Sign Language and Spoken Language for Children With Hearing Loss: A Systematic Review - American Academy of Pediatrics
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
Sign Language and Spoken Language for Children With Hearing Loss: A Systematic Review Elizabeth M. Fitzpatrick, PhD,a,b Candyce Hamel, MSc,c Adrienne Stevens, MSc,c,d Misty Pratt, MES,c David Moher, PhD,c,e Suzanne P. Doucet, MEd,f Deirdre Neuss, PhD,b,g Anita Bernstein, MSc,h Eunjung Na, MSca,b CONTEXT: Permanent hearing loss affects 1 to 3 per 1000 children and interferes with typical abstract communication development. Early detection through newborn hearing screening and hearing technology provide most children with the option of spoken language acquisition. However, no consensus exists on optimal interventions for spoken language development. OBJECTIVE: To conduct a systematic review of the effectiveness of early sign and oral language intervention compared with oral language intervention only for children with permanent hearing loss. DATA SOURCES: An a priori protocol was developed. Electronic databases (eg, Medline, Embase, CINAHL) from 1995 to June 2013 and gray literature sources were searched. Studies in English and French were included. STUDY SELECTION: Two reviewers screened potentially relevant articles. DATA EXTRACTION: Outcomes of interest were measures of auditory, vocabulary, language, and speech production skills. All data collection and risk of bias assessments were completed and then verified by a second person. Grades of Recommendation, Assessment, Development, and Evaluation (GRADE) was used to judge the strength of evidence. RESULTS: Eleven cohort studies metinclusion criteria, of which 8 included only children with severe to profound hearing loss with cochlear implants. Language development was the most frequently reported outcome. Other reported outcomes included speech and speech perception. LIMITATIONS: Several measures and metrics were reported across studies, and descriptions of interventions were sometimes unclear. CONCLUSIONS: Very limited, and hence insufficient, high-quality evidence exists to determine whethersign language in combination with oral language is more effective than oral language therapy alone. More research is needed to supplement the evidence base. aFaculty of Health Sciences and eSchool of Epidemiology, Faculty of Medicine, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada; bChildren’s Hospital of Eastern Ontario Research Institute, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada; cCentre for Practice-Changing Research, Ottawa Hospital Research Institute, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada; dTranslational Research in Biomedicine Graduate Program, University of Split School of Medicine, Split, Croatia; fConsultant, Moncton, New Brunswick; gAudiology Clinic, Children’s Hospital of Eastern Ontario, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada; and hVoice for Hearing-Impaired Children, Toronto, Ontario, Canada Dr Fitzpatrick conceptualized the project, finalized the protocol, was involved in all stages of the analysis and interpretation, wrote the first draft of this manuscript, and is the study guarantor; Ms Hamel and Ms Pratt were involved in screening articles and extracted data; Ms Hamel conducted the quality assessment; Ms Hamel, Ms Pratt, Ms Stevens, and Dr Moher provided input into the final manuscript; Ms Stevens contributed to the development of the methods, oversaw the screening, To cite: Fitzpatrick EM, Hamel C, Stevens A, et al. Sign Language and Spoken Language for Children With Hearing Loss: A Systematic Review. Pediatrics. 2016;137(1):e20151974 Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news by guest on September 16, 2021 PEDIATRICS Volume 137, number 1, January 2016:e20151974 REVIEW ARTICLE
Early detection of permanent which might disrupt or delay spoken better spoken language outcomes childhood hearing loss through language acquisition. However, when exposed to early intervention population-based newborn screening another body of research suggests that uses signs to support language has become standard care in much that visual languages such as compared with oral language of the world. Expectations are that American Sign Language (ASL) are intervention without sign language? early intervention through hearing processed in the brain in the same technology will improve spoken manner as spoken languages and language outcomes for children.1,2 are complementary to auditory METHODS Childhood hearing loss is a relatively stimulation, and therefore provide a The protocol and report for this frequent disorder, affecting strong foundation for learning oral review were prepared according 1 to 3 per 1000 live births,3–5 language.18,19 Therefore, adding to Preferred Reporting Items for that disrupts typical language sign language may develop bilingual Systematic Reviews and Meta- acquisition, placing children at risk skills and facilitate transition Analyses for Protocols,20 and for delays in language, literacy, and to spoken language acquisition. the protocol was published21 social development.6–8 There is Anecdotal evidence supports various and registered at PROSPERO strong consensus that specialized intervention options, but there is (Registration #CRD42013005426),22 intervention must be combined little scientifically based consensus. an international register of with early identification to develop This information is essential to (1) systematic review protocols. The communication skills in these guide families in making decisions review was undertaken using an children.1,9 about care for their children in integrated knowledge translation infancy, (2) inform clinicians so that Historically, considerable debate approach involving knowledge- they can tailor treatment plans to about optimal outcomes for children user clinicians from health and achieve the desired outcomes, and with hearing loss have resulted education in the early stages and as (3) inform policy makers so that they in the evolution of a plethora of required, to ensure relevance of the can make optimal investments in intervention methods that constitute project for parents, clinicians, and early intervention services. two broad but distinct philosophies. decision-makers. The oral approach aims to facilitate This review is timely given the spoken language and inclusion Literature Search worldwide proliferation of screening with normal-hearing peers, and programs in the past 10 years. In Ovid Medline In-Process & Other the manual approach focuses on 2008, the US Preventive Services Non-Indexed Citations and Ovid visual communication systems Task Force recommendations Medline (1946 to June week 2, (sign language) and a Deaf culture on newborn hearing screening 2013), Embase (1974 to June 25, identity.10,11 Although there is wide highlighted the need for further 2013), PsycINFO (1806 to June week recognition that various options research to demonstrate the 3 2013), and Cochrane CENTRAL should be available to families, effectiveness of the entire screening- (through May 2013 