The Evolution of Paternal Care with Overlapping Broods
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
vol. 164, no. 4 the american naturalist october 2004 The Evolution of Paternal Care with Overlapping Broods Andrea Manica* and Rufus A. Johnstone† Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge their mating frequency. Male care, on the other hand, is CB2 3EJ, United Kingdom predicted to be much rarer, since males can greatly increase their reproductive success by finding more mates. How- Submitted January 15, 2004; Accepted May 11, 2004; Electronically published September 1, 2004 ever, male care is predominant in fish and some groups of insects such as assassin bugs and giant water bugs Online enhancement: appendix. (Clutton-Brock 1991). Empiricists attempting to measure the costs of care have outlined three major categories of costs: energetic, survival, and mating costs (Gross and Sargent 1985; Clutton-Brock abstract: Most attempts to model the evolution of parental care 1991). Parental care is often energetically expensive (en- assume that caring and mating are mutually exclusive activities (i.e., ergetic costs) as a result of reduced feeding opportunities individuals acquire and guard broods “sequentially”). However, in or of direct calorific expense while caring, thus leading to most fish and certain insects, males can keep mating and collecting reduced future fertility (e.g., Clutton-Brock et al. 1989; additional eggs while continuing to guard broods obtained earlier Crowl and Alexander 1989). For example, in the Galilee (i.e., males guard “overlapping” broods). We present a model of St. Peter’s fish, a biparental mouthbrooding cichlid, parental care with overlapping broods in which males can mate and guard simultaneously, even though there is a trade-off between these mouthbrooding parents lose more weight than parents that two activities. Within this framework, we show that male care is have their brood removed after only 1 day of care favored by short female processing times and high population den- (Balshine-Earn 1995). Parental survival can also be affected sities, which minimize the mating cost of care. Relatively low mor- by care (survival costs) through the increased visibility of tality while guarding is also important for the stability of male care. caring adults to predators (e.g., Lack 1966; Shine 1980). Female care, on the other hand, is favored by long female processing In lumpsuckers, a fish with adhesive demersal eggs and times and low populations densities, which lead to longer intermating intervals. Biparental care is stable only when the cost to benefit ratio paternal care, males suffer very high levels of predation of care was not biased toward either sex. We derive quantitative by otters while guarding, whereas females of the same estimates of fitness for different strategies for two species of assassin species are rarely caught by this predator (H. Kruuk, un- bugs with male and female uniparental care and show that the model published data, in Clutton-Brock 1991). Finally, while car- predicts the correct form of care for both species. We believe our ing, individuals may suffer reduced access to mates (mat- model might help explain the prevalence of male uniparental care ing costs), a potentially important issue for males whose in certain taxa, such as fish. reproductive success is commonly limited by access to Keywords: parental care, population density, natural selection, sexual their mates (Bateman 1948). selection. The magnitude of mating costs probably differs between homeotherms, which have to keep their young warm and thus can look after only a small number of offspring Which sex should care for the offspring? The evolution of (Clutton-Brock 1991), and ectotherms, which can simul- any form of parental care depends on the cost to benefit taneously guard a large number of young (e.g., Blumer ratio of this strategy to each parent. Because of anisogamy, 1979; Sargent 1988). For the latter, providing parental care females often end up caring for the young, since they have and acquiring mates are not mutually exclusive activities a high investment in each of the gametes and cannot in- (i.e., they can obtain “overlapping” broods), and the mat- crease their reproductive success significantly by increasing ing cost of care is therefore reduced (Gross and Shine 1981; Gross and Sargent 1985). However, because of the asym- * E-mail: am315@cam.ac.uk. metry between the sexes in the cost of gamete production, † E-mail: raj1003@hermes.cam.ac.uk. only males are likely to be able to take full advantage of Am. Nat. 2004. Vol. 164, pp. 517–530. 䉷 2004 by The University of Chicago. this opportunity, since the time needed by females to pro- 0003-0147/2004/16404-40280$15.00. All rights reserved. duce more eggs is likely to be longer than the period of
518 The American Naturalist care (e.g., fish: Wootton 1984; insects: Thomas and Man- whereas pmc is the processing time for caring males (i.e., ica, in press). This asymmetry might explain the wide- the time taken by a male to mate and subsequently re- spread occurrence of paternal care in fish, where males plenish his gametes). can mate with a large number of females and simulta- In both females and males, care takes the form of egg neously guard all the eggs they acquire. It might also ex- guarding. The time required for fertilized eggs to hatch is plain the association between male care and territoriality denoted h, and the probability of offspring survival de- (Krebs and Davies), since males that mate on their ter- pends on the proportion of this time period during which ritories are likely to be able to continue doing so while either or both parents are present to protect the eggs. protecting the already acquired offspring. Male stickle- Specifically, we assume that unguarded eggs suffer a con- backs, for example, keep attracting females while guarding stant instantaneous mortality rate m. Guarding by the male up to 10 broods simultaneously (Wootton 1984). This pos- reduces egg mortality to a fraction M mc of the unguarded sibility has received little attention by theoreticians, who mortality m, whereas female guarding reduces it to M fc. generally assume that parents can care for only one brood When both parents guard the eggs simultaneously, mor- at a time (e.g., Maynard Smith 1977; Balshine-Earn and tality is reduced to a fraction equal to the