RESCUING FERSTER AND DEMYER'S BEHAVIORAL PHARMACOLOGY RESEARCH IN AUTISM FROM OBLIVION

 
CONTINUE READING
EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS                          2009, 10, 5 - 18                NUMBER 1 (SUMMER 2009)
                                                                                                                          5

            Rescuing Ferster and DeMyer’s
      behavioral pharmacology research in autism
                     from oblivion
                                                      Edward K. Morris
                                                       University of Kansas

                                                                 and

                                                 Charryse M. Fouquette
           University of Kansas and The Lovaas Institute for Early Intervention, Midwest Headquarters

  In 1961, Charles B. Ferster and Marian K. DeMyer published a one-page report in the Journal of the
  Experimental Analysis of Behavior titled, “Increased Performances of an Autistic Child with Prochlor-
  perazine Administration.” It may be the first and only report of basic behavioral pharmacology research
  using both operant apparatus and measures and a within-individual replication design to analyze the
  behavior of a child with autism. However, it is now lost in history. In this article, we rescue the report
  by providing historical context (e.g., a literature review, the setting), describing and elaborating on the
  report (e.g., the participant, method), and relating the report to the research of its day and afterward.
  We also discuss the grounds on which the report warrants being rescued and, in a postscript, summarize
  Ferster’s later work in autism.
      Keywords: Ferster, DeMyer, history, autism, behavior analysis, behavioral pharmacology

   In 1961, Charles B. Ferster and Marian K.                             only report of basic behavioral pharmacology
DeMyer (1961b) published a one-page report                               research using operant apparatus and measures
in the Journal of the Experimental Analysis of                           and a within-individual replication design to
Behavior (JEAB) titled, “Increased Performances                          analyze the behavior of a child with autism.
of an Autistic Child with Prochlorperazine                               However, it is now lost in history – in the his-
Administration.” It was perhaps the first and                            tory of autism, behavior analysis, and behavioral
                                                                         pharmacology. It is lost in what historians call
      This article is dedicated to Joseph V. Brady for his funda-        oblivion.1
mental contributions to behavioral pharmacology, strong ad-
vocacy for behavior analysis, and gentle encouragement of the                Using primary source publications, citation
first author. We thank Mike Aman, Len Green, Joe Hingtgen,               analyses, personal communications, and archival
Vic Laties, Al Poling, Steve Schroeder, Alan Silberberg, Travis
Thompson, Steve Warren, and Stan Weiss for assisting our review          records, we rescue Ferster and DeMyer’s report
of the literature for this manuscript and, in some cases, for com-       by placing it in historical context, describing the
menting on earlier drafts. We thank the Archives for the History
of American Psychology, the Harvard University Archives, and             research in greater detail, and offering evidence
the American University Archives for granting us access to their         that supports our observations about its priority
collections. And, we thank the manuscript’s several legions of
reviewers for their suggestions. Correspondence may be sent to           and uniqueness. The evidence notwithstanding,
the first author at the Department of Applied Behavioral Science,        the report is weak in several regards, leading us
Dole Human Development Center, University of Kansas, 1000
Sunnyside Avenue, Lawrence, KS 66045. Phone: 785.864.4840;               to discuss whether it warrants being rescued in
fax: 785.864.5202; email: ekm@ku.edu.                                    the first place. After weighing arguments for and
                                                                     5
6                                          Edward K. Morris and Charryse M. Fouquette

against this, we conclude that it does because it                    Medicine (est. 1903), located at the university’s
represents an as-yet developed area of research                      Medical Center in Indianapolis, IN. He was,
that has important clinical implications. At the                     in addition, appointed as a Principal Investiga-
very least, we will have added depth and detail                      tor in Experimental Psychology at the medical
to the early modern history of autism, behav-                        school’s Institute of Psychiatric Research (est.
ior analysis, and behavioral pharmacology. We                        1956), whose mission was to foster multidisci-
begin with some background.                                          plinary research in basic and applied biological
                                                                     psychiatry. At the Institute, Ferster conducted
                       Background                                    basic nonhuman research on, for instance,
                                                                     schedules of reinforcement and aversive control
Charles B. Ferster                                                   (e.g., Ferster, 1960a; Zimmerman & Ferster,
    Charles B. Ferster (1922-1981) is renowned                       1964) and conducted or consulted on applied
in the history of behavior analysis. After receiv-                   research on obesity (Ferster, Nurnberger, &
ing his undergraduate degree from Rutgers                            Levitt, 1962), stuttering (see J. P. Brady, 1990),
University in 1947, he entered the graduate                          and teaching (Ferster, 1960b).
program in psychology at Columbia University,                            Most notably, though, he and DeMyer
from which he earned an M.A. in 1948 and a                           began perhaps the earliest program of behavior-
Ph.D. in 1950. His mentors were Fred Keller                          analytic research on children with autism. This
and Nat Schoenfeld (on the Columbia program,                         included a behavioral interpretation of autism
see Dinsmoor, 1990; Keller, 1986). In 1950,                          (Ferster, 1961); human operant research on
he became a research fellow with B. F. Skinner                       children with autism, funded by the National
(1904-1990) in the Pigeon Lab at Harvard Uni-                        Association for Mental Health (NAMH) 2 and
versity, the main result of which was, of course,                    Smith, Kline, & French (Fester & DeMyer,
their text, Schedules of Reinforcement (Ferster &                    1961a, 1962; see Ferster, 1962); an application
Skinner, 1957; see Ferster, 1970; Skinner, 1981;                     of operant conditioning to autism, also funded
on the lab at that time, see Gollub, 2002). In                       by NAMH (DeMyer & Ferster, 1962); and their
1955, Ferster took a position as a research as-                      behavioral pharmacology research, for which no
sociate at the Yerkes Laboratories of Primate                        funding source was listed.
Biology in Orange Park, FL, where he, Roger                              At the time, DeMyer was an assistant profes-
Kelleher, and John Falk conducted operant                            sor in the Department of Psychiatry, a clinical
research with chimpanzees (Dewsbury, 2003;                           consultant and research collaborator at the In-
see, e.g., Ferster, 1957). At Yerkes, Ferster also                   stitute, and the Director of Children’s Services
drew up plans for JEAB, which was founded in                         at the Larue D. Carter Memorial Hospital. This
1957 (Kelleher & Morse, 1987; Laties, 1987).                         was a state hospital for inpatient mental health
He was its first editor -- 1958-1960.                                treatment and research, as well as a training site
    In 1957, Ferster took a position as an assis-                    for the medical school’s psychiatric residents and
tant professor in the Department of Psychiatry                       the site for her research with Ferster.
(est. 1940) at Indiana University’s School of
     1
       The report is available on-line through the Society for the   Ferster and DeMyer (1961b)
Experimental Analysis of Behavior (http://seab.envmed.rochester.         When Ferster and DeMyer (1961b) pub-
edu/) by searching for “Ferster DeMyer” (not “autism”), but its
three paragraphs are condensed into one and its figure, footnotes,   lished their report, behavioral pharmacology
and references are missing. An October 25, 2008, Pub Med             was still in its infancy. Although Skinner had
author search for “ferster cb demyer mk” yielded Ferster and
DeMyer (1961a, 1962), but not Ferster and DeMyer (1961b).            conducted related research in the 1930s (see
The full report, however, is accessible through the PubMed           Heron & Skinner, 1937; Skinner & Heron,
Central Archives link to Administrative Content (i.e., http://
www.Pubmedcentral.nih.gov/picrender.fcgi?artid=1403980&b             1937), the field was not founded until the
lobtype=pdf ). Ferster and DeMyer (1961b), the way, is not the
only report of behavioral pharmacology research in develop-               2
                                                                            The NAMH was founded in 1950, but its roots lie in the
mental disabilities lost in history. See, for example, McConahey,    National Committee for Mental Hygiene (est. 1909). In 2006,
Thompson, and Zimmerman’s (1977) chapter, “A Token System            the NAMH became Mental Health America, “the country’s oldest
for Retarded Women: Behavior Therapy, Drug Administration,           and largest nonprofit organization addressing all aspects of mental
and Their Combination.”                                              health and mental illness” (http://www.nmha.org/go/history).
Rescuing Ferster and Demyer                                      7

