Recognition of courtship song in the field cricket, Teleogryllus oceanicus

Page created by Joan Brown
 
CONTINUE READING
Anim. Behav., 1996, 51, 353–366

    Recognition of courtship song in the field cricket, Teleogryllus oceanicus
                    ROHINI BALAKRISHNAN & GERALD S. POLLACK
                           Department of Biology, McGill University

                        (Received 4 November 1994; initial acceptance 9 February 1995;
                              final acceptance 9 June 1995; MS. number: 7146)

Abstract. The courtship song of the cricket, Teleogryllus oceanicus plays an important role in inducing
the female to mount the male, which is necessary for mating. The song consists of a short,
amplitude-modulated chirp, followed by a long trill of constant intensity and high syllable rate. Using
playback techniques, it was determined which physical parameters of courtship song are necessary
and/or sufficient to evoke normal female mounting of muted, courting males. The higher harmonics of
natural courtship song were neither necessary nor sufficient for the effectiveness of the song. The chirp
component alone was sufficient to evoke normal levels of mounting, but the trill was only partially
effective on its own. The conspicuous amplitude modulation of the chirp was not necessary to evoke
normal responses. The results suggest that the high effectiveness of the chirp is due to its characteristic
temporal pattern. As in other cricket species, the song repertoire of T. oceanicus also includes distinct
calling and aggression songs, which contain chirps that are structurally similar to the courtship chirp.
Both calling and aggression songs evoked normal mounting responses when played back in the context
of courtship.                                           ? 1996 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour

Crickets use acoustic signals to communicate              and courtship songs are recognized share common
with each other. These signals take the form of           or similar features.
stereotyped, repetitive songs produced by stridu-            Studies of song recognition have focused on
lating males. Many cricket species, including Tel-        calling song. Female crickets recognize and pref-
eogryllus oceanicus, have a repertoire that includes      erentially orient towards the conspecific calling
three structurally distinct songs termed calling,         song, which they identify based on its fundamen-
courtship and aggression song, respectively               tal frequency and species-specific temporal param-
(Alexander 1961). The three songs serve distinct          eters (reviewed in Elsner & Popov 1978; Doherty
functions: calling song serves to attract females         & Hoy 1985). The prior identification of song
from afar, courtship song induces the female to           parameters required for behavioural effectiveness
mount the male, and aggression song is produced           has been invaluable in studies of the neuronal
during aggressive encounters with other males             mechanisms for calling song recognition
(Alexander 1962).                                         (Schildberger et al. 1989). Little is known about
   The evolutionary processes that have resulted in       the mechanisms, either behavioural or neuronal,
this repertoire of three songs are unknown, but it        of courtship song recognition in any cricket
is generally believed that song in crickets first         species.
evolved in the context of courtship (Alexander               The courtship behaviour of T. oceanicus, first
1962). The use of song for long-range attraction of       described in detail by Burk (1983), is very similar
females and in interactions with other males prob-        to that of other gryllid species (Alexander 1961;
ably evolved later. It is therefore interesting to        Loher & Dambach 1989; Adamo & Hoy 1994).
know whether the mechanisms by which calling              Briefly, courtship is initiated when the antennae of
                                                          the male contact the body of the female. The male
                                                          then vibrates his antennae, adopts a typical sing-
                                                          ing posture and begins to produce courtship song,
Correspondence: G. Pollack: Department of Biology,
McGill University, 1205 Dr Penfield Avenue,
                                                          simultaneously stroking the female with his anten-
Montreal, Quebec H3A 1B1, Canada (email:                  nae. He then turns away from her and presents his
gpollack@bio1.lan.mcgill.ca).                             abdomen, stridulating all the while. The female

0003–3472/96/020353+14 $12.00/0                      ?   1996 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour
                                                      353
354                                                            Animal Behaviour, 51, 2

                                      0

           Relative intensity (dB)
                                                                   (a)                                                         (b)
                                     –20

                                     –40

                                     –60

                                     –80

                                       0       6   12   18    24         30                     0           6   12   18   24     30
                                                                              Frequency (kHz)

                                           Chirp                                                    Trill

                                      200 ms

          (c)

                                                             30 ms
Figure 1. The courtship song of T. oceanicus. (c) The oscillogram shows the amplitude-modulated chirp and long,
constant-intensity trill. A portion of the trill is shown on an expanded time scale below. (a) Power spectrum of a
single (high-amplitude) chirp pulse. (b) Power spectrum of a single trill pulse. Both chirp and trill pulses have a
dominant peak at the fundamental frequency of 4–5 kHz and higher harmonics extending up to at least 30 kHz.
Points superimposed on the spectra are mean (&) values of the relative intensity levels and frequencies of the
harmonics. Data are based on analysis of at least 67 chirp and trill pulses in recordings from seven males.

reacts by following the male, approaching from                                     success in eliciting a mounting response from the
behind and touching his abdomen with her anten-                                    females. Mounting can be restored if courtship is
nae and palpi, whereupon the male spreads his                                      accompanied by playback of recorded courtship
hind wings and flattens his abdomen, allowing her                                  song.
to mount him. Mounting of the male by the                                             The courtship song of T. oceanicus (previously
female is a prerequisite for copulation.                                           described by Leroy 1966; Hutchings & Lewis
   The role of courtship song in T. oceanicus was                                  1984; Libersat et al. 1994) consists of a 7–10 pulse,
first examined by Burk (1983). From observations                                   amplitude-modulated chirp (Fig. 1), followed by a
of males under semi-natural conditions, he con-                                    long trill (2–4 s) of high pulse rate (33/s). Both
cluded that male mating success was correlated                                     chirp and trill are produced at a fundamental
with the production of courtship song. Muting                                      frequency of 4.5 kHz and contain higher harmon-
and playback experiments in different cricket                                      ics extending up to about 30 kHz. Libersat et al.
species support this conclusion (von Hörmann                                       (1994) demonstrated that the fundamental 4.5-
Heck 1957; Crankshaw 1979; Libersat et al. 1994).                                  kHz component of courtship song was both
Males that have been muted by removing their                                       necessary and sufficient to evoke normal female
tegmina perform all the acts of courtship nor-                                     mounting responses in T. oceanicus, and also
mally, including ‘singing’ (as seen by move-                                       showed that one of the higher frequency compo-
ments of their wing stubs), yet they have little                                   nents, 13.5 kHz, was not sufficient. In the present
Balakrishnan & Pollack: Cricket courtship song                          355

