RESEARCH LINES 2020-2021 - Master's Program in Behavior and Cognition Faculty of Psychology University of Barcelona - Universitat de Barcelona
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
RESEARCH LINES 2020-2021 Master’s Program in Behavior and Cognition Faculty of Psychology University of Barcelona 1
Stimuli Representations in Perceptual Learning Process Antonio A. Artigas Summary Mere exposure to more than one stimulus is known to trigger learning processes that modify their discriminability. In particular, intermixed exposure to two similar stimuli sharing common elements, AX and BX, where A and B refer to the unique elements of each stimulus and X to their common features, has been shown to result in improved discrimination—comparison made to a group in which AX and BX are exposed in separate blocks of trials (e.g., Symonds & Hall, 1995). This intermixed/blocked effect, an instance of perceptual learning, has been intensively researched and several mechanisms have been shown to contribute to the enhanced discriminability between AX and BX after intermixed pre-exposure (for a review, see Mitchell & Hall, 2014). Recently our research has produced an alternative approach to explain this Perceptual Learning phenomenon: the “differential representation hypothesis” (see Artigas & Prados, 2014; 2017). Right now our interest is to develop this approach systematically using a range of laboratory techniques both with non-humans animals (rats) and human participants. Keywords: perceptual learning; stimulus representation; salience modulation. Selected publications: Artigas, A. A., & Prados, J. (2014) Percpetual learning tranfer: salience of the common element as factor contributing to the intermixed/blocked effect. Journal experimental of psychology: Animal learning and cognition, 40(4), 419-424. Mitchell, C., & Hall, G. (2014). Can theories of animal discrimination explain perceptual learning in humans? Psychological Bulletin, 140, 283–307. 2
Violence Risk Assessment and Prediction Antonio Andrés-Pueyo (GEAV) Summary The recent advances in the psychological assessment techniques applied to the practice of criminology and criminal psychology have developed new and succeful techniques for violence risk assessment. First developed in the context of forensic psychiatry have become essential to the professional task in numerous people who work in legal and criminal law enforcement services or social and health services to prevent violence. Our research group, between criminology and psychology, the GEAV (www.ub.edu/geav) has specialized in the development and adaptation of risk assessment protocols to the spanish countries leading to violence. The GEAV has been adapted the HCR-20, SVR-20 and SARA RSVP and has also created new protocols, as the RVD-BCN and ETAPA (among others) for application in professional contexts. At present we are working with new protocols to assess the risk of harassment of women and violence against parents. These protocols allow assessing the risk of sexual violence, against women, against parents and that violence caused by people with serious criminal records or mental disorders. Our work is done in collaboration with various community services (correctional services, NGOs, police, penal, psychiatric hospitals, etc ..) that allow our research is conducted to address real problems of the first order. We also have a laboratory for the development of the experimental and quantitative studies (simulation) related to the technical problem of violence risk assessment. Keywords: Violence risk assessment. Criminal recidivism. Actuarial vs. Clinical prediction. Selected publications: Andrés-Pueyo, A., Arbach-Lucioni, K., & Redondo, S. (2018). The RisCanvi: a new tool for assessing risk for violence in prison and recidivism. Recidivism Risk Assessment: A Handbook for Practitioners, 255-268. Arbach-Lucioni, Karin, Marian Martinez-García, and Antonio Andrés-Pueyo. "Risk Factors for Violent Behavior in Prison Inmates A Cross-Cultural Contribution." Criminal Justice and Behavior 39.9 (2012): 1219-1239. Gallardo-Pujol, D., Andrés-Pueyo, A., & Maydeu-Olivares, A. (2013). MAOA genotype, social exclusion and aggression: an experimental test of a gene--environment interaction. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 12(1), 140–145. Gallardo-Pujol, D., Penelo, E., Sit, C., Jornet-Gibert, M., Suso, C., Buades-Rotger, M., ... & Bryant, F. B. (2019). The meaning of aggression varies across culture: testing the measurement invariance of the refined aggression questionnaire in samples from Spain, the United States, and Hong Kong. Journal of personality assessment, 1-6. López-Ossorio, J. J., González-Álvarez, J. L., Vicente, J. M. M., Cortés, C. U., & Andrés-Pueyo, A. (2019). Validation and Calibration of the Spanish Police Intimate Partner Violence Risk Assessment System (VioGén). Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 1-11. Martínez, V. C., & Andrés-Pueyo, A. (2015). The Spanish version of the Criminal Sentiment Scale Modified (CSS- M): Factor structure, reliability, and validity. The European Journal of Psychology Applied to Legal Context, 7(2), 67-72. 3
Cognitive basis of unwarranted beliefs Itxaso Barberia Javier Rodríguez-Ferreiro Summary The term “epistemologically unwarranted beliefs” refers to beliefs that remain in the absence of empirical evidence to support them, such as paranormal or pseudoscientific beliefs. These phenomena negatively impact in areas of great importance for our society such as health, education or political debate. Previous studies have linked the presence of epistemologically unwarranted beliefs to certain cognitive aspects. Knowing the cognitive basis of this type of belief is the basis for the design of effective intervention strategies specifically aimed at its reduction. Our current work, developed in the context of the project PID2019-106102GB-I00 (Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovación), focuses on different research lines: [1] The development of tools specifically designed to assess different types of unwarranted beliefs, as well as the study of their relationship with other related constructs, such as analytical thinking ability or probabilistic reasoning, among others. [2] The study of the possible relationship between the tendency to generate false memories and the presence of pseudoscientific beliefs. [3] The analysis of the relationship between causal illusion and the presence of pseudoscientific beliefs. [4] The assessment of the possible influence of lexical complexity on credulity, in relation to unwarranted beliefs. We conduct both offline and online behavioral experiments that include contingency learning tasks, psycholinguistic paradigms, false memory studies, and reasoning problems, as well as questionnaires measuring endorsement to pseudoscience and other unwarranted beliefs. Keywords: cognitive bias, pseudoscience, epistemologically unwarranted beliefs, pseudoscientific beliefs, causal illusion, false memory, contingency learning, causal learning Selected publications: Barberia, I., Vadillo, M. A., & Rodríguez-Ferreiro, J. (2019). Persistence of Causal Illusions After Extensive Training. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 24. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00024 Blanco, F., Barberia, I., & Matute, H. (2015). Individuals who believe in the paranormal expose themselves to biased information and develop more causal illusions than nonbelievers in the laboratory. PLoS ONE, 10(7), e0131378. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0131378 Rodríguez-Ferreiro, J., Aguilera, M., & Davies, R. (2020). Positive schizotypy increases the acceptance of unpresented materials in false memory tasks in non-clinical individuals. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 262. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00262 Torres, M. N., Barberia, I., & Rodríguez‐Ferreiro, J. (2020). Causal illusion as a cognitive basis of pseudoscientific beliefs. British Journal of Psychology, bjop.12441. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12441 4
