Recent progress in understanding mammalian color vision
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
Ophthal. Physiol. Opt. 2010 30: 422–434 The Verriest Lecture 2009 Recent progress in understanding mammalian color vision Gerald H. Jacobs Department of Psychology and Neuroscience Research Institute, University of California, Santa Barbara, CA 93106, USA Abstract There have been significant advances in our understanding of mammalian color vision over the past 15 years. This paper reviews a number of topics that have been central to these recent efforts, including: (1) the extent and nature of ultraviolet vision in mammals, (2) the evolutionary loss of short- wavelength-sensitive cones in some mammals, (3) the possible roles of rod signals in mammalian color vision, (4) the evolution of mammalian color vision, and (5) recent laboratory investigations of animal color vision. Successes in linking opsin genes and photopigments to color vision have been key to the progress made on each of these issues. Keywords: evolution of color vision, mammalian color vision, opsin genes, photopigments, ultraviolet vision Mammals were traditionally believed to constitute Introduction significant exceptions to this picture. In his classic Results obtained from comparative studies of retinas, treatise on vertebrate eyes, Gordon Walls encapsulated and inferences drawn from opsin gene phylogenies, that idea by noting that, although primates stand as a show that at an early stage in vertebrate history, almost clear exception, ÔWithin the mammals color vision is by certainly by the time jawless and jawed vertebrates no means widespreadÕ (Walls, 1942). Over the next diverged (540 million years ago (mya)), our ancestors 50 years this idea was echoed on numerous occasions by had already evolved four classes of cone photopigment other writers. WallsÕ explanation for the elaborate color and so possessed the photopigment basis for color vision vision detected in most contemporary vertebrate groups, (Collin et al., 2009). Shortly thereafter, colored oil and the simplified nature (or, indeed, complete absence) droplets, another retinal appurtenance usually associ- of a color vision capacity in most mammals, was that it ated with complex color vision, also appeared (Robin- simply reflected the early history of mammals when they son, 1994). Three or four classes of cone pigment are had undergone a long period of predominant noctur- present in many contemporary representatives from four nality, and as a consequence had largely abandoned the of the major vertebrate groups (fishes, birds, amphibians machinery required to support many quintessential and reptiles) while colored oil droplets are found in daylight visual capacities, including color vision. In an numerous members of the latter three groups (Bow- earlier review of the literature on mammalian color maker, 1991, 2008): thus, to varying degrees, many vision I concluded that, counter to the conclusions of vertebrates probably maintained a capacity for elabo- Walls and his followers, the presence of color vision, at rate color vision over the long sweep of their histories. least as it is technically defined, is actually quite widespread among contemporary mammals and that cones, rather than having sometimes been lost and Received: 10 September 2009 subsequently regained as Walls had surmised, were most Revised form: 18 December 2009 likely carried forward over the unbroken sweep of Accepted: 25 December 2009 mammalian history (Jacobs, 1993). The years since that Correspondence and reprint requests to: Gerald H. Jacobs. review have witnessed considerable further progress Tel.: 805 893 2446; Fax: 805 893 4303. towards understanding mammalian color vision. Here E-mail address: jacobs@psych.ucsb.edu I comment on several topics having to do with doi: 10.1111/j.1475-1313.2010.00719.x ª 2010 The Author, Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics ª 2010 The College of Optometrists
The Verriest Lecture: G. H. Jacobs 423 mammalian color vision that have come to the fore vision is common and much studied, it appears that UV during this period. sensitivity can be useful in both mate choice and foraging, among other activities, although it is unclear if avian UV vision actually evolved to subserve these Ultraviolet vision in mammals purposes (Church et al., 2001; Hart and Hunt, 2007). It has long been known that many terrestrial arthropods Thus far there is no evidence that mammalian UV vision have high sensitivity to ultraviolet (UV) light (Jacobs, can be employed to achieve similar goals; indeed, there 1992; Goldsmith, 1994). Detection of similar mecha- are some indications to the contrary: for instance, an nisms in vertebrates is more recent, but even as these explicit laboratory test of foraging in house mice found were eventually established in various birds, fishes, that these rodents were indifferent to the presence or reptiles and amphibians, it was still supposed that absence of UV-linked cues (Honkavaara et al., 2008). mammals were insensitive to UV, a conclusion princi- An alternative possibility has come from the observation pally based on the mistaken idea that the lenses of all that the urine of some rodent species has high reflectivity mammalian eyes have low transmissivity to short- to UV (Viitala et al., 1995), suggesting that in such wavelength light (Goldsmith, 1990). That