RESCUING FERSTER AND DEMYER'S BEHAVIORAL PHARMACOLOGY RESEARCH IN AUTISM FROM OBLIVION
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2009, 10, 5 - 18 NUMBER 1 (SUMMER 2009) 5 Rescuing Ferster and DeMyer’s behavioral pharmacology research in autism from oblivion Edward K. Morris University of Kansas and Charryse M. Fouquette University of Kansas and The Lovaas Institute for Early Intervention, Midwest Headquarters In 1961, Charles B. Ferster and Marian K. DeMyer published a one-page report in the Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior titled, “Increased Performances of an Autistic Child with Prochlor- perazine Administration.” It may be the first and only report of basic behavioral pharmacology research using both operant apparatus and measures and a within-individual replication design to analyze the behavior of a child with autism. However, it is now lost in history. In this article, we rescue the report by providing historical context (e.g., a literature review, the setting), describing and elaborating on the report (e.g., the participant, method), and relating the report to the research of its day and afterward. We also discuss the grounds on which the report warrants being rescued and, in a postscript, summarize Ferster’s later work in autism. Keywords: Ferster, DeMyer, history, autism, behavior analysis, behavioral pharmacology In 1961, Charles B. Ferster and Marian K. only report of basic behavioral pharmacology DeMyer (1961b) published a one-page report research using operant apparatus and measures in the Journal of the Experimental Analysis of and a within-individual replication design to Behavior (JEAB) titled, “Increased Performances analyze the behavior of a child with autism. of an Autistic Child with Prochlorperazine However, it is now lost in history – in the his- Administration.” It was perhaps the first and tory of autism, behavior analysis, and behavioral pharmacology. It is lost in what historians call This article is dedicated to Joseph V. Brady for his funda- oblivion.1 mental contributions to behavioral pharmacology, strong ad- vocacy for behavior analysis, and gentle encouragement of the Using primary source publications, citation first author. We thank Mike Aman, Len Green, Joe Hingtgen, analyses, personal communications, and archival Vic Laties, Al Poling, Steve Schroeder, Alan Silberberg, Travis Thompson, Steve Warren, and Stan Weiss for assisting our review records, we rescue Ferster and DeMyer’s report of the literature for this manuscript and, in some cases, for com- by placing it in historical context, describing the menting on earlier drafts. We thank the Archives for the History of American Psychology, the Harvard University Archives, and research in greater detail, and offering evidence the American University Archives for granting us access to their that supports our observations about its priority collections. And, we thank the manuscript’s several legions of reviewers for their suggestions. Correspondence may be sent to and uniqueness. The evidence notwithstanding, the first author at the Department of Applied Behavioral Science, the report is weak in several regards, leading us Dole Human Development Center, University of Kansas, 1000 Sunnyside Avenue, Lawrence, KS 66045. Phone: 785.864.4840; to discuss whether it warrants being rescued in fax: 785.864.5202; email: ekm@ku.edu. the first place. After weighing arguments for and 5
6 Edward K. Morris and Charryse M. Fouquette against this, we conclude that it does because it Medicine (est. 1903), located at the university’s represents an as-yet developed area of research Medical Center in Indianapolis, IN. He was, that has important clinical implications. At the in addition, appointed as a Principal Investiga- very least, we will have added depth and detail tor in Experimental Psychology at the medical to the early modern history of autism, behav- school’s Institute of Psychiatric Research (est. ior analysis, and behavioral pharmacology. We 1956), whose mission was to foster multidisci- begin with some background. plinary research in basic and applied biological psychiatry. At the Institute, Ferster conducted Background basic nonhuman research on, for instance, schedules of reinforcement and aversive control Charles B. Ferster (e.g., Ferster, 1960a; Zimmerman & Ferster, Charles B. Ferster (1922-1981) is renowned 1964) and conducted or consulted on applied in the history of behavior analysis. After receiv- research on obesity (Ferster, Nurnberger, & ing his undergraduate degree from Rutgers Levitt, 1962), stuttering (see J. P. Brady, 1990), University in 1947, he entered the graduate and teaching (Ferster, 1960b). program in psychology at Columbia University, Most notably, though, he and DeMyer from which he earned an M.A. in 1948 and a began perhaps the earliest program of behavior- Ph.D. in 1950. His mentors were Fred Keller analytic research on children with autism. This and Nat Schoenfeld (on the Columbia program, included a behavioral interpretation of autism see Dinsmoor, 1990; Keller, 1986). In 1950, (Ferster, 1961); human operant research on he became a research fellow with B. F. Skinner children with autism, funded by the National (1904-1990) in the Pigeon Lab at Harvard Uni- Association for Mental Health (NAMH) 2 and versity, the main result of which was, of course, Smith, Kline, & French (Fester & DeMyer, their text, Schedules of Reinforcement (Ferster & 1961a, 1962; see Ferster, 1962); an application Skinner, 1957; see Ferster, 1970; Skinner, 1981; of operant conditioning to autism, also funded on the lab at that time, see Gollub, 2002). In by NAMH (DeMyer & Ferster, 1962); and their 1955, Ferster took a position as a research as- behavioral pharmacology research, for which no sociate at the Yerkes Laboratories of Primate funding source was listed. Biology in Orange Park, FL, where he, Roger At the time, DeMyer was an assistant profes- Kelleher, and John Falk conducted operant sor in the Department of Psychiatry, a clinical research with chimpanzees (Dewsbury, 2003; consultant and research collaborator at the In- see, e.g., Ferster, 1957). At Yerkes, Ferster also stitute, and the Director of Children’s Services drew up plans for JEAB, which was founded in at the Larue D. Carter Memorial Hospital. This 1957 (Kelleher & Morse, 1987; Laties, 1987). was a state hospital for inpatient mental health He was its first editor -- 1958-1960. treatment and research, as well as a training site In 1957, Ferster took a position as an assis- for the medical school’s psychiatric residents and tant professor in the Department of Psychiatry the site for her research with Ferster. (est. 1940) at Indiana University’s School of 1 The report is available on-line through the Society for the Ferster and DeMyer (1961b) Experimental Analysis of Behavior (http://seab.envmed.rochester. When Ferster and DeMyer (1961b) pub- edu/) by searching for “Ferster DeMyer” (not “autism”), but its three paragraphs are condensed into one and its figure, footnotes, lished their report, behavioral pharmacology and references are missing. An October 25, 2008, Pub Med was still in its infancy. Although Skinner had author search for “ferster cb demyer mk” yielded Ferster and DeMyer (1961a, 1962), but not Ferster and DeMyer (1961b). conducted related research in the 1930s (see The full report, however, is accessible through the PubMed Heron & Skinner, 1937; Skinner & Heron, Central Archives link to Administrative Content (i.e., http:// www.Pubmedcentral.nih.gov/picrender.fcgi?artid=1403980&b 1937), the field was not founded until the lobtype=pdf ). Ferster and DeMyer (1961b), the way, is not the only report of behavioral pharmacology research in develop- 2 The NAMH was founded in 1950, but its roots lie in the mental disabilities lost in history. See, for example, McConahey, National Committee for Mental Hygiene (est. 1909). In 2006, Thompson, and Zimmerman’s (1977) chapter, “A Token System the NAMH became Mental Health America, “the country’s oldest for Retarded Women: Behavior Therapy, Drug Administration, and largest nonprofit organization addressing all aspects of mental and Their Combination.” health and mental illness” (http://www.nmha.org/go/history).