product of Earn 1997; Wade and Shuster 2002). M mc and M fc. We also assume that care is nondepreciable In this article, we develop a model of parental care in (sensu Clutton-Brock 1991); that is, its effectiveness does which males are able to simultaneously care and mate. We not depend on the number of offspring that receive it. show that, given the possibility, male care is favored in Parental care has three major costs (Gross and Sargent species found at high densities, whereas female care evolves 1985; Clutton-Brock 1991): energetic, mating, and survival more easily at low densities. We then test our model using costs. All these three costs are built into the model. Guard- data from two species of assassin bugs: Rhinocoris tristis ing individuals incur an energetic cost when they are not (Stål), which shows paternal care, and Rhinocoris carmelita able to meet the energetic demands of guarding by foraging Stål, which shows maternal care. These two species are while looking after the brood. In order to offset this en- sympatric and have very similar morphology and behavior ergetic cost, they will take a longer time to replenish their (Thomas 1994; Thomas and Manica 2003 and in press), gametes than individuals that do not care, thus leading to thus providing an ideal model system to investigate sex pc 1 pnc. Because of anisogamy, care is likely to affect the differences in the costs and benefits of parental care. processing time of females disproportionately more than that of males. In our model, females can produce and guard only one A Model of Parental Care with Overlapping Broods brood at a time, and thus they pay a high mating cost of We consider mating and offspring production in a large, care since they cannot mate while caring. When females stable, continuously breeding population. A proportion a guard a single brood until hatching, time out is equal to of newly born individuals are males and 1 ⫺ a are females. the sum of h and pfc. On the other hand, we assume that Individuals can be in either of two states: searching for caring males guard a territory and can continue to acquire mates (time in) or not available for mating (time out). broods while guarding, although the rate at which they While searching, an individual randomly encounters will- encounter females will be reduced by some proportion g ing members of the opposite sex at a rate proportional to (because of the males’ restricted ability to search out fe- their density d, with the constant of proportionality being males while remaining on their territory). This scenario denoted k. Since encounters are random, the time until is common: overlapping care by territorial individuals is the next encounter follows a negative exponential prob- the predominant form of male care in fish (Gross and ability distribution so that its mean (average search time Sargent 1985) and some insects (e.g., assassin bugs: Tho- s) is simply the inverse of encounter frequency l. In the mas and Manica, in press), whereas female carers are al- absence of any form of parental care, time out is equal to ways sequential brooders (e.g., intertidal fish from the fam- the processing time p, defined as the time needed to mate ily Stichaeidae: Coleman 1992; assassin bugs: Thomas and and to replenish egg or sperm supplies after a mating. Manica, in press). Because of anisogamy, processing as well as search time Parents guarding eggs or territory are likely to expose are likely to differ between the sexes. Subscripts “m” and themselves to predation, thus incurring survival costs. The “f” will therefore be used to denote parameters for males instantaneous mortality rates for guarding individuals of and females, respectively. Subscripts “c” and “nc” denote either sex, m mc and m fc, are thus assumed to be higher (or parameters for caring and noncaring individuals, respec- equal, in the case of no cost) to the mortality rates of tively. Following this notation, pfnc is the processing time nonguarding individuals, m mnc and m fnc, respectively. For for females in the absence of care (i.e., the time taken by caring males, we assume the higher mortality to be as- a female to mate and subsequently replenish her gametes), sociated with their limited movements while holding onto
Paternal Care with Overlapping Broods 519 Table 1: List of all the parameters in the model of factors such as population density on the stability of Parameter Definition male versus female care. A pair of male and female strategies is said to be stable c Clutch size if the male strategy maximizes the lifetime reproductive m Instantaneous egg mortality M Fraction of unguarded egg mortality suffered by success for males given the female strategy and vice versa. guarded eggs A population in which a stable pair of strategies is adopted s Search time cannot be invaded by a mutant male or female who adopts p Processing time (time taken to mate and some other strategy, since any such mutant would suffer replenish gametes) a lower lifetime reproductive success. Here, we consider h Hatching time of eggs the ancestral state of no care by either sex and investigate m Instantaneous adult mortality the stability of this system against invasion by caring in- g Proportional decrease in search efficiency of dividuals from either sex. A detailed treatment of the sta- guarding males bility of male-only, female-only, and biparental care can l Encounter rate be found in the appendix in the online edition of the d Population density American Naturalist. Mixed equilibria (where both strat- k Constant of proportionality between density and encounter rate egies coexist) are possible where a mutant strategy can a Primary sex ratio invade but is not able to become fully fixed within the population. For the sake of simplicity, we do not analyze in detail the relative frequencies of different strategies their territory and thus not be influenced by whether eggs within mixed equilibria but simply indicate the range are present in the territory. within which these equilibria occur. The adult sex ratio will differ from the primary ratio, depending on the mortality associated with the strategy adopted by each sex. Assuming a stable population, the Stability of No Care against the Invasion of Male Care proportion of adult males is given by We begin by looking at the stability of a system without am fy care against invasion by caring males. The mean payoff a mxfy p , (vmnc) from a single batch of eggs in our noncaring pop- a(m fy ⫺ m mx) ⫹ m mx ulation is equal to the number of eggs c multiplied by their where “x” and “y” represent the strategy (caring or non- survival probability when unguarded (e⫺hm). To obtain a caring) adopted by males and females, respectively. Table brood, a male will have first to survive the search phase 1 provides a summary of all the parameters used in the (with probability (1/s m)/(1/s m ⫹ m mnc ) p 1/(1 ⫹ s m m mnc )), model. after which it will obtain a payoff vmnc. Subsequently, the male will be able to reenter the search phase only if he survives the processing phase, with probability e⫺p mnc m mnc. Analysis Writing wmnc for the lifetime reproductive success of a nonguarding male, we thus have We will assume in the following analysis that females can produce and guard only one brood at a time while males 1 can acquire several overlapping broods and guard them wmnc p (v ⫹ e⫺p mnc m mnc wmnc ). simultaneously. For both males and females, several care 1 ⫹ s m m mnc mnc strategies are theoretically possible. A behavioral “rule” covering all situations would be very complex, and the set Substituting vmnc and solving for wmnc, we obtain of all possible strategies of this kind would be very large, especially for males that can care simultaneously for several ce⫺hm wmnc p . (1) broods of different ages. Here, for the sake of simplicity, 1 ⫹ s m m mnc ⫺ e⫺m mnc p mnc we consider only two strategies for each sex: no care, in which individuals immediately abandon a new clutch to The lifetime reproductive success of a guarding male seek out more mates, and full care, in which females guard mutant that arises in the population can be calculated eggs until hatching and males remain permanently in one using the same approach. The mean payoff (vmc) from a location, guarding any clutch they may acquire and waiting single batch of c eggs depends on the amount of time the for females to locate them (although we still refer to this male remains alive to guard them. If the male guarded the period of waiting as search time). Although simplistic, this brood up to hatching, the survival probability of the eggs limited strategy set still allows us to investigate the effects until hatching would be e⫺hmMmc. However, if we take into
Paternal Care with Overlapping Broods 521 account that the male might die at some time t ! h, the for mates or replenishing their sperm stores. We define average payoff is the proportion of searching males within the population as Ps mnc and the proportion of males that are replenishing 冕 冕 ⬁ h their sperm stores as Pc mnc. At any given moment in time, vmc p c ⫺m mct ⫺mMmct ⫺(h⫺t)m m mce e e ⫹ m mce⫺m mcte⫺hmMmc a proportion of males moves from the searching pool into tp0 tph the replenishing pool at a rate equal to their average en- c[e⫺h(mMmc⫹m mc)(mM mc ⫺ m) ⫹ e⫺hmm mc ] counter rate with females (l mnc p 1/s m). On the other p . hand, in a stable population, males will move out of the m mc ⫹ mM mc ⫺ m search pool either because they die (at rate m mnc) or because A guarding male will also suffer a mating cost, leading they have completed the whole replenishment period (with to longer search times (s m /(1 ⫺ g)) compared with the rest probability e⫺p mnc m mnc). Also, in a stable population, the rate of the population. Writing wmc for the lifetime reproductive of change in Pc mnc is equal to 0 (i.e., no change in the success of a guarding mutant, we thus have proportion of replenishing males), and it can be written as 1 wmc p (v ⫹ e⫺p mc m mc wmc ). 1 ⫹ [m mc s m /(1 ⫺ g)] mc dPc mnc p lPs mnc ⫺ m mnc Pc mnc ⫺ lPs mnce⫺p mnc m mnc p 0. Substituting vmc and solving for wmc, we obtain dt wmc p Since Pc mnc p (1 ⫺ Ps mnc ), we can rearrange the above equa- c[e⫺h(mMmc⫹m mc )(mM mc ⫺ m) ⫹ e⫺hmm mc ] tion as . (m mc ⫹ mM mc ⫺ m){1 ⫹ [m mc s m /(1 ⫺ g)] ⫺ e⫺p mc m mc} (2) 1 Ps mnc p . 1 ⫹ (l mnc /m mnc )(1 ⫺ e⫺p mnc m mnc) For stability, we require that wmnc 1 wmc, that is, that the nonguarding males that make up the population should have a higher lifetime reproductive success than the guard- Similarly, ing mutant. Writing out wmnc and wmc, we obtain ce⫺hm 1 Ps fnc p . 1 1 ⫹ (l fnc /m fnc )(1 ⫺ e⫺p fnc m fnc) 1 ⫹ s m m mnc ⫺ e⫺m mnc p mnc c[e⫺h(mMmc⫹m mc )(mM mc ⫺ m) ⫹ e⫺hmm mc ] . The rate at which individuals move from the search (m mc ⫹ mM mc ⫺ m){1 ⫹ [m mc s m /(1 ⫺ g)] ⫺ e⫺p mc m mc} phase to the processing phase (i.e., the encounter rate) is (3) proportional to the product of the population density (d), the proportion of females within the population (1 ⫺ To solve this inequality, we need an explicit formula for a mncfnc), and the proportion of individuals of the other sex male search time (s m) in a population of nonguarding available for mating (i.e., searching; Ps fnc). Thus, we can individuals. Males can be in either of two states: searching write Figure 1: Effect of population density and relative female processing time on the ability of male care to invade a noncaring population under different costs scenarios: (A) low (g p 0.3 ) and (B) high (g p 0.8 ) mating cost, (C) low (mmc p 0.015 ) and (D) high (mmc p 0.03) survival cost, and (E) low (pmc p 1.5) and (F) high (pmc p 3) energetic cost. Successively lighter shaded areas represent zones of stability for decreasing values of Mmc (i.e., higher benefit of care). When males suffered mating or mortality costs, the invasion of care was favored at high densities and low female processing times, whereas energetic costs favored the invasion of care at low densities and high female processing times. In each of the scenarios, the costs that were not being investigated were set to 0 (i.e., g p 0 , pmc p pmnc , mmc p mmnc , respectively). Each graph shows the range of densities and values of pfnc (relative to pmnc ) over which male care could invade for three different values of Mmc , the proportion of unguarded egg mortality suffered when guarding (A, 0.90, 0.91, 0.92; B, 0.55, 0.60, 0.65; C, 0.8560, 0.8561, 0.8562; D, 0.5950, 0.5955, 0.5960; E, 0.96, 0.97, 0.98; F, 0.7, 0.8, 0.9; unguarded egg mortality m was set to 0.3). Other parameter values were a p 0.5 (an even sex ratio), pmnc p 1 , k p 1, h p 10, mfnc p mmnc p 0.01 (no sex difference in mortality). Note that the units of density are undefined; the graph simply shows the effects of variation over a 10-fold range to invade a nonguarding population.