mid-1950s (e.g., Dews, 1955; see Laties,                                  The Report
2003; Pickens, 1977; Poling, 2000), with hu-
man behavioral pharmacology emerging soon              The Participant
thereafter (e.g., Dews & Morse, 1958; see                  Ferster and DeMyer’s (1961b) participant
Poling, 1986, pp. 3-20). Both were a melding           was a 10-year-old boy who had been an inpatient
of the experimental analysis of behavior with          at the Larue D. Carter Memorial Hospital for
pharmacology, a melding pursued in three               the preceding three years and who, during the
settings -- departments of psychology and              16 months prior to their pharmacology study,
pharmacology, and the pharmaceutical industry          had participated in their program of human
(Laties, 2003).                                        operant research (Ferster & DeMyer, 1961a,
    Although published early in the field’s            1962). In this research, children learned to press
founding, Ferster and DeMeyer (1961b) is               a key on a dispenser for “coins” that could be
not cited in the histories of behavioral phar-         used to operate “reinforcing devices” that lined
macology (e.g., Laties, 2003; Pickens, 1977);          the walls of a research room. Among the devices
in biographies of the field’s founders, for            were vending machines that dispensed food and
instance, those of Joe Brady (Barrett, 2008),          trinkets, a television, an electric train set, and
Peter Dews (Barrett, 2006), or Roger Kelleher          a three-window matching-to-sample (MTS)
(Branch, 2006); or in recent reviews of behav-         apparatus. With the coin dispenser and the de-
ioral pharmacology in autism (e.g., Handen &           vices, Ferster and DeMyer analyzed the effects of
Lubetsky, 2005; Schaal & Hackenberg, 1994;             reinforcement schedules (e.g., fixed-ratios) and
Tsai, 2005). It is also not cited in the histories     stimulus control (e.g., light-signaled reinforcer
of applied behavior analysis (e.g., Kazdin, 1978,      availability, matching-to-sample).
1982; Krasner, 1982) or in texts on applied                When Ferster and DeMyer prepared their
behavior analysis in autism (e.g., Ghezzi, Wil-        1962 human operant manuscript for publica-
liams, & Carr, 1999; Luiselli, Russo, Christian,       tion, not when they began that program of
& Wilczynski, 2008; Volkmar, Paul, Klin, &             research, their participants were an 11-year-old
Cohen, 2005).                                          girl and two boys, ages 3½ and 10. The older
     In a search of the Web of Science (April 27,      boy was thus the likely participant in their
2008), we did find nine citations to Ferster           pharmacology research. In their manuscript,
and DeMyer’s (1961b) article, but they were,           they described him this way:
on average, over 35 years old and published in             Thomas, aged 10 and hospitalized 3 years, has
journals outside mainstream behavior analysis.             been studied for 12 months. He had a normal
Three of the citations appeared in introductions           motor and speech development, speaking short
to studies in basic and applied human behav-               sentences until he was 2½, when he developed se-
ioral pharmacology, two of them conducted at               vere rage reactions, wandering away from home,
the Institute (i.e., J. P. Brady, Levitt, & Baydan,        gradual loss of speech, an excessive reaction to
1962; Zimmerman & Baydan, 1963; see also                   changes in his daily routine, and withdrawing
Davis, 1971). The other six appeared in reviews            to a corner where he would remain for weeks.
of childhood autism (i.e., Ornitz, 1973), drug             (p. 90)
effects on learning (i.e., Freeman, 1966) and on          In their pharmacology report, they noted
clinical outcome (i.e., Aman & Singh, 1986),           that he had no speech, few interactions with
behavior modification in childhood psychosis           people or toys, and “frequent severe temper tan-
(i.e., Leff, 1968), and the integration of drug        trums and atavistic performances” (p. 84). Over
interventions with behavioral and psychologi-          the course of their human operant research,
cal treatments (i.e., Sevin, Bowers-Stephens,          Ferster and DeMyer (1962) observed that the
Hamilton, & Ford, 2001; Willner & Gordon,              children’s “tantrums declined continuously”
1969). With this background, we turn to Fer-           (p. 93) and, in their pharmacology report,
ster and DeMyer’s report.                              noted that their participant’s performance had
                                                       “developed continuously and come under the
8                               Edward K. Morris and Charryse M. Fouquette

