The Electric and Magnetic Sense of Sharks, SkaS, andRays - by Adrianus J. Kalmijn
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
A shark swimming through the earth’s magnetic field induces electric fields giving the animal’s compass heading (the horizontal component of the earth’s magnetic field and the induced electrical currents are indicated). (From Kalmijn, 1974) The Electric and Magnetic Sense of Sharks, SkaS, andRays by Adrianus J. Kalmijn Scientists must know the sensory world of an that the elasmobranchs also detect the electric animal if they are to understand its behavior. fields they induce when swimming through Certainly most aquatic vertebrates have good the earth’s magnetic field, and thus sense their eyesight, hear well, and are endowed with a compass heading. Obviously, the electric sharp sense of smell and taste. Yet the physical sense of these ancient fishes has reached a characteristics of underwater light, sound, and high degree of sophistication. odor fields are quite different from those on Last year, the author and his student land. Moreover, animals use the sensory collaborators initiated field experiments in information in a variety of ways, depending on Vineyard Sound off Cape Cod, Massachusetts, the particular interest of the species. to verify the results of earlier laboratory Therefore, scientists cannot infer from man’s studies on the electrical aspects of predation. own sensory experience how an animal After chumming with chopped herring, we perceives its environment but have to learn observed the smooth dogfish Mu&e/us canis through behavioral tests and field search the bottom and viciously attack a observations. This point is dramatically current source simulating the bioelectric fields emphasized by animals that have sensory of the shark’s prey. Although attracted and capabilities that stretch man’s imagination. motivated by odor, the predators made their One of the most interesting examples of this is final approach exclusively relying on their the electromagnetic sensory performance of electric sense. Coming in with the sharks, the marine sharks, skates, and rays, commonly American eel Anguilla rostrata paid no referred to as elasmobranch fishes. attention to the fields, whereas in comparative In predation, sharks, skates, and rays tests the catfish lctalurus nebulosus repeatedly cunningly cue in on the weak, bioelectric dug at a current source hidden in the mud fields of their prey, even though it may be along the banks of a local freshwater pond. hiding under sand. These well-aimed feeding The magnetic orientation studies have responses clearly demonstrate the remarkable been conducted on the stingray Urolophus acuity of the elasmobranchs’ electric sense halleri at our land facilities, set up in the woods and testify to its biological significance in the of the quiet Quissett Campus of the Woods animals’ daily life. Recent research indicates Hole Oceanographic Institution. The stingrays 45
were trained to seek reward and avoid decelerations when applying electric fields of punishment by orienting to the earth’s voltage gradients as low as 0.01 microvolt per magnetic field. Between trials, the horizontal centimeter, which represents the highest component of the magnetic field was reversed electrical sensitivity known in the animal in a random order to eliminate the use of kingdom. In later behavioral tests, the alternative orientational cues. These frequency range of the animals’ response experiments have conclusively demonstrated appeared to extend from direct current (DC) up the elasmobranchs’ magnetic sensory abilities, to about 8 hertz. Reception of these which the author predicted on the basis of low-frequency, low-level electric fields was *_ their acute electric sense. shown to take place by means of the ampullae of Lorenzini, delicate sensory structures in the ,, Previous Studies protruding snout of the elasmobranch fishes (Figure 1). In 1917, G. H. Parker and A. P. Van Heusen published a historic paper on the curious behavior of Ictalurus, the common brown bullhead, with regard to metallic and non-metallic objects. They noticed that blindfolded specimens responded to the approach of metallic rods at distances of several centimeters, whereas a glass rod did not elicit a reaction until it actually touched the skin of the animals. In a series of simple but convincing experiments, Parker and Van Heusen demonstrated that these distant responses were due to galvanic currents Figure 1. Ampullae offorenziniandmechanicallateral-line system in head region of the shark Scyliorhinus canicula. generated at the interface between metal and (Solid dots: skin pores of electroreceptors. Small circles: aquarium water. Though very close to openings of lateral-line canals) (From Dijkgraafand