A.P.A.L. Group (Attention, Perception and Acquisition of Language) Laura Bosch Ferran Pons Summary Our research is focused on speech perception abilities, word learning, attention development and early language acquisition processes, both in normally developing infants (monolingual and bilingual) and in infants at risk for language and neurocognitive disorders. We are also interested in Second Language Acquisition (SLA), both in children and adults. Our methodological approach is mainly behavioral, with procedures that rely on attention, visual fixation and orientation latency or reaction time measures. Classical habituation and familiarization-preference paradigms in infancy research are used, with settings that rely on video (offline coding) and eye-tracking recordings. Measures from standardized infant developmental scales and parental reports on expressive and receptive language and social behavior are also used as tools in our research. Collaborative research with A. Rodriguez-Fornells group has extended our approach to include EEG/ERP methodologies applied to infants and young children. Language differentiation processes, audiovisual speech perception, phonetic categorization of native and non-native speech sounds, the beginning of word segmentation, phonological encoding, word recognition and word learning skills are ongoing areas of research in our infant Lab (UB - Hospital Sant Joan de Déu). Research involving children and adult populations can be undertaken at schools and at the UB Lab in Psychology. Keywords: infant speech, perception, attention, word learning, SLA Selected publications: Birulés, J., Bosch, L., Brieke, R., Pons, F., & Lewkowicz, D. J. (2019). Inside bilingualism: Language background modulates selective attention to a talker's mouth. Developmental science, 22(3), e12755. François, C., Teixidó, M., Takerkart, S., Agut, T., Bosch, L. & Rodríguez-Fornells, A. (2017). Enhanced neonatal brain responses to sung streams predict vocabulary outcomes by age 18 months. Scientific Reports, 7:12451. DOI.10.1038/s41598-017-12798-2 Martinez-Alvarez, A., Pons, F., & de Diego-Balaguer, R. (2017). Endogenous temporal attention in the absence of stimulus-driven cues emerges in the second year of life. PloS one, 12(9), e0184698. Pons, F., Bosch, L., & Lewkowicz, D. J. (2015). Bilingualism modulates infants’ selective attention to the mouth of a talking face. Psychological science, 0956797614568320. Bosch, L., & Ramon-Casas, M. (2014). First translation equivalents in bilingual toddlers’ expressive vocabulary. Does form similarity matter? International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38(4), 317-322. Bosch, L. (2011). Precursors to language in preterm infants: speech perception abilities in the first year of life. Progress in brain research, 189, 239-257. 5
Delineating fronto-striatal circuits in Huntington’s disease: a multimodal neuroimaging approach Estela Cámara Summary Converging data support the idea that the striatum forms a hub and integrates motor, cognitive control, and motivational circuits, which partially overlap and interact one with each other. There are individual differences in the degree of damage of these circuits in Huntington’s disease (HD), a genetic neurodegenerative disorder that manifests in mid-adulthood. The neurodegeneration in HD begins subcortically in the striatum and gradually extends to the cortex, leading to a progression of characteristic motor, cognitive and psychiatric symptoms. There is a high degree of phenotypic heterogeneity regarding the prominence of each type of symptom. To investigate the relationship between the different degree of neurodegeneration in each circuit and the clinical symptoms we used a combination of different magnetic resonance imaging modalities (functional and microstrucural), as well as a battery of questionnaires and neurological, neuropsychological and psychiatric assessments. Specifically, we are interested in investigating sources of individual differences in each symptom prominence by the delineation of the functional specialization of the striatum and their relation with specific cortico-striatal pathways in HD patients and controls. Keywords: Huntigton’s disease, individual differences, basal ganglia, neuroimaging, fMRI, DTI Selected publications: García-Gorro, C., de Diego-Balaguer, R., Martínez-Horta, S., Pérez-Pérez, J., Kulisevsky, J., Rodríguez-Dechichá, N., Vaquer, I., Subira, S., Calopa, M., Muñoz, E., Santacruz, P., Ruiz-Idiago, J., Mareca, C., Caballol, N., Càmara, E. (2018). Reduced striato-cortical and inhibitory transcallosal connectivity in the motor circuit of Huntington’s disease patients. Human Brain Mapping. 39(1):54-71 Hinzen, W., Rosselló, J., Morey, C., Càmara, E., García-Gorro, C., Salvador, R., de Diego-Balaguer, R. (2018). A systematic linguistic profile of spontaneous narrative speech in pre-symptomatic and early stage Huntington’s disease. Cortex. 100:71-83 García-Gorro, C., Càmara, E., de Diego-Balaguer, R. (2017). Neuroimaging as a tool to study the sources of phenotypic heterogeneity in Huntington’s disease. Current Opinion in Neurology. 30(4):398-404 Garcia-Gorro, C., Garau-Rolandi, M., Escrichs, A., Rodriguez-Dechicha, N., Vaquer, I., Subira, S., ... & Càmara, E. (2019). An active cognitive lifestyle as a potential neuroprotective factor in Huntington's disease. Neuropsychologia, 122, 116-124. 6