issue) databases 2 events in the past two decades to-intervention process.2 New were searched using the Ovid (newborn hearing screening and possibilities due to screening interface. CINAHL was searched on cochlear implant technology) have and technology have reignited June 26, 2013, using the EbscoHost made it possible for even children the discussion on best practices interface, and SpeechBITE was with profound deafness to develop for children with hearing loss. searched on June 26, 2013. The spoken language.7,12–14 Epidemiologic Accordingly, the primary purpose search strategy (Appendix 1) data confirm that >90% of children of this research was to examine the was developed in Medline by an with hearing impairment are born to evidence for the effects of various experienced information specialist, parents with normal hearing.15 intervention options for early- peer-reviewed using the Peer Although there is substantial identified children with hearing loss Review of Electronic Strategies evidence that children with hearing when the desired outcome is spoken (PRESS) standard,23 and adapted loss can develop oral language communication. Our interest was for the other databases. The search skills,7,14,16 there is no consensus in whether adding sign language was restricted to the pediatric age about optimal interventions.17 facilitates spoken language, because group,24 and the date was limited to There is a common expectation that of the increased focus on this material published in 1995 or later. children receiving oral language outcome since the advent of cochlear A broad methodological filter was intervention should develop better implantation. Specifically, the review applied, incorporating published language skills than those who addressed the following question: filters for controlled trials and are also exposed to sign language, Do children with hearing loss have other study designs.25,26 In addition, Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news by guest on September 16, 2021 2 FITZPATRICK et al
gray literature websites (A.G. Bell Relevancy of papers was assessed screened all potentially relevant Association for the Deaf, National based on the components of the full-text articles. Disagreements Acoustics Laboratory, National intervention described and not on were resolved by consultation and Health Services UK Newborn Hearing the program label. consensus with a third researcher Screening Program, CADTH Gray and knowledge-user clinicians as The comparator of interest was oral Matters Checklist), conference needed. language intervention without sign proceedings (Newborn Hearing language. Screening Conference Abstracts 2010 Data Extraction and 2012), two journals (Ear and Outcomes A study-specific data extraction form, Hearing and Journal of Deaf Studies finalized with input from knowledge- Primary outcomes included all and Deaf Education), and six books27– user clinicians, was developed in 33 were searched. measures of spoken language including auditory, receptive, and Distiller SR to extract predetermined expressive language skills (eg, data variables. Items extracted Eligibility Criteria vocabulary), speech production, and included (1) study characteristics Study Designs (citation, year, setting, country, intelligibility. These outcomes were The following study designs were selected as clinically relevant based language, publication status, and included: randomized controlled on a large body of literature.7,33,34 source of funding or other potential trials; controlled clinical trials and Secondary outcomes included conflict of interest), (2) study design, other quasi-experimental designs electrophysiologic outcomes (eg, (3) population characteristics (eg, that include comparator groups; and cortical responses). In addition, any sample size, gender, ethnicity, prospective and retrospective cohort adverse outcomes (eg, parent stress) etiology, age, severity of hearing studies. were noted. loss, hearing technology, and time with hearing technology), (4) Population Time Frame details (type) of intervention, (5) Studies were eligible for inclusion if We included studies from 1995 details of comparison groups, and they included children (1) with early- onward to capture studies after (7) outcome data. One researcher onset (before age 3 years) hearing wide implementation of cochlear extracted data and a second loss of any severity using hearing implantation and newborn hearing researcher independently verified aids and/or cochlear implants; (2) screening. Earlier studies were data. Discrepant and unclear data receiving early intervention (age excluded, as previous generations of were resolved through consensus ≤3 years); and (3)
bias, study design, confounders, Evaluation (GRADE) working group38 in tabular and narrative form. A blinding, data collection methods, to rate the quality of the overall meta-analysis was not possible due withdrawals and dropouts, integrity body of evidence for each outcome. to heterogeneity in designs, methods, of intervention, and study analysis. Primary outcomes were assessed and outcome metrics. A narrative One researcher conducted the quality across the domains of risk of bias, synthesis and table summary of assessment after 2 studies were consistency, directness, precision, data were therefore completed. We first assessed by 2 researchers, and and publication bias. GRADE results present continuous outcomes as the second verified the remaining in a rating of the quality of the body mean differences (MDs) with 95% assessments. A third researcher of evidence as high (very confident confidence intervals, where possible. with content expertise rechecked all that true effect is close to the effect Planned subgroup analyses to ratings and discussed any differences estimate), moderate (moderately examine variables related to hearing to reach consensus. confident), low (limited confidence), loss severity, age of identification, or very low (little confidence). and hearing technologies could not Grading Strength of Evidence be conducted owing to the lack of Data Synthesis studies. We applied methodology developed by the Grades of Recommendation, We summarized the characteristics of Assessment, Development, and study populations and interventions RESULTS We identified 432 records through database searching and 113 records through other sources, of which 470 were retained for screening after duplicates were removed. Figure 1 outlines the flow of records through the screening process. We assessed 352 full-text documents. As shown, the primary reasons for exclusion were (1) not a study design (58%) and (2) not an intervention of interest (32%). Eleven unique studies were included. Study Characteristics Table 1 summarizes the study characteristics. All were prospective cohort studies published between 1999 and 2013, 4 since 2004. The majority (n = 8) were conducted in the United States,32,39–45 and one each was conducted in Spain,46 the UK,47 and Denmark.48 Three studies44,46,47 were conducted specifically to examine whether signs added to spoken language intervention (sign + oral) improved outcomes. The remaining studies evaluated hearing technology or provided data about language intervention as 1 of several predictor variables in the analysis. Participants and Interventions Study size for reported outcomes FIGURE 1 ranged from 13 to 90 participants. Study selection. It is not possible to rule out overlap Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news by guest on September 16, 2021 4 FITZPATRICK et al