Rescuing Ferster and Demyer 7 mid-1950s (e.g., Dews, 1955; see Laties, The Report 2003; Pickens, 1977; Poling, 2000), with hu- man behavioral pharmacology emerging soon The Participant thereafter (e.g., Dews & Morse, 1958; see Ferster and DeMyer’s (1961b) participant Poling, 1986, pp. 3-20). Both were a melding was a 10-year-old boy who had been an inpatient of the experimental analysis of behavior with at the Larue D. Carter Memorial Hospital for pharmacology, a melding pursued in three the preceding three years and who, during the settings -- departments of psychology and 16 months prior to their pharmacology study, pharmacology, and the pharmaceutical industry had participated in their program of human (Laties, 2003). operant research (Ferster & DeMyer, 1961a, Although published early in the field’s 1962). In this research, children learned to press founding, Ferster and DeMeyer (1961b) is a key on a dispenser for “coins” that could be not cited in the histories of behavioral phar- used to operate “reinforcing devices” that lined macology (e.g., Laties, 2003; Pickens, 1977); the walls of a research room. Among the devices in biographies of the field’s founders, for were vending machines that dispensed food and instance, those of Joe Brady (Barrett, 2008), trinkets, a television, an electric train set, and Peter Dews (Barrett, 2006), or Roger Kelleher a three-window matching-to-sample (MTS) (Branch, 2006); or in recent reviews of behav- apparatus. With the coin dispenser and the de- ioral pharmacology in autism (e.g., Handen & vices, Ferster and DeMyer analyzed the effects of Lubetsky, 2005; Schaal & Hackenberg, 1994; reinforcement schedules (e.g., fixed-ratios) and Tsai, 2005). It is also not cited in the histories stimulus control (e.g., light-signaled reinforcer of applied behavior analysis (e.g., Kazdin, 1978, availability, matching-to-sample). 1982; Krasner, 1982) or in texts on applied When Ferster and DeMyer prepared their behavior analysis in autism (e.g., Ghezzi, Wil- 1962 human operant manuscript for publica- liams, & Carr, 1999; Luiselli, Russo, Christian, tion, not when they began that program of & Wilczynski, 2008; Volkmar, Paul, Klin, & research, their participants were an 11-year-old Cohen, 2005). girl and two boys, ages 3½ and 10. The older In a search of the Web of Science (April 27, boy was thus the likely participant in their 2008), we did find nine citations to Ferster pharmacology research. In their manuscript, and DeMyer’s (1961b) article, but they were, they described him this way: on average, over 35 years old and published in Thomas, aged 10 and hospitalized 3 years, has journals outside mainstream behavior analysis. been studied for 12 months. He had a normal Three of the citations appeared in introductions motor and speech development, speaking short to studies in basic and applied human behav- sentences until he was 2½, when he developed se- ioral pharmacology, two of them conducted at vere rage reactions, wandering away from home, the Institute (i.e., J. P. Brady, Levitt, & Baydan, gradual loss of speech, an excessive reaction to 1962; Zimmerman & Baydan, 1963; see also changes in his daily routine, and withdrawing Davis, 1971). The other six appeared in reviews to a corner where he would remain for weeks. of childhood autism (i.e., Ornitz, 1973), drug (p. 90) effects on learning (i.e., Freeman, 1966) and on In their pharmacology report, they noted clinical outcome (i.e., Aman & Singh, 1986), that he had no speech, few interactions with behavior modification in childhood psychosis people or toys, and “frequent severe temper tan- (i.e., Leff, 1968), and the integration of drug trums and atavistic performances” (p. 84). Over interventions with behavioral and psychologi- the course of their human operant research, cal treatments (i.e., Sevin, Bowers-Stephens, Ferster and DeMyer (1962) observed that the Hamilton, & Ford, 2001; Willner & Gordon, children’s “tantrums declined continuously” 1969). With this background, we turn to Fer- (p. 93) and, in their pharmacology report, ster and DeMyer’s report. noted that their participant’s performance had “developed continuously and come under the
8 Edward K. Morris and Charryse M. Fouquette control of the automatic devices” in the human behavioral neuroscience that was consistent operant research. with these interests (see Aprison, 2001). Over luncheons at the Institute, Ferster, DeMyer, and The Drug Aprison discussed conducting and collaborating The drug Ferster and DeMyer (1961b) used on research (J. Hingtgen, personal communica- in their pharmacology research was prochlorper- tion, October 14, 2008). With DeMyer, Ferster azine, which belongs to the phenothiazine class conducted research on autism; with Aprison, of “typical” or first-generation antipsychotic he collaborated on behavioral neuroscience re- drugs, of which chlorpromazine (Thorazine) is search (Aprison & Ferster, 1960, 1961a, 1961b, the best known. In the early 1950s, chlorpro- 1961c; see also Ferster & Aprison, 1960).3 mazine was found to improve the emotional and cognitive functioning of adult psychiatric The Apparatus patients and, in 1954, was approved by the The apparatus Ferster and DeMyer (1961b) US Food and Drug Administration as the used in their research was a three-window MTS first antipsychotic drug for this population. device. What they described of it was that Before then, patients were sedated, received sample stimuli – colored dots -- were presented electroconvulsive or insulin shock therapy, or in the lighted center window, while the side win- underwent psychosurgery. Chlorpromazine was dows remained dark. When the child touched so effective that tens of thousands of psychiatric the sample, the center window went dark and residents were released from institutions over the side windows were lighted – a successive, the next decade (Swazey, 1974). It was also used but not delayed MTS procedure. A colored to treat temper tantrums and hyperactivity in dot corresponding to the sample appeared in children with mental retardation and emotional one of the side windows, while a different col- disturbances (e.g., Bair & Herold, 1955; Freed 3 Ferster and Aprison came to collaborate in the following & Peifer, 1956). By the late 1960s, however, way. On arriving at the Institute, Ferster set up a display in its chlorpromazine was discontinued for long- lobby of a pigeon in an operant chamber and a sign that said that the pigeon would “work for food” if a penny were dropped into a term psychiatric treatment because it eventually slot (Aprison, 2001, p. 12). The pigeon “worked” on a multiple produced debilitating side-effects (e.g., tardive FR-50/FI-10’ schedule of reinforcement. At the time, Aprison was looking for an animal model of depression and, when he saw dyskinesia). Today, it is only prescibed for the the pigeon’s steady-state performances, he became “ecstatic” (p. short-term treatment of aggression, anxiety, 13) about the preparation and convinced Ferster to collaborate with him. As Aprison related later: nausea, and vomiting. Prochlorperazine (Com- The first step in this program was a specification of the pazine) is a derivative of chlorpromazine, yet 10 behavior of the animal for which the biochemical correlate was sought. Because the Skinnerian psychologists had already to 20 times more potent, but is also no longer developed techniques that could provide predictable baselines prescribed for long-term treatment for the same in a systematic account of animal behavior and because the reproducibility of the behavioral baseline was comparable reasons. It is used mainly for the short-term to that obtained in pharmacological bioassay techniques, we treatment of nausea and vomiting caused by believed that such advances made it possible to quantitatively measure the behavior of an animal objectively. (Aprison, chemotherapy and radiation therapy. 