study, we confirmed and extended their findings       Spectral and Temporal Pattern Analysis
on the exclusive importance of the fundamental
                                                        Power spectra were computed from digitized
frequency. We also investigated the importance of
                                                      songs using the Fast Fourier Transform. Tem-
the temporal parameters of the song. The goal of
                                                      poral pattern analysis was performed either by
these studies was to provide insight into the
                                                      manually measuring pulse durations and pulse
mechanisms underlying recognition of courtship
                                                      periods from digitized recordings of songs, or by
song.
                                                      routing tape-recorded songs through a custom-
                                                      built circuit that converted each sound pulse to a
                  METHODS                             rectangular voltage signal (details of the circuit
                                                      can be obtained from the authors). The onsets and
Crickets                                              offsets of these rectangular pulses were detected
  Teleogryllus oceanicus males and females were       and stored on computer, and used to calculate
reared in the laboratory at 25–30)C in 66-litre       pulse durations and periods. The calculated
plastic containers on a diet of Purina cat chow       spectral and temporal parameters are reported as
(Adult Formula) and given water ad libitum.           mean values&.
Male and female nymphs were separated
before the final moult. Adult males and virgin
                                                      Acoustic Stimuli
females were kept separately in plastic boxes
(38#20#23 cm) on a 12:12 h light:dark cycle for          We delivered digitized sound pulses (12-bit
at least 2 weeks before use. Experiments were         resolution, 100-kHz update rate), either recorded
performed within 3–6 h of the onset of scoto-         or synthesized, at computer-controlled intervals.
phase. We used 2–3-week-old virgin females and        The signals were routed through a calibrated
2–4-week-old males in all experiments.                attenuator (either Hewlett Packard 350D or
                                                      Grason Stadler 1292) before being amplified
                                                      (Sanken S1-1050G) and broadcast through a
Song Recording and Analysis                           loudspeaker (Realistic 40-1310). Acoustic signals
   We recorded courtship, calling and aggression      were digitized and synthesized with a National
songs of 2–4-week-old T. oceanicus males in the       Instruments ATMIO16F5 multi-function board,
laboratory. To record courtship song, we intro-       resident in a 80486-based computer. Signal analy-
duced a male and a female into a cylindrical          sis and synthesis were done with programs written
(14#13 cm) cotton gauze cage. Recordings of           with LabWindows software.
calling song were obtained in the same manner,
but from isolated males. To record aggression
                                                      The Behavioural Assay
song, we placed two males (of which one was
muted by cutting off his forewings) in a smaller        Courtship assays were performed in a cylindri-
cylindrical cage (6#2.5 cm). All recordings were      cal, anechoic chamber (13#10 cm) made of min-
carried out in an anechoic chamber, in the dark, at   eral wool covered with cheesecloth. A ring of
a temperature of 24–26)C.                             cellulose acetate was pinned around the rim and
   We made recordings using a Brüel and Kjaer         projected about 1 cm into the chamber to prevent
condenser microphone (type 4135) and measuring        the crickets from escaping. Experiments were
amplifier (type 2610). The microphone was placed      performed in the dark, at 24–26)C. Illumination
at a height of 5–6 cm close to the wall of the cage   was provided by a 60-W red light bulb suspended
(because the male was free to move and turn in        about 25 cm above and to one side of the
any direction, the reported intensity levels are      chamber.
likely to be underestimates of the actual sound         We selected courting males by pairing candi-
intensity experienced by the female). The output      dates with receptive females. Typically, the
of the measuring amplifier was digitized (12-bit      pair attempted repeatedly to mate, but was not
resolution, 100-kHz sampling rate) and stored for     permitted to do so. Males that showed courtship
later analysis. Some songs were recorded on mag-      behaviour were muted by cutting off both fore-
netic tape (upper frequency limit: 20 kHz); these     wings proximal to the file. They were allowed to
were used only for analysis of temporal pattern.      recover for 10–15 min and then, to ensure a high
356                                      Animal Behaviour, 51, 2