TABLE 1 Characteristics of Included Studies Study, Year, Design Main Study Objective and Participants, Age, Hearing Loss, Other Intervention Comparator Study Quality Country Setting Characteristics Miyamoto et al, Cohort To examine speech n = 14 for review, NR by group; CI; only TC: combined use OC Weak 1999, US39 perception, speech children implanted
TABLE 1 Continued Study, Year, Design Main Study Objective and Participants, Age, Hearing Loss, Other Intervention Comparator Study Quality Country Setting Characteristics Nicholas and Cohort To examine whether n = 76, 38 SC, 38 OC (OC from SC: emphasizes OC: emphasizes Moderate Geers, 2003, deaf children who have previously published study); age: the combination of the auditory signal US44 the addition of signs 18–54 mo; age tested: 18–54 mo; group spoken language with or without show an early linguistic scores reported at 18–30 mo (n = 16 with corresponding visual cues from advantage and greater per intervention group) and 36–54 mo manual signs speaker’s lips communicative function (n = 22 per intervention group); HL: maturity than those with better-ear PTA threshold ≥80 dBHL, spoken language input 103.21 vs 104.41 dBHL; HA: 27 vs 30; CI: 7 only; to examine whether vs 5; tactile aid: 0 vs 3; no device: 4 vs 0; deaf children who age onset: birth (n = 34 vs 33) and 1–18 receive speech and sign mo (n = 4 vs 5); age diagnosis: 10.42 vs lag behind children in OC 12.03 mo; maternal education: 14.14 vs in their use of speech in 14.71 y; duration CI: 7.5 (n = 7) vs. 3.8 preschool mo (n = 5) Connor and Cohort To examine the effects n = 91, 43 TC, 48 OC; 49.45% male; CI; TC: use of sign OC: use of spoken Weak Zwolan, 2004, of multiple variables age: 11.16 ± 2.83 vs. 10.81 ± 2.53 y (for language in language only US45 (SDT, age, SES, reading evaluation); age pre–post combination with communication method, vocabulary tests: NR; HL: >80 dBHL; age spoken language; pre- and postimplant onset: 0.12 vs 0.19 y; age CI: 7.40 vs 6.23 sign systems based vocabulary) on reading y; duration CI: 3.82 ± 1.56 vs. 4.63 ± 2.51 on English, including comprehensionb y; HL-SDT: 55.22 vs 51.72 Signed English and (pre- and postimplant Signing Exact English vocabulary scores were reported by group) Jiménez et al, Cohort To compare speech/ n = 18, NR by group; 61.11% male; CI; Bilingual: spoken + Monolingual: Moderate 2009, Spain46 language development age: mean 6.25 y, range 4 y 3 mo to sign language spoken language after CI in children 8 y; HL: profound; groups similar for educated using spoken gender, age (avg 6.25 y), age diagnosis language versus spoken (avg 10 mo), age CI (avg 3.2 y), duration and sign language CI (avg 3.1 y) (P > .05 for all) Nittrouer, 2010, Cohort To explain the n = 118, 44 sign support, 74 spoken Spoken + sign Spoken language Moderate US32 contributions of various language; 55% male; early-identified support; 28 of 44 independent measures (6 parents used ASL and to developmental mo) n = 15 vs 26; age: 12–48 mo; tested 16 a manually coded outcomes for children at 6-mo intervals; n varied per interval: English system; with hearing loss, 1 of 16 (12 mo) to 90 (48 mo); HL: moderate parents reported which was sign support to profound; 38 HA, 80 CI by end of using signs
of participants in some studies; for therapy (Table 2). Most studies (n = authors did not report scores example, 339,42,43 from 1999 to 2002 10) reported multiple outcomes of separately by group but concluded included a subset of children from 1 interest, whereas 1 study41 reported that not using total communication US cochlear implant program. Eight speech perception only (Table 3). As was 1 factor associated with greater studies included only children with shown in Table 2, different metrics odds of age-equivalent receptive severe to profound deafness who had (eg, standard score, raw score, age vocabulary. received cochlear implants. The 3 equivalency, language quotient) were remaining studies included a mix of reported in different studies. Studies Three studies also reported results children with respect to technology also reported variations in test for expressive vocabulary. The use.32,44,47 As shown in Table 1, administration. In 1 study, Spanish Danish study48 examined expressive authors used various descriptions test versions (which authors reported vocabulary using a Danish norm– and terms to describe both the as validated) were administered.46 referenced test but found no effect of intervention (eg, sign, bilingual) and Danish test adaptations48 were intervention mode. The US preschool comparator (oral, spoken language) used with American norms in study32 found that expressive groups. another study except for the vocabulary results favored oral expressive vocabulary test, which intervention only for late-identified Quality Assessment was developed and standardized in children (>12 months) and showed Study quality and scores are shown Danish. As noted in Table 1, tests no significant effects of intervention in Table 1. Studies were rated as were administered in the child’s for early-identified children (
8 TABLE 2 Summary of Evidence and GRADE Ratings Outcome Measure, Test, and Length of Follow-Up Sign + Oral vs Oral Onlyb Studies (Participants) Effect Estimatec Statistical Information Quality of Evidencee or Test Agea Provided by Authord Language PPVT39,42,46,48 Very low Unknown39,42f,g In 2 studies, mean LQ 0.30–0.72 vs 2 (28) MD (no CI): −0.02 to 0.10 NS for both studies 0.32–0.62 Mean 5.4 y46h Mean standard score 66.9 vs 73.6 1 (18) MD (no CI): −6.7 NS (P > .05) Mean 3.2 y48h (range 6 to >36 mo) Scores NR by group; reported as 1 (68) OR estimated to be P = .012 (favors oral) odds of having age-equivalent infinity by authors vocabulary Viborgmaterialet48 (Danish vocabulary) Very low Mean 3.2 yh (range 6 to >36 mo) Scores NR by group; reported as 1 (49) OR not reported NS odds of having age-equivalent vocabulary Picture Vocabulary Test/EOWPVT45i Very low Unknown45f,g Pre- and postimplant scores 1 (91) MD (no CI): 7.48 to 13.46 NS preimplant (P > .05); 66.54–74.60 vs 53.08–67.12 postimplant NR EOWPVT32j Very low 3.5 y (early id), 2–3 y (late id)32k; test age: Raw score: 32.89 ± 8.76 vs 32.18 1 (early: 52; late: 38) MD-early (95% confidence Early id: NS; late id: P = .002 48 mo ± 10.81 (early id); 21.50 ± 13.40 vs interval): 0.71 (−4.72 to (favors oral) 32.12 ± 7.86 (late id) 6.14); MD-late: −10.62 (−18.78 to −2.45) Reynell-R39,40,42,43,48 Very low Unknown39,42,43g Mean LQ 0.61–0.89 vs 0.64–0.91 3 (69) MD (no CI): −0.02 to NS for all studies −0.03 Unknown40g Mean 8- vs 9-mo increase in 1 (23) MD (no CI): −1-mo NS language age (no CIs) language age Mean 3.2 y, unknown by group48 