If the child hearing screening. with spina bifida was not in mainstream education, the comparison subject was DISABILITY SEVERITY SCORE chosen from an appropriate local school. Clinical severity for thoracic, lumbar and The comparison subjects had no known sacral lesions was quantified following the chronic illness, disability or special criteria of Wallander el al. (1989), who educational need (Table I). defined the degree of severity of spina bifida according to six medical parameters Medical assessment (Table 11). The score was not designed to The sample comprised 79 pairs of include cervical lesions, and therefore the subjects. Six subjects chose not t o have three cervical cases have been exctuded. 201
TABLE I I Medical parameters assessed for Wallander and colleagues' severity score Parameter Disability Clinically Total score evaluated sample (N 70) 2 ( N = 79) h' (%I N (To) Level o j lesion Sacral 0 I I (16) I I (14) Lumbar I 26 (37) 32 (41) Thoracic 2 30 (43) 33 (42) Cervical 3 (4 3 (4) Operations jor ventricular valve None 0 18 (26) 23 (29) I I I I (16) I2 ( I S ) 22 2 41 (59) 44 (56) Operationsfor skin ulcer below waist None 0 61 (87) 69 (87) 21 2 9 (13) 10 (13) Total surgical operations required Sone 0 1-2 I 23 2 Level oJ' independent amhulation No aids 0 16 (23) 18 (23) Braces I 31 (44) 36 (46) Wheelchair or carried 2 23 (33) 25 (32) Bladder junction Continent 0 14 (20) 16 (20) Cat heterised I 43 (61) 48 (61) Collection device 2 I! (19) IS (19) *Yo1 applicable to cervical lesions. PCLTIBECED SEVERITY SCORE between 17 years and 18 years 11 months, This functional severity assessment, the WAIS-R. The short form comprised originally devised by Lindon (1963), was Arithmetic, Vocabulary, Picture Arrange- used in the modified form described by ment and Block Design for both the Thomas et al. (1989). I t is not specific to WISC-R (Kaufman 1976, 1979) and the spina bifida. It uses a scoring of 12 items WAIS-R (Silverstein 1982, 1987). In (Table 111). scored from 1 to 4 according addition, all children were administered to defined degrees of severity. Digit Span. The disability can be graded in a number of ways, according to (a)the total THE REY AUDITORY LEARHING TEST number of problems across the scales, (b) This test (Rey 1958, Lezak 1983), admin- whether the main disabilities relate to istered, to all children, involved presen- physical function (items 1 to 5 , 11 and 12) 'tation of a 15-word list for five trials with or behavioural or communication diffi- free recall after each trial. A second culties (items 6 to lo), and (c) the 15-word (interference) list was then pre- functional severity of the problem. sented once for free recall, immediately followed by an unprompted recall trial Psychological interview and a recognition trial for the first list. As part of an interview, we used the Recorded responses were scored for revised versions of the Wechsler lntelligence words correctly recalled, repeats and Scale for Children (wISC-R) and the intrusions. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-R). All children received a four-subtest THE SELF-PERCEPTION PROFILE FOR short form of the age-appropriate test: up L E A R N I N G ~ I S A B L E STUDENTS D (HSPPLDS) 202 to 16 years ll'months, the WISC-R; and This self-report instrument (Renick and
TABLE I l l N Capacity assessed for Pultibeced Severity of Disability Score (N=70) I 00 Q’ Severrry Crude 6 M I 2 3 4 h’ (%) N (To) N (070) N (4’0) - Physical capacity 24 (34) 23 (33) 20 (29) 3 (4) Upper limbs-arms 49 (70) 17 (24) 4 (6) 0 (0) Upper limbs-hands 36 (15) 33 (47) 1 (I) 0 (0) Locomotion 4 (6) I 5 (21) IS (21) 36 (51) 2 Toileting 10 (14) 4 (6) 34 (49) 22 (31) Intellectual function 5 (7) 43 (61) 16 (23) 6 (9) G Behaviour 41 (59) 8 (11) 21 (30) 0 (0) Eyesight/vision 41 (59) 14 (20) 10 (14) 5 (7) Communication-speech 61 (87) 9 (13) 0 (0) 0 (0) Communication- hearing 65 (93) 3 (4) I (1) I (1) Eating and feeding 66 (94) 4 (6) 0 (0) 0 (0) Dressing 36 (51) 5 (7) 26 (37) 3 (4) Harter 1988) is based on the Self- tered to young people aged nine years and Perception Profile for Children (Harter over. This questionnaire asks children to 19850). It is designed for the assessment identify how important each of the nine of learning-disabled and normally achiev- domains of self-concept is to them per- ing children’s domain-specific judgements sonally. Items are scored on a four-point of their competence or adequacy in nine rating scale; high scores indicating greater self-concept domains, and their feelings perceived importance and low scores of global self-worth. The instruments representing lesser perceived importance. have well established validity and re- For calculation