Scent Marking, Sexual Behavior and Aggression in Male Gerbils: Comparative Analysis of Endocrine Control1

Page created by Jesse Greene
 
CONTINUE READING
AMER. ZOOL., 21:143-151 (1981)

           Scent Marking, Sexual Behavior and Aggression in Male Gerbils:
                    Comparative Analysis of Endocrine Control1
                                                 PAULINE YAHR
^                               Department of Psychobiology, University of California,
•                                            Irvine, California 92717

            SYNOPSIS. The aggressive, sexual, and scent marking behaviors of male gerbils (Meriones
            unguiculatus) are sensitive to gonadal androgens, but androgens are not equally important
            in the control of each behavior. In this species, territorial residency, prior aggressive
            experience, and unidentified factors that contribute to large individual differences in
            aggressiveness, influence the aggressive behavior of males at least as much as androgens
            do. To the extent that androgens affect aggression between male gerbils, they act partially

                                                                                                            Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/icb/article/21/1/143/134137 by guest on 23 January 2021
            by altering aggressiveness and partially by altering production of aggression-eliciting cues.
            The nature of these cues is unknown. Understanding the role of androgens in aggression
            in this species is further complicated by the observation that castration can either increase
            or decrease aggression depending on the age at which the surgery is performed. In
            contrast, androgens play a primary role in the control of sexual behavior and scent mark-
            ing. Both behaviors consistently decline following castration despite prior experience of
            the males. Both behaviors are also controlled by the medial preoptic area-anterior hy-
            pothalamus, an area of the brain often implicated in the control of male sociosexual
            behaviors. It appears, though, that the sites, and possibly the mechanisms, of hormone
            action underlying scent marking and sexual behavior differ. Studying both behaviors in
            the same species, and whenever possible in the same individuals, is proving to be a useful
            technique for identifying such differences between behaviors as their sensitivity to steroids
            and to brain lesions.

                   INTRODUCTION                           marking behaviors by which males actively
  Androgens secreted by the testes influ-                distribute olfactory signals about their en-
ence many of the sociosexual behaviors of                vironment (Thiessen and Rice, 1976). Ur-
males during development and/or adult-                    ine spraying by male cats (Felis domesticus)
hood. Androgens stimulate male copula-                    and the frequent leg-lift urination behav-
tory behavior in every vertebrate species                 ior of male dogs (Canis familiaris) are fa-
studied to date and androgen-dependent                    miliar examples of scent marking.
changes in aggressiveness are common-                        Male gerbils (Meriones unguiculatus)
place. Androgens also influence the com-                  scent mark by rubbing their bellies on ob-
munication patterns of males. The songs                  jects that protrude from the substrate.
of male birds (e.g., zebra finches, Poephila             This marking response deposits odorous
guttata, and canaries, Serinus canarius)                  secretions from the male's ventral seba-
and the postural displays of male lizards                ceous gland. Both the secretory activity of
(Anolis carolinensis) are examples of audi-               the gland and the marking behavior are
tory and visual displays controlled by an-               controlled by testicular androgens (Thies-
drogens (see reviews by Arnold, 1981;                     sen et al., 1968). After castration, the or-
Crews and Greenberg, 1981). In mam-                       angey secretion of the gland disappears
mals, androgens often affect olfactory                    and scent marking greatly decreases or
communication. Odors used as social sig-                 ceases entirely. Both scent gland activity
nals, such as the urinary pheromones of                   and marking behavior resume when cas-
male mice (Mus musculus), can vary with a                trated males are given testosterone. In this
male's hormonal status (Bronson, 1971).                   review, I summarize progress my research
Androgens also influence specialized scent-               group has made in relating the hormonal
                                                         basis of gerbil scent marking to the hor-
                                                          monal basis of two more traditionally-stud-
                                                          ied, androgen-sensitive behaviors, sexual
      1
        From the Symposium on SocialSignals—Comparativebehavior and aggression, that may be func-
    and Endocrine Aspects presented at the Annual Meet- tionally related to scent marking in this
    ing of the American Society of Zoologists, 27-30 De-
    cember 1979, at Tampa, Florida.                      species.
                                                        143
144                                     PAULINE YAHR

   The long-term goal of our research is to       in aggression vary with the context in
understand how androgens act on the               which aggression occurs, the previous ag-
brain to control and coordinate androgen-         gressive experience of the male, and the
dependent behaviors of males. Because we          age at which castration is performed. In-
are interested in identifying general prin-       dividual differences in aggressiveness are
ciples of androgen action, and in recogniz-       large.                                      #
ing their limitations, a comparative ap-             Male gerbils are more aggressive toward
proach is essential. The comparative              unfamiliar males intruding into their ter-
approach we selected is not the traditional       ritories than they are toward unfamiliar
one. Rather than compare androgen con-            males that they encounter in neutral areas