Scores NR by group; odds of age- 1 (71) OR estimated to be P = .013 (favors oral) equivalent language infinity by authors Reynell-E39,40,42,43 Very low Unknown39,42,43g Mean LQ 0.51–0.68 vs 0.69–0.93 3 (69) MD (no CI): −0.02 to 1 NS41; 1 NR42; 1 significant38 −0.26 Unknown40g Mean 7- vs 10-mo increase in 1 (23) MD (no CI): −3-mo NS language age language age ITPA (Spanish) Auditory Reception46 Very low 5.4 y Mean standard score 34.6 vs 56.0 1 (18) MD (no CI) −21.4 Significant Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news by guest on September 16, 2021 ITPA (Spanish) Auditory Association46 Very low 5.4 y Mean standard score 37.8 vs, 71.6 1 (18) MD (no CI) −33.8 Significant ITPA (Spanish) Auditory sequential memory46 Very low 5.4 y Mean standard score 44.8 vs 59.0 1 (18) MD (no CI)−14.2 NS ITPA (Spanish) Verbal Expression46 Very low 5.4 y Mean standard score 52.5 vs 39.2 1 (18) MD (no CI 13.3 Significant ITPA (Spanish) Grammatical closure46 Very low 5.4 y Mean standard score 39.2 vs 71.3 1 (18) MD (no CI)−32.1 Significant ICAP (Spanish) Social communication46 Very low 5.4 y Mean standard score 38.75 vs 53.0 1 (18) MD (no CI): −14.25 NS Auditory Comprehension-PLS-432j Very low FITZPATRICK et al
TABLE 2 Continued Outcome Measure, Test, and Length of Follow-Up Sign + Oral vs Oral Onlyb Studies (Participants) Effect Estimatec Statistical Information Quality of Evidencee or Test Agea Provided by Authord 3.5 y (early id), 2–3 y (late id)k; test age: Mean raw score: 45.39 ± 8.82 vs 1 (early: 52; late: 38) MD early id (95% Early id: NS,j P = .011 (favors 48 mo 42.59 ± 10.02 (early id); 35.92 ± confidence interval): oral) 12.80 vs 43.73 ± 8.59 (late id) −2.80 (−5.48 to 5.08); MD late id: −7.81 (−15.76 to 0.14) Language Comprehension–SIB- R32j Very low 3.5 y (early id), 2–3 y (late id)k; test age: Mean raw score: 19.78 ± 2.78 vs 1 (early: 52; late: 38) MD early id (95% Early id: NS; late id: NS 48 mo 19.15 ± 3.46 (early id); 19.83 ± 4.71 confidence interval): 0.63 vs 18.12 ± 3.43 (late id) (−1.10 to 2.36); MD late id: 1.71 (−1.26 to 4.68) Language Expression - SIB-R32j Very low PEDIATRICS Volume 137, number 1, January 2016 3.5 y (early id), 2–3 y (late id)k; test age: Mean raw score: 28.56 ± 1.89 vs 1(early: 52; late: 38) MD early id (95% Early id: NS; late id: P = .012 48 mo 27.03 ± 3.59 (early id); 23.42 ± 6.80 confidence interval): 1.53 (favors oral) vs 26.88 ± 3.19 (late id) (0.04 to 3.01); MD late id: −3.46 (−7.49 to 0.57) Number of spoken words-LDS32j Very low 3.5 y - early-id; 2-3 y - late-id groupk Test Mean: 185.79 ± 103.19 vs 157.08 ± 1 (early: 51 late: 16) MD early id (95% Early id: similar number of age: 30 mo 104.56 (early id); 38.75 ± 29.64 vs confidence interval): words; late id: sign had fewer 122.25 ± 69.34 (late id) 28.71 (−34.98 to 92.40); words (author report) MD late id: −83.5 (−132.31 to −34.68) BLADES47 Very low Unknown Delay 6–24 vs 12–24 mo 1 (13) See next column No difference between groups (author report) Number of different words (sign or spoken)44 Very low Rangel 7.6–19.6 mo (sign + oral), 6–18 mo Mean 13.88 ± 17.55 vs 2.65 ± 5.87 1 (76) MD (95% confidence NS (oral); test age 18–30 mo words interval): 11.23 (5.35 to 17.11) Rangel 25.6–43.6 (sign + oral), 24–42 mo Mean 45.50 ± 29.08 vs 38.90 ± 3.96 1 (76) MD (95% confidence NS (oral); test age: 36–54 mo words interval): 6.60 (−2.73 to 15.93) Words per utterance (sign or spoken)44 Very low Test age 18–30 mo Mean 0.99 ± 0.44 vs 0.31 ± 0.5 1 (76) MD (95% confidence Significant words interval): 0.68 (0.47 to Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news by guest on September 16, 2021 0.89) Test age 36–54 mo Mean 1.25 ± 0.18 vs 1.33 ± 0.36 1 (76) MD (95% confidence NS words interval): −0.08 (−0.21 to 0.05) Total number of spoken words44 Very low Test age 18–30 mo Mean 3.56 ± 5.19 vs 5.47 ± 14.1 1 (76) MD (95% confidence NS words interval): −1.91 (−6.69 to 2.87) Test age 36–54 mo Mean 23.09 ± 33.03 vs 117.88 ± 1 (76) MD (95% confidence NS 207.75 words interval): −94.79 (−161.67 to −27.91) 9
10 TABLE 2 Continued Outcome Measure, Test, and Length of Follow-Up Sign + Oral vs Oral Onlyb Studies (Participants) Effect Estimatec Statistical Information Quality of Evidencee or Test Agea Provided by Authord Number of different words spoken44 Very low Test age 18–30 mo Mean 2.62 ± 3.59 vs 1.94 ± 4.29 1 (76) MD (95% confidence NS words interval): 0.68 (−1.10 to 2.46) Test age 36–54 mo Mean 12.91 ± 17.38 vs 38.67 ± 44 1 (76) MD (95% confidence Significant words interval): −25.76 (−40.80 to −10.72) PIHF44 Very low Test age 18–30 mo Mean 0.23 ± 0.2 vs 0.04 ± 0.12 1 (76) MD (95% confidence Significant words interval): 0.19 (0.12 to 0.21) Test age 36–54 mo Mean 0.46 ± 0.17 vs mean 0.34 ± 1 (76) MD (95% confidence Significant 0.21 words interval): 0.12 (0.03 to 0.21) ICA44 Very low Test age 18–30 mo Mean 41.56 ± 35.71 vs 36.76 ± 1 (76) MD (95% confidence NS 35.68 words interval): 4.80 (−11.27 to 20.87) Test age 36–54 mo Mean 121 ± 75.25 vs 131.67 ± 70.79 1 (76) MD (95% confidence NS words interval): −10.67 (−43.52 to 22.18) Speech BIT39 Very low Unknown 10.57% vs 30.62% correct words 1 (14) MD (no CI) −20.05 Significant identified by listener CSIM32 Very low 3.5 y (early id), 2–3 y (late id)k; test age: Mean: 58% ± 19% vs 57% ± 17% 1 (early: 49 late: 36) MD early id (95% Early id: NS; late id: P < .001 48 mo (early id); 46% ± 25% vs 59 ± 16% confidence interval): 1.0 (favors oral) (late id) words recognized by (−9.78 to 11.78); MD late listener id: −13.0 (−29.05 to 3.05) Induced Phonological Register (Spanish)46 Very low 5.4 y Mean standard score 30 vs 60 1 (18) MD (no CI): −30 Significant (P < .05) (favors oral) Speech Perception Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news by guest on September 16, 2021 Mr. Potato Head-Words39,41–43 Very low Unknown Mean % correct 24%–56% vs 4 (105) MD (no CI): −9 to −39% 1 NS40; 1 NR42; 2 significant38,41 33%–95% (no CI) Mr. Potato Head – Sentences39 Very low Unknown Percent correct 39.76% vs 69.88% 1 (14) MD (no CI): −30.12% Significant ESP-Low Verbal41 Very low Unknown Percent correct 82% vs 90% 1 (36) MD (no CI): −8% Significant GAEL-P Words recognized39,42 Very low Unknown Percent correct 46%–87.38% vs 2 (28) MD (no CI): −7% to −9.3% Significant38,41 53%–96.68% GASP-W41 Very low FITZPATRICK et al