of importancdcom- liability (Harter 1985~.Renick and Harter petence discrepancy scores, Renick and 1988). The 10 subscales independently tap Harter (1988)-advised a cut-off of 3 on the children’s self-perceptions in the follow- four-point importance scale. Discrepancy ing areas: (1) general intellectual ability, scores (both domain-specific and overall (2) reading competence, (3) spelling discrepancy) therefore represent data competence, (4) writing competence, ( 5 ) solely on those domains in which indi- mathematics competence, (6) social vidual children place special personal acceptance, (7) athletic competence, (8) importance. Discrepancy scores were behavioural conduct, (9) physical appear- calculated by subtracting importance ance, and (10) global self-worth. The ratings from their respective competence separate domains for specific academic scores. In most cases this value was areas have been found to be useful for negative, since importance ratings tend to children with specific learning difficulties. be higher than competence scores. A Children were asked to evaluate their mean discrepancy score was calculated by perceived competence by completing three taking the sum of the discrepancy scores separate questionnaires entitled, ‘What I and dividing by the number of domains am like-first presentation’ (self-ratings rated as important. of competence in each self-concept domain), ‘Who I am like’ (specification THE SOCIAL SUPPORT SCALE FOR of chosen comparison group for each CHILDREN (SSSC) domain), and ‘What I am like-second This scale (Harter 19856)’ of well estab- presentation’ (repeated competence ratings lished reliability and validity, measures based on the comparison group not used the degree to which others like the child in the first presentation of the scale). the way he or she is, treat the child like a A questionnaire entitled, ‘How impor- person, care about his or her feelings, and a tant are these things to how you feel about act as if they feel that the child matters. yourself as a person?’ was thgn adrninis- The four sources of social support or 203
i positive regard in this instrument are information was not available in two z CJ parents, teachers, classmates and close cases. At birth, six of the lesions were friends. Each item is scored on a scale of covered by skin. 47 of the open lesions 1 to 4, I representing the lowest level of were operated on within the first week of support and 4 representing the highest. life. Hydrocephalus was present in 56 The format is similar to the i1SPPI.DS. cases, of whom 52 had valves in sifu. Three patients with hydrocephalus had m '13 Orher measures and inlerviews not had valves inserted, and one had had w The psychological interview also involved the valve removed. m quantitative measures of depression and .-cn v) coping not reported here. A qualitative SUBSEQUEST SURGERY section allowed young people to reflect on Details of orthopaedic, urological and -- 5 3 the development of their relationships. other surgery were collected. Patients had Separate interviews were conducted with a total number of surgical procedures parents or carers, and an occupational ranging from 0 to 36 (mean 8.2), with 53 00 C therapy assessment was offered to a having five operations or more and 21 2 0 subsample of young people. These data having 10 operations or more. t. will be the subject of further papers. %- 0 SUBSEQUENT MEDICAL PROBLEMS Procedure A variety of medical problems were The research was agreed by the Research recorded. 1 1 patients had had seizures at Ethics Committees of all participating some stage and four were currently health districts, and directors of education regarded as epileptic. Elevated blood gave their consent to the research being pressure was noted in three patients. conducted in participating schools. If the family indicated that they were willing to MOBILITY take part in the study, written consent was Sixteen walked independently and required then obtained from both the young no bracing. 23 were dependent on wheel- person and the parent or guardian. chairs. 52 young people required a Psychological data were collected by manual wheelchair for at least part of the four graduate research assistants, specially time, and six had the use of electric trained in interviewing skills. Extensive wheelchairs. 