                                                                                                  Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/icb/article/21/1/143/134137 by guest on 23 January 2021
trol of a single behavior across species or       (Wechkin and Cramer, 1971). Our data in-
strains, we set out to compare androgen           dicate that castration does not appreciably
control of scent marking, sexual behavior,        reduce the aggressiveness of resident
and aggression within a species. Our strat-       males toward intruders unless their inter-
egy was to develop a model system in              actions are prolonged (Yahr et al., 1977).
which several androgen-sensitive behav-           When males met in a series of brief (15-
iors could be studied simultaneously with-        min) tests, castrated residents and gonad-
in individual males. This comparative ap-         ally intact residents were equally likely to
proach would afford the advantage that            attack intruders. No differences in attack
any differences observed between behav-           latency, fight duration, or chasing were ap-
iors could not be attributed to differences       parent. We obtained similar results when
in genotype, developmental history, or            males remained together continuously for
amount of circulating hormone.                    several hours, either in a neutral arena or
   Our strategic goal has been partially          in one of the male's living space. However,
met. Comparisons of the scent-marking             if males remained together for an entire
and sexual behaviors of male gerbils are          day, the decreased aggressiveness of cas-
proving fruitful. In contrast, as discussed       trates showed up clearly. When fighting in
below, androgens are much less important          a neutral arena, 4 of 9 intact males killed
in determining the aggressive behavior of         their intact male opponents within 24 hr.
this species. Hence gerbil aggression is a        In the same setting, castrates never killed
less suitable model system for analyzing          their opponents, whether the opponent
the neural actions of androgens.                  was an intact or a castrated male. When
                                                  fighting in their own territories, 6 of 8 in-
       ANDROGENS AND AGGRESSION                   tact males killed intruders (these 6 males
   When we began our research, little was         killed 10 of the 18 intruders they faced),
 known about hormonal influences on the           whereas only 1 of 8 castrated residents
aggressive behavior of male gerbils. None-        killed intruders. These data suggest that
theless, the data that were available were        androgens stimulate male gerbils to per-
intriguing because of the contradictory re-       severe in their attacks but do not shorten
sults obtained in different laboratories.         the latency to initiate a fight.
Some researchers reported that castration            The decrease in aggressiveness follow-
decreased aggressiveness in male gerbils          ing castration was attenuated in males with
 (Sayler, 1970; Lumia et al., 1975), as it does   prior aggressive experience. When we
in many other species. Others obtained            treated castrated male residents with tes-
just the opposite result, i.e., castration in-    tosterone propionate, they became more
creased aggression (Anisko et al., 1973;          aggressive toward intruding males. For ex-
 Christenson etal., 1973). Our data provide       ample, some castrates that had not killed
support for both of these disparate results.      intruders previously did so after receiving
The behavioral changes that occur after           testosterone injections. In contrast, castra-
castration result partially from changes in       tion did not decrease the aggressiveness of
aggressiveness and partially from changes         male residents that gained aggressive ex-
in production of aggression-inducing cues.        perience while gonadally intact. In partic-
The magnitude and direction of changes            ular, resident males that killed intruders
SCENT MARKING, MATING AND AGGRESSION                               145

prior to castration continued to do so after    only subjects to reach a criterion of attack-
castration.                                      ing within 60 sec on 5 of 6 consecutive
   Gonadal androgens also influence              tests. In contrast, postpuberally castrated
aggression in this species by promoting          males formed the least aggressive group.
  reduction of aggression-inducing cues,         It is not clear why castration produced dif-
8 \., intact males elicit more aggression
than castrates do (Yahr et al., 1977). When
                                                 ferent effects on aggression when per-
                                                 formed before versus after puberty, but
we left intact males and castrates together      the differences do not necessarily reflect
for a day in a neutral arena, only 1 of 11      changes in the neuroendocrine mecha-
intact males killed his castrated opponent.      nisms underlying aggression. They could
As discussed above, deaths occurred in          just as easily reflect differences in the way
nearly half of the cases when intact males      intact male intruders behave toward dif-