discovering the catfish’s electric sense, these Kalmijn, 7963) scientists did not realize the biological implications of their remarkable results. By measuring the electric fields in the In 1934, the Dutch biologist S. Dijkgraaf laboratory habitat of the sharks and skates, it also observed a great sensitivity to metallic was found that aquatic animals produce objects in the small shark Scyliorhinus direct-current and low-frequency electric canicula. A quarter of a century later, he fields in the water, which stem mainly from suggested that the author, then his student in potential differences at the skin/water the Netherlands, investigate the possibility of interface. In fish, for example, the mucous electrical stimulation, as in the case of the membranes lining the mouth and the gill catfish, and evaluate the biological epithelia in the pharynx give rise to steady DC significance of the response. A few years fields, usually modulated by ventilatory earlier, H. W. Lissmann in England had movements. Externally, the bioelectric fields discovered that certain knife-fishes produce are of distributed dipole* configuration and, weak electrical discharges to probe their accordingly, fall off steeply with increasing environment. Sharks, however, lack distance. Yet the DC voltage gradients electrogenic organs; therefore, the main emanating from small fish and wounded crabs questions were: what might be the fields in often measured over 0.01 microvolt per nature that the sharks detect, and what could centimeter at distances up to 25 centimeters, be the role of these fields in the animals’ daily which suggested the role of the life? elasmobranchs’ electric sense in predation. Subsequent research soon revealed To elucidate the electrical aspects of that marine sharks, skates, and rays are indeed predation, the author decided to analyze the extremely sensitive to weak electric fields. By feeding responses of the shark Scyliorhinus recording the heartbeat of free-swimming specimens with implanted electrodes, the *A field produced by equal but opposite electrical p’oles, author established transient cardiac usually separated by a small distance. 46
(a) (b) Figure 2. Feeding responses of the shark Scvliorhinus caniculato: (a)’ flounder under sand, ib) flounder .~ in.._an --‘., .C C.__.._L ..N.- electrically transparent agarchamber, (c)pieces of ,.-- --.- whiting in an agar chamber, (d) flounder in an agar chamber covered with an electrically insulating plastic film, and flounder. Agar chamber not to scale. Solidarrows: responses of shark; dashed arrows: flow of seawater through agar chamber. (From Kalmijn, 1971) canicula and the skate Raja clavata to small purpose of the agar chamber was to conceal specimens of the flounder Pleuronectes the flounder visually, chemically, and platessa. The prey was cautiously introduced mechanically without impeding the animal’s into the seawater habitat, and after it had bioelectric field. Again, after odor motivation, hidden itself in the sand, a few drops of the sharks and skates made their well-aimed liquified whiting were diffusely spread feeding attacks from the same distance and in throughout the water. Motivated by the odor, the same frenzied manner, as if the prey were the sharks and skates began randomly not screened by agar at all (Figure 2b). searching the bottom of the pool. When To prove that the l-centimeter-thick coming within a distance of IO to 15 roof of the agar chamber was really effective in centimeters from the flounder, they made attenuating odor stimuli, the live flounder was well-aimed dives at their prey, uncovered it exchanged for pieces of whiting that had been from under the sand, and devoured it frozen for several days (whiting was the regular voraciously (Figure2a). Subsequently, the food for the experimental animals). In this flounder was placed in a flat chamber made case, the predators did not show the slightest out of 3 percent agar in seawater, and response to the food when swimming over the positioned under the sand on the bottom of agar chamber, although they-were strongly the pool. The chamber was aerated by a steady motivated by the odor of the seawater flow flow of seawater to keep the prey alive. The ventilating the chamber (Figure2c). Whether 47