changed with animals scent marking with urine might utilize a UV- the discovery that the retinas of several common species sensitive communication channel (Chavez et al., 2003). of rodent (including mice, rats, gerbils and gophers) in Whether that is true or not remains to be seen, but it is fact contain a separate spectral mechanism with max- noteworthy that high reflectivity is not characteristic of imal sensitivity in the UV (Jacobs et al., 1991). This the urine of many mammals, even those known to have claim was contested (Soucy et al., 1998), but subsequent UV cones, e. g., the house mouse (Kellie et al., 2004). electrophysiological (Lyubarsky et al., 1999), spectro- An additional point of concern is that most of the photometric (Yokoyama et al., 1998), and behavioral species so far known to have UV cones are nocturnal, measurements (Jacobs et al., 2001), have all verified the thus being predominantly active during a phase of the presence of cones containing UV pigment in rodent illumination cycle when UV light is not naturally very retinas. Although it is not the predominant arrangement abundant (Johnsen et al., 2004). Finally, it has been among mammals, UV cones have subsequently been suggested that natural fluctuations in UV light probably detected in a number of other rodent species (Peichl, play only a limited role in the entrainment of mamma- 2005) as well as in several species of bat (Wang et al., lian circadian systems (Hut et al., 2000). In summary, 2004; Muller et al., 2009; Zhao et al., 2009) and some although laboratory tests of mammals with UV cones marsupials (Strachan et al., 2004; Arrese et al., 2005; show clearly that they are capable of exploiting signals Hunt et al., 2009b). There are almost certainly other from these receptors to guide behavioral choices under mammalian species so far unstudied that also possess photopic test conditions (Jacobs et al., 2003, 2004) we UV cones. still have little idea of how this capacity may be A better understanding of the origin of UV cones in employed naturally. mammals has emerged from recent molecular genetic studies of photopigment opsin genes. All vertebrate cone Evolutionary loss of SWS1 cones pigments having maximum sensitivity (kmax) in the short wavelengths (360 nm to 440 nm) are specified by genes As noted, the photopigments of all mammalian short- from one (SWS1) of the four cone-opsin gene families wavelength-sensitive cones are specified by opsin genes (Bowmaker, 2008). Cross-species comparisons of the drawn from the SWS1 family. Some years ago studies residues implicated in the spectral tuning of the SWS1 involving both opsin immunolabelling (Wikler and cone opsins suggest that the ancestral mammalian SWS1 Rakic, 1990), and behavioral and electrophysiological pigment was in fact a UV pigment (Hunt et al., 2001). In measurements (Jacobs et al., 1993; Deegan and Jacobs, many mammalian lineages the occurrence of a small 1996), failed to detect functional short-wavelength- number of amino acid substitutions subsequently shifted sensitive cones in the retinas of two species of nocturnal the kmax of the cone pigment from the UV to a variety of primates—the anthropoid Aotus (owl monkey) and the locations in the visible spectrum (Yokoyama, 2009). strepsirrhine Otolemur (bushbaby). A subsequent genet- Evolutionary changes of this kind, though common, ic examination revealed that the absence of S cones in have not been universal; in particular, the mammalian these primates results from mutational changes in the species noted above have all retained the ancestral S-cone opsin genes that render them incapable of mammalian short-wavelength pigment. expressing opsin protein, i. e., they had become pseud- Why have some mammals retained their UV cones ogenes (Jacobs et al., 1996b). Since these two primates while others have not? Answering that question would are only distantly related, and since the structural nature be easier if we understood the relative values and costs of their SWS1 gene defects differed, it seemed likely that of UV vision for mammals. In diurnal birds, where UV the conversions of the SWS1 genes to pseudogene status ª 2010 The Author, Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics ª 2010 The College of Optometrists
424 Ophthal. Physiol. Opt. 2010 30: No. 5 must have occurred independently in the two lineages. Can similar functional explanations account for the Further, because both of these species are nocturnal it presence of mammalian S-opsin pseudogenes? In most seemed plausible to assume there must be other noctur- mammalian retinas SWS1-specified cones are infrequent nal mammals in whom gene mutations had also relative to the numbers of LWS-specified cones, typi- rendered their S-cone pigments nonfunctional (Jacobs cally making up no more than 10% of the total cone et al., 1996b). complement. Because of their relative sparsity, as well as This latter prediction has been amply borne out. limitations imposed by the optics of mammalian eyes, S Scattered species from four orders of eutherian mam- cones make little contribution to total photon capture mals (various rodents, primates, cetaceans, and carni- and they support significantly lower spatial and tempo- vores) similarly lack functional S cones, and in cases ral resolution than do the more abundant long wave- where it has been examined, their absence can be length cones (Calkins, 