TABLE 2 Continued Outcome Measure, Test, and Length of Follow-Up Sign + Oral vs Oral Onlyb Studies (Participants) Effect Estimatec Statistical Information Quality of Evidencee or Test Agea Provided by Authord Unknown Percent correct 50% vs 63% 1 (36) MD (no CI): −13% Significant PBK-Words41 Very low Unknown Percent correct 20% vs 24% 1 (36) MD (no CI): −4% Significant PBK-Phonemes41 Very low Unknown Percent correct 29% vs 50% 1 (36) MD (no CI): −21% Significant BIT, Beginner’s Intelligibility Test; BLADES, Bristol Language Developmental Scales; CI, confidence interval; CSIM, Children’s Speech Intelligibility Measure; EOWPVT, Expressive One Word Picture Vocabulary Test; ESP, Early Speech Perception; GAEL, Grammatical Analysis of Elicited Language (used as closed-set speech perception measure); GASP, Glendonald Auditory Screening Procedure; ICA, intentionally communicative act; ICAP, Inventory for Client and Agency Planning; ITPA, Illinois Test of Psycho-linguistic Abilities; LDS, Language Development Survey; id, identified; LQ, language quotient; MD, mean difference; NR, not reported; NS, not statistically significant; OR, odds ratio; PBK, Phonetically Balanced Kindergarten Test; PIHF, Proportion of Informative or Heuristice Functions; PLS, Preschool Language Scale; PPVT, Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test; SIB-R, Scales of Independent Behavior-Revised a Follow-up measured from time of intervention. b Outcomes data; higher score = better performance. PEDIATRICS Volume 137, number 1, January 2016 c Minus sign (−) indicates higher score in oral group. d No meta-analysis due to insufficient data and different metrics reported; statistics reported are those from authors. e Based on GRADE ratings. f Length of follow-up (from diagnosis of hearing loss) with language intervention of interest is unknown. Studies39–43,45 report duration of cochlear implant use and age at assessment (details in Table 1). g Authors stated that language measures were adapted and administered in child’s preferred communication modality. signed and spoken responses accepted,45 or instructions given nonverbally and certain vocabulary considered less likely to be known by children using sign was removed.32 h Calculated based on information provided in article. i Picture Vocabulary Test (PVT) of the Woodcock Johnson Test of Cognitive Ability or EOWPVT administered preimplant; PVT postimplant. j Study reports 25 different language scores at 6-mo test intervals from 12 to 48 mo; table includes 6 clinically relevant test measures that reflect overall findings at last test age (5 standardized language measures and 1 speech intelligibility measure). Remaining results (not shown in table) relate to specific aspects of language from language sample analysis (e.g., number of responses, number of pronouns): for early-identified children, 18 results showed no statistically significant differences and 1 favored oral intervention; for late-identified children, 14 results favored oral intervention. Statistical information provided is based on authors’ report of simple effects analysis (signs, prosthesis, test age) for early- and late- identified hearing loss. k Calculated based on early group identified at 12 mo of age up to 24 mo of age. Results for last test age reported. l Calculated from age of diagnosis and test age range; applies for all measures for study.44 test. Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news by guest on September 16, 2021 comprehension only. statistical significance. language intervention to be subtest only. One additional Various subtest scores were version of the Illinois Test of study from the UK47 (n = 13) for 2 standardized measures preschool study32 consistently Three additional studies32,46,47 language intervention reached of intervention compared with measures, all yielding different American norms of the Reynell using a Danish adaptation with also reported in the study of 18 2002. According to the authors, comprehension measure but no The same 4 US cochlear implant intervention for early-identified for late-identified children, with there were no significant effects children46 that used the Spanish quotients, and 1 study, language reported no significant effects of children at 48 months. However, Psycholinguistic Abilities (ITPA). results. As shown in Table 1, a US results favoring oral intervention received cochlear implants before for expressive language measured The authors reported significantly receptive and expressive language receptive subtest reported spoken language–only group. Mean scores higher standard scores in auditory reported language outcomes using the study produced mixed findings Only 1 study39 that favored spoken did not report expressive language. grammatical closure for the spoken reception, auditory association, and statistically significant, for language age scores). Only the Danish study48 on another language comprehension were significantly higher for the sign by the Reynell test. The Danish study studies also reported similar findings + oral group on the verbal expression significant difference in interventions controls (3 studies reported language (expressive language) and 1 language provided results for a variety of other 11
developmental scales to measure development. A study by Nicholas sign + oral group at 18 to 30 months; expressive language in the areas of and Geers44 reported findings for 2 number of different words spoken, pragmatics, semantics, and syntax. age groups, 18 to 30 months and 36 which favored the oral group at 36 The authors reported delays ranging to 54 months, for 38 children who to 54 months; and the Proportion of from 6 to 24 months in both groups received sign + oral intervention Informative or Heuristic Functions, and concluded that intervention did and compared results to a historical which favored sign + oral in both age not influence outcomes. cohort of 38 children in oral language groups. Of the 19 natural language intervention. As shown in Table 2, results reported in the Nittrouer32 Language Results: Natural Language of 12 different results, statistically study, 18 showed no statistically Sample Analysis significant differences were reported significant difference for early- Finally, 2 studies reported scores for only for words per utterance (sign identified children (18 of 51 had sign various aspects of communication or spoken words), which favored the + oral), whereas 1 measure (number TABLE 3 Overview of Outcome Measures for All Included Studies Measure/tool Study, reference no. Sign + Oral Oral 39 40 41 42 43 47 44 45 46 32 48 PPVT1 • • • • 1 Viborgmaterialeta • PVT/EOWPVT • • 1b Reynell Receptive • • • • • 1 Reynell Expressive • • • • 1 ITPAc Auditory Reception • 1 ITPA Auditory Association • 1 ITPA Auditory Sequential Memory • ITPA Verbal Expression • 1 ITPA Grammatical Closure • 1 ICAP • PLS-4 Auditory Comprehension • 1b Language Comprehension SIB-R • Language Comprehension SIB-R • 1b BLADES Expressive • Number of different wordsd • • Words per utteranced • 1 Total number of spoken wordsd • Number of different words spokend • 1 PIHFd • 2 Intentionally communicative actsd • Language-11 communication actse • 7f Language–5 grammar/syntaxe • 5b Language-3 vocalizationse • 2b Beginner’s Intelligibility Test • 1 CSIM • 1b Induced Phonological Register • 1 Mr. Potato Head—Words • • • • 2 Mr. Potato Head—Sentences • 1 ESP-Low Verbal • 1 GAEL-P Wordsg • • 2 GASP—Words • 1 PBK—Words • 1 PBK—Phonemes • 1 Total 4 36 Numbers in boxes refer to number of studies reporting statistical significance for sign + oral or oral group. BLADES, Bristol Language Developmental Scales; CSIM, Children’s Speech Intelligibility Measure; ESP, Early Speech Perception; EOWPVT, Expressive One Word Picture Vocabulary Test; GAEL-P, Grammatical Analysis of Expressive Language: Presentence Level; GASP, Glendonald Auditory Screening Procedure; ICAP, Inventory for Client and Agency Planning; Lang, language; PBK, Phonetically Balanced Kindergarten Test; PLS-4, Preschool Language Scale; PVT, Picture Vocabulary Test of the Woodcock Johnson Test of Cognitive Ability; SIB-R, Scales of Independent Behavior-Revised. a Danish version of test. b Nittrouer32 reported significant difference for late-identified sign group only; results for early-identified group were not significant. c Spanish version of ITPA for all subtests. d Results for 2 age groups (18–30 mo and 36–54 mo for each measure in this study43). e Various aspects of communication/language reported from analyses of language samples (e.g., number of responses, total words, number of pronouns, jargon). f Both early- and late-identified groups showed significant difference for 1 communication act (number of inquiries); 6 others showed significance for late-identified group only. g GAEL-P administered as closed-set speech perception test. Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news by guest on September 16, 2021 12 FITZPATRICK et al