Paternal Care with Overlapping Broods 523 1 nonguarding females is analogous to that of males de- l mnc p kd(1 ⫺ a mncfnc )Ps fnc p , scribed in equation (1): sm 1 ce⫺hm l fnc p kda mncfnc Ps mnc p . wfnc p . (5) sf 1 ⫹ m fnc s f ⫺ e⫺p fnc m mnc Substituting the above in the equations for Ps mnc and Ps fnc On the other hand, a guarding female mutant needs to and solving for s m and s f, we obtain survive both the guarding and the processing time before being able to produce another batch of eggs: (1 ⫹ m fnc s f ⫺ e⫺p fnc m fnc) sm p , 1 (1 ⫺ a mcfnc )dkm fnc s f wfc p [v ⫹ e⫺(h⫹p fc )m fc wfc ], 1 ⫹ s f m fc fc (1 ⫹ m mc s m ⫺ e⫺p mc m mc) sf p . a mcfncdkm mc s m where vfc can be computed by adapting the formula for vmc, giving This pair of equations can be solved for s m: c[e⫺h(mMfc⫹m fc )(mM fc ⫺ m) ⫹ e⫺hmm fc ] wfc p . (6) (mM fc ⫺ m ⫹ m fc )[1 ⫹ m fc s f ⫺ e⫺m fc(h⫹p fc )] sm p A ⫺ ([ 1 ⫺ e⫺p mnc m mnc a mncfnc(1 ⫺ e⫺p fnc m fnc) m mnc ⫹ m fnc(1 ⫺ a mncfnc ) ] For the nonguarding population to be stable against the invasion of a guarding mutant, we need wfnc 1 wfc. Writing 2 1 ⫺ e⫺p mnc m mnc a mncfnc(1 ⫺ e⫺p fnc m fnc) out wfnc and wfc, we obtain {[ ⫹ A⫺ m mnc ⫹ m fnc(1 ⫺ a mncfnc ) ] ce⫺hm 1 1 ⫹ m fnc s f ⫺ e⫺p fnc m mnc U 1/2 ⫹4 A(1 ⫺ e⫺p mnc m mnc) m mnc }) 2, c[e⫺h(mMfc⫹m fc )(mM fc ⫺ m) ⫹ e⫺hmm fc ] (mM fc ⫺ m ⫹ m fc )[1 ⫹ m fc s f ⫺ e⫺m fc(h⫹p fc )] . (7) (4) Using the approach described for equation (4), we can where obtain an expression for female search time: 1 Ap (1 ⫺ a mncfnc )kd . Substituting the above equation into equation (3), we can sf p A ⫺([ 1 ⫺ e⫺p fnc m fnc (1 ⫺ a mncfnc )(1 ⫺ e⫺p mnc m mnc) m fnc ⫹ m mnca mncfnc 2 ] 1 ⫺ e⫺p fnc m fnc (1 ⫺ a mncfnc )(1 ⫺ e⫺p mnc m mnc) thus predict when a noncaring population will be stable against the invasion of a caring male mutant. {[ ⫹ A⫺ m fnc ⫹ m mnca mncfnc ] U 1/2 Stability of No Care against the Invasion of Female Care Let us now consider the likelihood of the invasion of a ⫹4 A(1 ⫺ e⫺p fnc m fnc) m fnc }) 2, caring female mutant. Lifetime reproductive success for (8) Figure 2: Effect of population density and relative female processing time on the ability of female care to invade a noncaring population under different costs scenarios: (A) high (k p 2 ) and (B) low (k p 0.1 ) encounter rates, (C) low (mfc p 0.015) and (D) high (mfc p 0.03) survival cost, and (E) low (pfc p 1.5) and (F) high (pfc p 3 ) energetic cost. Successively lighter shaded areas represent zones of stability for decreasing values of Mfc (i.e., higher benefit of care). The invasion of care was favored at low densities and high female processing times. Each graph shows the range of densities and values of pmnc (relative to pfnc ) over which female care can invade for three different values of Mfc , the proportion of unguarded egg mortality suffered when guarding (A, 0.7, 0.8, 0.9; B, 0.5, 0.6, 0.7; C, 0.2, 0.3, 0.4; D, 0.01, 0.05, 0.10; E, 0.4, 0.5, 0.6; F, 0.3, 0.4, 0.5; unguarded egg mortality m was set to 0.5). Other parameter values are a p 0.5 (an even sex ratio), pfnc p 1 , k p 1 , h p 5 , mfnc p mmnc p 0.01 (no sex difference in mortality). Note that the units of density are undefined; the graph simply shows the effects of variation over a 10-fold range on the ability of female care to invade a nonguarding population.