2001). Rather, the principal role traced to corresponding opsin gene defects (Jacobs, subserved by mammalian S cones is to generate a signal 2009). Photic activity classifications are imprecise, but that can be contrasted to that derived from stimulation all the mammals so far found to lack S cones are of longer wavelength cones, with the combination thus principally nocturnal, as were the primates in whom providing the basis for a dimension of color vision. Since S-opsin pseudogenes were first detected. Supporting the most mammals have only a single type of LWS cone, the possibility of a causal link between pigment loss and loss of viable S cones eliminates the possibility of any photic activity cycle, is the observation that while most cone-based color vision, and that is just what has of the carnivore procyonids are nocturnal, and of these happened in those species in which the SWS1 genes have both Procyon (the raccoons) and Potos (kinkajous) become pseudogenes. lack short-wavelength cones, a closely-related procyo- What values and costs might be associated with nid (Nasua, the coati) is diurnal and retains functional abandoning a dimension of color vision? If animals are S cones (Jacobs and Deegan, 1992). Although it seems nocturnal, as at least most of these species seem to be, that nocturnality sets the stage for SWS1 cone opsin then they would normally be behaviorally active when genes to become pseudogenes, that feature cannot be ambient light levels are insufficient to support cone- the sole issue since many nocturnal mammals retain a based vision and thus color vision in such animals would full complement of functional short-wavelength cones, seem at first glance to offer minimal advantage. On the e.g., rats and mice. There are also the extreme other hand, many contemporary mammals classified as examples offered by some subterranean mammals, nocturnal are also active at dawn and dusk (Macdonald, animals that lead lives almost completely devoid of 2001), times when illumination conditions could well light exposure yet still retain fully functional short- support some role for cone vision. In addition, even the wavelength sensitive cones (Peichl et al., 2004; Williams most resolutely nocturnal species occasionally awaken et al., 2005). Finally, if a nocturnal lifestyle promotes and become active during daylight hours in order to the pseudogenization of SWS1 opsin genes it is curious initiate behaviors for which color vision might prove why this did not happen in widespread fashion during useful; for instance, to escape predation, to respond to the long period in their early history when mammals weather contingencies, or to initiate foraging driven by were principally nocturnal. the stress of food scarcity (Bearder et al., 2006). If there In recent years pseudogenes associated with receptor seems to be at least some potential value in retaining operation have been discovered in other sensory sys- color vision in a nominally nocturnal species, then tems; for example, within families of olfactory (Gilad perhaps one should look instead to the debit side of et al., 2004), pheromone (Zhang and Webb, 2003), and maintaining color vision. Energy efficiency has been gustatory (Go et al., 2005) receptor genes. A common shown to act as a strong selective force in brain suggestion is that the transition of genes to pseudogenes evolution (Niven and Laughlin, 2008) and conceivably occurs when the function(s) they support become that issue is at play here. Although there seems no way dispensable and, that being the case, this process and as yet of evaluating the possibility directly, the short- its dependence on details of the interaction of organisms wavelength sensitive cones required to support a dimen- with their environments may be particularly easy to sion of color vision are few in number, which would observe in sensory systems (Go et al., 2005) One seem to minimize their metabolic expense. In summary, possible example so cited is the large increase in the if there are any general adaptive reasons associated with proportion of anthropoid olfactory receptor genes that the inactivation of mammalian SWS1 cone opsin genes, are pseudogenes, relative to what is found in rodents. they are not yet apparent. That difference is attributed to the lessened importance Among mammals so far studied, SWS opsin pseud- of a keen sense of smell among the primates, perhaps ogenes seem sometimes to have emerged near the occurring in exchange for their increased dependence on evolutionary base of the lineage and in other cases only vision (Gilad et al., 2004). in the distal branches of the family. Among the latter ª 2010 The Author, Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics ª 2010 The College of Optometrists