20 of the wheelchair users training was given to the interviewers on were unable to transfer independently. the administration and scoring of the WISC-R and WAIS-R under the supervision URINARY CONTINENCE of C.E.E. Practice interviews (using video Sixteen cases were fully continent and 15 cameras and one-way mirrors) were totally incontinent. The majority, 41, completed with volunteer subjects before appeared to be managing their continence the interviewing of research subjects took independently. Intermittent catheterisation place. Interviews were carried out in a was used by 39 and long-term catheter- confidential setting within the school/ isation by five. Six patients had artificial college/workplace. Four older subjects urinary sphincters. Four had urinary who were living in a residential college for ' diversions. young people with special needs preferred a home-based interview. BOWEL CONTINENCE Twenty-four cases were fully continent, Results with 36 soiling intermittently and 10 Physical findings totally incontinent. 29 managed their An outline of the medical findings, levels bowels independently. of mobility and continence is shown in Table 11. CLINICAL FINDINGS Problems relating to the skin (ulceration, ' INITIAL MANAGEMENT oedema, poor circulation) were found in In only two cases had there been an ante- 48 patients. natal diagnosis of s ~ In. 66 cases there Spinal deformity was present in 47 204 had been no diagnosis before birth, and cases. Though most spinal scars were
TABLE IV Mean short-form IQscores for both groups Score Controls Sph P Mean (SO) bifda Mean (SO) .- Estimated IQ 100.8 (14.;) 78.9 (17.9)
- 4 L U TABLE V I Self-rated competence measures (llarter SelEPerccption Profile for Learning-Disabled Students) Competence doniain Controls .Spina bifida Paired-t test p Mean (SO) Mean (SD! I (dfl i a General intellectual ability 2-87 (0.56) 2.66 (0.71) 2-12 (78) 0.04 0 0 Reading competence 3.28 (0.72) 2.92 (0.86) 3.00 (78) 0.004 .- b Writing competence 2.05 (0.83) 2.55 (0.88) 2.99 (78) 0.004 m Spelling competence 3.03 (0.80) 3.00 (0.97) 0.22 (78) 0.83 .-2 Maths competence 2.85 (0.83) 2.36 (0-94) 3-42 (78) 0-001 c Social acceptance 3.30 (0.57) 3.03 (0.80) 2.39 (78) 0.02 rn Athletic competence 2.70 (0.76) 2.27 (0.84) 3.39 (78) 0.001 Behavioural conduct 3.06 (0.63) 2.95 (0.78) 1.06 (78) 0.29 Physical appearance 2.74 (0.67) 2.65 (0.91) 0.82 (78) 0.42 0, Global self-worth 3.17 (0.58) 2.98 (0.78) 1.69 (78) 0.10 n 8 P b 0 e 41 and stable across groups. The scales were c 0 U therefore appropriate for this study popu- zY lation, and the scoring system recom- c mended by Harter to derive subscale !2 scores was also appropriate. COMPETENCE (HSPPLDS) We conducted I tests for related samples, comparing groups. As can be seen from Table VI, young people with spina bifida regarded themselves as less competent than did controls in four of five academic domains. Mathematics competence was " Females Males regarded as low, both when compared with control scores and when compared M S p i n a bifida m A b l e - b o d i e d with other domains. Young people with Fig. 2. Imporlance o j physical appearance as a spina bifida also regarded themselves as function of group and gender (IISSPLDS). less socially accepted and less athletically competent than did controls. At this level of analysis there were no group differences evident in the domains of behavioural 41 conduct, physical appearance or global self-worth. ANOVAS were conducted for each domain, allowing us to study the impact of gender and age (greater vs. less than 160 months), as well as group. In addition to the group effects there were gender main effects (hl>F) for athletic com- petence (b11,150)= 13-75, p F) (F(1,150) = 13.01, p M ) (F(1,150)=7.77, pc0.01) and " Young Young Older 0:der global self-worth (F(1,150)=5.19, p < females males lema!es males 0.05). In the area of global self-worth, M S p t n a btftda BAbIe-bodied not only did gender appear as a main Fig. 3. Importance o j physical appearance as a effect-girls demonstrating lower self- 206 junction of group, age and gender (HSSPLDS). worth than boys-but a significant three-
TABLE VII v) Self-rated importance measures (Harter Self-Perception Profile for LearningDisabled Students) ~ a- m Comperence domain Controls Sprna brfida Mean (SO) Mean (SD) General intellectual ability 2.93 (0.86) 3.03 (0.92) 0.66 (71) 0.51 Reading competence 2.92 (0.81) 3.03 (1.001 0.70 (71) 0.48 Writing competence 2.72 (0.78) 2-85 (1.01) 0.88 (71) 0.38 Spelling competence 2.83 (0-91) 3.11 (0.89) 1-91 (71) 0.06 Marhs competence 2-81 (0.91) 3.03 (0,971 1.39 I711 0.17 Social acceptance 2.87 (0.811 3.07 (0.89) 1 . 5 1 (71) 0.14 Athletic competence 2.51 (0-96) 2-33 (1.05) 1.25 ( 7 1 ) 0.22 Rehat ioural conduct 3.35 (0.68) 3.42 (0.71) 0.56 (71) 0.58 Physical appearance 2.90 (0.81) .3.03 (1.07) 0.96 (71) 0-34 P way interaction was also evident between athletic competence, there was a highly group, age and gender (F(1,150)=3.75, significant age effect (F(1,137) = 14.75, p = 0.055). Examination of the tabulated pCO-OOl), older children feeling that this three-way ANOVA interaction indicated area was less important to them. In that older girls with spina bifida had the addition, there was a gender x age effect lowest mean self-worth scores and young (1.11,137)=4.80, p