of inquiries made by child) favored significant results for sign + oral and expressive language outcomes. oral intervention. However, results intervention and 36 for oral language Three studies addressing speech were markedly different for the late- only (9 related to speech perception, significantly favored oral language identified children (12 of 37 had sign 17 for late-identified group in US therapy only (except 1 early- + oral), in whom 14 of 19 results preschool study32). identified group32). Closed-set speech favored oral intervention. perception studies mostly favored spoken language only, whereas We retrieved only 1 study that DISCUSSION open-set speech perception studies addressed other aspects of language statistically favored spoken language functioning considered relevant to There continue to be questions about but with small clinical significance. this review. Using the Inventory for the advantages or disadvantages The majority of studies were Client and Agency Planning, Jiménez of adding sign language to methodologically weak, with a few et al46 reported no significant interventions focused on spoken scoring a moderate rating. However, differences between groups in social language development in children when rated according to GRADE communication skills. with hearing loss. This review found criteria, the overall quality of the that few studies have systematically Speech Results evidence per outcome is very low. addressed the issue. Eight of the Three studies addressed aspects 11 studies in this review included According to GRADE, cohort studies of speech production, 2 related to only children with severe to are initially considered as low quality speech intelligibility32,39 and 1 to profound deafness who were using before applying the assessment phonological production.46 In 2 cochlear implants. This is likely criteria, as they are at a greater studies,39,46 scores were significantly because many children received risk of bias, generally. The risk of better for the oral intervention preimplant intervention with a bias for the body of evidence for groups based on author reports, signing component in addition outcomes was deemed to have whereas in the US preschool study,32 to spoken language, owing to the serious limitations. The overall scores were significantly better profound nature of hearing loss, quality of evidence for each outcome for oral intervention in the late- particularly in the 1990s when we assessed was limited because of identified but not early-identified cochlear implantation was a new the small number of included studies, children. option. Evidence to address the issue which resulted in low ratings for the for children with less than profound precision of results and an inability Speech Perception degrees of hearing loss is severely to assess for consistency. There was Four cochlear implant studies39,41–43 lacking, as this review retrieved only also considerable variation in the (1999 to 2002) specifically 3 studies32,44,47 that included children measurements of outcomes, which investigated speech perception and with hearing loss using hearing aids. made comparison across studies included 3 closed-set measures difficult. Furthermore, the full Measures of language development, (response selected from defined process of care from time of diagnosis including receptive vocabulary word set) and 2 open-set measures is unknown, as is whether crossover and receptive and expressive (no word set available). All scores interventions occurred. Therefore, language skills, contributed the most were significantly better for the oral additional studies in contemporary information to this review. When group, with the exception of 1 closed- cohorts of children are needed to comparing sign and oral language set test (Mr. Potato Head, words) in supplement the evidence base. Most intervention with oral language 141 (n = 36) of the 4 studies, which studies were conducted before intervention alone, individual was not statistically significant. 2002 with children with severe to studies showed no differences However, for open-set monosyllabic profound deafness and therefore may between groups for receptive and words, the differences were clinically not reflect current practice, in which expressive vocabulary outcomes, small despite statistical significance children with congenital deafness with the exception of 1 study’s32 (eg, 20% vs 24%). No speech are likely to be identified and receive results that favored oral intervention recognition measures were reported early intervention well before 1 year only for late-identified children for children with hearing aids. of age. Accordingly, new information (n = 12 in sign group). However, about the effects of intervention Table 3 provides an overview of a variety of measures and metrics in children across the spectrum measures extracted in the included were used across studies that limit of hearing loss severity has the studies and the interventions that the generalizability of the evidence. potential to change the conclusions. showed statistically significant Overall, mixed results of statistical differences. Of 61 total test scores significance and no differences were Since completing our review, we reported, only 4 showed statistically observed in studies for receptive are aware of 1 recent retrospective Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news by guest on September 16, 2021 PEDIATRICS Volume 137, number 1, January 2016 13