The Verriest Lecture: G. H. Jacobs 425 examples would be the procyonids, described above, Rods and mammalian color vision where fairly closely-related genera can have either functional or non-functional S cone pigments. Perhaps Because rods and cones overlap in their operating most striking among the former are the marine mam- ranges (in human vision by some 4 log units of mals. A genetic survey of the SWS1 opsin genes in 16 intensity), and because the signals from these receptor species of cetaceans identified mutational changes in all types share neural pathways into the central visual of these species that should obviate the production of system, it has long been apparent that rod signals can functional S-cone pigment (Levenson and Dizon, 2003). potentially influence cone-based vision. Among those In fact, in one cetacean sub-order (the odontocetes) all demonstrated influences are cases where rod signals the species share in common a mis-sense mutation in cause complex alterations in color appearance (Volbr- their S-cone opsin genes implying that pseudogenes echt et al., 1995; Buck, 2004) and cases involving must have been present prior to the time these animals viewing conditions (mesopic light levels, large test fields) began to diverge in the Oligocene (25–38 mya). Further, where rod signals can be contrasted to signals derived in support of earlier evidence derived from opsin from a single class of cones to yield novel color vision immunolabelling (Peichl and Moutairou, 1998), a (Smith and Pokorny, 1977). The following examples genetic survey found that all the pinnipeds (seals, sea illustrate that similar influences from rod signals on lions, walrus) also have a gene-linked loss of S-cone color vision also operate in non-human mammals. function (Levenson et al., 2006). The complete absence An early behavioral experiment conducted on a of SWS1 cones in both of these two distinct mammalian strepsirrhine primate, the ring-tailed lemur (Lemur orders raises the possibility that such a loss may have catta), included tests of spectral sensitivity and color yielded some adaptive advantages. What those might be discrimination (Blakeslee and Jacobs, 1985). The latter is unclear, although some suggestions have been offered provided evidence for the presence of some (relatively (Peichl et al., 2001). Particularly puzzling in this regard feeble) color discrimination in the red-green portion of is that present day cetaceans and pinnipeds occupy the spectrum; specifically, these animals were able to distinctively different photic environments: the former make unique dichromatic color matches (540 nm + strictly aquatic, often active in environments where 645 nm = 570 nm) with the match proportions signi- photons are a scarce commodity; whereas pinnipeds are ficantly displaced in the protan direction relative to amphibious inhabiting both aquatic and terrestrial those made by normal human trichromats. Since habitats, the latter often characterized by high photopic subsequent results derived from both electrophysiolog- light loads. ical measurements (Jacobs and Deegan, 1993, 2003), Observations made on the owl monkey (Aotus) may and from an analysis of cone opsin genes (Tan and Li, argue against expecting any simple relationships be- 1999), show that this species expresses only a single cone tween photic environments and S-cone absence. Aotus is photopigment active in the middle to long-wavelength a nocturnal monkey, but is believed to have evolved portion of the spectrum (with kmax of 545 nm), the from diurnal ancestors some 12–15 mya (Setoguchi and color discriminations found in the earlier study must Rosenberger, 1987). Several contemporary species of perforce have derived from the ability of these animals Aotus share in common a mis-sense mutation which to jointly utilize rod and cone signals. renders their S-cone opsin gene nonfunctional and this That case is not unique; for example, genetic exam- implies that the pseudogene appeared early in the ination reveals that the pinniped California sea lion history of the genus, perhaps not long after the (Zalophus californaus) has only a single cone type transition to nocturnality (Levenson et al., 2007). (Levenson et al., 2006) yet it too seems capable of Although most of the animals comprising modern Aotus making color discriminations that would be technically have remained stringently nocturnal, one species, impossible without the exploitation of rod signals A. azarae, is cathemeral, i.e., it is frequently behavior- (Griebel and Schmid, 1992). One important point to ally active during daylight hours as well as at night be derived from these examples is that deductions about (Fernandez-Duque, 2003). Despite the absence of func- color vision based solely on knowledge of the cone tional S cones, and thus any possibility of a conven- complement, as for instance is commonly done follow- tional color vision capacity, this monkey forages quite ing examination of cone opsin genes, will miss possible successfully on colored fruits and tree flowers under influences from rod contributions. Such influences may lighting conditions where its vision must be based on be particularly relevant for those many mammals that signals from only a single type of cone pigment. If have heavily rod-dominated retinas because, as noted nothing else, this example underlines the fact that we are above, such animals often display photic rhythms that only at the beginning of understanding the extent and render them behaviorally active under illumination practical implications of the gene-driven losses of S-cone conditions favorable for supporting joint rod and cone that characterizes some mammals. contributions. ª 2010 The Author, Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics ª 2010 The College of Optometrists