2001, p. 14). Ferster and DeMyer may have been in- Their collaboration yielded among the earliest studies on the neurochemical correlates of behavior (Aprison & Ferster, 1960, terested in prochlorperazine because of chlo- 1961a, 1961b, 1961c; see Ferster & Aprison, 1960). It also fore- rpromazine’s effectiveness with children who told the value now placed on interdisciplinary research. In 1959, Aprison and Ferster submitted a three-year grant proposal to the had behavior problems similar to those of the National Institute of Mental Health (est. 1949), titled “Neuro- participants in their human operant research logical Correlates of Behavior.” Later that year, Aprison reported: I received a call from the [NIMH] study section secretary… (e.g., tantrums; Ferster & DeMyer, 1961a, who informed me that the grant was approved, and that the 1962). Ferster was already conducting research committee had recommended that it be extended for 2 ad- ditional years. I quickly accepted the report and asked how on chlorpromazine with pigeons at the Institute the decision had been made. He told me that the committee (e.g., Ferster, Appel, & Hiss, 1962). In addition, had not only thought the experiments were important but also they were pleased that two young and proven inves- Morris Aprison (1923-2007), a Principal Inves- tigators from different fields of study had chosen to work tigator in Biochemistry at the Institute (1956- together in an overlapping area of research that should be supported. (p. 15) 1997), had launched a program of research in
Rescuing Ferster and Demyer 9 ored dot appeared in the other. Correct color tions were higher on the FR-4 schedule (range: matches produced coins on a fixed-ratio (FR) 145-210) than on the FR-2 schedule (range: schedule that could be used to obtain “a variety 90-135). Given the schedule parameters, this of reinforcers such as food or toys, a view of a would have been expected (Ferster & Skinner, trained monkey, a television set, or an electric 1957), but its replication with children with train set.” Incorrect matches produced 2-sec autism could not have been assumed. Within time outs. This was likely the same MTS device the experiments, prochlorperazine increased Ferster and DeMyer (1962) used in their hu- the number of responses per condition rela- man operant research, but it was programmed tive to the placebo control. In Experiment I, differently there: It used a simultaneous MTS the increases were between 50% and 125%; procedure; the stimuli became progressively in Experiment 2, they were between 9% and more complex (e.g., from colored dots to geo- 45%. Although the ranges overlapped in both metric figures); and incorrect matches produced experiments, the differences became statistically 6-sec time outs. significant. Finally, the participant erred on only Ferster and DeMyer’s (1961b) use of the 5% of the MTS trials. In their human operant MTS apparatus in their pharmacology research research, Ferster and DeMyer (1962) presented was likely influenced by its use in their human cumulative records of his correct and incorrect operant research. In both cases, they might have MTS performances, showing how clearly dif- been influenced by Ferster’s ongoing research ferentiated they became. on the MTS performances of pigeons, in which he was studying the effects of intermittently The Discussion reinforcing correct matches and of time-out Ferster and DeMyer (1961b) did not sum- on errors (e.g., Ferster, 1960a; Ferster & Ap- marize their results, discuss any limitations of pel, 1961; Zimmerman & Ferster, 1963). Joe their methods, integrate their findings with Zimmerman was replicating this research at the the literature, or suggest future research. They Institute with college students (Zimmerman & did, though, include Dews (1958) and Kelle- Baydan, 1963). her, Riddle, and Cook (1960) among their references, but without citing them in the text. Method Without the citations, we are unsure of their Ferster and DeMyer’s dependent variable was function. They might have been a basis for their the median number of correct MTS responses in research or have been research with which their each condition of two experiments. In Experi- findings could be integrated. ment I, correct matches were reinforced on an Dews (1958) was a JEAB article reporting FR-2 schedule. The independent variable was that pigeons’ food-reinforced key pecking in- three 0.5mg doses of prochlorperazine admin- creased on the FI component of a mixed FR-FI istered at 8:00am, 2:00pm, and 8:00pm daily. schedule at high, but not low doses of chlorpro- The research design was an A-B-A (placebo- mazine. The higher doses attenuated pauses in drug-placebo) withdrawal design across 14, 7, responding between the FR and FI components. and 20 sessions, respectively. In Experiment II, Perhaps Ferster and DeMyer sought to replicate correct matches were reinforced on an FR-4 this rate-altering effect with prochlorperazine schedule. The independent variable was three and a child with autism. 10mg doses of prochlorperazine, administered Kelleher et al. (1960) was an abstract from at the same times of day. The research design the Federation Proceedings reporting that both was a B-A-B-A withdrawal design across 13, 11, chlorpromazine and prochlorperazine increased 3, and 11 sessions, respectively. pigeons’ rates of pecking an observation key which made food available for pecking a sec- The Results ond key. This replicated Dews’s (1958) results In both experiments, the participant’s MTS with chlorpromazine, so perhaps Ferster and responding in the drug and the placebo condi- DeMyer sought to replicate this with the MTS
10 Edward K. Morris and Charryse M. Fouquette performance of a child with autism, but we do discrimination learning on a “computer-con- not know. trolled operant conditioning apparatus” (e.g., discriminated lever-pressing to auditory-visual Discussion stimuli). In this context, discrimination learning was a means for assessing drug effects, not for In our introduction, we observed that Ferster analyzing them. Her research program was thus and DeMyer’s (1961b) report may be the first more applied than basic (see, e.g., Anderson, and only one of its kind. This can be supported Campbell, Adams, Small, Perry, & Shell, 1989; by several lines of evidence. First, in the nine Anderson, Campbell, Grega, Perry, Small, & Web of Science citations, Ferster and DeMyer Green, 1984; Campbell, M. et al., 1978, 1988; (1961b) was sometimes the only (i.e., Leff, Campbell, Anderson, Small, Perry, Green, & 1968), but always the earliest citation to basic Caplan 1982; Cohen, Campbell, Posner, Small, or applied behavioral pharmacology research Triebel, & Anderson, 1980). with children with autism (e.g., Aman & Singh, On the methodological count, Campbell 1986; Sevin, Bowers-Stephens, Hamilton, & randomly assigned the children to one of two Ford, 2001). Second, over the next decade, experimental groups – an A-B-A withdrawal behavioral pharmacology research with chil- design group and a B-A-B withdrawal design dren with developmental disabilities addressed group (A = placebo, B = drug) – for two to ten clinically relevant behavior (e.g., body rocking), sessions per condition. Her results were that which made it applied, not basic, research (e.g., haloperidol reduced the symptoms and facili- Davis, 1971; Hollis, 1968; Hollis & St. Omer, tated discrimination learning (i.e., changes in 