control of the automatic devices” in the human         behavioral neuroscience that was consistent
operant research.                                      with these interests (see Aprison, 2001). Over
                                                       luncheons at the Institute, Ferster, DeMyer, and
The Drug                                               Aprison discussed conducting and collaborating
    The drug Ferster and DeMyer (1961b) used           on research (J. Hingtgen, personal communica-
in their pharmacology research was prochlorper-        tion, October 14, 2008). With DeMyer, Ferster
azine, which belongs to the phenothiazine class        conducted research on autism; with Aprison,
of “typical” or first-generation antipsychotic         he collaborated on behavioral neuroscience re-
drugs, of which chlorpromazine (Thorazine) is          search (Aprison & Ferster, 1960, 1961a, 1961b,
the best known. In the early 1950s, chlorpro-          1961c; see also Ferster & Aprison, 1960).3
mazine was found to improve the emotional
and cognitive functioning of adult psychiatric         The Apparatus
patients and, in 1954, was approved by the                 The apparatus Ferster and DeMyer (1961b)
US Food and Drug Administration as the                 used in their research was a three-window MTS
first antipsychotic drug for this population.          device. What they described of it was that
Before then, patients were sedated, received           sample stimuli – colored dots -- were presented
electroconvulsive or insulin shock therapy, or         in the lighted center window, while the side win-
underwent psychosurgery. Chlorpromazine was            dows remained dark. When the child touched
so effective that tens of thousands of psychiatric     the sample, the center window went dark and
residents were released from institutions over         the side windows were lighted – a successive,
the next decade (Swazey, 1974). It was also used       but not delayed MTS procedure. A colored
to treat temper tantrums and hyperactivity in          dot corresponding to the sample appeared in
children with mental retardation and emotional         one of the side windows, while a different col-
disturbances (e.g., Bair & Herold, 1955; Freed              3
                                                              Ferster and Aprison came to collaborate in the following
& Peifer, 1956). By the late 1960s, however,           way. On arriving at the Institute, Ferster set up a display in its
chlorpromazine was discontinued for long-              lobby of a pigeon in an operant chamber and a sign that said that
                                                       the pigeon would “work for food” if a penny were dropped into a
term psychiatric treatment because it eventually       slot (Aprison, 2001, p. 12). The pigeon “worked” on a multiple
produced debilitating side-effects (e.g., tardive      FR-50/FI-10’ schedule of reinforcement. At the time, Aprison
                                                       was looking for an animal model of depression and, when he saw
dyskinesia). Today, it is only prescibed for the       the pigeon’s steady-state performances, he became “ecstatic” (p.
short-term treatment of aggression, anxiety,           13) about the preparation and convinced Ferster to collaborate
                                                       with him. As Aprison related later:
nausea, and vomiting. Prochlorperazine (Com-                The first step in this program was a specification of the
pazine) is a derivative of chlorpromazine, yet 10           behavior of the animal for which the biochemical correlate
                                                            was sought. Because the Skinnerian psychologists had already
to 20 times more potent, but is also no longer              developed techniques that could provide predictable baselines
prescribed for long-term treatment for the same             in a systematic account of animal behavior and because the
                                                            reproducibility of the behavioral baseline was comparable
reasons. It is used mainly for the short-term               to that obtained in pharmacological bioassay techniques, we
treatment of nausea and vomiting caused by                  believed that such advances made it possible to quantitatively
                                                            measure the behavior of an animal objectively. (Aprison,
chemotherapy and radiation therapy.                         2001, p. 14).
    Ferster and DeMyer may have been in-                    Their collaboration yielded among the earliest studies on the
                                                       neurochemical correlates of behavior (Aprison & Ferster, 1960,
terested in prochlorperazine because of chlo-          1961a, 1961b, 1961c; see Ferster & Aprison, 1960). It also fore-
rpromazine’s effectiveness with children who           told the value now placed on interdisciplinary research. In 1959,
                                                       Aprison and Ferster submitted a three-year grant proposal to the
had behavior problems similar to those of the          National Institute of Mental Health (est. 1949), titled “Neuro-
participants in their human operant research           logical Correlates of Behavior.” Later that year, Aprison reported:
                                                            I received a call from the [NIMH] study section secretary…
(e.g., tantrums; Ferster & DeMyer, 1961a,                   who informed me that the grant was approved, and that the
1962). Ferster was already conducting research              committee had recommended that it be extended for 2 ad-
                                                            ditional years. I quickly accepted the report and asked how
on chlorpromazine with pigeons at the Institute             the decision had been made. He told me that the committee
(e.g., Ferster, Appel, & Hiss, 1962). In addition,          had not only thought the experiments were important but
                                                            also they were pleased that two young and proven inves-
Morris Aprison (1923-2007), a Principal Inves-              tigators from different fields of study had chosen to work
tigator in Biochemistry at the Institute (1956-             together in an overlapping area of research that should be
                                                            supported. (p. 15)
1997), had launched a program of research in
Rescuing Ferster and Demyer                                     9

ored dot appeared in the other. Correct color       tions were higher on the FR-4 schedule (range:
matches produced coins on a fixed-ratio (FR)        145-210) than on the FR-2 schedule (range:
schedule that could be used to obtain “a variety    90-135). Given the schedule parameters, this
of reinforcers such as food or toys, a view of a    would have been expected (Ferster & Skinner,
trained monkey, a television set, or an electric    1957), but its replication with children with
train set.” Incorrect matches produced 2-sec        autism could not have been assumed. Within
time outs. This was likely the same MTS device      the experiments, prochlorperazine increased
Ferster and DeMyer (1962) used in their hu-         the number of responses per condition rela-
man operant research, but it was programmed         tive to the placebo control. In Experiment I,
differently there: It used a simultaneous MTS       the increases were between 50% and 125%;
procedure; the stimuli became progressively         in Experiment 2, they were between 9% and
more complex (e.g., from colored dots to geo-       45%. Although the ranges overlapped in both
metric figures); and incorrect matches produced     experiments, the differences became statistically
6-sec time outs.                                    significant. Finally, the participant erred on only
    Ferster and DeMyer’s (1961b) use of the         5% of the MTS trials. In their human operant
MTS apparatus in their pharmacology research        research, Ferster and DeMyer (1962) presented
was likely influenced by its use in their human     cumulative records of his correct and incorrect
operant research. In both cases, they might have    MTS performances, showing how clearly dif-
been influenced by Ferster’s ongoing research       ferentiated they became.
on the MTS performances of pigeons, in which
he was studying the effects of intermittently       The Discussion
reinforcing correct matches and of time-out             Ferster and DeMyer (1961b) did not sum-
on errors (e.g., Ferster, 1960a; Ferster & Ap-      marize their results, discuss any limitations of
pel, 1961; Zimmerman & Ferster, 1963). Joe          their methods, integrate their findings with
Zimmerman was replicating this research at the      the literature, or suggest future research. They
Institute with college students (Zimmerman &        did, though, include Dews (1958) and Kelle-
Baydan, 1963).                                      her, Riddle, and Cook (1960) among their
                                                    references, but without citing them in the text.
Method                                              Without the citations, we are unsure of their
    Ferster and DeMyer’s dependent variable was     function. They might have been a basis for their
the median number of correct MTS responses in       research or have been research with which their
each condition of two experiments. In Experi-       findings could be integrated.
ment I, correct matches were reinforced on an           Dews (1958) was a JEAB article reporting
FR-2 schedule. The independent variable was         that pigeons’ food-reinforced key pecking in-
three 0.5mg doses of prochlorperazine admin-        creased on the FI component of a mixed FR-FI
istered at 8:00am, 2:00pm, and 8:00pm daily.        schedule at high, but not low doses of chlorpro-
The research design was an A-B-A (placebo-          mazine. The higher doses attenuated pauses in
drug-placebo) withdrawal design across 14, 7,       responding between the FR and FI components.
and 20 sessions, respectively. In Experiment II,    Perhaps Ferster and DeMyer sought to replicate
correct matches were reinforced on an FR-4          this rate-altering effect with prochlorperazine
schedule. The independent variable was three        and a child with autism.
10mg doses of prochlorperazine, administered            Kelleher et al. (1960) was an abstract from
at the same times of day. The research design       the Federation Proceedings reporting that both
was a B-A-B-A withdrawal design across 13, 11,      chlorpromazine and prochlorperazine increased
3, and 11 sessions, respectively.                   pigeons’ rates of pecking an observation key
                                                    which made food available for pecking a sec-
The Results                                         ond key. This replicated Dews’s (1958) results
   In both experiments, the participant’s MTS       with chlorpromazine, so perhaps Ferster and
responding in the drug and the placebo condi-       DeMyer sought to replicate this with the MTS
10                               Edward K. Morris and Charryse M. Fouquette