level of motivation, they were ‘pre-treated’ by        differences in female response due to inter-male
being given access to a female. After 5–10 min of      variability because the same males were used in
vigorous courtship had been observed, we used          both control (playback of normal courtship song
the male for experimental trials. We introduced        or no playback) and test (playback of the test
the male into the chamber and, after he became         song) trials in a given experiment. We used a
calm, introduced the female, as far away from the      chi-squared test of independence with a table-wide
male as possible. Typically, the female was im-        significance level of 0.05 for each set of compari-
mobile for several seconds after being introduced      sons. The alpha errors were adjusted for multiple
into the arena, but quickly recovered and started      comparisons (within each figure in the Results)
exploring the chamber. Occasionally, the male          using the sequential Bonferroni procedure (Rice
approached the female before she had recovered.        1989).
We discarded these trials, as well as those in which
either cricket ran around in the chamber without
stopping. Males began to sing and court as soon        Song Patterns
as they encountered the female. In playback               The structures of the test songs were derived
experiments, each time the male attempted to sing      from the measured features of natural T.
a phrase of courtship song (as indicated by move-      oceanicus songs. Unless stated otherwise, all song
ments of the stumps of the forewings) a touch          patterns that lacked amplitude modulation were
of the computer keyboard by the experimenter           played back at 90 dB SPL (peak intensity). For
commanded the computer to play the test song           song patterns with amplitude modulation, ampli-
through a loudspeaker mounted directly above           tude varied from 87 to 100 dB SPL, to mimic the
the chamber at a height of 46 cm. The time from        natural range of modulation. Songs consisting of
the start of courtship ‘singing’ by the male to        short chirps alone had a duration of 0.65 s; the
mounting by the female was recorded. We termi-         duration of all other song patterns was 3.7 s unless
nated assays at 300 s in the absence of a mounting     indicated otherwise.
response. Two different types of ambiguous
responses sometimes occurred. (1) Females oc-
casionally mounted a male only partially, e.g. by      Normal courtship song
placing their forelegs on his abdomen without
                                                          The normal courtship song stimulus consisted
proceeding further. (2) Females sometimes repeat-
                                                       of a nine-pulse chirp produced using the recorded
edly, and rapidly, mounted and dismounted a
                                                       pulses of a single chirp from a single male, whose
male. We discarded these trials (approximately
                                                       spectral and temporal features had values close to
5–7% of the total). We used each female for only
                                                       the average (Figs 1, 7). The amplitude of each
one trial. We used each male for eight to 10 trials,
                                                       pulse was adjusted to match the average values
which always included the controls (playback of
                                                       shown in Fig. 7. The trill section of the song
normal courtship song and absence of playback)
                                                       consisted of a single representative trill pulse from
as well as one or, usually, two different test
                                                       the same male (peak intensity 90 dB SPL, dur-
songs.
                                                       ation 20 ms), iterated 100 times at periods of
   We used a sample size of 30 females for each
                                                       30.5 ms. Modified versions of courtship song were
song pattern tested. In addition, we included the
                                                       based on the foregoing parameters. Detailed
two controls in each day’s experiments. In each
                                                       explanations of their structures are presented in
case, we calculated the percentage of females that
                                                       the Results.
mounted the male within 60 s from the start of
courtship singing (the mounting frequency). There
were no significant differences in the mounting        Calling song (CALL)
frequencies to the controls in 10 different exper-
iments (no playback: ÷2 =1.353, N=235, P=0.852;          We constructed calling song using recorded
normal song: ÷2 =6.763, N=336, P=0.562). The           pulses of natural calling song. We used three pulse
mounting frequency for each test pattern, how-         types (fundamental frequency=4.7 kHz): (1) a
ever, was compared only to the controls in the         chirp pulse of 39 ms duration (modal value,
same experiment (N=30 for both control and             N=685 chirp pulses), (2) a pulse of 25 ms duration
experimental pattern). This procedure controls for     (modal value=26 ms, N=762 trill pulses) for the
Balakrishnan & Pollack: Cricket courtship song                           357

first pulse of the trill doublet and (3) a pulse of     mean duration of 34.0&6.1 ms (N=406 from
30 ms duration (modal value, N=762 trill pulses)        seven males) and the mean pulse period (period
for the second pulse of the trill doublet. A phrase     from the onset of one pulse to onset of the
consisted of five iterations of the chirp pulse (at     following pulse) is 73.8&6.1 ms (N=358 from
mean pulse period=63 ms, N=685) followed by             seven males). The trill consists of one to three
seven iterations (mean value, N=116) of the trill       trains of pulses produced at a mean intensity of
doublets. The intra-trill period was 35.5 ms (mean      91.5&1.3 dB SPL (N=30 from nine males). The
value, N=762) and the inter-trill period was            trill has a mean pulse duration of 17.9&2.8 ms
105.5 ms (mean value, N=645). The chirp–trill           (N=18 600 from seven males) and the mean pulse
period was 98 ms (mean value, N=110 phrases).           period is 29.0&4.8 ms (N=18 771 from seven
All the values reported here are based on data          males).
from the calling songs of seven males. The dur-            Spectral analysis of chirp and trill pulses
ation of the calling song was 1.23 s. To keep the       revealed that both have a dominant, fundamental
stimulus duration the same as that of the court-        frequency of 4–5 kHz (Fig. 1), and higher har-
ship song (3.7 s) during playback experiments,          monics extending up to at least 30 kHz. There are
the calling song stimulus was iterated three            minor spectral differences between the different
times. Calling song was played back at an in-           pulse types of the song (Fig. 1). Although these
tensity matched to that of courtship song, i.e.         spectral differences are statistically significant
with the trill at 90 dB SPL and the chirp at 92 dB      (analysis not shown) they are small, on the order
SPL.                                                    of 5–10 dB. Their biological relevance, if any, is
                                                        unclear.

Aggression song (AGG)
   We used recorded pulses of a single, represen-       Song is Necessary for Successful Courtship
tative phrase of aggression song (fundamental
frequency=4.4 kHz). To prevent confounding the             Males that were rendered mute by cutting off
results by the slight differences between the inten-    their forewings showed normal courtship behav-
sity profiles of courtship and aggression songs         iour, including ‘singing’, which was evident from
(Fig. 7), we played back the nine pulses of natural     movements of the wing stubs. Compared with
aggression song at the same intensity levels as         intact males, however, they had little success in
the courtship chirp (87–100 dB SPL). The pulse          eliciting a mounting response (Fig. 2a, b; Mann–
periods used were the calculated mean values for        Whitney U-test: U=3448.0, P
358                                                      Animal Behaviour, 51, 2

                                                                     Playback of courtship song restored the mounting
                                                              (a)
                                 N = 36                              frequency to nearly normal levels (Fig. 2; 78%
                                                                     versus 89%, ÷2 =2.337, P=0.126).
                      0.20

                                                                     Higher Harmonics are neither Necessary nor
                      0.15                                           Sufficient for Recognition
                                                                        To investigate whether the higher harmonics
                                                                     are necessary for recognition of courtship song,
                      0.05                                           we constructed two songs consisting of the funda-
                                                                     mental frequency alone. One of these (FILT: Fig.
                                                                     3) consisted of recorded chirp and trill pulses from
                                                                     which the higher harmonics had been filtered
                                                                     (Butterworth bandpass, 4.0–5.0 kHz, order 10).
                       0.6                                    (b)    The other (SYN: Fig. 3) was composed of syn-
                                 N = 235                             thetic 4.5-kHz pulses: each chirp pulse had a rise
                       0.5                                           time equal to 55% and a fall time equal to 42% of
                                                                     its duration (mean value, N=35 pulses from six
 Response frequency