                                                                                                  Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/icb/article/21/1/143/134137 by guest on 23 January 2021
were paired. Thus intact males are less          ferent types of residents. For example, in-
persistent when attacking castrates. Simi-      truders may provoke attacks from females
larly, resident males were twice as likely to   and prepuberal castrates by attempting to
attack and chase intact male intruders as        mount them. Prepuberal castration may be
castrated intruders. This was true regard-       more effective than postpuberal castration
less of the resident's gonadal status, indi-     for eliminating secondary sex characteris-
cating that both castrated and intact male      tics {e.g., odors) that distinguish males
residents are sensitive to the aggression-in-    from females.
ducing cues that intact intruders produce.          While the level of aggression male ger-
However, the importance of these aggres-        bils display in a territorial context is influ-
sion-inducing cues also decreases as males      enced to varying degrees by the males'
become more experienced fighters. When          hormonal condition, the outcome of inter-
we exposed aggressively experienced male        actions in this setting depends primarily on
residents (either castrated or intact) to in-   territorial (residential) status (Yahr et al.,
tact, castrated, or scent-glandectomized in-     1977). We found that during brief en-
truders for a day, the residents attacked       counters resident males were twice as likely
and chased the different types of intruders     to dominate intruders as the reverse, and
indiscriminately. Intruders from each cat-      this disparity increased when the interac-
egory were equally likely to be killed.         tions were prolonged. Intruders, for ex-
   The influence of castration on gerbil        ample, never killed a resident male. The
aggression is modified not only by aggres-      advantage to fighting in their own territory
sive experience, but also by the age at         occurred independent of either the resi-
which castration is performed. Castration       dent's or the intruder's hormonal state.
can actually increase the aggressiveness of     Similar results have recently been re-
male gerbils if they are castrated prior to     ported for rats (Rattus norvegicus; Christie
puberty (Yahr and Coquelin, 1980). In this      and Barfield, 1979).
study, male or female residents encoun-             As noted above, we originally hoped to
tered intact male intruders twice daily for     study three androgen-sensitive social be-
up to 2 wk. All female residents were go-       haviors, social signalling (scent marking),
nadally intact. The male residents either       sexual behavior, and aggression, simulta-
remained intact, were castrated before pu-      neously in male gerbils. Yet as our data
berty, or were castrated as adults. Each test   indicate, several other factors, including
ended after 5 min, or earlier if a fight be-    territorial residency, prior aggressive ex-
gan. Fights were infrequent under these         perience, and age at castration, limit the
conditions, but group differences ap-           role of androgens in the control of aggres-
peared among residents that initiated           sion in this species. It seems, therefore,
fights against 2 or more intruders. The         that straightforward analyses of androgen
most aggressive such fighters were females      control of aggression between male gerbils
and prepuberally castrated males. They          are not feasible until a test situation is de-
attacked more quickly and more often            veloped in which hormonal variables make
than intact male residents and were the         a larger contribution to aggression than
146                                              PAULINE YAHR

                     • Shorn-operated control*            often as paired or isolated males (Hull et
                                                          al, 1973; Yahr et al, 1980). The marking
                                                           frequencies of isolated males were related
                                                          to their sexual activity. Isolated males that
                                                          mounted scent marked 2-5 times as much
                                                          as sexually-inactive isolates.               •
                                                             Scent marking and sexual behavior show
                                                          a similar dependence on testicular andro-
                                                          gens (Yahr et al., 1979). Like scent mark-
                                                          ing, sexual behavior gradually declines af-

                                                                                                           Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/icb/article/21/1/143/134137 by guest on 23 January 2021
                  Weekly Tests After Surgery
                                                          ter castration, as illustrated in Figure 1.
FIG. 1. Changes in ejaculatory behavior of male ger-      Ejaculation disappeared first, followed by
bils after castration and during treatment with tes-
tosterone propionate (TP). TP was injected twice a        intromission and mounting. This pattern
week in the doses specified in the figure. Figure taken   of decline resembles that seen in other
from Yahr et al. (1979).                                  species (Grunt and Young, 1953; Young,
                                                           1961; Davidson, 1966a). Injections of tes-
                                                          tosterone propionate (75 pig twice weekly)
they do in the test settings that we have                 prevented loss of sexual activity and main-
employed.                                                 tained copulatory behavior at the level of
                                                          intact males. This dose of testosterone pro-
      HORMONAL CONTROL OF SEXUAL                          pionate did not restore sexual behavior
      BEHAVIOR AND SCENT MARKING                          once it had ceased, but larger doses (600
   Next we turned our attention to andro-                 ixg twice weekly) did. Thus gerbils resem-
 gen control of male sexual behavior. Al-                 ble rats: more testosterone is needed after
though gerbils have been popular subjects                 castration to reinstate mating behavior
for research on social behavior for years,                than is needed to maintain mating behav-
 no data were available on the hormonal                   ior (Davidson and Bloch, 1969).
basis of male copulatory behavior. It ap-                    Testosterone propionate injections also
 pears that this was due in part to difficul-             prevented and reversed castration-in-
ties in inducing gerbils to copulate in sit-              duced decreases in scent marking (see Fig-
uations where their behavior could be                     ure 2); however, the 75 //.g (twice weekly)
observed and quantified. Initially we were                dose of testosterone propionate that fully
plagued with problems in obtaining a rea-                 maintained mating did not fully maintain
sonable number of males that would cop-                   scent marking in the same males. This was
ulate reliably, e.g., males that would cop-               our first clue that scent marking and sex-
ulate to ejaculation in three of five                     ual behavior were differentially sensitive to
preliminary screening tests. By trying a va-              testosterone. Which behavior is more sen-
riety of environmental manipulations, we                  sitive to testosterone varies with the exper-
have since found that male gerbils are                    imental paradigm.
most likely to copulate if they are housed                   When male gerbils were implanted sev-
with other males and if they are tested dur-              eral weeks after castration with testoster-
ing the light rather than the dark phase of               one contained in silastic capsules, so that
the cycle (Yahr et al, 1980). Males housed                they were continuously exposed to hor-
in pairs or groups (of 4) were 3^1 times                  mone, both sexual behavior and scent
more likely to mount, intromit, and ejac-                 marking resumed as a function of hor-
ulate than isolated males. The percentage                 mone dose (capsule length), as shown in
of tests in which sexual behavior occurred                Figure 3 (Yahr et al, 1979). However, less
and the level of sexual activity among                    testosterone was needed to reinstate scent
males that mounted were both higher in                    marking than to reinstate sexual behavior.
the light than in the dark. Ejaculation, in               A dose of testosterone (2-mm capsules)
particular, was more likely to occur in the               that reinstated ejaculation in only 57% of
light and in grouped males. In contrast,                  castrates elevated scent marking frequen-
grouped males scent marked only half as                   cies above precastration levels in the same
SCENT MARKING, MATING AND AGGRESSION                                                            147