Cognition, Welfare and Conservation Montse Colell(UB) Mercedes Mayo(UB) Álvaro López Caicoya(UB) Jordi Galbany (UB) Maria Teresa Abelló (Zoo Barcelona) Summary We are interested in studying social behavior and physical/social cognition in different species, from primates (great apes, cercopithecines) to other mammals (ungulates, ex. giraffes, buffalo…), birds (ex. parrots and ravens) and reptiles (ex. Komodo Dragon). We keep an ethological perspective of study, so we always take into account the adaptive and evolutionary value of the behaviors that we study and we tried to apply the knowledge gathered in our researches to improve animal welfare and favor the development of conservation strategies in situ and ex situ. At this moment we have three projects in progress: “Cognition in ungulates”; “Play therapy applied to great apes welfare”; “Conservation in situ and ex situ of Cercocebus atys lunulatus”. We conduct our studies in collaboration with several institutions and research groups: Zoo of Barcelona, Psittacus Catalònia, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (MPI-EVA, Germany), WAPCA (Ghana), Zoo of Leipzig (Germany); Zoo of Madrid; Zoo of Accra (Ghana). Keywords: comparative ethology, primatology, physical cognition, laterality, social cognition, self- recognition, imitation, tool use, reciprocity, post-conflict behaviour, animal welfare, enrichment, primate conservation. Selected publications: Caicoya, Á. L., Amici, F., Ensenyat, C., & Colell, M. 2019. Object permanence in Giraffa camelopardalis: First steps in giraffes’ physical cognition. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 133(2), 207. Marín, H.; Chaves, A.B.; Posada, S.; Colell, M. 2017. Vertical string-pulling in green jays (Cyanocorax yncas). Behavioural Processes. 140, pp.74-80. Elsevier B.V. ISSN 0376-6357 Chaves, A.B.; Colell, M. 2017. String-pulling in Martin's spot-nosed monkey (Cercopithecus nictitans martini): evidence of physical continuity understanding. Behaviour. 154, pp. 719-740. E.J. Brill. ISSN 0005-7959 Amici, F; Colell, M; Von Borrell, C; Bueno-Guerra, N. 2017. Meerkats (Suricata suricatta) fail to prosocially donate food in an experimental set-up. Animal Cognition. 20(6), pp. 1059-1066. Springer Verlag. ISSN 1435-9448 Bueno-Guerra, N.; Leiva, D.; Colell, M.; Call, J. 2016. Do sex and age affect strategic behavior and inequity aversion in children? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 150, pp.285-300. Elsevier. ISSN 0022-0965 7
Executive Functions / Language Learning and Processing Toni Cunillera Summary My main area of interest is cognitive neuroscience. More specifically, I work on two main topics, a) executive functions and, b) language learning and processing. A. Executive Functions 1. Inhibitory function: I investigate how and when the cognitive brain is able to inhibit an action or how it reacts to withhold/withdraw a preponderant response. For this purpose, I use the approach of transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tDCS), a well-known neuromodulatory technique, together with EEG/ERP methodologies, to see the effects of the engagement of certain brain networks on behavior. There is an applied part of this research focused on how the inhibitory function is altered in eating disorders. B. Language Learning and Processing 2. Statistical Language Learning: I study the neural based and cognitive mechanisms recruited to success in the initial stages of learning a second language during the adulthood. My research is focused in the mechanism known as Statistical Learning. I have investigated different but related learning situations in which statistical learning can explain how we successfully learn the regularities embedded in the speech signal. Keywords: Executive Functions. Response inhibition. Statistical learning. Selected publications: Cunillera T, Brignani D, Cucurell D, Fuentemilla L, Miniussi C. (2016). The right inferior frontal cortex in response inhibition: A tDCS-ERP co-registration study. Neuroimage, 140, 66-75. Cunillera T, Càmara E, Toro JM, Marco-Pallares J, Sebastián-Galles N, Ortiz H, Pujol J, Rodríguez- Fornells A. (2009). Time course and functional neuroanatomy of speech segmentation in adults. Neuroimage, 48, 541-553. 8
Contextualising Psychosocial Wellbeing and Mental Health within Sociocultural Dynamics Francisco Jose Eiroá Orosa Summary The lack of contextualization of the science and applications of mental health and psychosocial interventions is the source of a large part of their criticisms. The fixation on experimental methodologies (such as clinical trials) to prove its efficacy, and the exportation of working methods manufactured in Western upper middle class contexts to developing countries and socially excluded populations, are some of the limitations of the body research in these disciplines. Both social and cultural decontextualisation of mental health and psychosocial interventions currently contribute to the gap between research and practice. From the lack of research on how social dynamics influence the well-being and mental health of citizens, or the lack of clearly defined public mental health policies at the European level, to the gaps in research and implementation of coordination among different disciplines, there are many constraints which now surround the implementation of these interventions. The objective of this project is to map and offer a bottom-up, source-embedded appreciation of the need to always seek to contextualise mental health and psychosocial interventions and the professional and institutional barriers to this process at the European level, using participatory action research methods with practitioners and users of psychological services as well as civil society groups. This will allow us to have an ecological approach while ensuring direct dissemination of the results. The project will be implemented integrating a website and a mobile application with face to face interviews, focus groups and continuing professional development workshops. Keywords: Mental health, psychosocial wellbeing, sociocultural dynamics, service users, participation, citizenship Selected publications: Eiroa-Orosa, F. J. (2018). The Sociocultural Context of Psychosocial Interventions. Frontiers in Psychology, 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01795 Eiroa-Orosa, F. J., & Lomascolo, M. (2018). Training mental health activists increases the well- being of participants with high baseline levels of self-stigma: results of the Obertament training evaluation. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry. https://doi.org/10.1037/ort0000376 Montague, A. C., & Eiroa-Orosa, F. J. (2018). In it together: Exploring how belonging to a youth activist group enhances well-being. Journal of Community Psychology, 46(1), 23–43. https://doi.org/10.1002/jcop.21914 Eiroa-Orosa, F. J., & Rowe, M. (2017). Taking the Concept of Citizenship in Mental Health across Countries. Reflections on Transferring Principles and Practice to Different Sociocultural Contexts. Frontiers in Psychology, 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01020 Eiroa-Orosa, F. J., Rodriguez-Urrutia, A., Accarino, A., Santamarina-Perez, P., Parramon, G., & Azpiroz, F. (2016). An exploratory study comparing psychological profiles and its congruence with clinical performance among patients with functional or motility digestive disorders. Journal of Health Psychology, 21(11), 2590–2599. https://doi.org/10.1177/1359105315581069. 9