1972). In addition, the participants in these discrimination indexes), while fenfluramine studies were drawn from undifferentiated popu- had little effect on the symptoms and hindered lations of children with developmental disabili- discrimination learning (e.g., Anderson et al., ties (e.g., autism, retardation, hyperactivity), 1984; Campbell et al., 1982, 1988). Although and thus may not have included any children Campbell used within-subject withdrawal de- with autism. Third, only one of the other 28 signs, she grouped the children by condition; behavioral pharmacology studies published in no individual data were reported. Thus, her JEAB in the three years before and after Ferster research was a within-group, not a within- and DeMyer’s publication used human partici- individual, experimental analysis of behavior. pants. This was Dews and Morse (1958), who For the record, Campbell neither cited nor ref- used medical students. Outside of JEAB, basic erenced Ferster and DeMyer (1961b) in this or human behavioral pharmacological research was any related research (e.g., Anderson et al., 1989; generally conducted with adult volunteers (e.g., Cohen et al., 1980) or in her major review of the J. P. Brady et al., 1962) or hospitalized adult pharmacotherapy research in autism (Campbell, psychiatric patients (e.g., Lindsley, 1956). Cohen, & Anderson, 1981). The main challenge to our observation that Other than Campbell’s studies, behavioral Ferster and DeMyer (1961b) was the earliest pharmacology research in autism has mainly as- and only report of its kind is, perhaps, Magda sessed the effects of drugs on clinically relevant Campbell’s program of research in the late 1970s behavior within individuals (e.g., aggression; and the 1980s, but the challenge fails on two Burgio, Page, & Capriotti, 1985) or across counts, one substantive, the other methodologi- them in clinical trials (see Barnard, Young, cal. On the substantive count, Campbell and Pearson, Geddes, & O’Brien, 2002; Kennedy & her colleagues studied the effects of haloperidol Meyer, 1998). As a result, Ferster and DeMyer’s and fenfluramine on (a) the behavioral “symp- (1961b) report is likely the earliest and only toms” of children with autism (e.g., attention basic behavioral pharmacology research using span, hyperactivity, object relations, stereotypy) both operant apparatus and measures and a measured via direct observation, automated within-individual replication design to analyze detectors, and rating scales and (b) the children’s the behavior of a child with autism (or of any
Rescuing Ferster and Demyer 11 children at all). If so, then it is, arguably, a land- develop. During the past two decades, autism mark report in the history of autism, behavior has become recognized as a problem of such analysis, and behavioral pharmacology. profound individual and social importance that behavioral neuroscience research has begun to Warranting a Rescue address it (e.g., Sparks et al., 2002; see Amaral, Notwithstanding the evidence supporting Schumann, & Wu Nordahl, 2008; Minshew & our observation regarding the priority and Williams, 2007). Basic behavioral pharmacolo- uniqueness of Ferster and DeMyer’s (1961b) gy research with children with autism, thus, may report, the report is deficient in several regards, follow (see J. V. Brady, 1991). In fact, it should which leads us to consider whether it warrants follow for an important reason: Drugs are be- our having rescued it. Several arguments may ing prescribed for children whose effects and be made for and against this. utility have only been studied and established Arguments against. Ferster and DeMyer’s with adults (Brown & Sammons, 2002). This report was deficient in three ways. First, the is both a clinical and ethical concern (Ameri- research was conducted with just one partici- can Psychological Association, 2006; Vitiello, pant, perhaps an idiosyncratic one, and was not 2003). Should a program of basic behavioral replicated across any others. It was also not pharmacology research emerge in autism, then obviously informed by the behavioral pharma- Ferster and DeMyer’s report will have proved, cology literature of the day, for instance, about at least, prescient. prochlorperazine or what the drug might have These arguments for rescuing Ferster and revealed about children with autism or their DeMyer’s (1961b) report from history still beg MTS performances. And, it did not analyze the question of why their report has not been which aspects of the child’s performance were generative. Among the reasons may be that, differentially sensitive to the drug, that is, the first, researchers have had ethical qualms about drug’s mechanism of action. Second, their re- conducting such research with children because port did not discuss the basis for the research their developmental course might be adversely or any limitations in its methods, integrate its affected by the drugs and because they can not findings with the extant literature, or suggest voluntarily consent to participate in research. any future research. It also listed articles in the Moreover, children with autism should be in reference section that were not cited in the text, treatment or, at least, participating in applied, which might have clarified these matters. Third, not basic research.4 Their parents may not even although the report may be a landmark in some consent to the latter. Second, typical adult respects, it neither influenced other research of its day nor was generative for subsequent lines 4 Whatever the reason for the lack of such research at the time, it was not due to professionally prescribed ethics or gov- of research. It is, perhaps, as one reviewer put ernment policies in the United Sates. As for ethics, although the it, just “an historical curiosity.” Nuremberg Code of 1947 required informed voluntary consent for medical research with humans (see Annas & Grodin, 1992), Arguments for. The deficiencies notwith- at the time Ferster and DeMyer published their report, the standing, Ferster and DeMyer’s (1961b) report American Psychological Association’s (1959) ethical standards required consent only “when the possibility of serious aftereffects” warrants being rescued for two reasons. First, existed (p. 282). The ethical principles of the American Medi- the interplay between basic behavioral phar- cal Association (1958) did not address consent at all, although obtaining it was consistent with the Hippocratic oath, as well as macology research and childhood autism were with the dictum attributed to Hippocrates -- Primum non nocere (and are) important and interesting enough in (i.e., “First, not to harm”; see Smith, 2005). As for government policies, the U. S. Public Health Service (est. 1798/1912) did not themselves that the report should have influ- have any policies for the independent review of human subjects enced and been generative for other research. research until 1966 (e.g., reviews by institutional review boards at, for instance, universities and medical schools). Moreover, no The historical curiosity may not be their report, reviews were actually required until the passage of the National but that it was not influential or generative. Research Act in 1974, followed later that year by regulations for establishing internal review boards -- Regulations for the Second, although no subsequent research Protection of Human Subjects of Biomedical and Behavioral was generated by the report, basic behavioral Research (Code of Federal Regulations, Title 45, Public Welfare, Department of Health and Human Services, Part 46, Protection pharmacology research in autism may still of Human Subjects).