524 The American Naturalist Figure 3: Effect of the benefit of male and female care on the stability of biparental care under three cost scenarios: (A) equal costs of care for the two sexes (pmc p pfc p 2.4, mmc p mfc p 0.012), (B) higher costs for males (pmc p 4 , mmc p 0.02), and (C) higher costs for females (pfc p 4, mfc p 0.02). Larger benefits of care are represented by smaller values of Mmc and Mfc . BC, Area of stability of biparental care; MC, area where a noncaring female mutant can invade, leading to male care; FC, area where a noncaring mutant can invade; U, unstable areas where both mutants can invade. Other parameter values are a p 0.5 (an even sex ratio), pmnc p pfnc p 2, mmnc p mfnc p 0.01, k p 1, d p 5, h p 10, m p 0.7. where M fc, in lighter gray). Also, it is intuitively obvious that smaller values of M mc and of M fc (i.e., large benefit of care) 1 are required for caring mutants to invade when care is Ap . a mncfnckd more costly (figs. 1, 2). Biparental care is stable only when the balance between the costs and benefits of care for the two sexes is comparable (fig. 3). A sex bias in terms of costs has to be counterbalanced by a bias in the benefits Results of care (fig. 3B, 3C). Any sex bias in the cost to benefit It is clear that when the benefit of care is high (represented ratio of care favors uniparental care by the sex with the in figs. 1 and 2 by a small M mc and M fc, respectively, in most advantageous ratio. However, a balance in the cost darker gray), uniparental care is likely to become estab- to benefit ratio in the two sexes would not be necessary lished in a noncaring population over a wider range of for the evolution of biparental care if the benefit of two conditions than when the benefit is low (large M mc and parents guarding the offspring was disproportionately
Paternal Care with Overlapping Broods 525 Table 2: Parameter estimates obtained from the literature Rhinocoris Rhinocoris Parameter Source tristis carmelita m Estimated in the wild by daily monitoring broods from which the guarding parent .0892 .2345 was removed.a Mmc Estimated in the wild by monitoring broods of R. tristis daily. Laboratory experi- .3037 .7252 ments determined that some of the mortality was due to filial cannibalism.a,b We assumed R. carmelita males would also cannibalize if they were to guard eggs. Since there is hardly any sexual dimorphism, we assumed both sexes to be equally capable of defending eggs. An estimate of egg mortality when R. carme- lita males provided protection was obtained by subtracting mortality due to can- nibalism from the estimate from females of the same species (see below). Mfc Estimated in the wild by daily monitoring broods of R. carmelita.a In R. tristis, we .0793 .3439 used the estimate obtained for males (see above) but discounted mortality due to filial cannibalism. pmnc The fastest intermating time recorded in the laboratory.c .3333 .3333 pmc The fastest intermating time for an R. tristis male that was allowed to keep his .3333 .3333 eggs.c Male R. tristis cannibalize some of their own eggs, minimizing the cost of care. We assumed R. carmelita males would be able to do the same if they were to guard eggs. pfnc The fastest interlaying time for R. tristis in the laboratory and for R. carmelita from 2 2 which the brood was artificially removed.c pfc Laboratory estimate for R. carmelita.c We assumed R. tristis female reproductive 15.6 15.6 physiology to be similar to that of R. carmelita (supported by the identical pfnc values). h Field estimates from demographic data.a 16.5 12 sm Field estimates from demographic data.a 4.3 … sf Field estimates from demographic data.a … 5.8 mmc p mmnc p mfc p mfnc Estimated from demographic data for R. tristis; there was no difference between .0286 .0286 sexes or between guarding and nonguarding individuals.a No data exist for R. carmelita, but we assumed mortality to be equal to R. tristis since the two species are sympatric and morphologically similar. g Derived from gas equation, assuming males and females move at the same speed .2929 .2929 when searching for mates. Males were assumed to be stationary when guarding. a Field estimates from demographic data.a .5 .5 d Field estimates from demographic data.a 5.4 1.2 c Field estimates from demographic data.a 19.63 35.82 k Estimated using the appropriate equation and the parameters detailed in this table. .2099 .2913 Note: Figures in bold were estimated directly either through demographic studies or through manipulative experiments. Values in italic represent indirect estimates either from first principle or by assuming the biology of one species to be similar to that of the other species for which information was available. a Data obtained from Thomas (1994). b Data obtained from Thomas and Manica (2003). c Data obtained from Thomas and Manica (in press). greater than the combined benefits arising from two in- time lead to an increase in encounter rates and thus a dividual carers (as opposed to our assumption that reduction in the proportion of time that males spend M bc p M mc # M fc). For simplicity’s sake, we do not in- searching/waiting for mates. A short search time mini- vestigate this possibility in detail, even though the model mizes the mating cost and decreases the probability that could be easily modified to do so. a guarding male will die because of the increased mortality When males suffer a mating (fig. 1A, 1B) or a survival while guarding. Comparison of figure 1A and 1B reveals cost (fig. 1C, 1D), male care is more likely to become that density is more important compared with processing established when density is high and when female pro- time when mating costs are higher (so that encounter rates cessing time is low relative to male processing time. This for caring males are lower). This is because an increased is because both high density and low female processing encounter rate for caring males means that even at low