Brainlab Carles Escera Iria SanMiguel Marc Via The Brainlab (www.ub.edu/brainlab; @Brainlab_UB) is an Excellence Research Group (SGR20117-974) that belongs to the Institute of Neurosciences of University of Barcelona and to the Sant Joan de Déu Research Institute (IRSJD). We offer a multidisciplinary (psychologist, biologists, engineers) and multicultural atmosphere to carry out a Master’s thesis project in the field of auditory cognitive neuroscience, with the possibility to use electrophysiological (EEG), behavioral (eye-tracking, psychophysics), neuroimaging (MRI) and genetic approaches, in adult as well as in newborn populations. Brainlab (1/3) – Subcortical encoding of speech sounds in adults and newborns PI: Carles Escera In the Subcortical encoding of speech sounds in adults and newborns research line (PI: Carles Escera), we aim at investigating how the sounds of human language are encoded in the auditory system and what are the modulating factors that shape this encoding, from experience-dependent plasticity and environmental to genetics. We employ a fascinating auditory evoked potential called Frequency-Following Response (FFR), which reflect compound neuronal activity in the ascending auditory pathway to cortex that is phase-locked to the spectrotemporal components of the acoustic signal, so that it faithfully mimics the eliciting stimulus. Using the FFR, our lab has shown predictive coding in the subcortical auditory system, the influence of the serotonin transporter gen in efficient speech-sound encoding, and that timing predictability enhances repetition suppression at subcortical level. The master projects will lie within this line, with the possibility to enroll in adult and even newborn recordings, and the opportunity to join an internationally reputed site for auditory cognitive neuroscience training and a thriving research environment. Check www.ub.edu/brainlab for ongoing specific projects and further details. Keywords: Auditory evoked potentials, Frequency-Following Response (FFR), auditory perception, speech and language disorders Selected references: Slabu, L., Grimm, S., & Escera, C. (2012). Novelty detection in the human auditory brainstem. Journal of Neuroscience, 34, 1447-1452. Recasens, M., Grimm, S, Capilla, A., Nowak, R., & Escera, C. (2014). Two sequential processes of change detection in hierarchically ordered areas of the human auditory cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 24, 143-153. Selinger, L., Zarnowiek, K., Via, M., Clemente, I.C., Escera, C. (2016). Involvement of the serotonin transporter gene in accurate subcortical speech encoding. Journal of Neuroscience, 36(42), 10782-10790. Gorina-Careta, N., Zarnowiec, K., Costa-Faidella, J., & Escera, C. (2016). Timing predictability enhances regularity encoding in the human subcortical auditory pathway. Scientific Reports, 6:37405. Parras, G.G., Nieto-Diego, J., Carbajal, G.V., Valdés-Baizabal, C., Escera. C., & Malmierca, M.S. (2017). Neurons along the auditory pathway exhibit a hierarchical organization of prediction error. Nature Communications, 8, 2148. Escera, C. (2017). The role of the auditory brain stem in regularity encoding and deviance detection. In: Kraus, N., Anderson, S., White-Schwoch, T., Fay, R. R., and Popper, A. N. (Eds.). The Frequency-following Response: A Window into Human Communication, (pp. 101-121). New York: Springer Nature. 10
Brainlab (2/3) – Electrophysiology of predictive processes in action-perception interactions and the sense of agency PI: Iria SanMiguel Active perception and predictive processing. Everyone is familiar with bistable stimuli like the Rubin vase, which can be perceived either as two faces or a vase. How is it possible that the same sensory stimulation can give rise to different percepts in different occasions? Such phenomena highlight the active nature of perception. The world we perceive is an interpretation of the information that arrives at our senses. A key factor in this interpretative process is prediction. The brain constantly and automatically formulates predictions regarding the sensory input. Sensory responses, and hence perception, are influenced by such predictions. We are interested in understanding the neural mechanisms that support predictive processing, and their effects on perception. Motor-driven sensory prediction. An important source of sensory predictions is our own motor behaviour. How is it possible that the visual image remains still, despite we are constantly moving our body and our eyes? The solution lies in predictive processing: the sensory consequences of the organisms’ motor actions are predicted by the nervous system, and this prediction is used to compensate the effects of self-action during sensory processing. In our research, we make use motor- driven prediction to study predictive processing in audition. In a typical experiment, participants deliver auditory stimuli to themselves by pressing buttons. Comparing responses to self- and externally-generated sounds we can study the effects of motor-driven prediction in auditory processing. Agency. How do we recognize ourselves as the agents of certain stimuli in the environment, when there is nothing that can differentiate such stimuli from other stimuli that we did not cause ourselves? The sensation of agency may arise from the effects of motor-driven sensory predictions on sensory processing. That is, whenever a sensation is cancelled out by a motor prediction, we may feel that we were the agent causing the stimulation. We are interested in understanding the precise relationship between motor-driven sensory predictions and the sensation of agency. Keywords: Predictive coding, sense of agency, EEG, ERPs, motor control, sensorimotor processes, perception Selected publications: Schröger, Marzecová & SanMiguel (2015) Attention and prediction in human audition: a lesson from cognitive psychophysiology. European Journal of Neuroscience, 41(5):641-64. SanMiguel, Widmann, Bendixen, Trujillo-Barreto & Schröger (2013) Hearing Silences: Human Auditory Processing Relies on Preactivation of Sound-Specific Brain Activity Patterns. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(20):8633-9. Bendixen, SanMiguel & Schröger (2012) Early electrophysiological indicators for predictive processing in audition: A review. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 83(2):120-31. Timm, Schönwiesner, Schröger & SanMiguel (2016) Sensory suppression of brain responses to self-generated sounds is observed with and without the perception of agency. Cortex, 80:5-20. Timm J, SanMiguel I, Keil J, Schröger E, Schönwiesner M. (2014). Motor intention determines sensory attenuation of brain responses to self-initiated sounds. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26(7):1481-9. 11