 males. Males that reliably copulated to eja-
                                                                                                 Sham-operated Controls
culation before castration rarely mounted
when given a lower dose of testosterone                                                          Castrates

(1-mm capsules), yet this dose reinstated
scent marking to 25% of precastration
Revels in a subset of these males that were
also reliable scent markers (i.e., that ob-
tained mean marking scores of at least five
 marks per 5-min test) before castration. In
 some males that regularly ejaculated but
 rarely scent marked before castration, low         Pre-op I   2   3   4     5   6   7   8   9   10   tl   12 13   14 15 16
doses of testosterone induced high mark-                                   Weekly Tests After     Surgery

                                                                                                                              Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/icb/article/21/1/143/134137 by guest on 23 January 2021
 ing frequencies without reinstating mount-     FIG. 2. Changes in scent marking frequency of male
 ing. Thus it was easier to reinstate scent     gerbils after castration and during treatment with tes-
 marking than sexual behavior using hor-        tosterone propionate (TP). TP was injected twice a
 mone implants, whereas in the previous         week in the doses specified in the figure. Data shown
study it was easier to maintain sexual be-      are for males that averaged five or more marks per
                                                test before castration. Figure taken from Yahr et al.
 havior than scent marking with hormone         (1979).
injections. These differences in the sensi-
tivity of scent marking and sexual behavior
to testosterone across studies may reflect
differences in the treatment paradigm           Wilson, 1968). It is presumed that dihy-
 (maintenance or reinstatement), the form       drotestosterone is not aromatized within
of hormone administration (injections or        the brain because it is not aromatized by
capsules), or the dose-response curves for      human placental microsomes (Ryan, 1960),
 the two behaviors.                             the standard assay system for assessing
   Differential sensitivity of scent marking    aromatization. If testosterone activates
and sexual behavior to hormonal stimula-        sexual behavior or scent marking via 5a-
tion is also apparent when other steroid        reduction, then dihydrotestosterone should
 hormones are substituted for testosterone      be more potent behaviorally than testos-
 (Yahr and Stephens, unpublished data).         terone. If aromatization is essential, dihy-
Our analysis of the effects of different ste-   drotestosterone should be ineffective,
 roids was motivated by an interest in the      whereas estradiol should be more potent
 role of metabolism in androgen action on       than testosterone. 19-Nortestosterone was
the brain and behavior. According to one        selected for study because it is converted
currently popular hypothesis, testosterone      to estrogen 20% as efficiently as testoster-
is a prohormone that stimulates male sex-       one in placental microsomes (Ryan, 1959).
 ual behavior via its metabolism to estrogen    Thus estradiol, testosterone, 19-nortestos-
 (aromatization) within the brain (see re-      terone, and dihydrotestosterone represent
view by Adkins, 1981). To test the role of      a graded series in terms of the amount of
aromatization in androgen control of sex        estrogen available either directly or via
and scent marking in male gerbils, we           aromatization.
studied the behavioral effects of estrogen         All of the steroids maintained and rein-
and of androgens that vary in their ability     stated scent marking in castrated males
to be metabolized to estrogens. More spe-       and few significant differences between
cifically, we compared the behavioral ef-       groups emerged. None of the steroids was
fects of dihydrotestosterone, 19-nortestos-     more potent than testosterone. This sug-
terone, testosterone, and estradiol.            gests that neither aromatization nor 5a-re-
   Dihydrotestosterone, a 5a-reduced me-        duction alone can account for the stimu-
tabolite of testosterone, is the active form    latory effects of testosterone on scent
of testosterone for promoting growth of         marking, although it is possible that the
tissues in the male reproductive tract (An-     doses of hormone used (2- and 5-mm cap-
derson and Liao, 1968; Bruchovsky and           sules) were too large to adequately distin-
148                                               PAULINE YAHR