performance of a child with autism, but we do           discrimination learning on a “computer-con-
not know.                                               trolled operant conditioning apparatus” (e.g.,
                                                        discriminated lever-pressing to auditory-visual
                   Discussion                           stimuli). In this context, discrimination learning
                                                        was a means for assessing drug effects, not for
    In our introduction, we observed that Ferster       analyzing them. Her research program was thus
and DeMyer’s (1961b) report may be the first            more applied than basic (see, e.g., Anderson,
and only one of its kind. This can be supported         Campbell, Adams, Small, Perry, & Shell, 1989;
by several lines of evidence. First, in the nine        Anderson, Campbell, Grega, Perry, Small, &
Web of Science citations, Ferster and DeMyer            Green, 1984; Campbell, M. et al., 1978, 1988;
(1961b) was sometimes the only (i.e., Leff,             Campbell, Anderson, Small, Perry, Green, &
1968), but always the earliest citation to basic        Caplan 1982; Cohen, Campbell, Posner, Small,
or applied behavioral pharmacology research             Triebel, & Anderson, 1980).
with children with autism (e.g., Aman & Singh,              On the methodological count, Campbell
1986; Sevin, Bowers-Stephens, Hamilton, &               randomly assigned the children to one of two
Ford, 2001). Second, over the next decade,              experimental groups – an A-B-A withdrawal
behavioral pharmacology research with chil-             design group and a B-A-B withdrawal design
dren with developmental disabilities addressed          group (A = placebo, B = drug) – for two to ten
clinically relevant behavior (e.g., body rocking),      sessions per condition. Her results were that
which made it applied, not basic, research (e.g.,       haloperidol reduced the symptoms and facili-
Davis, 1971; Hollis, 1968; Hollis & St. Omer,           tated discrimination learning (i.e., changes in
1972). In addition, the participants in these           discrimination indexes), while fenfluramine
studies were drawn from undifferentiated popu-          had little effect on the symptoms and hindered
lations of children with developmental disabili-        discrimination learning (e.g., Anderson et al.,
ties (e.g., autism, retardation, hyperactivity),        1984; Campbell et al., 1982, 1988). Although
and thus may not have included any children             Campbell used within-subject withdrawal de-
with autism. Third, only one of the other 28            signs, she grouped the children by condition;
behavioral pharmacology studies published in            no individual data were reported. Thus, her
JEAB in the three years before and after Ferster        research was a within-group, not a within-
and DeMyer’s publication used human partici-            individual, experimental analysis of behavior.
pants. This was Dews and Morse (1958), who              For the record, Campbell neither cited nor ref-
used medical students. Outside of JEAB, basic           erenced Ferster and DeMyer (1961b) in this or
human behavioral pharmacological research was           any related research (e.g., Anderson et al., 1989;
generally conducted with adult volunteers (e.g.,        Cohen et al., 1980) or in her major review of the
J. P. Brady et al., 1962) or hospitalized adult         pharmacotherapy research in autism (Campbell,
psychiatric patients (e.g., Lindsley, 1956).            Cohen, & Anderson, 1981).
    The main challenge to our observation that              Other than Campbell’s studies, behavioral
Ferster and DeMyer (1961b) was the earliest             pharmacology research in autism has mainly as-
and only report of its kind is, perhaps, Magda          sessed the effects of drugs on clinically relevant
Campbell’s program of research in the late 1970s        behavior within individuals (e.g., aggression;
and the 1980s, but the challenge fails on two           Burgio, Page, & Capriotti, 1985) or across
counts, one substantive, the other methodologi-         them in clinical trials (see Barnard, Young,
cal. On the substantive count, Campbell and             Pearson, Geddes, & O’Brien, 2002; Kennedy &
her colleagues studied the effects of haloperidol       Meyer, 1998). As a result, Ferster and DeMyer’s
and fenfluramine on (a) the behavioral “symp-           (1961b) report is likely the earliest and only
toms” of children with autism (e.g., attention          basic behavioral pharmacology research using
span, hyperactivity, object relations, stereotypy)      both operant apparatus and measures and a
measured via direct observation, automated              within-individual replication design to analyze
detectors, and rating scales and (b) the children’s     the behavior of a child with autism (or of any
Rescuing Ferster and Demyer                                                    11

children at all). If so, then it is, arguably, a land-   develop. During the past two decades, autism
mark report in the history of autism, behavior           has become recognized as a problem of such
analysis, and behavioral pharmacology.                   profound individual and social importance that
                                                         behavioral neuroscience research has begun to
Warranting a Rescue                                      address it (e.g., Sparks et al., 2002; see Amaral,
     Notwithstanding the evidence supporting             Schumann, & Wu Nordahl, 2008; Minshew &
our observation regarding the priority and               Williams, 2007). Basic behavioral pharmacolo-
uniqueness of Ferster and DeMyer’s (1961b)               gy research with children with autism, thus, may
report, the report is deficient in several regards,      follow (see J. V. Brady, 1991). In fact, it should
which leads us to consider whether it warrants           follow for an important reason: Drugs are be-
our having rescued it. Several arguments may             ing prescribed for children whose effects and
be made for and against this.                            utility have only been studied and established
     Arguments against. Ferster and DeMyer’s             with adults (Brown & Sammons, 2002). This
report was deficient in three ways. First, the           is both a clinical and ethical concern (Ameri-
research was conducted with just one partici-            can Psychological Association, 2006; Vitiello,
pant, perhaps an idiosyncratic one, and was not          2003). Should a program of basic behavioral
replicated across any others. It was also not            pharmacology research emerge in autism, then
obviously informed by the behavioral pharma-             Ferster and DeMyer’s report will have proved,
cology literature of the day, for instance, about        at least, prescient.
prochlorperazine or what the drug might have                 These arguments for rescuing Ferster and
revealed about children with autism or their             DeMyer’s (1961b) report from history still beg
MTS performances. And, it did not analyze                the question of why their report has not been
which aspects of the child’s performance were            generative. Among the reasons may be that,
differentially sensitive to the drug, that is, the       first, researchers have had ethical qualms about
drug’s mechanism of action. Second, their re-            conducting such research with children because
port did not discuss the basis for the research          their developmental course might be adversely
or any limitations in its methods, integrate its         affected by the drugs and because they can not
findings with the extant literature, or suggest          voluntarily consent to participate in research.
any future research. It also listed articles in the      Moreover, children with autism should be in
reference section that were not cited in the text,       treatment or, at least, participating in applied,
which might have clarified these matters. Third,         not basic research.4 Their parents may not even
although the report may be a landmark in some            consent to the latter. Second, typical adult
respects, it neither influenced other research of
its day nor was generative for subsequent lines                4
                                                                 Whatever the reason for the lack of such research at the
                                                         time, it was not due to professionally prescribed ethics or gov-
of research. It is, perhaps, as one reviewer put         ernment policies in the United Sates. As for ethics, although the
it, just “an historical curiosity.”                      Nuremberg Code of 1947 required informed voluntary consent
                                                         for medical research with humans (see Annas & Grodin, 1992),
     Arguments for. The deficiencies notwith-            at the time Ferster and DeMyer published their report, the
standing, Ferster and DeMyer’s (1961b) report            American Psychological Association’s (1959) ethical standards
                                                         required consent only “when the possibility of serious aftereffects”
warrants being rescued for two reasons. First,           existed (p. 282). The ethical principles of the American Medi-
the interplay between basic behavioral phar-             cal Association (1958) did not address consent at all, although
                                                         obtaining it was consistent with the Hippocratic oath, as well as
macology research and childhood autism were              with the dictum attributed to Hippocrates -- Primum non nocere
(and are) important and interesting enough in            (i.e., “First, not to harm”; see Smith, 2005). As for government
                                                         policies, the U. S. Public Health Service (est. 1798/1912) did not
themselves that the report should have influ-            have any policies for the independent review of human subjects
enced and been generative for other research.            research until 1966 (e.g., reviews by institutional review boards
                                                         at, for instance, universities and medical schools). Moreover, no
The historical curiosity may not be their report,        reviews were actually required until the passage of the National
but that it was not influential or generative.           Research Act in 1974, followed later that year by regulations
                                                         for establishing internal review boards -- Regulations for the
     Second, although no subsequent research             Protection of Human Subjects of Biomedical and Behavioral
was generated by the report, basic behavioral            Research (Code of Federal Regulations, Title 45, Public Welfare,
                                                         Department of Health and Human Services, Part 46, Protection
pharmacology research in autism may still                of Human Subjects).
12                                         Edward K. Morris and Charryse M. Fouquette