Brainlab (3/3) – Genetic modulators of brain potentials associated to speech and musical processing PI: Marc Via In the research line "Genetic modulators of brain potentials associated to speech and musical processing" (PI: Marc Via), we aim at establishing the role of genetic and epigenetic variants in the auditory processing of acoustic stimuli of different nature (speech vs. non-speech, syllables vs. running speech) at different levels of the auditory hierarchy (cortical vs. subcortical stages). In particular, we collect electroencephalographic (EEG) recordings from young healthy participants while conducting different experiments on sound processing. Additionally, we collect saliva samples to analyze genetic and epigenetic markers and behavioral data to measure the ability of participants in pitch discrimination tasks. Thus, we will identify genetic and epigenetic variants associated to measures of pitch extraction at the subcortical level, through the analysis of subcortical responses to acoustic stimuli of different complexity and to cortical responses to different linguistic and non-linguistic acoustic stimuli. Moreover, we will also assess the role of attentional processes and the ecological validity of the observed associations. Our new view on the molecular mechanisms involved in the cognitive neuroscience of audition, incorporating genetic markers and methylation profiling into the brain mechanisms of auditory cognition, constitute a breakthrough on current views in cognitive neuroscience. Results arising from the project will be relevant for specialists in very diverse fields (e.g. neurophysiology, molecular genetics and epigenetics, acoustics, or psycholinguistics) and may also lead to approaches to understand the pathophysiology of disorders such as dyslexia or language specific impairment. Keywords: EEG, FFR, genetics, epigenetics, audition, music, language Selected references: Garcia-Garcia, M., Via, M., Zarnowiec, K., SanMiguel, I., Escera, C., Clemente, I.C. (2017). COMT and DRD2/ANKK-1 gene-gene interaction account for resetting of gamma neural oscillations to auditory stimulus-driven attention. PLoS One, 12(2), e0172362. Selinger, L., Zarnowiek, K., Via, M., Clemente, I.C., Escera, C. (2016). Involvement of the serotonin transporter gene in accurate subcortical speech encoding. Journal of Neuroscience, 36(42), 10782-10790. 12
Construing Self and Others: Interventions in Clinical and Health Psychology Guillem Feixas Summary Our research group (SGR2017-642, see www.ub.edu/ipcs) is aimed at advancing the knowledge in the areas of personality and psychotherapy. We pay particular attention to the study of identity processes, cognitive conflicts, and their role in a variety of change processes. To this aim, personal construct and self-regulation theories (both based on constructivist epistemology) have been important sources of inspiration for both research and practice. Our work is conducted using methods derived from these approaches, such as textual analysis and the Repertory Grid Technique, with which we can identify conflicts in the cognitive structure of interviewees. These conflicts (or dilemmas) often become a hindrance for the progress either in personal development or in psychotherapy. With these methods we have studied the cognitive structures of people of different conditions (e.g., depression, eating disorders, fibromyalgia, victims of partner violence) and compared them to community and student samples. We have also created some therapy manuals for innovative interventions targeting identity processes and cognitive conflicts, and tested their efficacy and utility in randomized clinical trials. We have adapted and validated instruments for the assessment of the outcome of psychological interventions which can now be used by other researchers and practitioners (e.g., www.ub.edu/terdep/core). We have just finished a randomised controlled trial (RCT) comparing the efficacy of systematic case formulation based in personal construct therapy (PCT) with formulations based in cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) for the improvement of depressive symptoms of patients with fibromyalgia. Currently, we are beginning another RCT comparing the efficacy of CBT, PCT and PCT enhanced with virtual reality for young adults with mild-to- moderate depression. Keywords: Personality psychology, constructivism, psychotherapy, counseling, cognitive therapy, systemic therapy, cognitive conflict, self, identity, depression, repertory grid technique. Selected publications: Feixas, G., et al. (2014). Cognitive conflicts in major depression: Between desired change and personal coherence. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 53, 369-385. doi: 10.1111/bjc.12050 Montesano, A., López-González, M. A., Saúl, L. A., & Feixas, G. (2015). A review of cognitive conflicts research: A meta-analytic study of prevalence and relation to symptoms. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 11, 2997-3006. Trujillo, A., Feixas, G., et al. (2016). Psychometric properties of the Spanish version of the CORE-OM: Clinical Outcomes in Routine Evaluation-Outcome Measure. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 12, 1457–66. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S103079 Feixas, G.et al. (2016). Dilemma-focused intervention for depression: A multicenter randomized controlled trial with 3-month follow-up. Depression and Anxiety, 33, 862-869. doi: 10.1002/da.22510. Feixas, G., & Compañ, V. (2016). Dilemma-focused intervention for unipolar depression: a treatment manual. BMC Psychiatry, 16: 235. DOI 10.1186/s12888-016-0947-x Aguilera, M., Paz, C., Compañ, V., Medina, J. C., & Feixas, G. (2019). Cognitive rigidity in patients with depression and fibromyalgia. International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology, 19(2), 160–164. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijchp.2019.02.002 13
Dynamics of Memory Formation Lluís Fuentemilla Summary How do we form lasting memories of our everyday experiences? We want to understand how experiences are initially encoded, undergo further consolidation and are later retrieved. We use behavioural (including Eye movements), psychophysiological (Skin Conductance) and neural (fMRI, EEG, iEEG) measures to help us learn more about the cognitive and neural operations that contribute to episodic memory. We further extend our investigation to neurological patients, in special those with lesions in medial temporal lobe regions. In concrete, current topics of interest, and examples of related projects, include: 1) Brain mechanisms of how discrete episodic memories are formed 2) Brain mechanisms of how prior knowledge influences the formation of new memories 3) How memory reactivation supports memory formation and memory retrieval 4) The study of memory processes for autobiographical events collected individually through portable cameras Keywords: memory, hippocampus, EEG, neural oscillations Selected publications: Fuentemilla L, Miró J, Falip M, Ripollés P, Juncadella M, Castañer S, Rodríguez-Fornells A (2013). Hippocampus-dependent strengthening of targeted memories via reactivation during sleep in humans. Current Biology 23:1769-75. Fuentemilla L, Penny WD, Bunzeck N, Cashdollar N, Düzel E (2010). Theta coupled periodic replay in working memory. Current Biology 20:606-612. Griffiths, B.J., Fuentemilla, Ll. (In Press). Event conjunction: How the hippocampus integrates episodic memories across event boundaries. PsyArxiv. Jafarpour, A., Fuentemilla, L., Horner, A., Penny, W.D., Düzel, E. (2014). Replay of very early encoding representations during recollection. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1:242-8 Packard PA, Rodríguez-Fornells A, Stein LM, Nicolás B, Fuentemilla L (2014).Tracking explicit and implicit long-lasting traces of fearful memories in humans. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 116:96- 104. Silva, M., Baldassano, C., Fuentemilla, Ll. (In Press). Rapid memory reactivation at movie event boundaries promotes episodic encoding. Journal of Neuroscience. Sols, I., DuBrow, S., Davachi, L., Fuentemilla, Ll. (2017). Event Boundaries Trigger Rapid Memory Reinstatement of the Prior Events to Promote Their Representation in Long-Term Memory. Current Biology. 27(22):3499-3504. 14