12 Edward K. Morris and Charryse M. Fouquette participants might be easier to recruit and have ological note (Levison, Ferster, Niemann, & more useful collateral behavior (e.g., verbal Findley, 1964). Indeed, at the time, Travis reports). Third, the developmental course of Thompson reported that Ferster “expressed no atypically developing adults may be less likely to interest in pharmacology or anything remotely be affected by drugs. In addition, basic research neurobiological” (personal communication, with adults from whom treatments have failed May 3, 2008). Even at Indiana, Ferster may may be more ethically acceptable. In any event, not have been fully engaged in behavioral why Ferster and DeMyer’s research has not been neuroscience. In his research with Aprison, for replicated or extended awaits further analysis. instance, he humorously (or humorlessly) re- ferred to 5-hydroxytryptophan, a serotonin-like Postscript neurotransmitter, as “5-hydroxy-chicken-fat” (J. Hingtgen, personal communication, October 4, Although Ferster was promoted to associate 2008). Thus, the impetus for his pharmacology professor in psychiatry in 1961, he left Indiana research with DeMyer may have been more in 1962 to become the Executive Director of hers than his. She was interested in “develop- the Institute for Behavioral Research (IBR) in ing an integrated neurobiological approach to Silver Spring, MD.5 He remained in that and psychiatric disorders” (see, e.g., DeMyer, 1975a, related positions until 1968, when he was hired 1975b). Ferster was not (T. Thompson, personal as a professor in the Department of Psychology communication, May 3, 2008). at Georgetown University. In 1969, he moved This point notwithstanding, Ferster had to American University, where he remained evinced some interest in behavioral neurosci- until his death in 1981 (Boren, 1981; Keller, ence prior to moving to Indiana. As he was 1981).6 completing his work with Skinner, Skinner Between 1962 and 1981, Ferster published (1983) noted: both basic and applied behavioral research (e.g., For a time, he had planned to work with Walter Ferster, 1968b; Ferster & Hammer, 1965) and Rosenblith at MIT on the operant condition- developed an interest in psychotherapy (e.g., ing of nerve impulses from the brain of various Ferster, 1979), especially for depression (e.g., preparations, allowing efferent impulses to Ferster, 1973), but published no more research operate the programming equipment normally in behavioral pharmacology, only a method- operated by pecking a key or pressing a lever. (p. 5 When Ferster left Indiana, Joseph N. Hingtgen took his 111; see also Skinner, 1987) place, both with Aprison and DeMyer. With Aprison, he collabo- Moreover, the Archives for the History rated on animal model research on the neurochemical correlates of depression (Aprison & Hingtgen, 1970, 1993). With DeMyer, of American Psychology houses two boxes of he conducted behavior-analytic research with children with Ferster’s papers, one of which contains a folder autism (e.g., Hingtgen & Coulter, 1967; Hingtgen, Coulter, & Churchill, 1967; Hingtgen, Sanders, & DeMyer, 1965; Hingtgen labeled “Interpreting Drug-Behavior Effects & Trost, 1966). with a Functional Analysis of Behavior” (Box 6 Aprison remained at Indiana, where he had a distinguished career in behavioral neuroscience, discovering, for instance, gly- M93, Folder 1). Its contents are (a) a near-final cine’s role as a neurotransmitter inhibitor (Aprison & Werman, draft of a co-authored paper with James Appel 1965; see Aprison, 1990). In 1975, he received the Gold Medal Award from the Society of Biological Psychiatry and, in 1978, (not DeMyer) by the same title as the folder; was named Indiana University’s first Distinguished Professor of (b) an independent abstract, rough draft, and Neurobiology and Biochemistry. He retired in 1997 as profes- sor emeritus and passed away in 2007. DeMyer also remained draft of a presentation of the paper; and (c) some at Indiana, directing the Research Center for Early Childhood original data from the summer of 1960. Writ- Schizophrenia in a Children’s Services wing of the Larue D. Carter Memorial Hospital. In 1962, she received a grant from the ten while Ferster et al. (1962) was both in press National Institute of Mental Health that for many years funded and just published, the unpublished paper was, her research on autism’s diagnosis (DeMyer, Churchill, Pontius, & Gilkey, 1971), prognosis (e.g., DeMyer, Barton, DeMyer, Norton, as its title suggests, a behavioral interpretation Allen, & Steele, 1973), and biological basis (e.g., DeMyer, 1975a, of drug-behavior interactions, supported by a 1975b). She also wrote a major review of the autism literature (e.g., DeMyer, Hingtgen, & Jackson, 1981) and books on parents review of the basic behavioral pharmacology and their children with autism (DeMyer, 1979). Today, she is a research of the day (1955-1962) and illustrated professor emerita of psychiatry at the Indiana University School of Medicine. with original data, presumably the same data as
Rescuing Ferster and Demyer 13 (or similar to) those in the folder. The research References was funded by a grant from the National Sci- ence Foundation (G-7617), the same grant Aman, M. G., & Singh, N. N. (1986). A critical that supported Ferster and Appel (1961) and appraisal of recent drug research in mental Ferster et al. (1962). Ferster was presumably the retardation: The Coldwater studies. Journal principal investigator. of Mental Deficiency Research, 30, 203-216. Although he conducted no more pharma- Amaral, D. G., Schumann, C. M., & Wu Nor- cology research after leaving Indiana, Ferster dahl, C. (2008). Neuroanatomy of autism. did retain an interest in autism, publishing Trends in Neuroscience, 31, 137-145. articles and chapters on its analysis and treat- American Medical Association (1958). Prin- ment (e.g., Ferster, 1966, 1967, 1968a; Ferster ciples of medical ethics. The Journal of the & Simons, 1966). He also consulted with the American Medical Association (Special Edi- Linwood Children’s Center in Ellicott City, tion). Chicago, IL: AMA Press. MD. Founded in 1955 by Jeanne Simons American Psychological Association (1959). (1909-2005), the Center became known for its Ethical standards of psychologists. American “Linwood Method” of treating children with Psychologist, 14, 279-282. autism (Simons & Oishi, 1987). The method American Psychological Association (2006). appears similar to today’s Developmental, In- Report