p M ) for physical appearance (~11.92)= 8.12, p
TABLE V l l l - vl N Spearman rho correlations between domain discrepancy scores and m I global self-worth 0' ~~ ~~ Domain A l l subjects Control group m d m General intellectual ability 0.34 0.31 0.35 (96)' (46) (50) p
TABLE X Effects of different comparison groups on competence scores (HSPPLDS) of joung people with spina bifida - e 0 h Domoin Non -disabled Dira bled d Paired i iesi P 3 T Meun (SO) Meun (SO) i (dj) General intellectual ability 2.59 (0.82) 3.04 (0.89) 0.45 4.60 (76) 0~0001 Reading competence 2.87 (0.95) 3.12 (0.97) 0.25 2.06 (77) 0.04 Maths competence ' 2.34 (1.00) 2.70 (1.08) 0.36 3.07 (75) 0.003 Social acceptance 2-96 (0.83) 3.10 (0.83) 0.14 1-46 (74) 0.15 Athletic competence 2-07 (0.81) 2-70 (0.93) 0.63 7.13 (76) 0.001 Behavioural conduct 2-94 (0-84) 2.95 (0.83) 0.01 0.17 (73) 0.86 Physical appearance 2.61 (0.91) 3.06 (0.88) 0.45 5.12 (77) 0.001 Global self-worth 2.88 (0.77) 3 . 1 7 (0.73) 0.28 4.36 (75) 0.001 TABLL XI Scores on Harter Social Support Scale for Children Conirols Spino hrjido Poired I iesi p Mean (SO) Meon (SO) i (df, -- Parental support/regard 3.55 (0.56) 3.49 (0.61) 0.69 (78) . 0.49 Classmate supportlregard 3.38 (0.44) 3.05 (0.66) 3.41 (78) 0.001 Teacher support/regard 3.27 (0.56) 3.46 (0.59) 2.18 (78) 0.03 Close friend supportlregard 3-53 (0-61) 3-57 (0.58) 0.53 (78) 0-59 themselves with able-bodied peers across except spelling and writing. The data in all domains. This finding may well reflect Table X show that the subjects felt the fact that 74 per cent of the physically relatively less competent in academic, disabled subjects were in mainstream athletic and physical appearance domains schools. An analysis by school placement when comparing themselves with able- shows that those young people who were bodied peers than when comparisons were in a special school were much more likely made with other disabled young people. to compare themselves with physically The same effect was evident in global self- disabled peers, whereas those in main- worth. No significant differences were stream schools were very much more found in the areas of social acceptance likely to compare themselves with able- and behavioural conduct. bodied peers. For instance, in the domain In summary, hypothesis 6-that com- of general intellectual ability, 43 of 49 parison with other physically disabled young people in mainstream schools young people would be associated with compared themselves with able-bodied higher competence scores-wa$ largely peers, in contrast to four out of 16 in confirmed. However, the majority of the special schools (x2(2)=26.26, pC0-001). disabled group spontaneously compared The mean scores for the non-disabled themselves with able-bodied peers, an controls were close t o the mean com- effect associated with being in mainstream petence scores shown in Table VI. a school. further indication of the reliability of response in the spina bifida group. Young SOCIAL SUPPORT (SSSC) people with spina bifida were asked to We conducted f tests for related samples, rate themselves again o n the HSPPLDS comparing the groups for different using a comparison group which they had aspects of social support. As can be seen not chosen on the first occasion. The from Table XI, young people with spina 210 second presentation tapped all domains bifida felt equally as supported by parents
and friends as did their able-bodied peers. TABLE XI1 This contrasted with the finding for Spearman rho correlations between sources of social classmate support (SB < AB). A significant support and global self-worth difference in the opposite direction was W 0 Spino brjida Controls found for teacher support (SB > AB). (N = 79) ( N - 79) Three-factor ANOVAS were conducted to investigate the effects of group, gender Parental support 0.42 0.26 and age. For parental support there was a po) (F(1,150)= Classmate support 0.28 p=0*014 0.15 p=0.175 5.72, p < 0 . 0 5 ) and an interaction of Teacher support 0.30 0.05 groupxgender (fll.150)=5.69, p < p = 0.006 p = 0.686 2 0.05). girls with spina bifida reporting Close friend support 0.20 -0.12 2 significantly less parental support than p : 0.083 p=0.283 g others. For teacher support, there was a 0, E main effect for age ( Y > 0 ) (~11,150)= 2 h 5 - 5 3 . p