cohort study conducted in Australia that include comparator groups outcomes and reporting metrics with young children who used since previous reviews.51,52 Despite would be very useful in comparing cochlear implants that compared a strong conclusions based on expert future study results. sign and oral language intervention opinion and intervention results There is insufficient information to group (n = 10) with 2 oral without comparator groups, this guide parents about contemporary intervention groups (auditory-oral, n review indicates that very limited, cohorts of children who have = 14, and auditory-verbal, n = 18).50 and hence insufficient, evidence benefited from early detection and No significant differences in receptive exists to determine whether adding early access to hearing technology. vocabulary, auditory comprehension, sign language to spoken language is The field would gain from carefully or expressive language were found more effective than spoken language controlled prospective studies with across the 3 groups after controlling intervention alone to foster oral today’s children. for confounders such as age of language acquisition. hearing loss management and family involvement in the intervention APPENDIX 1: MEDLINE SEARCH program. These results are aligned CONCLUSIONS STRATEGY with the overall findings of our MEDLINE review and would not change the This review showed that there 1. exp Hearing Loss/ conclusions. are important gaps in knowledge concerning the effectiveness of sign 2. (hearing adj (loss or impair$ or Limitations and oral language intervention, disorder*)).tw. Despite a comprehensive search compared with oral language 3. deaf$.tw. by an information specialist, it is intervention only, for children with possible that some studies were 4. (prelingual$ or pre-lingual$).tw. hearing loss when spoken language not included owing to the range is the intended outcome. To date, 5. (sensorineural$ or sensori- of descriptions of intervention there is no evidence that adding neural$).tw. methods. During study selection, sign language facilitates spoken 6. congenital.mp. program descriptions were language acquisition. However, this sometimes difficult to discern. For 7. or/1-6 review also found no conclusive example, it was difficult to know evidence that adding sign language 8. auditory verbal.tw. whether interventions included the interferes with spoken language 9. ((speech or auditory) adj2 same level of emphasis on spoken development. Overall, the literature feedback).tw. language when sign language was related to intervention methods added. Although we contacted 10. cued speech.tw. for children with hearing loss lacks authors for clarity of interventions properly designed cohort studies 11. (listen* and (spoken or speak*)). and outcomes, we did not contact of today’s generation of children. tw. them for additional sample It will be important to conduct 12. oral approach$.tw. characteristics or statistical data. A more research to supplement this further important limitation is that 8 evidence base and to update this 13. aural.tw. of the 11 studies, although meeting review as those studies become 14. Lipreading/ inclusion criteria, were not designed available. Given the current context 15. (lipread$ or lip read$ or to directly examine the central of widespread neonatal screening, speechread$ or speech read$). question of this review. data can be collected prospectively tw. from diagnosis, allowing for more Implications for Practice 16. or/8-15 accurate data regarding onset, Although the question of intervention severity, and changes in hearing loss, 17. Manual Communication/ or Sign effectiveness and specifically the as well as specific characteristics and Language/ contribution of sign language changes related to intervention, than 18. (sign$ language or sign$ english). combined with oral language is was possible for study participants tw. longstanding, there is strikingly in this review. Interventions should limited new research to guide be reported with specific detail, and 19. ASL.tw. clinical or policy decision-making. if definitions can be developed and 20. visual language.tw. The question of the contribution of adopted on an international level, 21. (baby sign or infant sign).tw. sign language to the development of comparisons of similar programs spoken language remains elusive, and would be more feasible. In addition, 22. or/17-21 there has been little growth in studies agreement on a common set of core 23. Communication Methods, Total/ Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news by guest on September 16, 2021 14 FITZPATRICK et al
24. total communication.tw. 39. ((consecutive or clinical) adj2 45. 42 and (child* or adolescent or 25. simultaneous communication.tw. case$).tw. infan*).mp. 26. (multilingual or multi-lingual). 40. ((control$ or intervention or 46. 44 or 45 tw. evaluation or comparative or effectiveness or evaluation or 47. limit 46 to yr=”1985 -Current” 27. (bicultural or bi-cultural).tw. feasibility) adj3 (trial or studies 28. (bilingual or bi-lingual).tw. or study or program or design)). 29. bi bi.tw. tw. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 30. or/23-29 41. or/31-40 We thank Margaret Sampson, MLIS, 31. exp clinical trial/ 42. 7 and ((16 and 22) or 30) and 41 PhD, AHIP (Children’s Hospital of 43. 42 and ((Infan* or newborn* Eastern Ontario), for developing the 32. clinical trial.pt. or randomized. electronic search strategies and Janet ti,ab. or placebo.ti,ab. or or new-born* or perinat* or neonat* or baby or baby* or Joyce, MLIS, for peer review of the randomly.ti,ab. or trial.ti,ab. or MEDLINE search strategy. We also groups.ti,ab. babies or toddler* or minors or minors* or boy or boys or thank Pauline Quach for assistance 33. ((control$ or clinical or boyfriend or boyhood or girl* with the quality assessment. comparative$) adj2 (trial$ or or kid or kids or child or child* stud$)).mp. or children* or schoolchild* ABBREVIATIONS 34. exp Epidemiologic studies/ or schoolchild).mp. or school or Case-control studies/ or child.ti,ab. or school child*.ti,ab. ASL: American Sign Language Retrospective studies/ or Cohort or (adolescen* or juvenil* or GRADE: Grades of studies/ or Longitudinal studies/ youth* or teen* or under*age* Recommendation, or Cross-sectional studies/ or pubescen*).mp. or exp Assessment, pediatrics/ or (pediatric* or Development, and 35. between group design$.mp. paediatric* or peadiatric*).mp. Evaluation 36. control group$.mp. or school.ti,ab. or school*.ti,ab. or ITPA: Illinois Test of 37. (cohort stud$ or longitudinal). (prematur* or preterm*).mp.) Psycholinguistic Abilities mp. MD: mean difference 44. limit 43 to (“in data review” PPVT: Peabody Picture 38. (case adj2 (series or control$)). or in process or “pubmed not Vocabulary Test mp. medline”) was involved in extracting data and quality assessment, conducted the GRADE ratings, assisted with data interpretation; Dr Moher assisted with the development of the study protocol and consulted on the quality assessment and data interpretation as required; Ms Doucet (a knowledge-user clinician), Dr Neuss (a knowledge-user clinician), and Ms Bernstein (a knowledge-user clinician) provided input into the inclusion criteria and data abstraction form, helped clarify data interpretation when needed, and commented on the manuscript; Ms Na verified and provided input into the synthesis tables and made comments on the manuscript; and all authors approved the final manuscript. This review is registered at PROSPERO: #CRD42013005426, http://www.crd.york.ac.uk/PROSPERO/display_record.asp?ID=CRD42013005426#.VWeaU-vqc6I. DOI: 10.1542/peds.2015-1974 Accepted for publication Oct 7, 2015 