426 Ophthal. Physiol. Opt. 2010 30: No. 5 Among the most noteworthy features of mammalian The schematic of Figure 2 suggests the evolutionary retinas are the large species variations in rod/cone ratios fate of these four cone opsin gene families in mammals. and in the pattern of distribution of cones in the retinal As for amphibians, cone opsin genes from the Rh2 mosaic (Ahnelt and Kolb, 2000). These variations will family are not found in any contemporary mammal significantly impact the thresholds and dynamic ranges suggesting it was lost prior to the onset of mammalian for rod and cone vision and they will influence the limits divergence. Contemporary monotremes (platypus and of color discrimination. Both of these facts were earlier echidna) have photopigments from the SWS2 and LWS taken to suggest that the relative rod/cone mix and their families and their genomes also contain a pseudogene spatial distributions could be targets for selection in the from the SWS1 family (Davies et al., 2007). Genes evolution of color vision (Jacobs, 1993). That idea begins drawn from the SWS2 family are not present in either to seem more plausible in the face of recent research that marsupial (Strachan et al., 2004; Cowing et al., 2008) or compared structural features of the visual cortex and the eutherian mammals and must, therefore, also have been retina in a variety of nocturnal and diurnal mammals and lost prior to the divergence of these two lineages showed that, indeed, relative rod and cone complements (Figure 2). Both of these lines retain viable representa- are very sensitive to niche-specific selection pressures and tives from the SWS1 and LWS families. These various that plasticity stands in striking contrast to the much gene losses are usually suggested to have occurred greater conservatism of the size of central visual struc- during the long period of early mammalian nocturnality, tures (Kaskan et al., 2005). The relative numbers of but exactly how that exposure may have fostered such a retinal cell types can be readily altered through nothing loss is not known. Whatever the reason, the outcome more elaborate than changes in the schedule of retinal has been to limit most animals in these groups to only neurogenesis, and such schedule alterations could thus two types of cone pigment although, as noted below, provide a proximate mechanism through which selection significant exceptions to this rule occur among primates. might impact the relative influences of rods on mamma- Sequence comparisons of cone opsin genes from con- lian color vision (Finlay, 2008). temporary eutherian mammals suggest that the SWS1 and LWS gene families provided ancestral eutherian mammals with cone pigments having kmax values of Evolution of color vision in mammals 360 nm and 560 nm (Hunt et al., 2001; Yokoyama At the time of the 1993 review, a renewed interest in the evolution of color vision was just beginning to manifest itself. Triggered by a substantial accrual of information about opsin genes, as well as by new examinations of the ecology of color vision, much more has now been learned about this. A number of reviews dealing with various aspects of this topic have appeared in recent years (for the most recent of these see Osorio and Vorobyev, 2008; Collin et al., 2009; Hunt et al., 2009a; Jacobs, 2009; Yokoyama, 2009) so I provide here only a brief summary of the relevant findings. Evolution of opsin genes Phylogenetic analysis shows that all vertebrate photo- pigments are specified by opsin genes belonging to five families—four for the cone opsins, the other for rod opsins (Yokoyama, 2000). Each of these gene families produce opsins structured to yield photopigments that cover the range of spectral peaks indicated in Figure 1 Figure 1. Spectral range of vertebrate photopigments. All vertebrate (top). As a result of prior gene duplications, these cone photopigment opsins are specified by members of the five opsin opsin gene families are believed to have already emerged gene families listed at the top. When combined with an 11-cis-retinal at a point early in vertebrate history. Pigments drawn chromophore, variations in the gene sequences yield photopigments whose kmax values cover the spectral ranges indicated by the from each of the four cone opsin gene families are found horizontal lines. Cone photopigments in eutherian mammals come in various present-day birds, fishes, and reptiles. Rep- exclusively from the SWS1 and LWS families. The two ancestral resentation from only three of these families (Rh2 is cone pigments found in these animals are believed to have had the missing) has so far been detected among contemporary spectral absorption curves sketched at the bottom. (Modified from amphibians (Bowmaker, 2008). Jacobs, 2009). ª 2010 The Author, Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics ª 2010 The College of Optometrists