defences we examined. In fact, given the relationship, i.e. that with a parent. This group differences in discrepancies, and perception could be based on feelings of the correlations between discrepancies ‘not being worthy of love’. I t is not and global self-worth, it is surprising that possible to discriminate between these global self-worth was not lower in the possibilities with the current data set. young people with spina bifida. Several It is not at all clear why correlations points are relevant. First, as noted above, between social support and global self- the most powerful disability effects in this worth were attenuated for able-bodied study were those that emerged in inter- children. These findings are different m action with gender, rather than main from published data on US samples .-C P cn effects. Second, Harter (1986) viewed (Harter 19856). Support from parents, global self-worth as a ‘core variable’, in- teachers and classmates were significantly fluenced by (and presumably influencing) correlated with global self-worth in the a wide range of component parts of the disabled group. person’s psychosocial experience. Un- Harter (1986) regarded self-concept to C measured compensatory factors in the domains and social support areas as a experience of the physically disabled independent variables, predictive of global 0 L group may have been important, and self-worth. It seems to us that, with 0 possibly would have emerged in a current information, it is equally plausible qualitative study. Third, an over-emphasis that global self-worth has reciprocal on group difference methodology influence on specific domains and per- precludes investigation of factors within ceived support. For instance, low support the spina bifida group that promote or may indeed cause a lowering of self- impede the development of optimum self- worth, but low self-worth may in turn esteem. A series of studies of psychosocial cause the disabled young person to attend adjustment in physical disability by selectively to negative aspects of experience Wallander and others’(see review by Varni (Gotlib and Hammcn 1992) such as and Wallander 1988) has used a within- scholastic problems and problems of group design. physical appearance, thereby setting in It is encouraging that, on a simple motion cognitive-interpersonal negative group-comparison basis, the young chain-reactions (Rutter 1987). i.ongitudina1 people with spina bifida felt equally as and intervention research designs are supported by friends and parents as their required to address these questions. able-bodied peers. The disabled group felt What are the implications for prac- more supported by teachers, perhaps an titioners? First, that the use of instru- effect associated with the special classes ments such as the HSPPIBS can be useful and greater amount of individual as part of multidisciplinary assessment. attention required by a physically disabled The primary focus of assessment, around child. It is of concern that disabled girls which other assessments are built, must appeared to feel less supported by parents now be the young person’s own view of than did able-bodied children and dis- his or her life situation, wishes, interests abled boys. This finding requires and plans (Department of Health 1989). replication, but is another indication of Tools such as the HSPPLDS allow prac- the importance of gender as a potential titioners to plan management, and effect modifier in psychosocial aspects of evaluate change and outcome, using physical disability. If the finding is not variables that are relevant to the young spurious, then at least two causal interpre- person. Treatment plans failing to take tations are possible. First, disabled girls account of these fundamental sources of may objectively be more difficult to care motivation are likely to prove less fruitful for in adolescence because of their (Harter 1991). relatively low self-esteem and associated Second, peer social acceptance and factors, leading to a relative reduction in social comparison appear to be key issues. received parental support. Second, because Work with the able-bodied peer-group of low self-esteem, the disabled girls may itself can be valuable (e.g. Armstrong ef perceive themselves as receiving less al. 1987). Work with disabled young 212 support from the most important close people o n social skills (Inderbitzen-