526 The American Naturalist Figure 4: Sensitivity analysis of the stability of care in (top) Rhinocoris tristis and (bottom) Rhinocoris carmelita. Plausible transitions (when the lifetime reproductive success of a mutant is larger than that of the rest of the population) are shown by solid black arrows, whereas dashed arrows show implausible transitions. Note that sex-specific traits can affect transitions that refer to the other sex (e.g., the transition between biparental care and male-only care depends on the lifetime reproductive success of caring and noncaring females, but these are strongly influenced by male mortality, which determines the likelihood that a caring male will survive long enough to guard the brood for a significant amount of time). The ratio between the lifetime reproductive success of the mutant and the rest of the population is given together with a 95% confidence interval obtained from simulations in which all parameters were allowed to vary simultaneously. Parameters that destabilize a transition during simulations, changing only one parameter at a time (with a coefficient of variation p 0.1), are presented next to the transition they affected. density, females mate very soon after they become avail- density is the most important factor determining the abil- able, so that female processing has the main influence on ity of male care to invade a noncaring population. the proportion of time that males spend searching/waiting When females guard the eggs, on the other hand, the for a mate. When encounter rates are low, by contrast, effect of male processing time and density is different. For
Paternal Care with Overlapping Broods 527 Figure 5: Effect of density and male mating cost on the stability of parental care in (A) Rhinocoris tristis and (B) Rhinocoris carmelita. The asterisks denote the estimates of these two parameters obtained from the literature. MC, Area of stability of male care; FC, area of female care; BC, biparental care; NC, no care. Areas where both male and female care could become stable, depending on which one evolves first, are denoted by MC or FC. Small zones of instability (i.e., where two strategies would coexist) around the boundaries between areas of stability are not shown for the sake of simplicity. All these zones are very narrow. Parameter values are given in table 2. any given value of M fc, female care is more likely to invade An Empirical Test of the Model when density is low and when female processing time is high. This is because both low density and high female The ability of the model to predict the evolutionarily stable processing time lead to an increase in female intermating form of care was tested on two species of assassin bugs: interval, with the result that care involves a proportionately Rhinocoris tristis, which exhibits paternal care, and Rhino- smaller decrease in mating rate. Contrary to what was coris carmelita, which exhibits maternal care. Despite observed for male care, stability was negatively affected by showing different patterns of care, the two species are short search time even with a survival cost. This is the sympatric and morphologically very similar, thus provid- result of guarding time acting in a similar fashion to a ing an ideal test for our model. We have detailed infor- large energetic cost of care for males, forcing the female mation on several population parameters (such as en- to have a longer time out. Stability would be favored by counter rates and population density) and on the impact a relatively short search time only if hatching time was of care on both adult and egg survival as well as gamete extremely short relative to the processing time of non- replenishment times for both species (Thomas 1994; Tho- guarding females. It is also apparent, comparing figure 2A mas and Manica 2003 and in press). The parameters of and 2B, that the relative importance of female processing the model were estimated from either demographic data time compared with density increases when encounter obtained by daily monitoring of individuals in the field or rates are high. This is because an increased encounter rate simple manipulations (table 2 and references therein). For means that even at low density, females mate very soon each species, it was obviously not possible to estimate di- after they become available. Consequently, it is female pro- rectly the impact of care for the noncaring sex (e.g., we cessing time rather than density that has the main influ- cannot directly measure how long a mutant caring R. tristis ence on the intermating interval. When encounter rates female would need to replenish her gametes after guarding are low, by contrast, density is the critical factor. A similar the eggs, since females of this species never guard). Since effect is seen when males pay an energetic cost. Under this the two species have very similar ecology and morphology, scenario, when processing time for the mutant guarding we assumed the impact of care on the noncaring sex in males is extremely long relative to the nonguarding males, each species to be equal to the impact measured in the a relatively long search time (given by high density and other species (e.g., the impact of care on processing time long female processing time) minimizes the advantage that for a mutant caring R. tristis female was assumed to be nonguarding males have in processing broods at a faster the same as measured in caring R. carmelita females). The rate. only parameter that could not be estimated directly from
528 The American Naturalist Table 3: Predictions on the stability of care Discussion Type of care Favored by Insights from the Model Male High population density Our model shows that, when males can guard overlapping Short female processing time Low mortality for caring males broods, the evolution of exclusive paternal care is not an Female Low population density unlikely event as suggested by models that assume that Long female processing time guarding and mating are mutually exclusive (e.g., Maynard Biparental Small sex bias in the cost to benefit ratio of Smith 1977; Balshine-Earn and Earn 1997; Wade and Shus- care ter 2002). When males are able to care for overlapping broods, their cost of care is drastically reduced compared with caring males that can only guard broods sequentially. Unless the benefit of biparental care is disproportionately empirical data for either species was the mating cost of greater than the combination of the benefits provided by guarding males. Caring males are stationary and have to each parent individually, an asymmetry in the costs and wait for a female to find them, whereas noncaring indi- benefits of care to the two sexes has