participants might be easier to recruit and have                     ological note (Levison, Ferster, Niemann, &
more useful collateral behavior (e.g., verbal                        Findley, 1964). Indeed, at the time, Travis
reports). Third, the developmental course of                         Thompson reported that Ferster “expressed no
atypically developing adults may be less likely to                   interest in pharmacology or anything remotely
be affected by drugs. In addition, basic research                    neurobiological” (personal communication,
with adults from whom treatments have failed                         May 3, 2008). Even at Indiana, Ferster may
may be more ethically acceptable. In any event,                      not have been fully engaged in behavioral
why Ferster and DeMyer’s research has not been                       neuroscience. In his research with Aprison, for
replicated or extended awaits further analysis.                      instance, he humorously (or humorlessly) re-
                                                                     ferred to 5-hydroxytryptophan, a serotonin-like
                         Postscript                                  neurotransmitter, as “5-hydroxy-chicken-fat” (J.
                                                                     Hingtgen, personal communication, October 4,
    Although Ferster was promoted to associate                       2008). Thus, the impetus for his pharmacology
professor in psychiatry in 1961, he left Indiana                     research with DeMyer may have been more
in 1962 to become the Executive Director of                          hers than his. She was interested in “develop-
the Institute for Behavioral Research (IBR) in                       ing an integrated neurobiological approach to
Silver Spring, MD.5 He remained in that and                          psychiatric disorders” (see, e.g., DeMyer, 1975a,
related positions until 1968, when he was hired                      1975b). Ferster was not (T. Thompson, personal
as a professor in the Department of Psychology                       communication, May 3, 2008).
at Georgetown University. In 1969, he moved                              This point notwithstanding, Ferster had
to American University, where he remained                            evinced some interest in behavioral neurosci-
until his death in 1981 (Boren, 1981; Keller,                        ence prior to moving to Indiana. As he was
1981).6                                                              completing his work with Skinner, Skinner
    Between 1962 and 1981, Ferster published                         (1983) noted:
both basic and applied behavioral research (e.g.,                       For a time, he had planned to work with Walter
Ferster, 1968b; Ferster & Hammer, 1965) and                             Rosenblith at MIT on the operant condition-
developed an interest in psychotherapy (e.g.,                           ing of nerve impulses from the brain of various
Ferster, 1979), especially for depression (e.g.,                        preparations, allowing efferent impulses to
Ferster, 1973), but published no more research                          operate the programming equipment normally
in behavioral pharmacology, only a method-                              operated by pecking a key or pressing a lever. (p.
      5
        When Ferster left Indiana, Joseph N. Hingtgen took his          111; see also Skinner, 1987)
place, both with Aprison and DeMyer. With Aprison, he collabo-           Moreover, the Archives for the History
rated on animal model research on the neurochemical correlates
of depression (Aprison & Hingtgen, 1970, 1993). With DeMyer,         of American Psychology houses two boxes of
he conducted behavior-analytic research with children with           Ferster’s papers, one of which contains a folder
autism (e.g., Hingtgen & Coulter, 1967; Hingtgen, Coulter, &
Churchill, 1967; Hingtgen, Sanders, & DeMyer, 1965; Hingtgen         labeled “Interpreting Drug-Behavior Effects
& Trost, 1966).                                                      with a Functional Analysis of Behavior” (Box
      6
        Aprison remained at Indiana, where he had a distinguished
career in behavioral neuroscience, discovering, for instance, gly-   M93, Folder 1). Its contents are (a) a near-final
cine’s role as a neurotransmitter inhibitor (Aprison & Werman,       draft of a co-authored paper with James Appel
1965; see Aprison, 1990). In 1975, he received the Gold Medal
Award from the Society of Biological Psychiatry and, in 1978,        (not DeMyer) by the same title as the folder;
was named Indiana University’s first Distinguished Professor of      (b) an independent abstract, rough draft, and
Neurobiology and Biochemistry. He retired in 1997 as profes-
sor emeritus and passed away in 2007. DeMyer also remained           draft of a presentation of the paper; and (c) some
at Indiana, directing the Research Center for Early Childhood        original data from the summer of 1960. Writ-
Schizophrenia in a Children’s Services wing of the Larue D.
Carter Memorial Hospital. In 1962, she received a grant from the     ten while Ferster et al. (1962) was both in press
National Institute of Mental Health that for many years funded       and just published, the unpublished paper was,
her research on autism’s diagnosis (DeMyer, Churchill, Pontius, &
Gilkey, 1971), prognosis (e.g., DeMyer, Barton, DeMyer, Norton,      as its title suggests, a behavioral interpretation
Allen, & Steele, 1973), and biological basis (e.g., DeMyer, 1975a,   of drug-behavior interactions, supported by a
1975b). She also wrote a major review of the autism literature
(e.g., DeMyer, Hingtgen, & Jackson, 1981) and books on parents       review of the basic behavioral pharmacology
and their children with autism (DeMyer, 1979). Today, she is a       research of the day (1955-1962) and illustrated
professor emerita of psychiatry at the Indiana University School
of Medicine.                                                         with original data, presumably the same data as
Rescuing Ferster and Demyer                                    13

(or similar to) those in the folder. The research                        References
was funded by a grant from the National Sci-
ence Foundation (G-7617), the same grant               Aman, M. G., & Singh, N. N. (1986). A critical
that supported Ferster and Appel (1961) and               appraisal of recent drug research in mental
Ferster et al. (1962). Ferster was presumably the         retardation: The Coldwater studies. Journal
principal investigator.                                   of Mental Deficiency Research, 30, 203-216.
    Although he conducted no more pharma-              Amaral, D. G., Schumann, C. M., & Wu Nor-
cology research after leaving Indiana, Ferster            dahl, C. (2008). Neuroanatomy of autism.
did retain an interest in autism, publishing              Trends in Neuroscience, 31, 137-145.
articles and chapters on its analysis and treat-       American Medical Association (1958). Prin-
ment (e.g., Ferster, 1966, 1967, 1968a; Ferster           ciples of medical ethics. The Journal of the
& Simons, 1966). He also consulted with the               American Medical Association (Special Edi-
Linwood Children’s Center in Ellicott City,               tion). Chicago, IL: AMA Press.
MD. Founded in 1955 by Jeanne Simons                   American Psychological Association (1959).
(1909-2005), the Center became known for its              Ethical standards of psychologists. American
“Linwood Method” of treating children with                Psychologist, 14, 279-282.
autism (Simons & Oishi, 1987). The method              American Psychological Association (2006).
appears similar to today’s Developmental, In-             Report of the Working Group on Psychotropic
dividual Difference, Relationship (DIR) model             Medications for Children and Adolescents:
known as “Floor Time” (Greenspan & Wieder,                Pharmacological, psychosocial, and combined
2006). Although neither the Linwood Method                interventions for childhood disorders: Evidence
nor DIR is based on a science of behavior or              base, contextual factors, and future directions.
is evidence-based, they appear to involve the             Washington, DC: Author. (see http://www.
intensive free-operant differential reinforcement         apa.org/releases/PsychotropicMedication-
of clinically-relevant behavior (e.g., parent-child       sReport.pdf )
interactions) through successive approxima-            Anderson, L. T., Campbell, M., Adams, P.,
tions (i.e., shaping), along with some incidental         Small, A. M., Perry, R., & Shell, J. (1989).
teaching.                                                 The effects of haloperidol on discrimination
    At Linwood, Ferster observed the method,              learning and behavioral symptoms in autistic
interpreted it behaviorally, described its similari-      children. Journal of Autism and Developmen-
ties with operant conditioning (e.g., reinforce-          tal Disorders, 19, 227-239.
ment, extinction, shaping), and vouched for its        Anderson, L. T., Campbell, M., Grega, D. M.,
effectiveness (Ferster & Simons, 1966). He also           Perry, R., Small, A. M., & Green, M. D.
assisted Simons in operationalizing it, which             (1984). Haloperidol in the treatment of
reportedly improved staff training and treat-             infantile autism: Effects on learning and
ment fidelity (Ferster, 1967). This work was sup-         behavior. American Journal of Psychiatry,
ported by a $220,000 grant to Ferster through             141, 1195-1202.
IBR from the Bureau of Research at the Office          Annas, G. J., & Grodin, M. A. (Eds.). (1992).
of Education in the U.S. Department of Health,            The Nazi doctors and the Nuremberg Code:
Education, and Welfare, titled “Treatment and             Human rights in human experimentation.
Education of Autistic Children: Combined Ap-              New York: Oxford University Press.
plication of Clinical and Laboratory Methods”          Aprison, M. H. (1990). The discovery of the
(1965-1968). After the grant’s completion,                neurotransmitter role of glycine. In O. P.
Ferster’s interests turned more to psychotherapy.         Otterson & J. Storm-Mathesen (Eds.),
However, just prior to his death, he accepted an          Glycine neurotransmission (pp. 1-23). New
appointment to serve on the Advisory Board of             York: Wiley.
the Linwood Children’s Center, so perhaps his          Aprison, M (2001). Morris H. Aprison. In L.
work in autism was unfinished.                            R. Squire (Ed.), A history of neuroscience in
                                                          autobiography (Vol. 3, pp.-37). San Diego,
14                                Edward K. Morris and Charryse M. Fouquette