                                                                          but ejaculatory behavior ceased in most

                                                     1
                                                                          estradiol-treated castrates within 5 wk.
 •o 25                                loo-                                The decline in ejaculations may reflect an
                                        80                                inability of estradiol to maintain penile re-
2 20

 £ IS-
           *       k

                        1I            1 60
                                      1
                                      g
                                                      1—|
                                                                          flexes and morphology, as has been re-
                                                                          ported for rats (Sodersten, 1975; Haijfe
                                                                          1979). However, estradiol proved less et-
2 10                                 ilT 40                               fective than either testosterone or 19-nor-
=    5-
                                     'i                                   testosterone in restoring mounting behav-
                                      « 20-
                                                                          ior. Castrates given 2-mm capsules of

                                                                                                                          Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/icb/article/21/1/143/134137 by guest on 23 January 2021
                                                                          estradiol mounted in only 8% of their tests.
                2 mm 5 mm 10
                          T

     Length of Silastic Capsule of T
                                               I mm   2 mm 5mm |0mm
                                                                          When the estradiol dose was increased (5-
                                          Length of Silastic Capsule of T
                                                                          mm capsules), mounting still occurred in
Fic. 3. Dose-response relationships for reinstate- only 33% of the tests. Thus gerbils differ
ment by testosterone (T) of ejaculatory behavior and considerably from rats which resume
scent marking in castrated male gerbils. Asterisks in-
dicate mean marking scores before castration. Mark- mounting much more readily in response
ing scores shown are for males that averaged five or to estradiol than in response to testoster-
more marks per test before castration. All males cop- one (Baum and Vreeburg, 1973).
ulated to ejaculation in at least 2 of 3 precastration
tests. Figure taken from Yahr et at. (1979).                                The levels of mounting behavior in-
                                                                          duced in gerbils by estradiol were actually
                                                                          lower than those induced by implants of
                                                                          dihydrotestosterone. Dihydrotestosterone
guish their effects on marking behavior. (2-mm capsules) did not maintain mount-
In contrast, the steroids differed consid- ing behavior after castration. Yet castrates
erably in their effects on mating.                                        given 2- and 5-mm capsules of dihydrotes-
    Testosterone and 19-nortestosterone tosterone in a reinstatement paradigm
were the most potent steroids for stimu- mounted in 17% and 56% of their tests,
lating sexual behavior. When administered respectively. In terms of reinstating eja-
in 2-mm capsules implanted at castration, culation, dihydrotestosterone was roughly
these steroids maintained ejaculation fre- as potent as testosterone. When given in 2-
quency at 109% and 93% of precastration and 5-mm capsules, dihydrotestosterone
levels, respectively. They also reinstated restored ejaculation frequencies to 22%
sexual activity once it had ceased in the and 55% of precastration levels, respec-
absence of hormonal stimulation, although tively, compared to 8% and 67% for tes-
19-nortestosterone was more potent than tosterone. Thus to some extent gerbils re-
testosterone in this paradigm. Within semble guinea pigs {Cavia porcellus) and
three to five weeks after implantation of rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatto) in terms
19-nortestosterone in 2-mm capsules, eja- of their ability to respond to dihydrotes-
culation frequencies of castrates returned tosterone (Alsum and Goy, 1974; Phoenix,
to 109% of precastration values. Ejacula- 1974).
tion frequencies of castrates given testos-                                  Our comparisons of the behavioral ef-
terone returned to only 8% of precastra- fects of different steroids must be tem-
tion levels in the same time. A larger dose pered by the fact that they are released
of testosterone (5-mm capsules) boosted from silastic capsules at different rates.
ejaculation frequencies to 67% of precas- (They are, of course, also released at dif-
tration levels. The observation that 19-nor- ferent rates from oil depots produced by
testosterone reinstated sexual behavior at subcutaneous injections.) Nonetheless, it
a lower dose than testosterone seems in- seems difficult to account for all of the data
compatible with the notion that androgens on the basis of differential release rates.
act via their estrogenic metabolites.                                     The release of estradiol would have to be
    Estradiol (2-mm capsules) maintained very low to be less than the amount of es-
high levels of mounting after castration, as tradiol produced by aromatization of tes-
predicted by the aromatization hypothesis, tosterone within the brain since the con-
SCENT MARKING, MATING AND AGGRESSION                                                                                                                                  149