Person-Environment transactions and Individual Differences David Gallardo-Pujol Summary Why we behave in a certain way or another? It is an axiom in behavioral sciences that behavior is a function of personal characteristics and the environment [B=f(P,E)]. However, little research has been conducted taking into account both elements of the equation for over hundred years. We aim to understand how individual differences operate and cause behavior depending on specific contexts. To this end, we conduct different experiments and collect data at multiple levels (genes, brain function, individual differences in personality and cognitive abilities, social and cultural). We combine these data with the latest analytic approaches within the framework of person-environment transactions. This academic year we are specifically interested in 1) how situational pressures interact with individual differences on moral decision-making; and 2) how situations are differentially perceived across cultures. Keywords: Personality, Individual Differences, Culture, Brain Structure, Geolocation, Structural Equation Models, Person-Environment Transactions. Selected publications: Gallardo-Pujol, D.; Maydeu-Olivares, A.; & Andrés-Pueyo (2013). An experimental test of a gene-environment interaction: MAOA genotype, social exclusion and aggression. Genes, Brain & Behavior. 12(1);140-145. DOI: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2012.00868.x Guillaume, E., Baranski, E., Todd, E., Bastian, B., Bronin, I., Ivanova, C., Cheng, J.T., de Kock, F.S., Denissen, J.J.A., Gallardo-Pujol, D., Halama, Pl, Han, G.Q., Bae, J., Moon, J., Hong, R.Y., Hřebíčková, M., Graf, S., Izdebski, P., Lundmann, L., Penke, L., Perugini, M., Costantini, G., Rauthmann, J., Ziegler, M., Realo, A., Elme, L., Sato, T., Kawamoto, S., Szarota, P., Tracy, J.L., van Aken, M.A.G., Yang, Y., & Funder, D.C. (2015, May 7th). The world at 7: Comparing the experience of situations across 20 countries. Journal of Personality. DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12176 Pascual, L.; Rodrigues, P.; & Gallardo-Pujol, D. (2013). How does morality work in the brain? A structural perspective of moral behavior. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 7;65. DOI: 10.3389/fnint.2013.00065 Rauthman, J.F.; Gallardo-Pujol, D.; Guillaume, E.M.; Todd, E.; Nave, C.; Sherman, R.A.; Ziegler, M.; Jones, A.B. & Funder, D.C. (2014). The Situational Eight DIAMONDS: A taxonomy of major dimensions of situation characteristics. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107(4), 677-718. 15
Validation of psychological tests Georgina Guilera Summary Psychological measurement instruments play an important role in research, clinical practice and educational and health assessment. Measurement involves assigning scores to individuals so that they represent some characteristic of them. But how do we know that the scores actually represent that characteristic? In addition, measurement implies drawing conclusions about individuals. But how do we know that our conclusions are fair for them? To answer these and other questions about the quality of psychological tests, their psychometric properties in terms of reliability and validity evidence should be explored. This research line is focused on the evaluation of psychometric properties of several psychological tests, using classical test theory and advanced methodology to do so. This academic year we are especially interested in the: a) validation of the Pure Procrastination Scale (PPS) in the framework vof Item Response Theory models; b) reliability generalization meta-analysis of the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS); and c) development of normative data of the Screen for Cognitive Impairment in Psychiatry in elderly people. However, we are open to new ideas and challenges. Share yours with us! Keywords: psychometrics; test-retest reliability; internal consistency; validity evidence; factor analysis; meta-analysis. Selected publications: Gómez‐Benito, J., Berrío, Á. I., Guilera, G., Rojo, E., Purdon, S., & Pino, O. (2018). The Screen for Cognitive Impairment in Psychiatry: Proposal for a polytomous scoring system. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 27(3), e1598. Guilera, G., Barrios, M., Penelo, E., Morin, C., Steel, P., & Gómez-Benito, J. (2018). Validation of the Spanish version of the Irrational Procrastination Scale (IPS). PloS One, 13(1), e0190806. Guilera, G., Gómez-Benito, J., Pino, Ó., Rojo, E., Vieta, E., Cuesta, M. J., ... & Martínez-Arán, A. (2015). Disability in bipolar I disorder: the 36-item World Health Organization Disability Assessment Schedule 2.0. Journal of Affective Disorders, 174, 353-360. Guilera, G., Pereda, N., Paños, A., & Abad, J. (2015). Assessing resilience in adolescence: The Spanish adaptation of the Adolescent Resilience Questionnaire. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes, 13(1), 1. Pereda, N., Gallardo-Pujol, D., & Guilera, G. (2018). Good practices in the assessment of victimization: The Spanish adaptation of the Juvenile Victimization Questionnaire. Psychology of Violence, 8(1), 76. 16
Neurolinguistics, Multilingualism and Cognition Mireia Hernández Summary 1. Bilingualism and decision-making1. Would you kill one person to save five? Even if it were for a greater good, many people would find the action of killing someone incompatible with their morality… UNLESS… they face this dilemma in their foreign language. Difficult as it may be to believe, it has been consistently found that most of us tend to make more utilitarian decisions when using our foreign language. In addition, this “foreign language effect” seems to go beyond moral choices. For instance, using a foreign language reduces risk aversion in decision-making contexts: risks appear smaller. But, how could our choices depend on whether we made them in our native tongue or foreign language? It has been proposed that a foreign language increases the psychological distance with certain contents. However, the answer to this question remains, in fact, unclear. Indeed, this is one of the most controversial and hotly debated current questions in the fields of cognitive neuroscience of language, neuroeconomics and multilingualism. 2. Mechanisms of “bilingual language control” (bLC)2. Cases of bilingual neurological patients who lost control of their languages have evidenced how much bilinguals depend on bLC––e.g., patent AH (reported by Abutalebi et al. (2000). Neurocase) could not help mixing English and Italian: “I cannot communicare con you; Oggi I cannot say il mio nome to you; I’m a disastro today.” This type of patients is rather scarce. Hence, bLC has been typically studied with healthy bilinguals, using the so-called language-switching paradigm. Studies with this paradigm have revealed that the efficiency of bLC mechanisms may vary on a number of factors related with the individuals’ bilingual language history as well as non-linguistic mechanisms of cognitive control. Determining what these factors are and how they interact would represent a major contribution in the field of cognitive neuroscience of language and multilingualism. 