of the Working Group on Psychotropic dividual Difference, Relationship (DIR) model Medications for Children and Adolescents: known as “Floor Time” (Greenspan & Wieder, Pharmacological, psychosocial, and combined 2006). Although neither the Linwood Method interventions for childhood disorders: Evidence nor DIR is based on a science of behavior or base, contextual factors, and future directions. is evidence-based, they appear to involve the Washington, DC: Author. (see http://www. intensive free-operant differential reinforcement apa.org/releases/PsychotropicMedication- of clinically-relevant behavior (e.g., parent-child sReport.pdf ) interactions) through successive approxima- Anderson, L. T., Campbell, M., Adams, P., tions (i.e., shaping), along with some incidental Small, A. M., Perry, R., & Shell, J. (1989). teaching. The effects of haloperidol on discrimination At Linwood, Ferster observed the method, learning and behavioral symptoms in autistic interpreted it behaviorally, described its similari- children. Journal of Autism and Developmen- ties with operant conditioning (e.g., reinforce- tal Disorders, 19, 227-239. ment, extinction, shaping), and vouched for its Anderson, L. T., Campbell, M., Grega, D. M., effectiveness (Ferster & Simons, 1966). He also Perry, R., Small, A. M., & Green, M. D. assisted Simons in operationalizing it, which (1984). Haloperidol in the treatment of reportedly improved staff training and treat- infantile autism: Effects on learning and ment fidelity (Ferster, 1967). This work was sup- behavior. American Journal of Psychiatry, ported by a $220,000 grant to Ferster through 141, 1195-1202. IBR from the Bureau of Research at the Office Annas, G. J., & Grodin, M. A. (Eds.). (1992). of Education in the U.S. Department of Health, The Nazi doctors and the Nuremberg Code: Education, and Welfare, titled “Treatment and Human rights in human experimentation. Education of Autistic Children: Combined Ap- New York: Oxford University Press. plication of Clinical and Laboratory Methods” Aprison, M. H. (1990). The discovery of the (1965-1968). After the grant’s completion, neurotransmitter role of glycine. In O. P. Ferster’s interests turned more to psychotherapy. Otterson & J. Storm-Mathesen (Eds.), However, just prior to his death, he accepted an Glycine neurotransmission (pp. 1-23). New appointment to serve on the Advisory Board of York: Wiley. the Linwood Children’s Center, so perhaps his Aprison, M (2001). Morris H. Aprison. In L. work in autism was unfinished. R. Squire (Ed.), A history of neuroscience in autobiography (Vol. 3, pp.-37). San Diego,
14 Edward K. Morris and Charryse M. Fouquette CA: Academic Press. ior, 90, 405-415. Aprison, M. H., & Ferster, C. B. (1960). Be- Boren, J. J. (1981). Charles B. Ferster, 1922- havioral effects of 5-hydroxytryptophan. 1981. The Behavior Analyst, 4, 155-156. Experientia, 16, 159-162. Brady, J. P. (1990). The shaping of behavior Aprison, M. H., & Ferster, C. B. (1961a). therapy: A historical perspective: Ferster Neurochemical correlates of behavior. I. was often first. The Behavior Therapist, 13, Quantitative measurements of the behav- 113-114. ioral effects of the serotonin precursor, Brady, J. P., Levitt, E. E., & Baydan, N. (1962). 5-hydroxytrytophan. Journal of Pharmacol- The operant-reinforcement paradigm in the ogy and Experimental Therapeutics, 131, study of drug effects. Diseases of the Nervous 100-107. System, 23, 497-502. Aprison, M. H., & Ferster, C. B. (1961b). Neu- Brady, J. V. (1991). Behavioral pharmacology in rochemical correlates of behavior. II. Cor- the era of neuroscience. Journal of the Experi- relation of brain monoamine oxidase activity mental Analysis of Behavior, 56, 167-169. with behavioral changes after iproniazed Branch, M. N. (2006). Roger Kelleher, behavior and 5 hydroxytryptophan administration. analyst. Journal of the Experimental Analysis Journal of Neurochemistry, 6, 350-357. of Behavior, 86, 381-384. Aprison, M. H., & Ferster, C. B. (1961c). Brown, R. T., & Sammons, M. T. (2002). Serotonin and behavior. Recent Advances in Pediatric psychopharmacology: A review Biological Psychiatry, 3, 151-162. of new developments and recent research. Aprison, M. H., & Hingtgen, J. N. (1970). Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, Neurochemical correlates of behavior. 33, 135-147. International Review of Neurobiology, 13, Burgio, L. D., Page, T. J., & Capriotti, R. M. 325-341. (1985). Clinical behavioral pharmacology: Aprison, M. H., & Hingtgen, J. N. (1993). Methods for evaluating medications and A neurochemist’s perspective on human contingency management. Journal of Applied depression and stress. In T. Kariya & M. Behavior Analysis, 18, 45-59. Nakagawara (Eds.), Affective disorders: Per- Campbell, M., Adams, P., Small, A. M., Cur- spectives on basic research and clinical practice ran, E. L., Overall, J. E., Anderson, L. T., (pp. 3-25). Tokyo: Seiwa Shoten. Lynch, N., & Perry, R. (1988). Efficacy and Aprison, M. H., & Werman, R. (1965). The safety of fenfluramine in autistic children. distribution of glycine in cat spinal cord and Journal of the American Academy of Child and roots. Life Science, 4, 2075-2083. Adolescent Psychiatry, 27, 434-439. Bair, H. V., & Herold, W. (1955). Efficacy of Campbell, M., Anderson, L. T., Meier, M., Co- chlorpromazine in hyperactive mentally hen, I. L., Small, A. M., Samit, C., & Sachar, retarded children. Archives of Neurology and E. J. (1978). A comparison of haloperidol, Psychiatry, 74, 363-364. behavior therapy, and their interaction in Barnard, A. L., Young, H., Pearson, A. D. J., autistic children. Journal of the American Geddes, J, & O’Brien, G. (2002). A system- Academy of Child Psychiatry, 17, 640-655. atic review of the use of atypical antipsychot- Campbell, M., Anderson, L. T., Small, A. M., ics in autism. Journal of Psychopharmacology, Perry, R., Green, W. H., & Caplan, R. 16, 93-101. (1982). The effects of haloperidol on learn- Barrett, J. E. (2006). Behavioral determinants ing and behavior in autistic children. Journal of drug action: Contributions of Peter B. of Autism and Developmental Disabilities, Dews. Journal of the Experimental Analysis 12, 167-175. of Behavior, 86, 359-370. Campbell, M., Cohen, I. L., & Anderson, L. Barrett, J. E. (2008). Pioneer in behavioral T. (1981). Pharmacotherapy for autistic pharmacology: A tribute to Joseph V. Brady. children: A summary of research. Canadian Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behav- Journal of Psychiatry, 26, 265-273.