the agar was adequate to mechanically shield During the summer of 1976, we learned the live flounder was indirectly tested by from longline fishing off Cape Cod that the repeating the original experiment, this time, smooth dogfish Mustelus regularly frequents however, with a thin, electrically insulating the shallow, inshore waters of Vineyard Sound polyethylene film covering the agar chamber. on its nightly feeding excursions. This Now, the sharks and skates no longer predatory shark is a warm-season visitor, responded to the prey, although they eagerly arriving at Woods Hole in May and leaving for searched about and often passed directly over the South again in late October or shortly it (Figure 2d). This drastic effect could not thereafter. It is an active bottom hunter, conceivably be due to the mechaniical preying on small fish as well as crustaceans and properties of the thin (IO micrometer) and other invertebrate animals. The females reach extremely pliable polyethylene film, since an average length of 115 centimeters; the even the stiff, l-centimeter-thick agar roof to males are slightly smaller. The smooth dogfish which the film was added did not nioticeably is truly live-bearing; the new-born measure 29 weaken the feeding responses. Hence, the to 37 centimeters. sharks and skates evidently were able to attack To observe the sharks’ feeding a flounder hiding in the sand without the aid of behavior, we worked from an inflatable rubber visual, chemical, or mechanical cues. raft (Zodiac Mark II) free of any metal under The results of these behavioral tests the waterline. On station in 2.5 to suggested that the sharks and skates 3.0-meter-deep water over a sand patch devoid electrically located the agar-screened prey. of seaweed, we attracted the sharks by This would also explain the all-or-none effect squeezing liquified herring through a long of the polyethylene film, which offered Tygon tube that ran from the raft to the bottom an extremely high electrical resistamce, of the sea. The Tygon chumming tube was whereas the agar layer did not distort the attached to a polypropylene line, suspended bioelectric field of the flounder to any extent. from a Styrofoam float and stretched over the To provide direct evidence for the electrical ocean floor between two polyvinyl pipes hypothesis, the presence of the flounder was anchored in low-profile cinder blocks (Figure simulated by passing electrical current 3). Starting after dark, we illuminated the area between two salt-bridge electrodes’ buried in with a loo-watt, battery-operated underwater the sand. After odor motivation, the predators light. To break the water surface, we used a displayed the same characteristic feeding glass-bottom viewing box secured behind the responses to the electrodes (whether or not stern of the raft. covered with agar) as they did to the actual prey (Figure 2e). They dug tenaciously at the Two pairs of agar-filled, salt-bridge source of the field, responding again and again electrodeswere tied to the polypropylene line when coming across the electrodes. These and positioned on the sand, one on either side results proved that the electric sense of sharks of the odor source and 30 centimeters from it. and skates plays an important role in the Mekka underwater plugs with stainless steel animals’ lives. pins and integral cables connected the thin, 30 to 90-centimeter-long Silastic salt-bridge tubes to the electrical equipment set up in the rubber raft. The use of a constant-current Field Observations on Cape Cod Sharks source virtually eliminated the adverse effects Following the studies on captive shalrks, we of polarization at the stainless steel/seawater have sought to verify the results in tests on wild interfaces. From the raft, we could specimens, roaming freely in their natural conveniently vary the strength of the field and habitat. The main problem, however, has been select the pair of electrodes to be energized, approaching the animals without introducing the other pair functioning as the control. The galvanic fields or other perturbations into the applied direct-current dipole moments ranged environment that might interfere with the from 1 to 8 microamperes x 5 centimeters fishes’ normal behavior. Remember that the (dipole current x distance between electrical sensitivity of catfish and sharks first electrodes), roughly corresponding to the revealed itself through the animals’ Iunusual bioelectric fields of small prey at a seawater responses to metallic objects. resistivity of 20.0 to 20.5 ohm.centimeters and 48
f . Figure 3. Field setup for observing the feeding responses of sharks to electrically stimulated prey. Though attracted by fish extract exuding from chumming tube, the shark Mustelus caniszeroes in on electricaldipole fieldin Vineyard Sound. a temperature of 19 to 22 degrees Celsius. attracted the sharks from a distance. Since the After entering the area, the smooth odor of wounded prey lingers long after the dogfish began frantically searching over the animal is gone, the sharks obviously need sand, apparently trying to locate the odor more precise directional cues to locate their source. Both young and mature sharks were prey accurately, enabling them to seize it with observed, sometimes alone, sometimes in one quick move. groups of two to