                       0.4                                           males). Trill pulses (duration=19 ms) had rise and
                                                                     fall times of 11 and 7 ms, respectively (mean
                       0.3                                           values, N=39 pulses from six males). The tem-
                                                                     poral and amplitude-modulation features of both
                       0.2                                           songs were identical to the control, which was a
                                                                     phrase of normal courtship song (see Methods).
                       0.1                                           All three songs were played back at 90 dB SPL
                                                                     (trill). In playback experiments, the songs that
                                                                     lacked higher harmonics elicited normal levels of
                                                                     mounting (Fig. 3; ÷2 =0.131, P=0.718 for FILT;
                                                              (c)    ÷2 =1.364, P=0.243 for SYN). Thus, the higher
                                 N = 336
                                                                     harmonics are not necessary for recognition of
                      0.20
                                                                     courtship song.
                                                                        To determine whether the higher harmonics
                      0.15                                           were sufficient, we constructed a synthetic song
                                                                     that consisted of the higher harmonics alone
                                                                     (HAR: Fig. 3). The spectral features of the syn-
                      0.10                                           thetic chirp and trill pulses reflected those of
                                                                     natural song (Fig. 1), except for the absence of the
                      0.05                                           fundamental. The temporal features of HAR were
                                                                     similar to those of natural courtship song. Most of
                                                                     the amplitude differences in the natural song, both
                             0       60    120    180   240   300    within the chirp and between the chirp and trill,
                                            Time (s)                 are due to the fundamental component of the
                                                                     signal. When this component is removed by filter-
Figure 2. Mounting responses to male courtship with                  ing, all of the sound pulses are nearly equal in
and without song. Mounting response is the time elapsed
                                                                     amplitude, with peak intensities of 77 dB SPL.
from the start of courtship singing by the male to
                                                                     HAR was therefore played back at this intensity.
mounting by the female. (a) Response to normal, court-
ing males with intact wings. (b) Response to courtship of            This song was ineffective; response to the harmon-
muted males in the absence of playback. (c) Response to              ics alone was not significantly different from that
courtship of muted males when accompanied by play-                   to mute males in the absence of playback
back of normal courtship song at 90 dB SPL. The last                 (SILENT; Fig. 3; ÷2 =0.303, P=0.582). Thus, the
bar in (b) and (c) represents the frequency of females               higher harmonics are not sufficient for recognition
that did not mount within 300 s.                                     of courtship song.
Balakrishnan & Pollack: Cricket courtship song                                                                       359

                 100                                                                    No playback (SILENT)
                       Normal song          Test song                                   Normal song                           Filtered song (FILT)
                                            Silent                                      (4.5 kHz + harmonics)                 (4.5 kHz)
                                                                                    0                                     0
                                                                                                                        –20
                 80                                                               –20
                                                                                                                        –40
                                                                                  –40
                                                                                                                        –60
                                                                                  –60                                   –80

                                                        Relative intensity (dB)
                                                                                                                    –100
  Response (%)

                 60                                                               –80

                                                                                        0      6    12   18   24   30         0   6   12   18   24   30

                 40                                                                     Synthetic song (SYN)                  Harmonics alone (HAR)
                                                                                        (4.5 kHz) alone
                                                                                    0                                     0
                                                                                  –20                                   –20

                                                                                  –40                                   –40
                 20                                                                                                     –60
                                                                                  –60
                                                                                                                        –80
                                                                                  –80
                                                                                                                    –100
                                                                         –100                                       –120

                  0                                                                     0       6   12   18   24   30         0   6   12   18   24   30
                          FILT       SYN   HAR
Figure 3. The role of the higher harmonics in courtship song recognition. This and subsequent graphs show the
percentage of females that mounted mute, courting males within 60 s from the start of courtship singing. Each pair
of open and solid bars shows the percentage mounting to normal song and test pattern, respectively, in a given
experiment. Circles above the bars indicate response frequencies significantly different from those to playback of
normal song in the same experiment. N=30 in each case.

The Temporal Pattern of the Chirp is Sufficient to                                          poral pattern and not its amplitude modulation.
Evoke Normal Responses                                                                      We tested this further by presenting a song
   The effectiveness of the chirp and trill compo-                                          (CHRP-AM) that retained the temporal features
nents was tested by playing them back indepen-                                              of the natural chirp but lacked amplitude modu-
dently. The long natural trill, when presented                                              lation (Fig. 5). This non-amplitude-modulated
alone (TRILL), was not sufficient to restore                                                chirp (produced by nine iterations of a single,
the mounting response to normal levels (Fig. 4;                                             4.5 kHz, 35-ms chirp pulse at pulse periods of
÷2 =5.554, P=0.036). A song consisting of the                                               73.8 ms) was also sufficient to evoke normal
natural chirp alone (CHRP) however, was suf-                                                levels of mounting (Fig. 5; ÷2 =2.857, P=0.091).
ficient to evoke mounting responses that were                                               To confirm that the amplitude modulation of the
not significantly different from normal (Fig. 4;                                            chirp was not necessary in the context of the
÷2 =2.857, P=0.091).                                                                        normal, two-part song, we replaced the amplitude-
   The relatively high effectiveness of the chirp                                           modulated chirp of the normal song with a single,
compared to the trill component could be due                                                4.5-kHz chirp pulse (duration=35 ms), iterated
either to the characteristic temporal pattern of the                                        nine times (pulse period=73.8 ms) at constant
chirp or to its conspicuous amplitude modulation.                                           intensity (90 dB; i.e. the same intensity as the trill).
To test this, we constructed a song (CONT                                                   This constant-intensity song (NORM-AM) was as
CHRP: Fig. 5) that differed from the natural trill                                          effective as the normal song (Fig. 5; ÷2 =0.0,
only in temporal pattern. This song was produced                                            P=1.0), confirming that amplitude modulation is
by 50 iterations of a single, 4.5-kHz chirp pulse                                           not necessary for recognition of courtship song.
(duration=35 ms) at pulse periods of 73.8 ms. The                                              The relative ineffectiveness of the trill afforded
response to this stimulus (CONT CHRP) did not                                               us an opportunity to determine whether the
differ significantly from that to the normal song                                           amplitude modulation that occurs during the
(Fig. 5; ÷2 =0.317, P=0.573), suggesting that the                                           chirp, while not necessary, might nevertheless play
high effectiveness of the chirp is due to its tem-                                          a role in recognition. We imposed amplitude
360                                              Animal Behaviour, 51, 2

                           100
                                 Normal song     Test
                                                 song               Normal song

                           80
            Response (%)

                           60
                                                                   Chirp alone (CHRP)

                           40

                           20                                      Trill alone (TRILL)

                            0
                                      CHRP     TRILL
Figure 4. Effectiveness of the chirp and trill components in evoking the mounting response. N=30 in each case.