the obvious effect of viduals (both males and females) can freely move searching favoring uniparental care by the sex with the best balance; for mates. We assume the latter to move randomly through the ability to care for overlapping broods together with space, with both sexes being equally efficient at searching anisogamy makes male care relatively more likely to evolve for mates. Under this scenario, the movement of individ- than female care. uals can be modeled using the gas equation (e.g., Dusen- Out of the factors we investigated, male mortality had bery 2000), and the decrease in encounter rates for sta- the strongest influence on the stability of paternal care (see tionary males can be estimated as g p 1 ⫺ 1/2(1/2). fig. 1C, 1D, where very small steps in M mc lead to sharp Using the parameter estimates detailed in table 2, the shifts in the ability of care to invade, and fig. 4 [bottom], model predicted the correct form of care in both species, where the stability of care in Rhinocoris carmelita was with paternal care being the only stable strategy for R. mostly influenced by this parameter). This result is a con- tristis and maternal care for R. carmelita (fig. 4). The sen- sequence of the ability of males to continuously add new sitivity of these predictions was investigated through sim- broods, thus leading to potentially very high payoffs for ulations. Initially, we allowed all parameter values to vary increased longevity. This factor is particularly important simultaneously. Each parameter value was randomly sam- in R. carmelita, where the benefit of care is relatively low pled from a normal distribution with the estimated value when compared with Rhinocoris tristis. Since caring for from table 2 as the mean and a coefficient of variation each brood gives only a limited payoff to caring males, equal to 0.05. Confidence intervals from 1,000 simulations their ability to process a large number of broods when are given in figure 4. Subsequently, we investigated the caring is fundamental in determining the stability of this effect of each individual parameter by keeping all other strategy. parameters equal to the estimates given in table 2. For this Density was shown to be an important ecological factor second set of simulations, the coefficient of variation for that could influence the stability of care. The effect of the parameter being investigated was raised to 0.1. Param- density depends on the relative magnitude of the three eters that affected the stability of care in more than 5% costs (promiscuity, mortality, and energetic). In general, of 1,000 simulations are given in figure 4. The predictions we would expect male care to be most stable in high- for R. tristis are very robust with respect to the exact pa- density populations, since short search times minimize rameter estimates, whereas the predictions for R. carmelita both the mating cost of care and the probability of dying are more sensitive to the actual values of the benefit of without having obtained many matings. In most species, care (m, M mc) and male mortality (m mnc and m mc; fig. 4). this effect is unlikely to be counteracted by the energetic We further investigated the sensitivity of our results with cost of care, which is often relatively small for males and respect to the reduction in mating efficiency g, since the can be further minimized through filial cannibalism (Man- mating cost was not estimated from field data, and pop- ica 2002). On the other hand, low densities are more likely ulation density d, since this parameter is likely to vary to favor female care, since females tend to pay a high widely across populations of the same species. The stability energetic cost of care because of their expensive gametes of care in the two species shows similar patterns (fig. 5). and thus have a relatively long time out when guarding Female care is stable only at relatively low densities, because of their inability to guard multiple broods si- whereas male care becomes stable at higher densities. The multaneously. Similarly, we would expect short female pro- effect of the promiscuity parameter g decreases slightly cessing times to favor paternal care and long female pro- with increasing population density. cessing times to favor female care.
Paternal Care with Overlapping Broods 529 Density might play an important role in the evolution location where females can still locate mates, whereas non- of paternal care in assassin bugs. Whereas R. carmelita is predatory species are more likely to be unable to effectively found at low densities and thus is prone to potentially guard more eggs than can be covered by their body (e.g., high costs for guarding males, R. tristis is found at very Mappes and Kaitala 1994). high densities, possibly because of its specialized associ- Sexual selection has been recently suggested as a po- ation with the legume Stylosanthes (Thomas 1994). Inter- tentially important factor in the evolution of uniparental estingly, the other three known paternal species of assassin male care, but only verbal models have been used to infer bugs, Rhinocoris albopilosus, Rhinocoris albopunctatus, and the effect of density and processing times on the evolution Zelus sp., are also found at relatively high densities as well of care (Tallamy 2000, 2001). Our model could be used as being specialized for hunting on the flowers of certain to investigate the effect of sexual selection by allowing the species of legumes (Odhiambo 1959; Ralston 1977; Tho- mating cost to be mitigated or even to be turned into a mas 1994). benefit (such that s mc ! s mnc), if females prefer guarding males and actively search for them. Applicability of the Model Conclusions The model accurately predicted the caring sex in assassin bugs. However, its most obvious application would be to Our model makes a set of testable predictions for when investigate why paternal care is so common in fish. Trivers parental care should evolve and which sex should look (1972) suggested that this association might be a conse- after the offspring in species where males can obtain over- quence of gametic proximity. He speculated that the sex lapping broods (see table 3 for a summary). We believe that is last associated with the eggs is most likely to care, our framework to be general enough to fit most