Rescuing Ferster and Demyer 15 Cohen, I. L., Campbell, M., Posner, D., Small, servations on an operant in human subjects A. M., Triebel, D, & Anderson, L. T. (1980). and its modification by dextroamphetamine. Behavioral effects of haloperidol in young Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behav- autistic children. Journal of the American ior, 1, 359-364. Academy of Child Psychiatry, 19, 665-667. Dewsbury, D. A. (2003). Conflicting ap- Davis, K. V. (1971). The effects of drugs on proaches: Operant psychology arrives at a stereotyped and nonstereotyped operant be- primate laboratory. The Behavior Analyst, haviors in retardates. Psychopharamacologia, 26, 253-265. 22, 195-213. Dinsmoor, J. A. (1990). Academic roots: Co- DeMyer, M. K. (1975a). The nature of the lumbia University, 1943-1951. Journal of neuropsychological disability in autistic the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 54, children. Journal of Autism and Childhood 129-149. Schizophrenia, 5, 109-128. Ferster, C. B. (1957). Concurrent schedules of DeMyer, M. K. (1975b). Research in infantile reinforcement in the chimpanzee. Science, autism: A strategy and its results. Biological 125, 1090-1091. Psychiatry, 10, 433-452. Ferster, C. B. (1960a). Intermittent reinforce- DeMyer, M. K. (1979). Parents and children in ment of matching to sample in the pigeon. autism. Washington, DC: V. H. Winston Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Be- & Sons. havior, 3, 259-272. DeMyer, M. K., Barton, S., DeMyer, W. E., Ferster, C. B. (1960b). A semi-automatic teach- Norton, J. A., Allen, J. & Steele, R. (1973). ing machine. Journal of the Experimental Prognosis in autism: A follow-up study. Jour- Analysis of Behavior, 3, 245-246. nal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, Ferster, C. B. (1961). Positive reinforcement 3, 199-246. and behavioral deficits in autistic children. DeMyer, M. K., Churchill, D. W., Pontius, Child Development, 32, 437-456. W., & Gilkey, K. M. (1971). A comparison Ferster, C. B. (1962). Performance development of five diagnostic systems for childhood in autistic children (16mm film). Unpub- schizophrenia and infantile autism. Journal lished. of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 1, Ferster, C. B. (1966a). The repertoire of the 175-189. autistic child in relation to principles of DeMyer, M. K., & Ferster, C. B. (1962). Teach- reinforcement. In L. A. Gottschalk & A. ing new social behavior to schizophrenic AuerLack (Eds.), Methods of research in children. Journal of the American Academy psychotherapy (pp. 312-330). New York: of Child Psychiatry, 1, 443-461. Appleton-Century-Crofts. DeMyer, M. K. Hingtgen, J. N., & Jackson, Ferster, C. B. (1967). Perspectives in psychol- R. K. (1981). Infantile autism reviewed: A ogy: XXV Transition from animal labora- decade of research. Schizophrenia Bulletin, tory to clinic. The Psychological Record, 17, 7, 388-451. 145-150. Dews, P. B. (1955). Differential sensitivity to Ferster, C. B. (1968a, November). The autistic pentobarbital of pecking performance in child. Psychology Today, 61, 35-37. pigeons depending on the schedule of re- Ferster, C. B. (1968b). Individualized instruc- ward. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental tion in a large introductory psychology Therapeutics, 113, 393–401 college course. The Psychological Record, 18, Dews, P. B. (1958). The effects of chlorprom- 521-532. azine and promazine on performance on a Ferster, C. B. (1970). Schedules of reinforce- mixed schedule of reinforcement. Journal ment with B. F. Skinner. In P. B. Dews (Ed.), of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1, Festschrift for B. F. Skinner (pp. 37-46). New 73-82. York: Irvington. [Reprinted in the Journal of Dews, P. B., & Morse, W. H. (1958). Some ob- the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2002,
16 Edward K. Morris and Charryse M. Fouquette 77, 303-311.] Freed, H., & Peifer, C. A. (1956). Treatment of Ferster, C. B. (1973). A functional analysis hyperkinetic emotionally disturbed children of depression. American Psychologist, 28, with prolonged administration of chlorpro- 857-870. mazine. American Journal of Psychiatry, 113, Ferster, C. B. (1979). Psychotherapy from the 22-26. standpoint of a behaviorist. In J. D. Keehn Freeman, R. D. (1966). Drug effects on learn- (Ed.), Psychopathology in animals (pp. 279- ing in children: A selective review of the past 304). New York: Academic Press. thirty years. Journal of Special Education, 1, Ferster, C. B., & Appel, J. B. (1961). Pun- 17-44. ishment of S- responding in matching to Ghezzi, P. M., Williams, W. L., & Carr, J. E. sample by time out from positive reinforce- (Eds.). (1999). Autism: Behavior-analytic ment. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of perspectives. Reno, NV: Context Press. Behavior, 4, 45-56. Gollub, L. R. (2002). Between the waves: Ferster, C. B., Appel, J. B., & Hiss, R. A. Harvard Pigeon Lab 1955-1960. Journal (1962). The effect of drugs in a fixed-ratio of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 77, performance suppressed by a pre-time-out 319-326. stimulus. Journal of the Experimental Analysis Greenspan, S. I., & Wieder, S. (2006). Engaging of Behavior, 5, 73-88. autism: Using the DIR floortime approach to Ferster, C. B., & Aprison, M. H. (1960). A help children relate, communicate, and think. multiple-unit pigeon apparatus. Journal Cambridge, MA: Du Capo Press. of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 3, Handen, B. L., & Lubetsky, M. (2005). Phar- 165-166. macotherapy in autism and related disorders. Ferster, C. B., & DeMyer, M. K. (1961a). The School Psychology Quarterly, 20, 155-171. development of performances in autistic Heron, W. T., & Skinner, B. F. (1937). The children in an automatically controlled effects of certain drugs and hormones on environment. Journal of Chronic Diseases, conditioning and extinction [Abstract]. 13, 312-345. Psychological Bulletin, 34, 741-742. Ferster, C. B., & DeMyer, M. K. (1961b). Hingtgen, J. N., & Coulter, S. K. (1967). Au- Increased performances of an autistic child ditory control of operant behavior in mute with prochlorperazine administration. Jour- autistic children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, nal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 25, 561-565. 4, 84. Hingtgen, J. N., Coulter, S. K., & Churchill, Ferster, C. B., & DeMyer, M. K. (1962). A D. W. (1967). Intensive reinforcement of method for the experimental analysis of the imitative behavior in mute autistic children. autistic child. American Journal of Orthopsy- Archives of General Psychiatry, 17, 36-43. chiatry, 32, 89-98. Hingtgen, J. N., Sanders, B. J., & DeMyer, M. Ferster, C. B., & Hammer, C. (1965). Variables K. (1976). Shaping cooperative responses determining the effects of delay in reinforce- in early childhood schizophrenics. In L. ment. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Ullmann & L. Krasner (Eds.), Case studies Behavior, 8, 243-254. in behavior modification (pp. 130-138). New Ferster, C. B., Nurnberger, J. I., & Levitt, E. B. York: Holt. (1962). The control of eating. The Journal of Hingtgen, J. N., & Trost, F. C. (1966). Shap- Mathetics, 1, 87-109. ing cooperative responses in early childhood Ferster, C. B., & Simons, J. (1966). Behavior schizophrenia: II. Reinforcement of mutual therapy with children. The Psychological physical contact and vocal responses. In R. Record, 16, 65-71. Ulrich, T. Stachnik, & J. Marby (Eds.), Ferster, C. B., & Skinner, B. F. (1957). Sched- Control of human behavior (pp. 110-113). ules of reinforcement. New York: Appleton Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman. Century-Crofts. Hollis, J. H. (1968). Chlorpromazine: Direct