                        100
                                 Normal song   Test                Normal song
                                               song

                           80

                                                                   Normal song without
         Response (%)

                           60                                      amplitude modulation (NORM – AM)

                           40
                                                                   Continuous chirp
                                                                   (CONT CHRP)

                           20

                            0                                      Chirp without amplitude
                                  NORM – CONT CHRP –               modulation (CHRP – AM)
                                    AM   CHRP  AM

Figure 5. Relation between the conspicuous amplitude modulation and the effectiveness of the chirp. CONT CHRP
and CHRP-AM: Songs that retained the temporal pattern of the chirp but lacked amplitude modulation;
NORM-AM: a two-part song that lacked amplitude modulation. N=30 in each case.

modulation on the temporal pattern of the trill in           AMTR2, the modulation was applied over a time
two ways (Fig. 6). In the song AMTR1, the                    period similar to the duration of the natural chirp
modulation occurred over the first nine pulses of            (610 ms), by changing amplitude every second
the trill, as it normally does in the chirp. In              pulse for the first 18 pulses. In both cases, the
Balakrishnan & Pollack: Cricket courtship song                           361

                         100
                                                                 Normal song
                               Normal song     Test
                                               song
                          80
          Response (%)

                          60                                     Amplitude modulation over first
                                                                 9 pulses of trill (AMTR1)

                          40

                         20                                      Amplitude modulation of trill
                                                                 over duration of chirp (AMTR2)

                           0
                                  AMTR1      AMTR2
Figure 6. Relation between amplitude modulation and the effectiveness of the trill. A 13-dB amplitude modulation
(AM) was imposed on the temporal pattern of the natural trill (played back at 90 dB SPL). In the first case
(AMTR1), the AM was imposed over the first nine pulses of the trill (same number as the natural chirp). In the
second case (AMTR2), the AM was applied over 610 ms (to mimic the duration of the natural chirp). N=30 in each
case.

range of amplitude modulation was identical to               consists of a chirp (Fig. 7) of seven to 22 pulses
that of natural chirp (13 dB) and was achieved by            (11.2&3.0, N=93 from 12 males).
adjusting the peak-to-peak amplitudes of nine trill             The chirps of calling, courtship and aggression
pulses to the mean intensity levels of successive            song are similar in their spectral features (Leroy
pulses in natural chirps. AMTR1 restored the                 1966; Loher & Rence 1978; Hutchings & Lewis
response to a level not significantly different from         1984). All have a dominant, fundamental fre-
normal (Fig. 6; ÷2 =2.7, P=0.1), showing that                quency of 4–5 kHz, with higher harmonics extend-
amplitude modulation can enhance the effective-              ing up to 30 kHz (Fig. 7). The second harmonic is
ness of a temporal pattern that is less attractive           at least 20 dB less intense than the fundamental in
than that of the chirp. AMTR2, however, did not              all three chirps, and the higher harmonics are even
restore the response to normal levels (Fig. 6;               less intense. Detailed statistical analysis (not
÷2 =6.787, P=0.018). In fact, the response to                shown) revealed some minor (5–10 dB) but statis-
AMTR2 was almost identical to that evoked by                 tically significant differences between the mean
the trill without amplitude modulation (40%                  relative intensity levels of some of the harmonics.
versus 43%: see Fig. 4).                                        The aggression song is very similar to the
                                                             courtship chirp in pulse number (mode=9 in both
                                                             cases), pulse durations and pulse periods (Fig. 7)
Calling and Aggression Songs can also Elicit
                                                             and in the presence of conspicuous amplitude
Normal Mounting Responses
                                                             modulation. The calling chirp has fewer pulses,
   Like the courtship song, the calling song of              lacks amplitude modulation and has a shorter
T. oceanicus (Leroy 1966; Bentley & Hoy 1972;                mean pulse period, but is similar in terms of pulse
Hill et al. 1972) also consists of two parts: a short        duration (Fig. 7).
chirp of four to eight pulses (X&=5.9&1.0,                    On the basis of the structural similarities of the
N=116 from seven males) followed by a complex                three chirps described above, we predicted that
trill that consists of six to 10 pulse doublets              aggression and calling song should also elicit
(6.9&1.1, N=116 from seven males). The aggres-               responses in the context of courtship. To test this
sion song (Leroy 1966; Hutchings & Lewis 1984)               prediction, we performed playback experiments
362                                                                     Animal Behaviour, 51, 2

                    (a)
                                     Courtship                                     Calling                                          Aggression

                    (b)
                           0
   Intensity (dB)

                      –20

                      –40

                      –60

                      –80
                           0         6   12   18   24   30 0                 6      12 18 24         30                 0     6      12   18     24   30
                                                                                   Frequency (dB)
                      110                                               50                                              100
                               (c)                                           (d)                                              (e)
                                                                                                                        90
                                                                        40
                                                        Duration (ms)
   Intensity (dB)

                                                                                                          Period (ms)
                      100
                                                                                                                         80
                                                                        30
                                                                                        Courtship                        70
                          90
                                                                        20              Aggression
                                                                                                                        60
                                                                                        Calling
                          80                                            10                                              50
                               1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9                             1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9                                1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
                               Chirp pulse number                            Chirp pulse number                               Pulse period number
Figure 7. Structure of courtship, calling and aggression chirps. (a) Oscillograms of representative chirps of courtship,
calling and aggression songs. (b) Power spectra of single courtship, calling and aggression chirp pulses. Data points
in the spectra represent means (&s) of the relative intensity levels and frequencies of the harmonics. Data are
based on analysis of 67 (courtship), 21 (calling) and 71 (aggression) chirp pulses from seven males in each case. (c)
Graph shows the mean (+) intensity level (dB SPL) of each successive pulse of courtship (N=9), calling (N§17)
and aggression (N=10) chirps. (d) Graph shows the mean (+) duration of successive pulses in courtship (N§24),
calling (N§77 for pulses 1–6, 29 for pulse 7) and aggression (N§17) chirps. (e) Graph shows mean (+) pulse
period for successive pulses in courtship (N§24), calling (N§79 for periods 1–5, 29 for period 6) and aggression
(N§17) chirps. Recordings were obtained from seven males.