   CA: Academic Press.                                      ior, 90, 405-415.
Aprison, M. H., & Ferster, C. B. (1960). Be-             Boren, J. J. (1981). Charles B. Ferster, 1922-
   havioral effects of 5-hydroxytryptophan.                 1981. The Behavior Analyst, 4, 155-156.
   Experientia, 16, 159-162.                             Brady, J. P. (1990). The shaping of behavior
Aprison, M. H., & Ferster, C. B. (1961a).                   therapy: A historical perspective: Ferster
   Neurochemical correlates of behavior. I.                 was often first. The Behavior Therapist, 13,
   Quantitative measurements of the behav-                  113-114.
   ioral effects of the serotonin precursor,             Brady, J. P., Levitt, E. E., & Baydan, N. (1962).
   5-hydroxytrytophan. Journal of Pharmacol-                The operant-reinforcement paradigm in the
   ogy and Experimental Therapeutics, 131,                  study of drug effects. Diseases of the Nervous
   100-107.                                                 System, 23, 497-502.
Aprison, M. H., & Ferster, C. B. (1961b). Neu-           Brady, J. V. (1991). Behavioral pharmacology in
   rochemical correlates of behavior. II. Cor-              the era of neuroscience. Journal of the Experi-
   relation of brain monoamine oxidase activity             mental Analysis of Behavior, 56, 167-169.
   with behavioral changes after iproniazed              Branch, M. N. (2006). Roger Kelleher, behavior
   and 5 hydroxytryptophan administration.                  analyst. Journal of the Experimental Analysis
   Journal of Neurochemistry, 6, 350-357.                   of Behavior, 86, 381-384.
Aprison, M. H., & Ferster, C. B. (1961c).                 Brown, R. T., & Sammons, M. T. (2002).
   Serotonin and behavior. Recent Advances in               Pediatric psychopharmacology: A review
   Biological Psychiatry, 3, 151-162.                       of new developments and recent research.
Aprison, M. H., & Hingtgen, J. N. (1970).                   Professional Psychology: Research and Practice,
   Neurochemical correlates of behavior.                    33, 135-147.
   International Review of Neurobiology, 13,             Burgio, L. D., Page, T. J., & Capriotti, R. M.
   325-341.                                                 (1985). Clinical behavioral pharmacology:
Aprison, M. H., & Hingtgen, J. N. (1993).                   Methods for evaluating medications and
   A neurochemist’s perspective on human                    contingency management. Journal of Applied
   depression and stress. In T. Kariya & M.                 Behavior Analysis, 18, 45-59.
   Nakagawara (Eds.), Affective disorders: Per-          Campbell, M., Adams, P., Small, A. M., Cur-
   spectives on basic research and clinical practice        ran, E. L., Overall, J. E., Anderson, L. T.,
   (pp. 3-25). Tokyo: Seiwa Shoten.                         Lynch, N., & Perry, R. (1988). Efficacy and
Aprison, M. H., & Werman, R. (1965). The                    safety of fenfluramine in autistic children.
   distribution of glycine in cat spinal cord and           Journal of the American Academy of Child and
   roots. Life Science, 4, 2075-2083.                       Adolescent Psychiatry, 27, 434-439.
Bair, H. V., & Herold, W. (1955). Efficacy of            Campbell, M., Anderson, L. T., Meier, M., Co-
   chlorpromazine in hyperactive mentally                   hen, I. L., Small, A. M., Samit, C., & Sachar,
   retarded children. Archives of Neurology and             E. J. (1978). A comparison of haloperidol,
   Psychiatry, 74, 363-364.                                 behavior therapy, and their interaction in
Barnard, A. L., Young, H., Pearson, A. D. J.,               autistic children. Journal of the American
   Geddes, J, & O’Brien, G. (2002). A system-               Academy of Child Psychiatry, 17, 640-655.
   atic review of the use of atypical antipsychot-       Campbell, M., Anderson, L. T., Small, A. M.,
   ics in autism. Journal of Psychopharmacology,            Perry, R., Green, W. H., & Caplan, R.
   16, 93-101.                                              (1982). The effects of haloperidol on learn-
Barrett, J. E. (2006). Behavioral determinants              ing and behavior in autistic children. Journal
   of drug action: Contributions of Peter B.                of Autism and Developmental Disabilities,
   Dews. Journal of the Experimental Analysis               12, 167-175.
   of Behavior, 86, 359-370.                             Campbell, M., Cohen, I. L., & Anderson, L.
Barrett, J. E. (2008). Pioneer in behavioral                T. (1981). Pharmacotherapy for autistic
   pharmacology: A tribute to Joseph V. Brady.              children: A summary of research. Canadian
   Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behav-           Journal of Psychiatry, 26, 265-273.
Rescuing Ferster and Demyer                                  15