 version ratio is less than 0.1% (Naftolin et     5
                                                  4     0
al., 1975). Similarly, if 19-nortestosterone      3
 acted via aromatization yet is more potent       2
                                                                                                                                                                 • TP in Am POA

 than testosterone, it must be released more      II
                                                  0
 than five times as rapidly as testosterone       9
^iven that it is aromatized only one-fifth as     8                                  /
 well.                                            7
                                                  6
                                                                                 /       v                                 \
                                                                                                               /      \                                      «TP in Pos! POA

                                                                                                 Xv
                                                  5
  NEURAL CONTROL OF SEXUAL BEHAVIOR               4                 -^       /
          AND SCENT MARKING                       3             ,,                       ~y                \              f..       Js>~=-—"TPinLotPOA
                                                   2
  The medial preoptic area-anterior hy-            1
                                                                                         * * • • - - « .    > < _ /

                                                                                                           ^-tf''^*'^
                                                                                                                           *    '   "   ^    "
                                                                                                                                                     T   P

                                                                                                                                            ^»---*Cholesterol
                                                                                                                                                             '   "   "   "   '
                                                                                                                                                                                 P   O   a

pothalamus mediates androgen control of           0             e        ^

                                                                                                                                                                                                   Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/icb/article/21/1/143/134137 by guest on 23 January 2021
                                                       Pre Post I        2   3           4            5    6          7        8        9        1
male copulatory behavior in various ver-               Castration
                                                                     Test Periods offer Brain Implants
tebrate species, including lizards, frogs       FIG. 4. Changes in scent marking frequencies of cas-
 (Rana pipiens), ring doves (Streptopelia ri-   trated male gerbils after implantation of testosterone
soria), rats, cats, dogs and rhesus monkeys     propionate (TP) into either the anterior (Ant), middle
(Davidson, 19666; Heimer and Larsson,           (Mid), or posterior (Post) portion of the medial
 1966; Slimp et al, 1968; Barfield, 1971;       preoptic area (POA), or into the lateral preoptic area
Hart, 1974; Hart et al., 1974; Wada and         (Lat POA). Controls received cholesterol implants at
                                                the same locations.
Gorbman, 1977; Morgantaler and Crews,
 1978; Wheeler and Crews, 1978). This
area of the brain also controls various pat-
terns of social signalling in males. For ex-    after castration, but implants into the mid-
ample, lesions that impair copulatory be-       dle portion of the medial preoptic area or
havior also disrupt assertion and challenge     into the lateral preoptic area did not.
displays in male lizards and eliminate uri-     These data are shown in Figure 4.
nary scent marking in male dogs and cats           In contrast, the middle portion of the
(Hart, 1974; Hart and Voith, 1978; Wheel-       medial preoptic area seems critical for the
er and Crews, 1978). Implanting testoster-      display of sexual behavior in male gerbils.
one directly into the medial preoptic area-     This conclusion is based on the effects of
anterior hypothalamus stimulates display        lesions in the medial preoptic area-ante-
behavior in lizards, mate-calling in frogs,     rior hypothalamus (Yahr et al., unpub-
and scent marking in gerbils (Thiessen et       lished data). These lesions eliminated or
al., 1973; Wada and Gorbman, 1977; Mor-         severely impaired mating behavior in go-
 gantaler and Crews, 1978). We were inter-      nadally intact males. After lesioning, most
ested in determining whether the medial         males (13/17) stopped ejaculating and
 preoptic area-anterior hypothalamus con-       nearly half (7/17) never mounted. Those
 trols sexual behavior in male gerbils and      that did mount did so much less often after
 whether the cells mediating androgen con-      lesioning than before. These deficits were
 trol of scent marking and sexual behavior      neither prevented nor reversed by admin-
 were the same. Our data indicate that dif-     istration of exogenous testosterone, indi-
 ferent, although possibly overlapping, cell    cating that the behavioral decrements were
 populations in this region control the two     not due to decreased gonadotropin and
 behaviors.                                     hence testicular androgen secretion. Le-
   The most important areas for mediating       sions that eliminated mounting were clus-
 androgen control of scent marking are the      tered in the middle portion of the medial
 anterior aspect of the medial preoptic         preoptic area.
 area, near the diagonal band of Broca, and        Interestingly, the same lesioned males
 the posterior aspect of the medial preoptic    showed only temporary decreases in the
 area, near the anterior hypothalamus           frequency of scent marking. Even this tem-
 (Yahr et al., unpublished data). Testoster-    porary decline did not occur in males im-
 one implants into these portions of the        planted with silastic capsules of testoster-
 preoptic area induced marking behavior         one at lesioning. Since the lesions generally
150                                           PAULINE YAHR