five. Neither the raft nor the Behaviorally, elasmobranchs readily underwater light appeared to bother them. respond to direct-current fields. Most interestingly, when nearing the odor Electrophysiologically, however, the ampullae source, the animals did not bite at the opening of Lorenzini are not true DC receptors, of the chumming tube but from distances up to although they do detect frequencies as low als 25 centimeters turned sharply to the 0.1 hertz. That is, to sense a prey’s DC field, current-electrodes, viciously attacking the elasmobranch fishes have to move relative to electrically simulated prey. After snapping the their prey, or vice versa. Alternatively, they line with their teeth right at the position of the may detect the low-frequency components electrodes, the sharks usually attempted to rip that accompany the prey’s ventilatory and them apart-and one night they succeeded. body movements. Odor-motivated sharks and When the current was switched to the other skates do zero in on DC, as well as pair of electrodes, the animals let go, circled low-frequency dipole fields of biological around for awhile, and attacked again, but at strengths. In their final approach, they aim the electrodes on the other side of the odor directly at their prey, deriving its location frorn source. At the lower current levels, the sharks the spatial configuration of the animal’s kept responding, though from increasingly bioelectric field. shorter distances. The American eels that visited our test These observations convincingly area often nibbled at the opening of the demonstrate that odor-motivated sharks are chumming tube, but theydidnot pay any capable of detecting and taking prey by the attention to the current-passing electrodes. exclusive use of their electric sense, not only The eels’ behavior was particularly noteworthy under well-controlled laboratory conditions, because the fish had been reported to exhibit but also in their electrically more noisy, ocean similar cardiac decelerations when subjected habitat. Of course, other sense organs, in to weak electric fields, as previously observed particular the fish’s lateral-line in elasmobranch fishes. The eels’ responses, mechanoreceptors, also can play an important however, have not been independently part in predation. At short range, the electric confirmed, nor are these fish known to have fields appear to dominate in our experiments specific electroreceptors. In predation at least, over the much vaguer odor cues that initially they evidently are more chemically inclined. 49
Toward the end of the season, we author has endeavored to collect hard conducted comparative tests on the common experimental evidence of the elasmobranchs’ brown bullhead, which is endemic to the magnetic abilities predicted from the animals’ Cape’s freshwater ponds. With small pieces of known electric sense and the theory of beef liver, we lured the fish into shallow water, electromagnetic induction. and offered them direct-current dipole fields Our first, most simple magnetic tests of 0.5 to 4.0 microamperes passed between were performed on the leopard shark, Triakis two salt-bridge electrodes with openings I semifasciata, in outdoor, all-fiberglass pools centimeter apart buried in the mud. Along the on the bluffs of Scripps Institution of banks of the pond, the resistivity of the water Oceanography’s campus at La Jolla, California. measured 19 kiloohm-centimeters at a With the fish swimming steadily along the temperature of 20 degrees Celsius. As with the circumference of their circular habitat, we sharks, the catfish repeatedly dug at the source introduced a local magnetic field by passing an of the field in apparent attempts to devour the electrical current through a small induction electrically simulated prey. coil (20 centimeters in diameter) held external Although we primarily conceived this to the tank. The field was switched on when field work with the undisturbed bioelectric the sharks were at the far side of the pool. sensory world of the sharks in mind, the Then, seconds later, upon completing their lap results of our tests also indicated that attacks and swimming into the imposed field, the on humans and underwater gear may be sharks suddenly veered off to the center of the elicited and guided by electric fields tank, although the coil current did not distort resembling those of regular prey. The human the earth’s ambient magnetic field by more body, especially when the skin is damaged, than 25 percent. creates DC bioelectric fields that sharks in the Next, we noticed that the leopard ocean may detect from distances up to 1 or 2 sharks each morning before dawn gathered in meters. The galvanic fields of metallic objects the northern area of the tank. To eliminate the on the body may be even