3. The benefits of bilingualism in general cognition3. It seems that bLC mechanisms partially overlap with general cognition. Many researchers have proposed that the constant need of bilinguals of applying bLC would result in them being over-trained (compared to monolinguals) in general cognition––to the point of even delaying the onset of the earliest symptoms of dementia. However, it remains to be determined what specific aspects of general cognition become benefited by bilingualism. 1 Hayakawa S., Costa A., Foucart, A., & Keysar, B. (2016). Using a Foreign Language Changes Our Choices. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(11), 791–793. 2 Bobb, S. C., & Wodniecka, Z. (2013). Language switching in picture naming: What asymmetric switch costs (do not) tell us about inhibition in bilingual speech planning. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 568–585. 3 Yow, W. Q., & Li, X. (2015). Balanced bilingualism and early age of second language acquisition as the underlying mechanisms of a bilingual executive control advantage: why variations in bilingual experiences matter. Frontiers in Psychology, 6:164. 17
Primate Behaviour, Ecology and Cognition Adriana Hernández-Aguilar Summary My research focuses on the behaviour and ecology of chimpanzees and other primates in the wild and in captivity, and the implications these have for understanding human behaviour and evolution. My research involves several areas of the behavioural sciences: primatology, ethology, primate archaeology, anthropology and comparative psychology. I work on two main research lines: A) Behavioural ecology of savanna chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and other primates The main goals of this research are 1) to understand how the ecological characteristics of savanna habitats influence the behaviour of primates, 2) how their behaviour differs from that of conspecifics living in more forested habitats, and 3) especially in the case of chimpanzees and baboons (Papio spp.), to help modelling the possible behaviour and adaptations of early hominins who lived in similar habitats. Opportunities are offered to do fieldwork and/or analyse already collected data or video footage from camera traps under the Research and Conservation Project run by the Jane Goodall Institute Spain in Senegal (https://www.janegoodallsenegal.org/research) or in collaboration with the University of Dar es Salaam in Tanzania. B) Captive primate research The main goal of this research is to contribute to our understanding of the evolution of human behaviour through conducting (non-invasive) cognition experiments and behavioural studies with captive primates. Opportunities are offered to collect data in captive settings or to analyse video footage in collaboration with the Kristiansand Zoo (Norway) as well as with other zoos. There is also potential to contribute to studies for the project ManyPrimates https://manyprimates.github.io, a large scale international collaboration in comparative primate cognition research (including more than 12 institutions around the world), of which I am a member. ManyPrimates, including Hernandez-Aguilar R. A. (2019) Establishing an infrastructure for collaboration in primate cognition research. Plos One DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0223675. Motes-Rodrigo, A., Majlesi, P., Pickering, T. R., Laska, M., Axelsen, H., Minchin, T. C., Tennie, C. & Hernandez-Aguilar R. A. (2019) Chimpanzee extractive foraging with excavating tools: Experimental modeling of the origins of human technology. Plos One DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0215644 Haslam, M., Hernandez-Aguilar R. A., Ling, V., Carvalho, S., de la Torre, I., DeStefano, A., Du, A., Hardy, B., Harris, J., Marchant, L. F., Matsuzawa, T., McGrew, W., Mercader, J., Mora, R., Petraglia, M., Roche, H., Visalberghi, E., Warren, R. (2009) Primate Archaeology. Nature 460:339-334. Hernandez-Aguilar R. A., Moore, J. & Pickering, T. R. (2007) Savanna chimpanzees use tools to harvest the underground storage organs of plants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (PNAS) 104:19210- 19213. 18
Computational Neuroscience, Machine Learning and Computer Vision Matthias S. Keil Summary My principal research interest is to understand how the visual system of the human brain and insects process and interpret real-world stimuli. For example, stimuli could comprise gray level images for brightness perception, or video sequences for motion perception. My methods are mathematical modeling and computer simulations. A typical of my models complies with two goals: (1) ability to process real-world stimuli, and (2) predict psychophysical and/or neurophysiological results. In this way, is my research connected to computer vision and biologically-inspired image processing algorithms, respectively. My models use adaptive mechanisms and innovative learning methods, and in this way connect to deep learning. A new research line aims at reducing the time and number of learning examples of deep learning methods with new learning paradigms, Topics that I have been working on include human brightness/lightness perception, information processing in the retina (dynamic range reduction, predictive coding, luminance encoding), information processing with syncytia, analog and unconventional computing, face perception, optical flow, time-to contact perception and collision thread detection. Keywords: Computational Neuroscience, Machine Learning, Computer Vision, Mathematical Modeling, Dynamic Systems, Computer Simulations, Unconventional Computation, Image Processing, Predictive Coding. Selected publications: Keil M. Dendritic pooling of noisy threshold processes can explain many properties of a collision- sensitive visual neuron,” PLoS Computational Biology, vol. 11, no. 10, p. e1004479, 2015. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004479. Keil M and López-Moliner J. Unifying time to contact estimation and collision avoidance across species. PLoS Computational Biology, vol. 8, no. 8, p. e1002625, 2012. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002625. Keil M. ‘I Look in Your Eyes, Honey’: Internal face features induce spatial frequency preference for human face processing. PLoS Computational Biology, vol. 5, no. 3, p. e10003290, 2009. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000329; Keil M. Gradient representations and the perception of luminosity. Vision Research, vol. 47, pp. 3360–3372, 2007. Also available at http://arxiv.org/abs/0709.3237. 19