Cohen, I. L., Campbell, M., Posner, D., Small,         servations on an operant in human subjects
  A. M., Triebel, D, & Anderson, L. T. (1980).         and its modification by dextroamphetamine.
  Behavioral effects of haloperidol in young           Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behav-
  autistic children. Journal of the American           ior, 1, 359-364.
  Academy of Child Psychiatry, 19, 665-667.         Dewsbury, D. A. (2003). Conflicting ap-
Davis, K. V. (1971). The effects of drugs on           proaches: Operant psychology arrives at a
  stereotyped and nonstereotyped operant be-           primate laboratory. The Behavior Analyst,
  haviors in retardates. Psychopharamacologia,         26, 253-265.
  22, 195-213.                                      Dinsmoor, J. A. (1990). Academic roots: Co-
DeMyer, M. K. (1975a). The nature of the               lumbia University, 1943-1951. Journal of
  neuropsychological disability in autistic            the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 54,
  children. Journal of Autism and Childhood            129-149.
  Schizophrenia, 5, 109-128.                        Ferster, C. B. (1957). Concurrent schedules of
DeMyer, M. K. (1975b). Research in infantile           reinforcement in the chimpanzee. Science,
  autism: A strategy and its results. Biological       125, 1090-1091.
  Psychiatry, 10, 433-452.                          Ferster, C. B. (1960a). Intermittent reinforce-
DeMyer, M. K. (1979). Parents and children in          ment of matching to sample in the pigeon.
  autism. Washington, DC: V. H. Winston                Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Be-
  & Sons.                                              havior, 3, 259-272.
DeMyer, M. K., Barton, S., DeMyer, W. E.,           Ferster, C. B. (1960b). A semi-automatic teach-
  Norton, J. A., Allen, J. & Steele, R. (1973).        ing machine. Journal of the Experimental
  Prognosis in autism: A follow-up study. Jour-        Analysis of Behavior, 3, 245-246.
  nal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia,        Ferster, C. B. (1961). Positive reinforcement
  3, 199-246.                                          and behavioral deficits in autistic children.
DeMyer, M. K., Churchill, D. W., Pontius,              Child Development, 32, 437-456.
  W., & Gilkey, K. M. (1971). A comparison          Ferster, C. B. (1962). Performance development
  of five diagnostic systems for childhood             in autistic children (16mm film). Unpub-
  schizophrenia and infantile autism. Journal          lished.
  of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 1,         Ferster, C. B. (1966a). The repertoire of the
  175-189.                                             autistic child in relation to principles of
DeMyer, M. K., & Ferster, C. B. (1962). Teach-         reinforcement. In L. A. Gottschalk & A.
  ing new social behavior to schizophrenic             AuerLack (Eds.), Methods of research in
  children. Journal of the American Academy            psychotherapy (pp. 312-330). New York:
  of Child Psychiatry, 1, 443-461.                     Appleton-Century-Crofts.
DeMyer, M. K. Hingtgen, J. N., & Jackson,           Ferster, C. B. (1967). Perspectives in psychol-
  R. K. (1981). Infantile autism reviewed: A           ogy: XXV Transition from animal labora-
  decade of research. Schizophrenia Bulletin,          tory to clinic. The Psychological Record, 17,
  7, 388-451.                                          145-150.
Dews, P. B. (1955). Differential sensitivity to     Ferster, C. B. (1968a, November). The autistic
  pentobarbital of pecking performance in              child. Psychology Today, 61, 35-37.
  pigeons depending on the schedule of re-          Ferster, C. B. (1968b). Individualized instruc-
  ward. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental       tion in a large introductory psychology
  Therapeutics, 113, 393–401                           college course. The Psychological Record, 18,
Dews, P. B. (1958). The effects of chlorprom-          521-532.
  azine and promazine on performance on a           Ferster, C. B. (1970). Schedules of reinforce-
  mixed schedule of reinforcement. Journal             ment with B. F. Skinner. In P. B. Dews (Ed.),
  of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1,         Festschrift for B. F. Skinner (pp. 37-46). New
  73-82.                                               York: Irvington. [Reprinted in the Journal of
Dews, P. B., & Morse, W. H. (1958). Some ob-           the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2002,
16                              Edward K. Morris and Charryse M. Fouquette

   77, 303-311.]                                       Freed, H., & Peifer, C. A. (1956). Treatment of
Ferster, C. B. (1973). A functional analysis              hyperkinetic emotionally disturbed children
   of depression. American Psychologist, 28,              with prolonged administration of chlorpro-
   857-870.                                               mazine. American Journal of Psychiatry, 113,
Ferster, C. B. (1979). Psychotherapy from the             22-26.
   standpoint of a behaviorist. In J. D. Keehn         Freeman, R. D. (1966). Drug effects on learn-
   (Ed.), Psychopathology in animals (pp. 279-            ing in children: A selective review of the past
   304). New York: Academic Press.                        thirty years. Journal of Special Education, 1,
Ferster, C. B., & Appel, J. B. (1961). Pun-               17-44.
   ishment of S- responding in matching to             Ghezzi, P. M., Williams, W. L., & Carr, J. E.
   sample by time out from positive reinforce-            (Eds.). (1999). Autism: Behavior-analytic
   ment. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of          perspectives. Reno, NV: Context Press.
   Behavior, 4, 45-56.                                 Gollub, L. R. (2002). Between the waves:
Ferster, C. B., Appel, J. B., & Hiss, R. A.               Harvard Pigeon Lab 1955-1960. Journal
   (1962). The effect of drugs in a fixed-ratio           of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 77,
   performance suppressed by a pre-time-out               319-326.
   stimulus. Journal of the Experimental Analysis      Greenspan, S. I., & Wieder, S. (2006). Engaging
   of Behavior, 5, 73-88.                                 autism: Using the DIR floortime approach to
Ferster, C. B., & Aprison, M. H. (1960). A                help children relate, communicate, and think.
   multiple-unit pigeon apparatus. Journal                Cambridge, MA: Du Capo Press.
   of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 3,        Handen, B. L., & Lubetsky, M. (2005). Phar-
   165-166.                                               macotherapy in autism and related disorders.
Ferster, C. B., & DeMyer, M. K. (1961a). The              School Psychology Quarterly, 20, 155-171.
   development of performances in autistic             Heron, W. T., & Skinner, B. F. (1937). The
   children in an automatically controlled                effects of certain drugs and hormones on
   environment. Journal of Chronic Diseases,              conditioning and extinction [Abstract].
   13, 312-345.                                           Psychological Bulletin, 34, 741-742.
Ferster, C. B., & DeMyer, M. K. (1961b).               Hingtgen, J. N., & Coulter, S. K. (1967). Au-
   Increased performances of an autistic child            ditory control of operant behavior in mute
   with prochlorperazine administration. Jour-            autistic children. Perceptual and Motor Skills,
   nal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior,          25, 561-565.
   4, 84.                                              Hingtgen, J. N., Coulter, S. K., & Churchill,
Ferster, C. B., & DeMyer, M. K. (1962). A                 D. W. (1967). Intensive reinforcement of
   method for the experimental analysis of the            imitative behavior in mute autistic children.
   autistic child. American Journal of Orthopsy-          Archives of General Psychiatry, 17, 36-43.
   chiatry, 32, 89-98.                                 Hingtgen, J. N., Sanders, B. J., & DeMyer, M.
Ferster, C. B., & Hammer, C. (1965). Variables            K. (1976). Shaping cooperative responses
   determining the effects of delay in reinforce-         in early childhood schizophrenics. In L.
   ment. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of          Ullmann & L. Krasner (Eds.), Case studies
   Behavior, 8, 243-254.                                  in behavior modification (pp. 130-138). New
Ferster, C. B., Nurnberger, J. I., & Levitt, E. B.        York: Holt.
   (1962). The control of eating. The Journal of       Hingtgen, J. N., & Trost, F. C. (1966). Shap-
   Mathetics, 1, 87-109.                                  ing cooperative responses in early childhood
Ferster, C. B., & Simons, J. (1966). Behavior             schizophrenia: II. Reinforcement of mutual
   therapy with children. The Psychological               physical contact and vocal responses. In R.
   Record, 16, 65-71.                                     Ulrich, T. Stachnik, & J. Marby (Eds.),
Ferster, C. B., & Skinner, B. F. (1957). Sched-           Control of human behavior (pp. 110-113).
   ules of reinforcement. New York: Appleton              Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman.
   Century-Crofts.                                     Hollis, J. H. (1968). Chlorpromazine: Direct
Rescuing Ferster and Demyer                                       17