spared the most anterior and the most pos-        golian gerbil (Meriones unguiculatus). Physiol. Be-
terior aspects of the medial preoptic area        hav. 10:989-994.
                                              Christie, M. H. and R. J. Barfield. 1979. Effect of
(i.e., the areas implicated in the control of     castration and home cage residency on aggres-
scent marking), this may account for the          sive behavior in rats. Horm. Behav. 13:85-91.
sparing of scent marking function. This Crews, D and N. Greenberg. 1981. Function and
possibility gains support from the obser-         causation of social signals in lizards. Amer. Zoofe
vation that the few males that seemed to          21:273-294.
suffer permanent disruption of scent Davidson,          J. M. 1966a. Characteristics of sex behav-
                                                  iour in male rats following castration. Anim. Be-
marking, despite hormone therapy, had             hav. 14:266-272.
lesions that included one or the other of Davidson, J. M. 1966ft. Activation of the male rat's

                                                                                                                 Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/icb/article/21/1/143/134137 by guest on 23 January 2021
these sites. These data suggest that gerbils      sexual behavior by intracerebral implantation of
differ from lizards, dogs, and cats in that       androgen. Endocrinology 79:783-794.
                                              Davidson, J. M. and G. J. Bloch. 1969. Neuroen-
different neural loci control social signal-      docrine aspects of male reproduction. Biol. Re-
ling and sexual behavior. Perhaps more            prod. 1 (Suppl.):67-92.
discrete lesions within the medial preoptic Grunt, J. A. and W. C. Young. 1953. Consistency of
area-anterior hypothalamus would reveal           sexual behavior patterns in individual male
specificity of function in these other            guinea pigs following castration and androgen
                                                  therapy. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 46:138-144.
species as well.                              Hart, B. L. 1974. Medial preoptic-anterior hypotha-
                                                           lamic area and the sociosexual behavior of male
              ACKNOWLEDGMENTS                              dogs: A comparative neuropsychological analy-
                                                           sis. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 86:328-349.
   The research summarized here was sup- Hart, B. L. 1979. Activation of sexual reflexes of
ported by NIMH research grant No. MH-                      male rats by dihydrotestosterone but not estro-
26481.                                                     gen. Physiol. Behav. 23:107-109.
                                                       Hart, B. L., C. M. Haugen, and D. M. Peterson. 1973.
                    REFERENCES                             Effects of medial preoptic-anterior hypothalamic
                                                           lesions on the mating behavior of male cats.
Adkins-Regan, E. 1981. Hormone specificity, andro-         Brain Res. 54:177-191.
    gen metabolism and social behavior. Amer. Zool. Hart, B. L. and V. L. Voith. 1978. Changes in urine
    21:257-270.                                            spraying, feeding and sleep behavior of cats fol-
Alsum, P. and R. W. Goy. 1974. Actions of esters of        lowing medial preoptic-anterior hypothalamic le-
    testosterone, dihydrotestosterone or estradiol on      sions. Brain Res. 145:406-409.
    sexual behavior in castrated male guinea pigs. Heimer, L. and K. Larsson. 1966. Impairment of
     Horm. Behav. 5:207-217.                               mating behavior in male rats following lesions in
Anderson, K. M. and S. Liao. 1968. Selective reten-        the preoptic-anterior hypothalamic continuum.
    tion of dihydrotesterone by prostatic nuclei. Na-      Brain Res. 3:248-263.
    ture 219:277-279.                                  Hull, E. M., C. J. Langan, and L. Roselli. 1973. Pop-
Anisko, J. J., T. Christenson, and M. G. Buehler.          ulation density and social, territorial, and phys-
     1973. Effects of androgen on fighting behavior        iological measures in the gerbil (Meriones ungui-
    in male and female Mongolian gerbils (Meriones         culatus). J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 84:414-422.
    unguiculatus). Horm. Behav. 4:199-208.             Lumia, A. R., M. O. Westervelt, and C. A. Reider.
Arnold, A. P. 1981. Logical levels of steroid hor-         1975. Effects of olfactory bulb ablation and an-
    mone action in the control of vertebrate behav-        drogen on marking and agonistic behavior in
    ior. Amer. Zool. 21:233-242.                           male Mongolian gerbils (Meriones unguiculatus).
Barfield, R. J. 1971. Activation of sexual and ag-        J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 89:1091-1099.
    gressive behavior by androgen implantation into Morgantaler, A. and D. Crews. 1978. Role of ante-
    the male ring dove brain. Endocrinology 89:1470—       rior hypothalamus-preoptic area in the regula-
     1476.                                                 tion of reproductive behavior in the lizard, Anolis
Baum, M. J. and J. T. M. Vreeburg. 1973. Copula-           carolinensis: Implantation studies. Horm. Behav.
    tion in castrated male rats following combined          11:61-73.
    treatment with estradiol and dihydrotestoster- Naftolin, F., K. J. Ryan, I. J. Davies, V. V. Reddy, F.
    one. Science 182:283-285.                              Flores, Z. Petro, and M. Kuhn. 1975. The for-
Bronson, F. H. 1971. Rodent pheromones. Biol. Re-          mation of estrogens by central neuroendocrine
    prod. 4:344-357.                                       tissues. Rec. Prog. Horm. Res. 31:295-315.
Bruchovsky, N. and J. D. Wilson. 1968. The con- Phoenix, C. H. 1974. Effect of dihydrotestosterone
    version of testosterone to 5a-androstan-17/3-ol-3-     on sexual behavior of castrated male rhesus
    one by rat prostate in vivo and in vitro. J. Biol.     monkeys. Physiol. Behav. 12:1045-1055.
    Chem. 243:2012-2021.                               Ryan, K. J. 1959. Biological aromatization of ste-
Christenson, T., K. Wallen, B. A. Brown, and S. E.         roids. J. Biol. Chem. 234:268-272.
    Glickman. 1973. Effects of castration, blindness, Ryan, K. J. 1960. Estrogen formation by the human
    and anosmia on social reactivity in the male Mon-      placenta: Studies on the mechanisms of steroid
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/icb/article/21/1/143/134137 by guest on 23 January 2021
                            SCENT MARKING, MATING AND AGGRESSION                                       151

