The Model of Achievement Competence Motivation (MACM)
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
The Model of Achievement Competence Motivation (MACM) A: Introduction and Background (1st in multi-module series) (K. McGrew 01-04-2021) © Institute for Applied Psychometrics (IAP), Dr. Kevin McGrew, 01-04-2021 These slides are provided as supplements to The Model of Achievement Competence Motivation (MACM): Standing on the shoulders of giants (McGrew, in press, 2021—for special issue on motivation in Canadian Journal of School Psychology). The slides in this PPT/PDF module can be used without permission for educational (not commercial) purposes
The Model of Achievement Competence Motivation (MACM) (K. McGrew 01-04-2021) Part A: Why was MACM Developed? • Educational Reform and Policymakers • Educational/Psychological Research & Theory • Brief History of Conative Abilities • Models of School Learning • The Snow Academic Aptitude Framework
Why?: Going “beyond cognitive abilities or IQ” has been an area of study in education, psychology and policy for decades
The Model of Achievement Competence Motivation (MACM) Part A: Why was MACM Developed? • Educational Reform and Policymakers • Educational/Psychological Research & Theory • Brief History of Conative Abilities • Models of School Learning • The Snow Academic Aptitude Framework
Why: A response to NCLB Education Policy Initiatives http://www.iapsych.com/articles/mcgrew2004.pdf.
The Model of Achievement Competence Motivation (MACM) Part A: Why was MACM Developed? • Educational Reform and Policymakers • Educational/Psychological Research & Theory • Brief History of Conative Abilities • Models of School Learning • The Snow Academic Aptitude Framework
Intelligence tests are important and powerful predictors of achievement: But they are fallible predictors Cattell’s (1987) wise words, written over 30 years ago, still apply (unfortunately) to the state-of-the-art of psychology’s limited conceptual integration of cognitive, conative and affective constructs in understanding student learning —"The school psychologists of the first half of this century made a big mistake in trying to estimate school performance and scholarship readiness from the I.Q. alone. Typically, only half the variance in grades is thus accounted for, and, as we now realize …much of the rest can be accounted for by predictions from personality and motivation measures [emphasis added]” (p. 435). (McGrew, in press, 2021)
Intelligence tests are important and powerful predictors of achievement: But they are fallible predictors
Intelligence tests are important and powerful predictors of achievement: But they are fallible predictors McGrew and Evans (2004) reminded educators, psychologists, and policy makers that with the best IQ tests, and for any particular IQ score, there is a normal distribution of achievement scores around each IQ score (after adjusting for regression to the mean). Expected achievement scores for any IQ score could show a band of expected achievement standard scores that range close to 22 points (+ 11) for approximately 2/3 of the population. The point was clear—IQ test scores, or related diagnostic categories, should not be used as an excuse to formulate lower academic expectations for students with disabilities. (McGrew, in press, 2021)
The Model of Achievement Competence Motivation (MACM) Part A: Why was MACM Developed? • Educational Reform and Policymakers • Educational/Psychological Research & Theory • Brief History of Conative Abilities • Models of School Learning • The Snow Academic Aptitude Framework
Why was MACM developed? Back to the Future Going Beyond IQ
Why: Educational & psychological research has identified “non-cognitive factors” as important for school learning (e.g., Messick, 1979)
Why: Educational & psychological research has identified “non-cognitive factors” as important for school learning (e.g., Messick, 1979)
Spearman on “conative” abilities (1927) “The process of cognition cannot possibly be treated apart from those of conation and affection, seeing that all these are but inseparable aspects in the instincts and behavior of a single individual, who himself, as the vary name implies, is essentially indivisible” (p. 2)
Alfred Binet’s definition of Intelligence (Corno et al., 2002 translation by Terman, 1916) “The tendency to take and maintain a definite direction; the capacity to make adaptations for the purpose of attaining a desired end; and the power of auto- criticism” (translation by Terman, 1916, p. 45). All three of these phrases refer at least as much to conative processes and attitudes as to reasoning powers.
David Wechsler (1944) on “non-intellective factors” “When our scales measure the non- intellective as well as the intellectual factors in intelligence, they will more nearly measure what in actual life corresponds to intelligent behavior” (p. 103)
Conative and noncognitive: The jingle jangle jungle
“Noncognitive:” Too many cooks in the kitchen Noncognitive skills have drawn the interest of psychologists, educators, economists and policymakers over the past 30 years. The research literature….is vast and the noncognitive domain has drawn the interest of a wide cross-section of individuals outside scientific psychology (e.g., economists, educators, practitioners, policymakers).
Noncognitive: “Everybody hates this term” (Kell, 2018) There is long-standing and widespread dissatisfaction with the label “noncognitive skills” “Everybody hates this term” (Easton, 2013, p. 8). Mostly simply, the term indicates that noncognitive skills are whatever cognitive skills are not. “noncognitive” implies that the constructs and measures do not entail cognition, a virtual impossibility.
The jingle-jangle-jungle in the motivation (conation) literature The jingle-jangle-jungle is when erroneous assumptions are made that two different things are the same because they have the same name (jingle fallacy) or are identical or almost identical things are different because they are labeled differently (jangle fallacy). (Schneider & McGrew, 2018) (Kelly, 1927)
The jingle-jangle-jungle in the motivation (conation) literature: A recent example--Grit An interest in what Duckworth and colleagues refer to as grit, perseverance, and consistency is not new to psychology. Studies of attributes such as will power, tenacity, determination, persistence of motives, and volitional perseveration date back over 80 years.