species, because it is faced with a “cruel bind.” Since fish mostly and the parameters should be relatively easy to measure show external fertilization (Gross and Sargent 1985), males empirically. The model successfully predicted the stable would usually be left behind to care for the eggs. However, form of care in two species of assassin bugs with uniparen- this hypothesis does not explain why several fish species tal care, but further testing in other species (especially in with internal fertilization show paternal care (Clutton- fish) would be very informative. Brock 1991) nor why paternal care in internally fertilizing insects such as assassin bugs strongly resembles care shown Acknowledgments by fish (Thomas and Manica 2003 and in press). The ability We are indebted to L. Thomas, who was the first to suggest to look after a large number of offspring (i.e., multiple that high population densities could favor paternal care broods) is likely to be a key factor in explaining paternal with overlapping broods and encouraged us to look at this care in fish and insects, which both have relatively small issue, providing advice and data on assassin bugs. We also eggs that need to be guarded only until hatching, with no thank T. Clutton-Brock, N. Davies, H. Kokko, and A. or little provisioning for the larvae. Young for providing useful comments on the manuscript. The major difficulty in applying our model to fish is the limited presence of species with female care, thus mak- Literature Cited ing the parameterization of the model problematic. One possible solution is to design laboratory manipulations Balshine-Earn, S. 1995. The costs of parental care in Galilee that force females to go through activities similar to those St. Peters fish, Sarotherodon galilaeus. Animal Behaviour undertaken by caring males. However, an interspecific 50:1–7. comparison similar to the one we presented for assassin Balshine-Earn, S., and D. J. D. Earn. 1997. An evolutionary bugs might be possible using species from the Stichaeidae, model of parental care in St. Peter’s Fish. Journal of a little studied family of intertidal fishes. Several species Theoretical Biology 184:423–431. within this family show either male care, with males guard- Bateman, A. J. 1948. Intra-sexual selection in Drosophila. ing multiple broods simultaneously, or female care, with Heredity 2:349–368. female guarders processing one batch of eggs at a time Blumer, L. S. 1979. Male parental care in the bony fishes. (reviewed in Coleman 1992). Quarterly Review of Biology 54:149–161. Further testing of our model with other insect systems Clutton-Brock, T. H. 1991. The evolution of parental care. could also provide interesting insights. For example, pa- Princeton University Press, Princeton, N.J. ternal care in insects is found almost exclusively in pred- Clutton-Brock, T. H., S. D. Albon, and F. E. Guinness. atory species (Smith 1980; Tallamy and Wood 1986). Pred- 1989. Fitness costs of gestation and lactation in wild atory species are the best candidates for being able to mammals. Nature 337:260–262. aggressively protect a large number of eggs in an exposed Coleman, R. M. 1992. Reproductive biology and female
530 The American Naturalist parental care in the cockscomb prickleback, Anaplarchus increase with clutch size in the fathead minnow, Pi- purpurescens (Pisces: Stichaeidae). Environmental Bi- mephales promelas. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology ology of Fishes 35:177–186. 23:33–37. Crowl, T. A., and J. E. Alexander. 1989. Parental care and Shine, R. 1980. “Costs” of reproduction in reptiles. Oeco- foraging ability in male water bugs (Belostoma flumi- logia (Berlin) 46:92–100. neum). Canadian Journal of Zoology 67:513–515. Smith, R. L. 1980. Evolution of exclusive postcopulatory Dusenbery, D. B. 2000. Selection for high gamete en- paternal care in the insects. Florida Entomologist 63: counter rates explains the success of male and female 65–78. mating types. Journal of Theoretical Biology 202:1–10. Tallamy, D. W. 2000. Sexual selection and the evolution Gross, M. R., and R. C. Sargent. 1985. The evolution of of exclusive paternal care in arthropods. Animal Be- male and female parental care in fishes. American Zo- haviour 60:559–567. ologist 25:807–822. ———. 2001. Evolution of exclusive paternal care in ar- Gross, M. R., and R. Shine. 1981. Parental care and mode thropods. Annual Review of Entomology 46:139–165. of fertilization in ectothermic vertebrates. Evolution 35: Tallamy, D. W., and T. K. Wood. 1986. Convergence pat- 775–793. terns in subsocial insects. Annual Review of Entomology Lack, D. 1966. Population studies of birds. Clarendon, 31:369–390. Oxford. Thomas, L. K. 1994. The evolution of parental care in Manica, A. 2002. Filial cannibalism in teleost fish. Bio- assassin bugs. University of Cambridge, Cambridge. logical Reviews 77:261–277. Thomas, L. K., and A. Manica. 2003. Filial cannibalism in Mappes, J., and A. Kaitala. 1994. Experiments with Elas- an assassin bug. Animal Behaviour 66:205–210. mucha grisea L. (Heteroptera: Acanthosomatidae): does ———. In press. Intrasexual competition and mate choice a female parental bug lay as many eggs as she can de- in assassin bugs with uniparental male and female care. fend? Behavioral Ecology 5:314–317. Animal Behaviour. Maynard Smith, J. 1977. Parental investment: a prospective Trivers, R. L. 1972. Parental investment and sexual selec- analysis. Animal Behaviour 25:1–9. tion. Pages 136–179 in B. Campbell, ed. Sexual selection Odhiambo, T. R. 1959. An account of parental care in and the descent of man, 1871–1971. Aldine-Atherton, Rhinocoris albopilosus Signoret (Hemiptera-Heteroptera: Chicago. Reduviidae), with notes in its life history. Proceedings Wade, M. J., and S. M. Shuster. 2002. The evolution of parental care in the context of sexual selection: a critical of the Royal Entomological Society of London A General reassessment of parental investment theory. American Entomology 34:175–185. Naturalist 160:285–292. Ralston, J. S. 1977. Egg guarding by male assassin bugs of Wootton, R. J. 1984. A functional biology of sticklebacks. the genus Zelus (Hemiptera: Reduvidae). Psyche 87: Croom Helm, London. 103–107. Sargent, R. C. 1988. Paternal care and egg survival both Associate Editor: Allen J. Moore
You can also read