stronger. In this possibility of visual orientation, we covered connection, we expect that the U.S. Navy’s their pool with a large sheet of black plastic. polyvinyl anti-shark bag, which is suspended We also took all objects out of the water and from an inflatable floatation collar and even rotated the whole tank. Surprisingly, this designed to visually and olfactorily conceal a did not change the sharks’ early-morning mariner in distress, electrically screens the orientation. However, when we roughly person as well. Sidetracking the sharks to neutralized the earth’s ambient magnetic field alternative sources of electricity that by passing electrical current through two large secondarily release a discouraging agent may, induction coils diametrically attached to the under certain circumstances, be another outside of the tank, the animals lost their means of warding off electrically evoked shark positional preference and distributed bites. It should be emphasized here that the randomly. electrical aspects of the animals’ environment Although the outcome of these should be taken into account in all behavioral preliminary tests was consistent with the idea studies on elasmobranchs and, if possible, left of geomagnetic orientation, none were fully undisturbed. conclusive. The avoidance reactions nroved the sharks’ sensitivity to fields of geomagnetic Evidence of Geomagnetic Orientation strength but were not of obvious biological When cruising through the earth’s magnetic significance. The spontaneous orientation was field, marine sharks, skates, and rays induce biologically more interesting, but at that time electric fields that are well within the dynamic we were not technically prepared to reverse range of their highly-sensitive electroreceptor the field in order to verify the magnetic nature system. As the induced voltage gradients of the response. depend on the direction in which the animals In the meantime, the author moved to are heading, they may form the physical basis Massachusetts to pursue his magnetic studies of an electromagnetic compass sense in these at the Woods Hole Oceanographic Institution. fishes. To substantiate the biological feasibility To scale down the technical problems of of this novel orientation mechanism, the controlling the ambient magnetic field, we 50
(4 lb) Figure 4. Magnetic test facility on the Quissett campus of the Woods Hole Oceanographic institution (a). The round stingray Urolophus halleri, swimming toward the east S enclosure to secure food, undernormalandreversed field conditions (b). Norma I Reversed looked for a good experimental animal of coils, each 5 meters in diameter, were smaller size, which one of the students found mounted along the north-south axis to control in the round stingray Urolophus halleri. The the horizontal component of the earth’s round stingray is a hardy, very alert and active magnetic field in the tank (Figure 4a). We elasmobranch of subtropical and tropical seas, chose for our tests a magnetic induction of 0.34 reaching an average span of 25 centimeters. To gauss, corresponding to the horizontal fit our tanks, we selected five specimens component of the Southern California region measuring only 15 to 20 centimeters. from where the animals were taken. The stingrays were tested in a circular, During the 1 to 2-hour training sessions, fiberglass pool, 1.8 meters in diameter, which twelve concealed lights illuminated the ceiling rested on Teflon blocks to insulate it from over the tank to produce an even, low-level ground. The pool was filled with natural light distribution. For the rest of the day, the seawater to a depth of 15 centimeters. Coarse lights were programmed to simulate the sand covered the bottom, and two airlifts sunshift and daily variation in brightness of the maintained a slow, internal circulation. We sky in accordance with the direction of the surrounded the pool with a lighttight, ambient magnetic field. Lights, heaters, and twelve-sided hut devoid of any ferromagnetic coils were DC-powered from distantly located materials. The seawater was kept at 20 degrees voltage and current sources. The wiring was Celsius by regulating the air temperature in the tightly twisted and judiciously installed to hut. Outside the hut, two large Helmholtz prevent unwanted electric and magnetic fields 51
from straying into the animals’ habitat. a follow-up series, we changed the direction of Each morning, two observers ran a the field again on a daily basis, but this time in series of 15 to 20 trials after first turning on the random order. Under this regime, our most lights, shutting off the airlifts, and checking active stingray, without further training, made the direction of the horizontal component of 120 correct choices out of 184 trials (P
You can also read