Dyadic Data Analysis David Leiva Summary Dyads are the most basic configuration or relationship in a group and they represent the unit of analysis adopted in most interpersonal interaction studies. Many social researchers have focused on dyadic dependence in explaining social interaction. Thus, dyad is the unit of analysis for the study of social interaction and, similarly, the research of mutual influence is founded on pairs of individuals. Interdependence has, beyond all doubt, constituted the main feature of dyadic data analyses. The study of social interaction is, therefore, the study of nonindependence and any assessment of this interdependence is concerned with the specific type of dyadic design. Here, several designs have been proposed to evaluate nonindependence for distinguishable and non- distinguishable or exchangeable dyads and for several dyadic data structures, including the standard dyadic and the Social Relations Model (SRM) designs. This research line is related to the development of new statistical techniques for quantifying and testing different social aspects using a dyadic approach and under different dyadic designs. The work to be developed under this line mainly relies on running simulations for assessing statistical properties of estimators and developing statistical tests useful in applied studies. Furthermore, developing statistical programs (preferably under an open-source environment as R) to ease the use of these new procedures by applied researchers in areas as social psychology or ethology, constitutes an important aim of this research line. Keywords: Dyadic data, dyadic designs, statistical modeling. Selected publications: Sbragaglia, V., Leiva, D., Arias, A., García, J.A., Aguzzi, J., & Breithaupt, T. (2017). Fighting over burrows: The emergence of dominance hierarchies in the Norway lobster (Nephrops norvergicus). Journal of Experimental Biology, 220, 4624-4633. Solanas, A., Leiva, D., Salafranca, Ll. (2010). Bias and standard error for social reciprocity measurements. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 63(1), 139-161. Leiva, D., Solanas, A., Salafranca, Ll. (2008). Testing reciprocity in social interactions: A comparison between the directional consistency and skew-symmetry statistics. Behavior Research Methods, 40(2), 626-634. 20
Decision Making and Eye Movements in Perception & Action Joan López-Moliner Summary An ubiquitous process in human daily-life behaviour is deciding between competing actions within complex and rich environments. First, optimal decisional processes depend on people representing different states of the world accurately and reliably. By means of tools rooted in the Statistical Decision Theory, we analyse how human encode different states and whether this encoding is optimal, that is, minimizes the uncertainty. Second, the range of sensory stimulation out there often exceeds by far the operating range of our senses. The nature’s solution to overcome this limitation is adaptation to the changing conditions of stimulation. This allows people to operate optimally across many different sensory conditions. We then study the underlying processes of adaptation in sensory and sensorimotor domains. Third, our actions have consequences on the states of the world. The brain predicts these consequences (forward models) and these predictions can be integrated with the actual sensory feedback to improve the precision with which we perceive the consequences of our actions on the world. We then study how humans combine these predictions with the incoming feedback in the control of our actions. Finally, these action consequences have an associated gain (or reward) or cost. We are interested in the processes that lead to optimal actions or decisions (i.e. maximize expected gain), mainly in changing environmental conditions. We use different methodologies to tackle these problems including psychophysics, eye movements, neural modelling, virtual reality, and neuroimage. Keywords: decision making in complex environments, perception and action; eye movements; sensory prediction; decision under uncertainty & risk; virtual reality Selected publications: Linares, D., Aguilar-Lleyda, D., and López-Moliner, J. (2019). Decoupling sensory from decisional choice biases in perceptual decision making. eLife, 8:e43994. López-Moliner, J. and Brenner, E. (2016). Flexible timing of eye movements when catching a ball. J Vis, 16(5):1–11. de la Malla, C., and López-Moliner, J. (2015). Predictive plus online visual information optimizes temporal precision in interception, Journal of Experimental Psychology: HPP. DOI: 10.1037/xhp0000075 Kokkinara, E., Slater, M., and López-Moliner, J. (2015). The Effects of Visuomotor Calibration to the Perceived Space and Body, through Embodiment in Immersive Virtual Reality. ACM Transactions on Applied Perception (TAP), 13(1):3. 21
Single-case designs data analysis Rumen Manolov Summary The research line is focused on testing, comparing, and proposing analytical techniques for data obtained through single-case experimental designs. Several aspects define this field of research. First, due to the longitudinal nature of the data and the usual shortness of the data series classical statistical procedures cannot be employed. Second, applied researchers using single-case designs (e.g., behavioral interventions in special education, developmental disabilities) have commonly relied only on visual analysis, not using statistical tools. Third, the amount of data features that can be modeled (initial level, change in level, baseline trend, change in trend, autocorrelation, variation across the cases within a study) has triggered a myriad of proposals, accompanied by a lack of consensus regarding which proposals are optimal. One of the challenges of the field that we are trying to tackle are: to bring the statistical proposals closer to applied researchers, by identifying easily understood, but statistically sound procedures and developing software implementations. Other challenges include the continuous development and testing of already existing or newly proposed analytical techniques, as well as summarizing the evidence on their performance. For that purpose, the research can be formalized in different kinds of papers: proposals, illustrations with published behavioral data, tests via Monte Carlo simulation, discussions. Furthermore, software development and description is another relevant task for making the analytical techniques attractive for applied researchers. Keywords: single-case designs; effect size, trend, autocorrelation Selected publications: Manolov, R., Gast, D. L., Perdices, M., & Evans, J. J. (2014). Single-case experimental designs: Reflections on conduct and analysis. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 24(3-4), 634–660. https://doi.org/10.1080/09602011.2014.903199 Manolov, R., & Moeyaert, M. (2017). How can single-case data be analyzed? Software resources, tutorial, and reflections on analysis. Behavior Modification, 41(2), 179–228. https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445516664307 Manolov, R., & Moeyaert, M. (2017). Recommendations for choosing single-case data analytical techniques. Behavior Therapy, 48(1), 97–114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2016.04.008 Manolov, R., & Onghena, P. (2018). Analyzing data from single-case alternating treatments designs. Psychological Methods, 23(3), 480–504. https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000133 22
You can also read