The Verriest Lecture: G. H. Jacobs 427 all the naturally observed variations (Neitz et al., 1991; Carroll and Jacobs, 2008). A similarly small number of amino acid substitutions are linked to variations in the mammalian photopigments specified by the SWS1 genes (Hunt et al., 2004). Although interactions between environmental signals and sensory capacities impacting evolution can be complex (Endler, 1992), one common assumption is that the spectral positioning and number of cone pigment types that evolve reflect those best adapted to support the visual tasks requisite for survival (Lythgoe and Partridge, 1989). In contemporary mammals pig- ments from the LWS family span a range of spectral positions having kmax values from 500 to 560 nm. If, as believed (above), the spectral location of the ancestral LWS pigment was close to the latter location, there must have been numerous shifts in the spectral position of this pigment toward the shorter wavelengths. Most euthe- rian mammals also have an SWS1 cone pigment. Figure 2. Suggested fate of the four cone opsin gene families during Through an analysis of a collection of natural images mammalian evolution. The range of photopigment absorption prop- viewed in conjunction with a popular model of color erties of pigments derived from the four families is shown in Figure 1. discrimination Chiao et al. (2000) examined how photo- All four gene families are believed to have arisen early in vertebrate pigment spectral positioning might influence color evolution. The Rh2 gene family is not present in any contemporary discrimination. For pigment combinations involving mammals and so is presumed to have been lost during the early short-wavelength pigments with kmax >400 nm, varia- evolution of mammals. The distribution of the extant gene families among the three groups of contemporary mammals is given at the tions in the positioning of the LWS pigment from its top; SWS1 is a pseudogene in present-day monotremes. Repre- longest to its shortest position had only very modest sentation of the SWS2 gene family was lost prior to the divergence of effects on predicted discriminability. From their com- marsupial and eutherian mammals. (Modified from Jacobs, 2009). putations these authors additionally inferred that color discrimination in such dichromats could be maximized et al., 2008). These cone pigments (bottom of Figure 1) by increasing the spectral separation between the two represent the shortest and longest spectral positions that pigments, irrespective of the nature of the visual can be generated from cone opsins linked to a retinal-1 environment. Modeling analyses such as this one thus chromophore and would have provided the photopig- provide no obvious explanation for the significant ment potential for dichromatic color vision. variations in the position of the LWS pigment across these dichromatic mammals. One possibility is that in such cases the spectral tuning of the LWS pigment has Spectral positioning of mammalian cone pigments been more impacted by the demands of those capacities Since all mammalian photopigments are constructed supported by achromatic vision (Chiao et al., 2000; from the same chromophore, retinal-1, variations in Osorio and Vorobyev, 2005). Another is that, within their spectral absorption properties must be due to opsin some fairly broad limits, pigment positioning is not variations. Molecular genetic studies show that varia- critically important for supporting visual needs; that tions at a limited number of positions in the opsin rather the observed variations seen among mammals molecule are largely responsible for all the variations in better reflect events that occurred in the earlier history the spectral positioning of photopigments. In the case of of the various animal groups, than it does in matching mammalian LWS pigments, for example, dimorphic current visual demands. This latter scenario merits variations at only five amino acid sites cause variations attention because it at least seems to provide the best in pigment spectral positioning, with combinations of explanation for variations in photopigment positioning changes occurring at these critical sites allowing for the in many insects (Briscoe and Chittka, 2001). production of pigments occupying quite a number of possible spectral positions (Yokoyama and Radlwim- The primate story mer, 2001). Mutagenesis studies show that four amino acid positions can potentially influence the spectral Primates have long been known to constitute a special tuning of the primate LWS pigments (Asenjo et al., case, but it is only in recent years that a fuller 1994), with only three of these accounting pretty well for appreciation of the diversity of primate color vision ª 2010 The Author, Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics ª 2010 The College of Optometrists
428 Ophthal. Physiol. Opt. 2010 30: No. 5 emerged, and along with it a more detailed understand- most of the other contemporary platyrrhines (Kainz ing of its evolution. In large part these changes were et al., 1998; Dulai et al., 1999). fostered by studies of the cone opsin genes and cone The third group of primates, the strepsirrhines, is photopigments in many different primates. Several usually described as more primitive. Animals of this recent reviews may be consulted for access to what is group feature afoveate, more rod-dominated, retinas now an extensive literature on this topic (Regan et al., and their eyes often contain a tapetum. To date these 2001; Osorio et al., 2004; Jacobs, 2007, 2008). primates have been less well studied, but they too show Although the idea is not without its critics (e. g., Tan significant variations in their cone photopigment com- et al., 2005), it is usually believed that the earliest plements. Three principal variants have been identified. primates were nocturnal, and thus like most eutherian Two of these have been described above—some are like mammals probably had two types of cone pigment the bushbaby (Otolemur) in having only a single type of drawn, respectively, from the SWS1 and LWS opsin cone pigment and thus lacking color vision; while