with recorded calling and aggression songs. Nor-                                       Burk (1983) observed that T. oceanicus females
mal mounting responses (Fig. 8) were evoked by                                         would not mount males that did not produce
both aggression (÷2 =1.920, P=0.166) and calling                                       courtship song. However, some studies on Acheta
songs (÷2 =0.098, P=0.754).                                                            (Ghouri & McFarlane 1957) and T. commodus
                                                                                       (Loher & Rence 1978) did not reveal a significant
                                     DISCUSSION                                        decrease in male mating success as a result of
                                                                                       muting males or deafening females. Differences in
Role of Song in Courtship                                                              results could be due to differences in behavioural
   Our results agree with earlier work on field                                        paradigms, including factors such as duration of
crickets that demonstrated the importance of song                                      the assay, arena size, age and prior exposure of
as a courtship signal. Muted males had little                                          females to courting males (Adamo & Hoy 1994).
success in eliciting a mounting response from                                          Muting experiments in the field have confirmed
females (von Hormann-Heck 1957; Crankshaw                                              the importance of song for male mating success in
1979; Adamo & Hoy 1994; Libersat et al. 1994).                                         grasshoppers (Kriegbaum & von Helversen 1992)
Balakrishnan & Pollack: Cricket courtship song                           363

                           100
                                 Courtship song         Test
                                                        song        Courtship song

                           80
            Response (%)

                           60

                                                                    Calling song (CALL)

                           40

                           20                                       Aggression song (AGG)

                             0
                                      CALL        AGG
Figure 8. Response to calling and aggression songs in the context of courtship. N=30 in each case.

and sagebrush crickets (Snedden & Sakaluk 1992).               when the male was not singing, so that their
Acoustic signalling as a necessity for successful              females were exposed to much greater levels of
courtship is well documented for the hissing                   acoustic stimulation than in our case. Also,
cockroach, Gromphadorhina portentosa (Nelson                   Libersat et al. used a different experimental design
& Fraser 1980), the tree cricket, Oecanthus                    that involved repeated testing of individual pairs
nigricornis (Bell 1980) and several species of                 of crickets.
Drosophila (Ewing 1989; Bixler et al. 1992;                       We found that the chirp component of court-
Liimatainen et al. 1992). In most cases, however,              ship song was sufficient to evoke normal mounting
muting did not completely abolish male mating                  responses, the trill being dispensable as an acoustic
success, showing that song is not an absolute                  cue. Our results indicate that the mechanism for
requirement for successful courtship.                          courtship song recognition in T. oceanicus is
                                                               temporal pattern-dependent. Temporal pattern is
                                                               the major cue for recognition in many signalling
The Necessary Acoustic Parameters of
                                                               systems; for example, calling song recognition in
Courtship Song
                                                               orthopterans (Elsner & Popov 1978; Ewing 1989)
   Our experiments indicate that the two necessary             and recognition of the vibrational mating songs
acoustic parameters for an effective courtship                 of planthoppers, Nilaparvata lugens (Claridge
song are the fundamental frequency (4.5 kHz) and               et al. 1984) and green lacewings, Chrysoperla
the temporal pattern of the chirp. Our results                 plorabunda (Wells & Henry 1992). Temporal pat-
agree with those of Libersat et al. (1994), who                tern is also important for recognition of courtship
demonstrated that the fundamental frequency of                 song in D. melanogaster (Bennett-Clark & Ewing
T. oceanicus courtship song is both necessary and              1969; Kyriacou & Hall 1982), and is the major cue
sufficient to evoke normal mounting responses.                 in the vibratory courtship signals of the wandering
In contrast to their results, however, we found                spider, Cupiennius salei (Schüch & Barth 1990).
that the courtship trill presented on its own was              Neurons that show selectivity for temporal pattern
only partially effective and not sufficient to elicit          exist in the central nervous systems of a variety of
normal levels of mounting. The discrepancy in                  organisms (Rose 1986). Bandpass filter neurons
our results could be due to differences in the                 that are tuned to the temporal pattern of calling
paradigms used: their assays were of longer dur-               song have been identified in the brain of the
ation (5 min versus 1 min) and their stimuli were              field cricket, Gryllus bimaculatus (Schildberger
played continuously during this period, even                   1984).
364                                      Animal Behaviour, 51, 2