Address correspondence to Elizabeth M. Fitzpatrick, Audiology and Speech-Language Pathology Program, Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Ottawa, 451 Smyth Road, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada K1H 8L1. E-mail: elizabeth.fitzpatrick@uottawa.ca PEDIATRICS (ISSN Numbers: Print, 0031-4005; Online, 1098-4275). Copyright © 2016 by the American Academy of Pediatrics FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose. FUNDING: This systematic review was funded by a Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR) Knowledge Synthesis Grant (FRN-124600). The funding agency was not involved in developing the protocol and was not involved in any aspect of the review (data collection, analysis, or interpretation) or publications. Dr Fitzpatrick’s work is also supported by a CIHR New Investigator and a Canadian Child Health Clinician Scientist Program award. Dr Moher holds a University of Ottawa Research Chair. All researchers are independent from the funding agencies. POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: The authors have indicated they have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose. Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news by guest on September 16, 2021 PEDIATRICS Volume 137, number 1, January 2016 15
REFERENCES 1. American Academy of Pediatrics, Joint hearing: an international consensus 19. Petitto LA, Zatorre RJ, Gauna K, Nikelski Committee on Infant Hearing. Year statement. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ. EJ, Dostie D, Evans AC. Speech-like 2007 position statement: principles 2013;18(4):429–445 cerebral activity in profoundly deaf and guidelines for early hearing 10. Marschark M, Spencer PE. Spoken people processing signed languages: detection and intervention programs. language development of deaf and implications for the neural basis of Pediatrics. 2007;120(4). Available at: hard-of-hearing children: Historical human language. Proc Natl Acad Sci www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/ and theoretical perspectives. In: USA. 2000;97(25):13961–13966 120/4/e898 Spencer PE, Marschark M, eds. 20. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman 2. Nelson HD, Bougatsos C, Nygren P; Advances in the spoken language DG; PRISMA Group. Preferred reporting 2001 US Preventive Services Task development of deaf and hard-of- items for systematic reviews and Force. Universal newborn hearing hearing children. New York: Oxford meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. screening: systematic review to update University Press; 2006:3–21 BMJ. 2009;339:b2535 the 2001 US Preventive Services Task 11. Durieux-Smith A, Fitzpatrick EM. History 21. Fitzpatrick EM, Stevens A, Garritty C, Force Recommendation. Pediatrics. of the management of hearing loss Moher D. The effects of sign language 2008;122(1). Available at: www. in children. In: Seewald R, Tharpe on spoken language acquisition pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/122/1/ AM, eds. Comprehensive handbook in children with hearing loss: a e266 of pediatric audiology. San Diego, CA: systematic review protocol. Syst Rev. 3. Fortnum HM, Summerfield AQ, Plural Publishing; 2011:617–629 2013;2:108 Marshall DH, Davis AC, Bamford JM. 12. Thoutenhoofd ED, Archbold SM, Prevalence of permanent childhood 22. Fitzpatrick EM, Stevens A, Garritty C, et Gregory S, Lutman ME, Nikolopoulos al. A systematic review of the effects hearing impairment in the United TP, Sach TH. Paediatric cochlear Kingdom and implications for of using sign language on spoken implantation: Evaluating outcomes. language acquisition in children universal neonatal hearing screening: London: Whurr Publishers Ltd.; 2005 questionnaire based ascertainment with early-identified hearing loss. study. BMJ. 2001;323(7312):536–540 13. Geers AE, Moog JS, Biedenstein J, PROSPERO 2013:CRD42013005426 Brenner C, Hayes H. Spoken language Available at: www.crd.york.ac.uk/ 4. Prieve BA, Stevens F. The New York scores of children using cochlear PROSPERO/display_record.asp?ID= State universal newborn hearing implants compared to hearing age- CRD42013005426#.VWeaU-vqc6I. screening demonstration project: mates at school entry. J Deaf Stud Deaf Accessed October 13, 2015 introduction and overview. Ear Hear. Educ. 2009;14(3):371–385 2000;21(2):85–91 23. Mcgowan J, Sampson M, Lefebvre C. An 14. Fitzpatrick EM, Crawford L, Ni A, evidence based checklist for the peer 5. Watkin PM, Baldwin M. Identifying Durieux-Smith A. A descriptive analysis review of electronic search strategies deafness in early childhood: of language and speech skills in 4- to (PRESS EBC). Evid Based Libr Inf Pract. requirements after the newborn 5-yr-old children with hearing loss. Ear 2010;5(1):149–154 hearing screen. Arch Dis Child. Hear. 2011;32(5):605–616 2011;96(1):62–66 24. Leclercq E, Leeflang MMG, van Dalen 15. Mitchell R, Karchmer M. Chasing the EC, Kremer LC. Validation of search 6. Kennedy CR, McCann DC, Campbell mythical ten percent: parental hearing filters for identifying pediatric studies MJ, et al. Language ability after early status of deaf and hard of hearing in PubMed. J Pediatr. 2013;162(3):629– detection of permanent childhood students in the United States. Sign 634.e2 hearing impairment. N Engl J Med. Lang Stud. 2004;4(2):138–163 2006;354(20):2131–2141 25. Glanville JM, Lefebvre C, Miles JNV, 16. Niparko JK, Tobey EA, Thal DJ, et al; Camosso-Stefinovic J. How to identify 7. Ching TY, Dillon H, Marnane V, et al. CDaCI Investigative Team. Spoken randomized controlled trials in Outcomes of early- and late-identified language development in children MEDLINE: ten years on. J Med Libr children at 3 years of age: findings following cochlear implantation. JAMA. Assoc. 2006;94(2):130–136 from a prospective population-based 2010;303(15):1498–1506 study. Ear Hear. 2013;34(5):535–552 26. Scottish Intercollegiate Guidelines 17. Spencer PE, Marschark M. Evidence- Network. Search filters. Available at: 8. Wake M, Poulakis Z, Hughes EK, Carey- based practice in educating deaf and www.sign.ac.uk/methodology/filters. Sargeant C, Rickards FW. Hearing hard-of-hearing students. New York: html. Accessed June 27, 2013 impairment: a population study of age Oxford University Press; 2010 at diagnosis, severity, and language 27. Spencer PE, Marschark M, eds. 18. Petitto LA, Katerelos M, Levy BG, outcomes at 7-8 years. Arch Dis Child. Advances in spoken language Gauna K, Tétreault K, Ferraro V. 2005;90(3):238–244 development of deaf and hard-of- Bilingual signed and spoken language hearing children. New York: Oxford 9. Moeller MP, Carr G, Seaver L, Stredler- acquisition from birth: implications University Press; 2006 Brown A, Holzinger D. Best practices for the mechanisms underlying early in family-centered early intervention bilingual language acquisition. J Child 28. Brisk ME, Harrington MM. Literacy for children who are deaf or hard of Lang. 2001;28(2):453–496 and bilingualism: a handbook for all Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news by guest on September 16, 2021 16 FITZPATRICK et al
You can also read