     aromatization by mammalian tissue. Acta Endo-       Wechkin, S. and R. C. Cramer. 1971. The role of
     crinol. 35 (Suppl.):697-698.                           site familiarity in aggression toward strangers in
 Sayler, A. 1970. The effect of anti-androgens on ag-       the Mongolian gerbil. Psychon. Sci. 23:335-336.
     gressive behavior in the gerbil. Physiol. Behav.    Wheeler, J. M. and D. Crews. 1978. Role of the an-
     5:667-671.                                             terior hypothalamus-preoptic area in the regu-
 Slimp, J. C, B. L. Hart, and R. W. Goy. 1978. Het-          lation of male reproductive behavior in the liz-
0    erosexual and sociosexual behavior of adult male       ard, Anolis carolinensis: Lesion studies. Horm.
     rhesus monkeys with medial preoptic-anterior            Behav. 11:42-60.
     hypothalamic lesions. Brain Res. 142:105-122.       Yahr, P. and A. Coquelin. 1980. Effect of pre-versus
 Sodersten, P. 1975. Mounting behavior and lordosis          post-pubertal castration on aggression between
     behavior in castrated male rats treated with tes-       male gerbils. Behav. Neur. Biol. 28:496-500.
     tosterone propionate, or with estradiol benzoate    Yahr, P., A. Coquelin, A. Martin, and C. W. Scouten.
     or dihydrotestosterone in combination with tes-         1977. Effects of castration on aggression be-
     tosterone propionate. Horm. Behav. 6:109-126.           tween male Mongolian gerbils. Behav. Biol.
 Thiessen, D. D., H. C. Friend, and G. Lindzey. 1968.        19:189-205.
     Androgen control of territorial marking in the      Yahr, P., J. C. Jackson, A. Newman, D. R. Stephens,
     Mongolian gerbil. Science 160:432^134.                  and A. N. Clancy. 1980. Paradigm for compar-
 Thiessen, D. and M. Rice. 1976. Mammalian scent             ing sexual behavior and scent marking in male
     gland marking and social behavior. Psych. Bull.        gerbils. Physiol. Behav. 24:263-266.
     83:505-539.                                         Yahr, P., A. Newman, and D. R. Stephens. 1979.
 Thiessen, D. D., P. I. Yahr, and K. Owens. 1973.           Sexual behavior and scent marking in male ger-
     Regulatory mechanisms of territorial marking in         bils: Comparison of changes after castration and
     the Mongolian gerbil. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol.        testosterone replacement. Horm. Behav. 13:175—
     82:382-393.                                             184.
 Wada, M. and A. Gorbman. 1977. Relation of mode         Young, W. C. 1961. The hormones and mating be-
     of administration of testosterone to evocation of       havior. In W. C. Young (ed.), Sex and internal
     male sexual behavior in frogs. Horm. Behav.            secretions, pp. 1173-1239. Williams and Wilkins,
     8:310-319.                                              Baltimore.
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/icb/article/21/1/143/134137 by guest on 23 January 2021
You can also read