The Role of Amplitude Modulation                        she is distant from the male, but by mounting the
                                                        male if the two have come into contact. Similarly,
   The conspicuous amplitude modulation of the          chirps help males to gain and maintain social
chirp was not necessary for its function. When          dominance or territory (Nolen et al. 1992) when
amplitude modulation was imposed over the first         produced during close-range interactions with
nine pulses of the trill temporal pattern, however,     other males, but evoke mounting responses if
it enhanced the effectiveness of the trill so that      the recipient is female. This dependence of the
mounting was restored to nearly normal levels.          response on factors extrinsic to the signal, such as
This result implies that the amplitude modulation       sex, context and additional sensory inputs, was
present in courtship song can be evaluated by the       foreseen more than 30 years ago by Alexander
nervous system, but its effect is redundant when        (1962).
the temporal pattern is highly attractive. Our             In T. oceanicus, then, we speculate that a single
results with the second amplitude-modulated trill       acoustic signal, the chirp, detected by a single
pattern (AMTR2) indicate that the parameter that        neuronal recognizer, has evolved diverse func-
is used to evaluate amplitude modulation is the         tions: long-range mate attraction during calling,
number of pulses rather than the duration over          enhancing the probability of female mounting
which it occurs.                                        during courtship, and determining male social
   There is evidence for the use of amplitude           status.
modulation for song recognition in some orthop-
terans; in the recognition of calling song in the
cricket, Melanogryllus desertus (Elsner & Popov         The Role of the Trill Components
1978), and the grasshopper, Chorthippus biguttulus         Why do calling and courtship songs contain trill
(von Helversen & von Helversen 1983).                   components if their chirps are sufficient to attract
                                                        mates and elicit female mounting? One possibility
A Single ‘Chirp Recognizer?’                            is that the chirp and trill serve different functions:
                                                        the different components of a complex signal may
   Calling and courtship song recognition in            be directed to different receivers, as in the case
T. oceanicus share striking similarities. Both songs    of the frog, Eleutherodactylus coqui (Narins &
are complex, consisting of chirp and trill compo-       Capranica 1976). Pollack (1982) showed that, in
nents. In both cases, the song parameters that are      flight phonotaxis assays, T. oceanicus males sig-
important to evoke normal responses (female             nificantly preferred the temporal pattern of the
phonotaxis to calling song or female mounting in        calling trill to that of the chirp, raising the possi-
response to courtship song) are the fundamental         bility that the calling trill is involved in male–male
frequency of 4–5 kHz and the temporal pattern of        interactions. Evidence suggests that T. oceanicus
the chirp (Moiseff et al. 1978; Pollack & Hoy           males form aggregates in the wild (Cade 1981),
1981). In calling song, as in courtship song, the       but the possible role of the calling trill in this
chirp temporal pattern is sufficient to elicit normal   behaviour has not been tested.
responses (Pollack & Hoy 1981). The chirps of              Although our experiments indicate that the
calling, courtship and aggression songs are very        courtship trill is dispensable as an acoustic cue, we
similar in structure and all three songs were           believe that it plays an important role during
equally effective in eliciting mounting responses in    courtship for two major reasons. (1) Males pro-
the context of courtship. This similarity suggests      gressively increase the length of their trills as the
that a single neuronal filter (‘chirp recognizer’)      courtship bout progresses (Libersat et al. 1994;
may mediate the responses to calling, courtship         personal observations), even though this is likely
and aggressive chirps.                                  to be energetically expensive (Prestwich & Walker
   If the above speculation is correct, then the        1981). (2) Burk (1983) observed that females did
different behavioural responses to the three songs      not mount males that produced only chirps. In
may depend not primarily on their mediation by          our experiments, the chirp alone was very effective
different signal recognizers but rather on factors      in eliciting mounting. We believe that the reason
such as the sex of the recipient and on behavioural     for the discrepancy in the two observations is that
context. For example, a receptive female responds       in our experiments with playback of the chirp
to the chirp pattern by performing phonotaxis if        alone, the muted male was still performing the
Balakrishnan & Pollack: Cricket courtship song                                    365

movements associated with trilling. Stridulatory              Sexual Competition in a Diverse Group of Insects (Ed.
movements generate vibrations that could be                   by D. T. Gwynne & G. K. Morris), pp. 97–119.
                                                              Boulder, Colorado: Westview Press.
perceived by the female either through the sub-             Cade, W. H. 1981. Field cricket spacing, and the
strate or through direct body contact during                  phonotaxis of crickets and parasitoid flies to clumped
courtship (Gogala 1985). We speculate that,                   and isolated cricket songs. Z. Tierpsychol., 55, 365–
during courtship, two important signals are nec-              375.
essary to evoke normal mounting: an acoustic                Claridge, M. F., den Hollander, J. & Morgan, J. C.
                                                              1984. Specificity of acoustic signals and mate choice in
component provided primarily by the courtship                 the brown planthopper Nilaparvata lugens. Entomol.
chirp, and a non-acoustic component (perhaps                  exp. appl., 35, 221–226.
vibration) provided largely by the trill.                   Crankshaw, O. S. 1979. Female choice in relation to
   The observed dispensability of the acoustic                calling and courtship songs in Acheta domesticus.
                                                              Anim. Behav., 27, 1274–1275.
component of the trill in our experiments could             Doherty, J. & Hoy, R. 1985. Communication in insects.
also be ascribed to an age-dependent loss of                  III. The auditory behavior of crickets: some views of
selectivity in T. oceanicus females. We used 2–3-             genetic coupling, song recognition, and predator
week-old females in all our experiments: younger              detection. Q. Rev. Biol., 60, 457–472.
females may be more selective (Walikonis et al.             Elsner, N. & Popov, A. V. 1978. Neuroethology of
                                                              acoustic communication. Adv. Insect Physiol., 13,
1991) and may require the acoustic component of               229–355.
the trill for normal mounting.                              Ewing, A. W. 1989. Arthropod Bioacoustics; Neuro-
                                                              biology and Behavior. Ithaca, New York: Cornell
                                                              University Press.
           ACKNOWLEDGMENTS                                  Ghouri, A. S. K. & McFarlane, J. E. 1957. Reproductive
                                                              isolation in the house cricket (Orthoptera: Gryllidae).
We thank two annonymous referees whose com-                   Psyche, 64, 30–36.
                                                            Gogala, M. 1985. Vibrational communication in insects
ments considerably improved the manuscript.                   (biophysical and behavioural aspects). In: Acoustic
This work was supported by the Natural Sciences               and Vibrational Communication in Insects (Ed. by K.
and Engineering Research Council of Canada.                   Kalmring & N. Elsner), pp. 117–126. Berlin: Verlag
                                                              Paul Parey.
                                                            von Helversen, D. & von Helversen, O. 1983. Species
                                                              recognition and acoustic localization in acridid grass-
                  REFERENCES                                  hoppers: a behavioral approach. In: Neuroethology
                                                              and Behavioral Physiology (Ed. by F. Huber & H.
Adamo, S. A. & Hoy, R. R. 1994. Mating behaviour of           Markl), pp. 95–107. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
  the field cricket Gryllus bimaculatus and its depend-     Hill, K. G., Loftus-Hills, J. J. & Gartside, D. F. 1972.
  ence on social and environmental cues. Anim. Behav.,        Premating isolation between the Australian field
  47, 857–868.                                                crickets Teleogryllus commodus and T. oceanicus
Alexander, R. D. 1961. Aggressiveness, territoriality,        (Orthoptera: Gryllidae). Aust. J. Zool., 20, 153–
  and sexual behavior in field crickets (Orthoptera:          163.
  Gryllidae). Behaviour, 17, 130–223.                       von Hörmann-Heck, S. 1957. Untersuchungen über den
Alexander, R. D. 1962. Evolutionary change in cricket         Erbgang einiger Verhaltenwiesen bei Grillenbastarden
  acoustical communication. Evolution, 16, 443–467.           (Gryllus campestris L.2Gryllus bimaculatus DeGeer).
Bell, P. D. 1980. Multimodal communication by the             Z. Tierpsychol., 14, 137–183.
  black-horned tree cricket, Oecanthus nigricornis          Hutchings, M. & Lewis, B. 1984. The role of two-tone
  (Walker) (Orthoptera: Gryllidae). Can. J. Zool., 58,        suppression in song coding by ventral cord neurones
  1861–1868.                                                  in the cricket Teleogryllus oceanicus (Le Guillou).
Bennett-Clark, H. C. & Ewing, A. W. 1969. Pulse               J. comp. Physiol. A, 154, 103–112.
  interval as a critical parameter in the courtship song    Kriegbaum, H. & von Helversen, O. 1992. Influence of
  of Drosophila melanogaster. Anim. Behav., 17, 755–          male songs on female mating behavior in the grass-
  759.                                                        hopper Chorthippus biguttulus (Orthoptera: Acridi-
Bentley, D. R. & Hoy, R. R. 1972. Genetic control of          dae). Ethology, 91, 248–254.
  the neuronal network generating cricket (Teleogryllus     Kyriacou, C. P. & Hall, J. C. 1982. The function of
  Gryllus) song patterns. Anim. Behav., 20, 478–492.          courtship song rhythms in Drosophila. Anim. Behav.,
Bixler, A., Jenkins, J. B., Tompkins, L. & McRobert,          30, 794–801.
  S. P. 1992. Identification of acoustic stimuli that me-   Leroy, Y. 1966. Signaux acoustiques, comportement
  diate sexual behavior in Drosophila busckii (Diptera:       et systématique de quelque espèces de Gryllides-
  Drosophilidae). J. Insect Behav., 5, 469–478.               (Orthoptères, Ensifères). Bull. biol. Fr. Belg., 100,
Burk, T. 1983. Male aggression and female choice in a         1–134.
  field cricket (Teleogryllus oceanicus): the importance    Libersat, F., Murray, J. A. & Hoy, R. R. 1994. Fre-
  of courtship song. In: Orthopteran Mating Systems:          quency as a releaser in the courtship song of two
366                                           Animal Behaviour, 51, 2