   measurement of differential behavioral ef-             letin, 69, 396-409.
   fect. Science, 159, 1487-1489.                      Levison, P. K., Ferster, C. B., Niemann, W. H.,
Hollis, J. H., & St. Omer, (1972). Direct mea-            & Findley, J. D. (1964). A method for train-
   surement of psychopharmacologic response:              ing unrestrained primates to receive drug
   Effects of chlorpromazine on motor behav-              injections. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of
   ior of retarded children. American Journal of          Behavior, 7, 253–254
   Mental Deficiency, 4, 397-407.                      Lindsley, O. R. (1956). Operant condition-
Kazdin, A. E. (1978). History of behavior modi-           ing methods applied to research in chronic
   fication: Experimental foundations of contem-          schizophrenia. Psychiatric Research Reports,
   porary research. Baltimore, MD: University             5, 118-139.
   Park Press.                                         Luiselli, J. K., Russo, D. C., Christian, W. P., &
Kazdin, A. E. (1982). History of behavior modi-           Wilczynski, S. M. (Eds.). (2008). Effective
   fication. In A. S. Bellack, M. Hersen, & A.            practices for children with autism. New York:
   E. Kazdin (Eds.), International handbook of            Oxford University Press.
   behavior modification and therapy (pp. 3-32).       McConahey, O. L., Thompson, T., & Zim-
   New York: Plenum.                                      merman, R. (1977). A token system for
Kelleher, R. T., & Morse, W. H. (1987). The Ye-           retarded women: Behavior therapy, drug
   rkes connection. Journal of the Experimental           administration, and their combination. In T.
   Analysis of Behavior, 48, 456-457.                     Thompson & J. Grabowski (Eds.), Behavior
Kelleher, R. T., Riddle, W. C., & Cook, L. A.             modification of the mentally retarded (2nd ed.,
   (1960). A behavioral analysis of qualitative           pp. 167-234). New York: Oxford.
   differences among phenothiazines. Federa-           Minshew, M. J., & Williams, D. L. (2007).
   tion Proceedings, 19(1), 22.                           The new neurobiology of autism: Context,
Keller, F. S. (1981). Charles Boris Ferster               connectivity, and neuronal organization.
   (1922-1981): An appreciation. Journal of               Archives of Neurology, 64, 945-950.
   the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 36,          Ornitz, E. M. (1973). Childhood autism: A re-
   299-301.                                               view of clinical and experimental literature.
Keller, F. S. (1986). A fire at Shermerhorn exten-        California Medicine, 118, 21-47.
   sion. The Behavior Analyst, 9, 139-146.             Pickens, R. (1977). Behavioral pharmacology: A
Kennedy, C. H., & Meyer, K. A. (1998). The                brief history. In T. Thompson & P. B. Dews
   use of psychotropic drugs for people with              (Eds.), Advances in behavioral pharmacology
   severe disabilities and challenging behavior:          (Vol. 1, pp. 229-257). New York: Academic
   Current status and future directions. Jour-            Press.
   nal of the Association for Persons with Severe      Poling, A. (1986). A primer of human behavioral
   Handicaps, 23, 83-97.                                  pharmacology. New York: Plenum.
Krasner, L. (1990). History of behavior modi-          Poling, A. (2000). Introduction. In A. Poling
   fication. In A. S. Bellack, M. Hersen, & A.            & T. Byrne (Eds.), Behavioral pharmacology
   E. Kazdin (Eds.), International handbook of            (pp. 9-24). Reno, NV: Context Press.
   behavior modification and therapy (pp. 3-25).       Schaal, D. W., & Hackenberg, T. (1994).
   New York: Plenum.                                      Toward a functional analysis of drug treat-
Laties, V. G. (1987). Society of the Experimen-           ment for behavior problems of people with
   tal Analysis of Behavior: The first thirty years       developmental disabilities. American Journal
   (1957-1987). Journal of the Experimental               on Mental Retardation, 99, 123-140.
   Analysis of Behavior, 48, 495-512.                  Sevin, J. A., Bowers-Stephens, C., Hamilton,
Laties, V. G. (2003). Behavior analysis and the           M. L., & Ford, A. (2001). Integrating be-
   growth of behavioral pharmacology. The                 havioral and pharmacological interventions
   Behavior Analyst, 26, 235-252.                         in treating clients with psychiatric disorders
Leff, R. (1968). Behavior modification and the            and mental retardation. Research in Develop-
   psychoses of childhood. Psychological Bul-             mental Disabilities, 22, 463-485.
18                              Edward K. Morris and Charryse M. Fouquette

Simons, J., & Oishi, S. (1987). The hidden child:         disorders: Identification, education, and treat-
   The Linwood method for reaching the autistic           ment (3rd ed.) (pp. 395-492). Mahwah, NJ:
   child. Bethesda, MD: Woodbine House                    Erlbaum.
Skinner, B. F. (1981). Charles B. Ferster -- A         Willner, M. M., & Gordon, L. E. (1969). Psy-
   personal memoir. Journal of the Experimental           chotropic drugs in children: Pharmacology
   Analysis of Behavior, 35, 259-261.                     as an adjunct to counseling and psycho-
Skinner, B. F. (1983). A matter of consequences.          therapy. Clinical Pediatrics, 8, 193-200.
   New York: Knopf.                                    Vitiello, B. (2003). Ethical considerations in
Skinner, B. F. (1987). Antecedents. Journal of            psychopharmacological research involving
   the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 48,             children and adolescents. Psychopharmacol-
 447-448.                                                 ogy, 171, 86-91.
Skinner, B. F., & Heron, W. T. (1937). Effects         Volkmar, F. R., Paul, R., Klin, A. & Cohen,
   of caffeine and benzedrine upon condition-             D. (Eds.). (2005). Handbook of autism and
   ing and extinction. The Psychological Record,          pervasive developmental disorders (3rd ed.).
   1, 340-346.                                            New York: Wiley.
Smith, C. M. (2005). Origin and uses of Pri-           Zimmerman, J., & Baydan, N. T. (1963).
   mum non nocere—Above all, do no harm!                  Punishment of S∆ responding of human
   Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 45, 371-             conditional matching to sample by time-
   377.                                                   out. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of
Sparks, B. F., Friedman, S. D., Shaw, D. W.,              Behavior, 6, 589-597.
   Aylward, E. H., Echelard, D., Artru, A. A.,         Zimmerman, J., & Ferster, C. B. (1963). In-
   et al. (2002). Brain structural abnormalities          termittent punishment of S∆ responding in
   in young children with autism spectrum                 matching to sample. Journal of the Experi-
   disorder. Neurology, 59, 184-192.                      mental Analysis of Behavior, 6, 349-355.
Swazey, J. (1974). Chlorpromazine in psychiatry:       Zimmerman, J., & Ferster, C. B. (1964). Some
   A study in therapeutic innovation. Cam-                notes on time out from reinforcement. Jour-
   bridge, MA: MIT Press.                                 nal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior,
Tsai, L. Y. (2005). Medical treatment in au-              7, 13-19.
   tism. In D. B. Zager (Ed.), Autism spectrum
You can also read