The jingle-jangle-jungle in the motivation (conation) literature: A recent example--Grit • 584 effect sizes from 88 independent samples representing 66,807 individuals. • The higher order structure of grit is not confirmed. • Grit is very strongly correlated with conscientiousness. • Overall grit explains no variance in either overall academic performance or high school GPA after controlling for conscientiousness. • Interventions designed to enhance grit may only have weak effects on performance and success. • That the construct validity of grit is in question.
The jingle-jangle-jungle in the motivation (conation) literature: A recent example--Grit Indeed, the correlation between overall grit and conscientiousness, and between persistence and conscientiousness (.89) is much stronger than what is typically found between scores on two different global measures of conscientiousness (.63; Pace & Brannick, 2010). This, in turn, suggests that grit research may have fallen victim to the jangle fallacy and that grit as currently measured is simply a repackaging of conscientiousness or one of the facets of conscientiousness.
The Model of Achievement Competence Motivation (MACM) Part A: Why was MACM Developed? • Educational Reform and Policymakers • Educational/Psychological Research & Theory • Brief History of Conative Abilities • Models of School Learning • The Snow Academic Aptitude Framework
Learner characteristics Research findings: Wahlberg et al.s’ series of grand narrative and meta-analyses reviews and theoretical testing of models Figure 5. Visual schematic summary of Walberg’s synthesis of the major models of school learning
Research-based reasons: Models of School Learning John B. Carroll’s 1963 elegant Model of School Learning, which spawned a variety of models of school learning and educational productivity, reminds us that individual difference variables (e.g., IQ) are only PART of the equation of school learning. Other variables OUTSIDE of the individual help Learner characteristics explain why someone achieves above or below their IQ score.
Research-based reasons: Models of School Learning
Research-based reasons: Models of School Learning Learner characteristics
Learner characteristics
Research findings: A series Learner characteristics of grand narrative and meta- analyses reviews and theoretical testing of models Walberg et al.’s model of educational productivity (simplified)
Learner characteristics Walberg 1984
The Model of Achievement Competence Motivation (MACM) Part A: Why was MACM Developed? • Educational Reform and Policymakers • Educational/Psychological Research & Theory • Brief History of Conative Abilities • Models of School Learning • The Snow Academic Aptitude Framework
Salient research findings: Student learner characteristics are important (McGrew et al., 2004) The direct intervention in the psychological determinants of learning promise the most effective avenues for reform” (Wang et al., 1997). Targeted student learning characteristics (i.e., social, behavioral, motivational, affective, cognitive, and metacognitive) are the set of variables with the most potential for modification that could significantly positively effect student outcomes (DiPernal et al., 2002; in McGrew et al., 2004).
Salient research findings: Student learner characteristics are important (Detterman, 2016)
Salient research findings: Student learner characteristics are important--Motivation and intelligence meta-analysis
Salient research findings: Motivation interventions meta-analysis
Average ES = .49 !
Salient research findings: Self-regulated learning interventions meta-analysis
Average ES = .69 !
What is conation or conative? The APA Dictionary of Psychology (VandenBos, 2007) defines conation as “the proactive (as opposed to habitual) part of motivation that connects knowledge, affect, drives, desires, and instincts to behavior. Along with COGNITION and affect, conation is one of the three traditionally identified components of mind” (p. 210).
The Model of Achievement Competence Motivation (MACM) Part I: Why was MACM Developed? • Educational Reform and Policymakers • Educational/Psychological Research & Theory • Brief History of Conative Abilities • Models of School Learning • The Snow Academic Aptitude Framework
The need for A conative taxonomy There have been few solid attempts to develop a research and theory-based taxonomy of individual difference constructs important for school learning Such a grand model or taxonomy requires integrating many different strands of theoretical and empirical research
The need for A conative taxonomy Snow’s Academic Aptitude Model A “provisional” taxonomy to help “see the forest and the trees.” Based on: (2002) • A systematic program of educational research • Integration of the extant literature (4 existing taxonomies) • Emphasis on relatively stable constructs related to educational performance
The trilogy-of-the-mind taxonomy
The trilogy-of-the-mind taxonomy The cognition, affection, and conation trilogy-of-the-mind originated in the German faculty psychology of the eighteenth century and has endured as a model for describing the division of labor that characterizes intellectual functioning (Hilgard, 1980). Eventually conation experienced a demotion (when compared to cognition) and was ignored or was merged with affection and the two considered mere associates of cognition (Snow & Farr, 1987). (McGrew, in press, 2021)
The trilogy-of-the-mind taxonomy A central thesis of this article is that this ageless trilogy, and conation in particular, be resurrected as an overarching and revised aptitude framework from which psychologists can conceptualize motivational and other conative constructs. (McGrew, in press, 2021)
Snow, Corno & Jackson, 1996 (Handbook of Educational Psychology)
Snow, Corno & Jackson, 1996
The big picture: An adapted Snow (Corno et al., 2002) model of aptitude (MACM revised; 10-13-16) Intellect Knowing Willing Feeling Physical Cognitive Conative Affective Physical Cognitive Temper- abilities Motivation ament traits processes Psycho- Acquired Volitional Character- motor knowledge controls istic moods abilities systems Sensory- perceptual abilities Personality Cool intelligences Hot intelligences (Note: Social abilities have been integrated in these major domains: Gei [cognitive aspects of social intelligence] now in Cognitive/CHC model. Social behavior characteristics now subsumed under personality).
We have an embarrassment of riches—but a serious need to make order out of chaos A major MACM goal is to facilitate the process of developing a common nomenclature for these constructs…like the CHC periodic table of cognitive elements
The big picture: Richard Snows concept of aptitude
The MACM model (combined with cognitive and affective constructs) and the Crossing the Rubicon “commitment pathway” to self-regulated learning model will be described in subsequent modules
You can also read