  crickets, Gryllus bimaculatus (deGeer) and Teleogryl-       Prestwich, K. N. & Walker, T. J. 1981. Energetics of
  lus oceanicus: a neuroethological analysis. J. comp.          singing in crickets: effect of temperature in three
  Physiol. A, 174, 485–494.                                     trilling species (Orthoptera: Gryllidae). J. comp.
Liimatainen, J., Hoikkala, A., Aspi, J. & Welbergen, P.         Physiol., 143, 199–212.
  1992. Courtship in Drosophila montana: the effects of       Rice, W. R. 1989. Analyzing tables of statistical tests.
  male auditory signals on the behaviour of flies. Anim.        Evolution, 43, 223–225.
  Behav., 43, 35–48.                                          Rose, G. 1986. A temporal-processing mechanism for all
Loher, W. & Dambach, M. 1989. Reproductive behav-               species? Brain Behav. Evol., 28, 134–144.
  ior. In: Cricket Behavior and Neurobiology (Ed. by F.       Schildberger, K. 1984. Temporal selectivity of identified
  Huber, T. E. Moore & W. Loher), pp. 43–82. Ithaca,            auditory neurons in the cricket brain. J. comp.
  New York: Cornell University Press.                           Physiol., 155, 171–185.
Loher, W. & Rence, B. 1978. The mating behavior of            Schildberger, K., Huber, F. & Wohlers, D. W. 1989.
  Teleogryllus commodus (Walker) and its central and            Central auditory pathway: neuronal correlates of
  peripheral control. Z. Tierpsychol., 46, 225–259.             phonotactic behavior. In: Cricket Behavior and
Moiseff, A., Pollack, G. S. & Hoy, R. R. 1978. Steering         Neurobiology (Ed. by F. Huber, T. E. Moore & W.
  responses of flying crickets to sound and ultrasound:
                                                                Loher), pp. 423–459. Ithaca, New York: Cornell
  mate attraction and predator avoidance. Proc. natn.           University Press.
  Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 75, 4052–4056.
Narins, P. M. & Capranica, R. R. 1976. Sexual differ-         Schüch, W. & Barth, F. G. 1990. Vibratory communi-
  ences in the auditory system of the tree frog Eleuthero-      cation in a spider: female responses to synthetic male
  dactylus coqui. Science, 192, 378–380.                        vibrations. J. comp. Physiol. A, 166, 817–826.
Nelson, M. C. & Fraser, J. 1980. Sound production in          Snedden, W. A. & Sakaluk, S. K. 1992. Acoustic sig-
  the cockroach, Gromphadorhina portentosa: evidence            nalling and its relation to male mating success in
  for communication by hissing. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.,        sagebrush crickets. Anim. Behav., 44, 633–639.
  6, 305–314.                                                 Walikonis, R., Schoun, D., Zacharias, D., Henley, J.,
Nolen, T. C., Lam, C., Wong, J., Luayon, L. & Luayon,           Coburn, P. & Stout, J. 1991. Attractiveness of the
  J. 1992. High frequency components in the rivalry             male Acheta domesticus calling song to females. III.
  song of territorial male crickets: multiple functions for     The relation of age-correlated changes in syllable
  aversion to ultrasound. Proc. Int. Congr. Neuroethol.,        period recognition and phonotactic threshold to
  3.                                                            juvenile hormone III biosynthesis. J. comp. Physiol. A,
Pollack, G. S. 1982. Sexual differences in cricket calling      169, 751–764.
  song recognition. J. comp. Physiol., 146, 217–221.          Wells, M. M. & Henry, C. S. 1992. Behavioural
Pollack, G. S. & Hoy, R. R. 1981. Phonotaxis to                 responses of green lacewings (Neuroptera: Chrysopi-
  individual rhythmic components of a complex cricket           dae: Chrysoperla) to synthetic mating songs. Anim.
  calling song. J. comp. Physiol., 144, 367–373.                Behav., 44, 641–652.
You can also read