others gene families. In mammals the LWS cone opsin genes resemble the ring-tailed lemur (Lemur catta), and many are located on the X-chromosome, but, unlike other other mammals, in having two types of cone pigment and mammals, catarrhine primates (Old World monkeys, dichromatic color vision (Kawamura and Kubotera, apes and humans) have two different LWS genes that 2004). In a third variant, some species from this group specify cone photopigments with peaks at about 530 nm have polymorphic X-chromosome opsin genes and thus, and 560 nm (commonly called M and L respectively). similar to the platyrrhines, have the photopigment basis Since these two are effectively conserved across all the to support a mixture of dichromatic and trichromatic catarrhines they apparently emerged as a consequence of phenotypes (Tan and Li, 1999; Jacobs et al., 2002; a gene duplication that occurred close to the base of the Velleux and Bolnick, 2009). An understanding of the catarrhine radiation, some 30–40 mya (Nathans et al., evolution of opsin genes and color vision in this group 1986). In conjunction with the pigment product of an of primates remains very much a goal for future studies. autosomal SWS1 gene, all of the species of this group The production of color vision requires, as a mini- express three classes of cone photopigment and have mum, multiple types of receptor containing different trichromatic color vision. Thus catarrhine primates, photopigments and a nervous system capable of con- alone among eutherian mammals, have been able to add trasting the pattern of photon absorption in the different a second version of an LWS gene and exploit its pigment types of photoreceptor. Two such neural arrangements product to acquire a new dimension of color vision. are generally believed to characterize mammalian reti- The other large group of anthropoid primates, the nas (Lee, 2004; Wässle, 2004). One involves a dedicated New World platyrrhine monkeys, has highly diverse class of bipolar cells (the S-cone bipolars) that selectively color vision and, as a consequence, has been much contact short-wavelength cones. Signals from these studied in recent years (Jacobs, 2007). With only two cones are fed via S-cone bipolar cells to a class of small apparent exceptions, this entire group features X-chro- bi-stratified ganglion cells that also receive antagonistic mosome opsin gene polymorphisms, the most common inputs from a group of bipolar cells that contact M/L arrangement featuring three alternate forms of the LWS cones. The combination of these inputs provides the gene with each allele specifying a photopigment with basis for a spectrally-opponent pathway that can sup- kmax in the 530–562 nm range. As a consequence of port a dimension of color vision. Although the compar- early X-chromosome inactivation, heterozygous females ative evidence is still somewhat scanty, it seems likely express two types of M/L pigment and derive trichro- that this neural pathway is characteristic of the retinas matic color vision while homozygous females and all of all eutherian mammals and thus has been conserved males have a single M/L pigment and are dichromatic. throughout the history of this group. The other circuit This arrangement yields a total of six distinct color for extracting color information is unique to primate vision phenotypes. The two exceptions are Aotus, which retinas. It originates from the M or L cone inputs to has only a single LWS pigment and thus lacks conven- midget bipolar cells which in turn synapse on midget tional color vision (above), and the howler monkey ganglion cells where that signal is combined in opponent Alouatta which resembles the catarrhine norm in having fashion with signals originating from neighboring M or two populations of M/L cone pigments (Jacobs et al., L cones. These form the substrate for the second 1996a) and being uniformly trichromatic (Araujo et al., spectrally-opponent channel, setting the stage for an 2008). Evidence suggests that the addition of a second additional dimension of color vision (Martin, 1998). The X-chromosome opsin gene in the howler monkey midget cell pathway has been identified as being present occurred independently from the gene addition that in retinas of a number of disparate primate species, even occurred in the catarrhine primates; in the case of those lacking trichromatic color vision, and so is howler monkeys probably emerging against a back- believed to have appeared early in primate evolution ground of earlier polymorphisms similar to that seen in (Silveira et al., 2005). There remains lively debate as to ª 2010 The Author, Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics ª 2010 The College of Optometrists