Rescuing Ferster and Demyer 17 measurement of differential behavioral ef- letin, 69, 396-409. fect. Science, 159, 1487-1489. Levison, P. K., Ferster, C. B., Niemann, W. H., Hollis, J. H., & St. Omer, (1972). Direct mea- & Findley, J. D. (1964). A method for train- surement of psychopharmacologic response: ing unrestrained primates to receive drug Effects of chlorpromazine on motor behav- injections. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of ior of retarded children. American Journal of Behavior, 7, 253–254 Mental Deficiency, 4, 397-407. Lindsley, O. R. (1956). Operant condition- Kazdin, A. E. (1978). History of behavior modi- ing methods applied to research in chronic fication: Experimental foundations of contem- schizophrenia. Psychiatric Research Reports, porary research. Baltimore, MD: University 5, 118-139. Park Press. Luiselli, J. K., Russo, D. C., Christian, W. P., & Kazdin, A. E. (1982). History of behavior modi- Wilczynski, S. M. (Eds.). (2008). Effective fication. In A. S. Bellack, M. Hersen, & A. practices for children with autism. New York: E. Kazdin (Eds.), International handbook of Oxford University Press. behavior modification and therapy (pp. 3-32). McConahey, O. L., Thompson, T., & Zim- New York: Plenum. merman, R. (1977). A token system for Kelleher, R. T., & Morse, W. H. (1987). The Ye- retarded women: Behavior therapy, drug rkes connection. Journal of the Experimental administration, and their combination. In T. Analysis of Behavior, 48, 456-457. Thompson & J. Grabowski (Eds.), Behavior Kelleher, R. T., Riddle, W. C., & Cook, L. A. modification of the mentally retarded (2nd ed., (1960). A behavioral analysis of qualitative pp. 167-234). New York: Oxford. differences among phenothiazines. Federa- Minshew, M. J., & Williams, D. L. (2007). tion Proceedings, 19(1), 22. The new neurobiology of autism: Context, Keller, F. S. (1981). Charles Boris Ferster connectivity, and neuronal organization. (1922-1981): An appreciation. Journal of Archives of Neurology, 64, 945-950. the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 36, Ornitz, E. M. (1973). Childhood autism: A re- 299-301. view of clinical and experimental literature. Keller, F. S. (1986). A fire at Shermerhorn exten- California Medicine, 118, 21-47. sion. The Behavior Analyst, 9, 139-146. Pickens, R. (1977). Behavioral pharmacology: A Kennedy, C. H., & Meyer, K. A. (1998). The brief history. In T. Thompson & P. B. Dews use of psychotropic drugs for people with (Eds.), Advances in behavioral pharmacology severe disabilities and challenging behavior: (Vol. 1, pp. 229-257). New York: Academic Current status and future directions. Jour- Press. nal of the Association for Persons with Severe Poling, A. (1986). A primer of human behavioral Handicaps, 23, 83-97. pharmacology. New York: Plenum. Krasner, L. (1990). History of behavior modi- Poling, A. (2000). Introduction. In A. Poling fication. In A. S. Bellack, M. Hersen, & A. & T. Byrne (Eds.), Behavioral pharmacology E. Kazdin (Eds.), International handbook of (pp. 9-24). Reno, NV: Context Press. behavior modification and therapy (pp. 3-25). Schaal, D. W., & Hackenberg, T. (1994). New York: Plenum. Toward a functional analysis of drug treat- Laties, V. G. (1987). Society of the Experimen- ment for behavior problems of people with tal Analysis of Behavior: The first thirty years developmental disabilities. American Journal (1957-1987). Journal of the Experimental on Mental Retardation, 99, 123-140. Analysis of Behavior, 48, 495-512. Sevin, J. A., Bowers-Stephens, C., Hamilton, Laties, V. G. (2003). Behavior analysis and the M. L., & Ford, A. (2001). Integrating be- growth of behavioral pharmacology. The havioral and pharmacological interventions Behavior Analyst, 26, 235-252. in treating clients with psychiatric disorders Leff, R. (1968). Behavior modification and the and mental retardation. Research in Develop- psychoses of childhood. Psychological Bul- mental Disabilities, 22, 463-485.
18 Edward K. Morris and Charryse M. Fouquette Simons, J., & Oishi, S. (1987). The hidden child: disorders: Identification, education, and treat- The Linwood method for reaching the autistic ment (3rd ed.) (pp. 395-492). Mahwah, NJ: child. Bethesda, MD: Woodbine House Erlbaum. Skinner, B. F. (1981). Charles B. Ferster -- A Willner, M. M., & Gordon, L. E. (1969). Psy- personal memoir. Journal of the Experimental chotropic drugs in children: Pharmacology Analysis of Behavior, 35, 259-261. as an adjunct to counseling and psycho- Skinner, B. F. (1983). A matter of consequences. therapy. Clinical Pediatrics, 8, 193-200. New York: Knopf. Vitiello, B. (2003). Ethical considerations in Skinner, B. F. (1987). Antecedents. Journal of psychopharmacological research involving the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 48, children and adolescents. Psychopharmacol- 447-448. ogy, 171, 86-91. Skinner, B. F., & Heron, W. T. (1937). Effects Volkmar, F. R., Paul, R., Klin, A. & Cohen, of caffeine and benzedrine upon condition- D. (Eds.). (2005). Handbook of autism and ing and extinction. The Psychological Record, pervasive developmental disorders (3rd ed.). 1, 340-346. New York: Wiley. Smith, C. M. (2005). Origin and uses of Pri- Zimmerman, J., & Baydan, N. T. (1963). mum non nocere—Above all, do no harm! Punishment of S∆ responding of human Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 45, 371- conditional matching to sample by time- 377. out. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Sparks, B. F., Friedman, S. D., Shaw, D. W., Behavior, 6, 589-597. Aylward, E. H., Echelard, D., Artru, A. A., Zimmerman, J., & Ferster, C. B. (1963). In- et al. (2002). Brain structural abnormalities termittent punishment of S∆ responding in in young children with autism spectrum matching to sample. Journal of the Experi- disorder. Neurology, 59, 184-192. mental Analysis of Behavior, 6, 349-355. Swazey, J. (1974). Chlorpromazine in psychiatry: Zimmerman, J., & Ferster, C. B. (1964). Some A study in therapeutic innovation. Cam- notes on time out from reinforcement. Jour- bridge, MA: MIT Press. nal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, Tsai, L. Y. (2005). Medical treatment in au- 7, 13-19. tism. In D. B. Zager (Ed.), Autism spectrum
You can also read