Pisaruk and Foster 1990) must recognise (Department of Health 1989), on self- the distinction between individual perceptions of the body and how these are friendship-making skills, and the linked, in each case, to overall self- experience of feeling ‘part of’ a peer esteem. Such cognitive-behavioural work group (Parker and Asher 1993). Our data, (Harter 1991, Gotlib and Hammen 1992) i 01 and Harter’s data (1986) on able-bodied may be viewed as a first stage, or Q‘ children, suggest that the latter set of foundation stage, out of which the young group processes is as important as indi- person may wish to engage in specific vidual friendships, if not more so. Much pieces of collaborative work on self-care. is known about the cognitive and social aspects of group integration (see Turner Accepied for publication 81h August 1993. 1991); application of this knowledge to Acknowledgements - special-needs settings is important. ‘By the We are very grateful to the young people and P time children reach adolescence, peer families who participated in the study. We are group identities and the feeling of being grateful to ASBAH for generous funding, and ctrong interest and support. Clwyd Social Services part of a larger community are very and Clwyd Health Authority also provided funding. important aspects of the young person’s Research Assistants Val Lawson. Colin Clerkin, life (Widdicombe 1988, Parker and Asher Ann Llewelyn and I’rish Gilroy, and Medical Interviewers Dr Val Klirnach, Dr Gill Clements, Dr 1993). Robert Pugh and Dr Roger Blackmore. made the Finally, it is clear that the young study possible. Participating schools were interested and supportive. Dr Martin Rax, Medical Adviser to person’s self-perception of physical ASBAH. was most helpful, especially in the early appearance may need addressing as part planning stages of the project. Parts of this paper of an overall intervention programme. were reported at the European Academy of Childhood Disability, Italy, 1992. While it may be true that physically disabled young people find other sources Auihors ’ Appointments of reward in their lives, there is little ‘Peter Appleton, Clinical Psychology Services doubt that many (especially girls) are Manager; Colin Elliott. Principal Clinical Psychologist, distressed by their physical disabilities and Wrexham Child and Family Service; appearance. This will have specific impli- Clwydian Community Care. cations for attitudes towards developing Philip Minchom. Consultant Paediatrician, Wrexham Maelor Hospital. continence and other self-care skills. I f Nick Ellis, Senior Lecturer in Psychology, young people have negative perceptions University of Wales, Bangor. Vicki Boll. Team Manager, Child Health and of their physical appearance, and this is Disability Service; closely tied to their overall view of them- Pat Jones, Rehabilitation Officer; selves, motivation for self-care pro- South Divisions, Clwyd Social Services. grammes is likely to be low. Therapists’ *Correspondence lo firs1 author at Wrexham attention should therefore focus, in Maelor Hospital. Croesnewydd Road, Clwyd. Wales partnership with the young person LL13 7TD, UK. SUMMARY Seventy-nine young people with spina bifida were given a psychological, medical. carer and occupational therapy assessment. 79 matched able-bodied young people received the psychosocial interview. The disabled group felt themselves to be less competent in academic, athletic and social aspects of self-concept, less supported by classmates, equally supported by parents and friends and more supported by teachers than the able-bodied group. Disabled subjects did not discount the importance of any area of personal-social functioning, and experienced greater discrepancies between competence and importance in most academic, athletic, social and physical appearance aspects of self-concept. Disabled girls assigned very high importance to physical appearance. Physical appearance was more strongly associated with general self-esteem than any other area of self-concept. RGSUMG L ‘appreciation de soi de jeunes spina bifida: une elude de population Une entrevue concernant les aspects psychologiques, medicaux de prise en charge de soins et de reeducation a ete proposee a 79 jeunes spina bifida. La mOme entrevue psychosocial fut proposke a 79 jeunes sujets valides apparies. Les mcmbres du groupe avec incapacites, se sentaient moins competents dans une auto-evaluation sur les aspects scolaires. athletiques et sociaux, moins aidCs par leurs camarades de classes, egalement aides par leurs parents et amis, et mieux aides par leurs professeurs que les jeunes du groupe de valides. Les sujets avec incapacites en minimisaient I’importance d’aucun aspect de I’activite individuelle et sociale, et faisaient une plus grande . 213
distinction entre competence et importance dans la plupart des aspects d’auto-evaluation sur les do- maines scolaires, sportifs, sociaus et d’apparence physique. Les filles avec incapacites attribuaient une tres grande importe h I’apparence physique. L’apparence physique etait plus fortement associee avec I’estime d e soi generale qu’i n’import quelle autre aspect d’auto-evaluation. ZUSAMMENFASSUNG Die Selbsteinsciibt:img junger Leute niit Spina byida: eine Populationssludic 79 junge Leute mit Spina bifida wurden anhand von Interviews beziiglich ihrer psychologischen, tnedizinschen, pflegerischen und beschaftigungsthcrapeutischen Situation befragt. 79 gesunde Kontrollen wurden mit einem psychosozialen Interview untersucht. Die behinderten Probanden fuhlten sich in akademischen, sportlichen und sozialen Bereichen weniger kompetent, durch Klassenkameraden weniger, durch Eltern und Freunde gleich und durch Lehrer mehr unterstutzt als die gesunden Kindern. Die behinderten Patienten waren sich der Bedeutung der allgemeinen psychosozialen Eingliederung bewuflt und machten die Erfahrung groflerer Diskrepanzen zwischen ihren Fahigkeiten und der Bedeutung, die sie den meisten akademischen, sportlichen, sozialen und korperlichen Erscheinungsbildern in ihrer Selbsteinschatzung zuschrieben. Behinderte Madchen legten sehr grossen Wert auf die korperliche Erscheinung. Umgekehrt war die korperliche Erscheinung starker mit dem allgemeinen Selbstwertgefuhl vernknupft als irgendein anderer Bereich der Selbsteinschatzung. RESUM EN A irtoconcepto de jovenes con espina bi5da. Estudio de poblacion Setenta y neuve jovenes con espina bifida pasaron una entrevista psicologica, medica, de cuidador y de terapia ocupacional. Otro grupo de 79 jovenes sin minusvalencia fisica pasarori por una entrevista psicosocial. El grupo con minusvalencia se sentia menos competente en 10s conceptos academico, atletico y social d e su autoconcepto menos apoyados por 10s compalleros d e clase, igualmente apoyados por padres y amigos y m i s apoyados por 10s profesores, que el grupo sin minusvalencia. Los individuos minusvalentes no descontaban la impoitancia de ninguna area del functionamiento personal y social y experimentaban grandes discrepancias entre la competencia y la importancia de la mayoria de 10s aspectos academicos, atleticos, sociales y aspecto fisico del autoconcepto. k.as chicas minusvhlidas daban una muy gran importancia al aspecto fisico. El aspecto fisico estaba mas fuertemente asociado con la autoestima en general, que con cualquier otra area del autoconcepto. References Armsrrong, K. W., Rosenbaum, P. I . . . King. S. 31. A . (Ed. 1 Chtlditood Assessnrent: Through the (1987) ‘A randomized controlled trial of a ‘buddy’ Eyes of a Child. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon. programme to improve children’s attitudes -- (1991) ‘Developmental differences in the nature toward the disabled.’ Developnrental Medicine of self-representations: implications for the and Child Neurology. 29, 327-336. understanding, assessment. and treatment of Campbell, M. M., Hayden. P. W., Davenport. maladaptive behaviour.’ Cognitive Therupy und S. L. t i . (1977) ‘Psychological adjustment of Research, 14, 113-142. adolescents with myelodysplasia.’ Journal of Hinde. R. A.. Dennis, A. (1986) ‘Categorizing Youth and Adolescence. 6, 397-407. individuals: an alternative to linear analysis.’ Crocker, J . , Major, B. (1989) ‘Social stigma and International Journal of Rehavioitral Develop- self-esteem: the self-protective properties of ment. 9, 105-1 19. stigma.’ Psychological Review. 96,608-630. Inderbitzen-Pisaruk, H., Foster, S. I.. (1990) Cull. C., Wyke, M. A . (1984) ‘Memory function of ‘Adolescent friendships and peer acceptance: . children with spina bifida and shunted hydro- implications for social skills training.’ Clinical cephalus.’ Developmental Medicine and Child Psychology Review, 10, 425-439. Neurology, 26, 177- 183. Kaufman, A. S. (1976) ‘A four-test short form of Department of Health (1989) Children Acr Guidance the WISC-R.’ Contemporary Educational ’ and Regulations, Vol. 6: Children with Dis- PSychology, 1, I 80- 196. abilities. London: HMSO. - (1979) Intelligence Testing with the WISC-R. Gotlib, 1. ti., Hammen, C. L. (1992) Psychological New York: John Wiley. Aspects of Depression: Toward a Cognitive Lavigne, 1. V., Nolan, D., McLone, D. G. (1988) Interpersonal Integration. Chichester: John Wiley. ‘Temperament, coping, and psychological adjust- Harper, D. C. (1991) ‘Paradigms for investigating ment in young children with myelomeningocele.’ rehabilitation and adaptation to childhood dis- Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 13, 363-378. ability and chronic illness.’ Journal of Pediatric Lezak. M. D. (1983) Neuropsychological Assess- Psychology, 16. 533-542. ment. 2nd Edn. New York: Oxford University llarter, S. (1985a) Manual for the Self-Perception Press. Profile for Children. Denver: University of Lindon, R. L. (1963) ‘Pultibec system for the Denver Press. medical assessment of handicapped children.’ - ( 1 9856) Manual for the Social Support Scalefor Developmenral Medicine and Child .Veurology. 5. Children. Denver: University of Denver Press. 125-145. - (1986) ‘Processes underlying the construction. Lord, J.. Varzos. N.. Behrman. B., Wicks. J., maintenance, and enhancement of the self- Wicks, D. (1990) ‘Implications of mainstream concept in children.’ I n Suls, J., Greenwald, A . classrooms for adolescents with spina bifida.’ G . (Eds.) Psychological Perspectives On The Sey. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. Vol. 3. London: Erlbaum. 32, 20-29. - (1990) ‘Issues in the assessment of the self- Marsh. H. W. (1989) ‘Age and sex effects in multiple 214 concept of children and adolescents.’ In La Greca, dimensions of self-concept: preadolescence to
early adulthood.’ Journal of Educational ~sychology,43, 7 14-721. Psycholo,~y.81, 417-430. Spaulding. 13. R., Morgan, S. B. (1986) ‘Spina bifida Murch, R. L.. Cohen. L. H . (1989) ‘Relationships children and their parents: a population prone to among life stress, perceived family environment, family dysfunction’?’ Journal of Pediatric and the psychological distre5s of spina bifida PSyChology, I I , 359-374. adolescents.’ Journal of Pediarric Psychology. 14, Thomas. A. P.. Has, M. C. 0.. Smyth, D. P. 1.. 193-214. (1989) 7he Health and Social Needs of Y O U I I ~ ’arkcr, J . G., Asher, S. K. (1993) ‘Friendship and Adults wirh Physical Disabiliries. Clinics i n friendship quality in middle childhood: links with De\’elopmental Medicine. No. 106. London: Mac peer group acceptance and feelings of lonelincss Keith Press. and social dissatisfaction.’ Developtrrenial Turner, J. C. (1991) Social Injluence. Milton Psychology. 29, 4. 61 1-621. Keynes: Open University Press. ’earson. A . M. (1985) 7he self-concepr o j i’arni, J . W., Wallander, J. L. (1988) ‘Pediatric adolescen/s wi/h spina bi’ftda. (Thesis .) Un iversi t y chronic disabilities: hemophilia and spina bifida of London, Institute of Education. as examples.’ I n Routh. D. K. (Ed.) Handbook of Perersen. A. C., Sariyiani, P. A.. Kennedy, R. E . Pediatric Psychology. New York: Guilford Press. ( I Y Y I ) ‘Adolescent depression: why more girls?’ - Rubenfield, M. A,, Talbot. D., Setoguchi, Y . Journal o j Youth and Adolescence, 20, 247-27 I . (1989) ‘Determinants of self-esteem in children Pope, A. W.,Mctlale, S. IM., Craighead, W. E. with congenital/acquired limb deficiencies.’ ( 1988) Self-esreem Enhancement with Children De velopmenral and Beha vioural Paediarrics. 10. and Adolescents. Oxford: Pergamon Press. 13-16. Renick, %I. J., Harter, S. (1988) Manual for [he - Setoguchi, Y., Rappaport, 1.. R., Talbot, D. Self-perception Profile for Learning Disabled (1992) ‘Psychological adjustment and perceived Srudenrs. Denver: University of Denver Press. social support in children with congenital/ - - (19x9) ‘Impact of social comparisons on acquired limb deficiencies.’ Journal of the developing $elf-perceptions of learning Behavioural Medicine, 15, 3 1-44. disabled students.’ Joirrnal of Educarional Wallander. J. L., Feldman. b‘. S., Varni, J . W. PSYChOIORy. 81, 631-638. (1989) ‘Physical status and psychosocial adjust- Rey, A . (1958) I. ’examen Clinique en Psychologie. ment in children with spina bifida.’ Journal of Paris: Presses Universitaircs. Pediatric Psychology. 14, 89- 102. Rutter. bl. (1987) ‘Psychosocial resilience and Wechsler, D. (1974) Manual for /he Wechsler protective mechanisms.’ American Journal of Inrelligence Scale for Children-Revised. Kew Orlhopsychiarry. 57, 317-331. York: Psychological Corporation; Windsor, - (1989) ‘Age as an ambiguous variable in Berks: NFER. developmental research: some epidemiological - (1986) A Brirish Supplemenr 10 the Manual of considerations from developmental psycho- the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale- Revised. pathology.’ International Journal of Behavioural London: Harcourt Brace Jovsnovich. Development, 12, 1-34. Widdicombe, S . (1988) ‘Dimensions of adolescent Silverstein, A. 8 . (1982) ‘Two- and four-subtest identity.’ f?uropean Journal of social psycho log^. short forms of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence 18,471-483. Scale-Revised.’ Journal of Consulring and Wills, K. E., Holmbeck, G. N., Dillon, K., .Mcl.one, Clinical Psychology, 50, 41 5-418. D. G . (1990) ‘Intelligence and achievement in - (1987) ‘Equal weighting vs. differential weight- children with myelomeningocele.’ Joirrnal of ing of Subtesl scores on short forms of Wechsler’s Pedrarric Psychology, 15, 16 I - 176. Intelligence Scales.’ Journal of Clinical 215
You can also read