The Verriest Lecture: G. H. Jacobs 429 the nature of spatial arrangements of L and M cone Mollon, 2000; Dominy and Lucas, 2001; Regan et al., signals to this second pathway (Solomon and Lennie, 2001; Parraga et al., 2002). 2007) and of the function(s) that this pathway may have With their dramatic individual variations in color subserved in primates prior to the points at which a vision platyrrhine monkeys provide a rich resource for second type of M/L cone appeared (Mollon, 1989; Lee, examining the linkages between color vision capacity 2004), and behavior. Since there is evidence that the M/L cone Recent years have seen a marked increase in the pigments of the platyrrhines have been under selection number of studies asking how well suited the various for a considerable period of time (Surridge et al., 2003), forms of primate color vision are for various life- one might confidently expect to find among these supporting visual behaviors and, by extension, perhaps monkeys individual differences in behavior that corre- thereby shedding some light on the circumstances that late with individual differences in color vision. Exper- led to the evolution of the mechanisms underlying color iments conducted in laboratory settings have in fact vision. Such investigations typically start with detailed detected some differences in foraging efficiency for measurements of natural spectral environments and monkeys of different phenotypes (Caine and Mundy, then use one or other of the computational models of 2000; Smith et al., 2003); however, studies of several visual processing to predict discriminative performance. different platyrrhine species in their natural habitats These exercises show consistently that the discrimina- have so far proven singularly unsuccessful in detecting tion capacities inherent in primate trichromacy are well individual variations in behavior that can be compel- suited to support the demands of foraging, whether the lingly traced to individual variations in color vision targets are edible fruits or foliage viewed in their natural (Dominy et al., 2003; Smith et al., 2003; Vogel et al., surrounds (Osorio and Vorobyev, 1996; Sumner and 2007; Hiramatsu et al., 2008; Bunce, 2009). Why this Table 1. Recent laboratory investigations of mammalian color vision Exemplars Order (Genus, common name) Goal of study* Reference Marsupalia Macropus (wallaby) Dichromacy Hemmi, 1999 Sminthopsis (dunnart) Trichromacy Arrese et al., 2006 Rodentia Mus (mouse) Dichromacy Jacobs et al., 2004 Rattus (rat) Dichromacy Jacobs et al., 2001 Cavia (guinea pig) Dichromacy Jacobs and Deegan,1994b Meriones (gerbil) Dichromacy Jacobs and Deegan, 1994a Spermophilus (ground squirrel) Color thresholds van Arsdel and Loop, 2004 Primate Alouatta (howler monkey) Trichromacy Araujo et al., 2008 Callithrix (marmoset) Distinctiveness of color Derrington et al., 2002 Callithrix (marmoset) Polymorphism Pessoa et al., 2005a Cebus (capuchin monkey) Stimulus size and color vision Gomes et al., 2005 Eulemur (black lemur) Presence of color vision Gosset and Roeder, 2000 Leontopithecus (golden lion) Polymorphism Pessoa et al., 2005b Pan (chimpanzee) Color classification Matsuno et al., 2004 Papio (baboon) Color categorization Fagot et al., 2006 Saguinus (tamarin) Polymorphism Pessoa et al., 2003 Scandentia Tupaia (tree shrew) Color thresholds van Arsdel and Loop, 2004 Cetacea Tursiops (dolphin) Rod contributions to color Griebel and Schmid, 2002 Artiodactyla Bos (cow) Dichromacy Phillips and Lomas, 2001 Dama (fallow deer) Presence of color vision Birgersson et al., 2001 Perissodactyla Equus (horse) Dichromacy Pick et al., 1994 Dichromacy Macuda and Timney, 1999 Dichromacy Smith and Goldman, 1999 Dichromacy Geisbauer et al., 2004 Dichromacy Hanggi et al., 2007 Dichromacy Ahmadinejad et al., 2008 Color Thresholds Roth et al., 2008 Carnivora Felis (cat) Presence of color vision Tritsch, 1993 Color thresholds Tritsch, 1995 Sirenia Trichechus (manatee) Dichromacy Griebel and Schmid, 1996 *The meanings of the comments are explained in the text. ª 2010 The Author, Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics ª 2010 The College of Optometrists
430 Ophthal. Physiol. Opt. 2010 30: No. 5 should be so is puzzling and remains under active Conclusion investigation. Recent progress toward gaining a more complete picture of mammalian color vision can be largely attributed to Laboratory investigations of mammalian color vision technical advances in molecular genetics, cell biology, A large majority of the publications on the topic of and electrophysiology, each of which has significantly mammalian color vision produced over the past expanded our understanding of the current picture of 15 years, including most of those referenced above, deal the distribution of cone pigments across extant mam- not with color vision but with various biological mals, of what these pigments predict about color vision, mechanisms linked to that capacity. There are probably and of how these arrangements may have evolved. For at least two factors that have contributed to this reasons noted above, progress in the challenging task of imbalance. For one thing, behavioral studies of color measuring color vision in non-human subjects has been vision in non-human species are especially challenging slower, while a detailed understanding of how various and time consuming relative to studies of mechanisms, animals employ color vision in support of their survival often taking months, even years, to complete. A second remains largely a task for the future. Finally, recent impediment is that in current times funding agencies experiments have opened the door to actively manipu- have show only modest inclination to support such lating color vision either through direct alterations of ventures. Despite these challenges, there have neverthe- the opsin gene complement (Jacobs et al., 2007) or by less been a number of investigations that posed direct changes in the photopigment array induced by a gene questions about color vision in various mammals. transfer paradigm (Mancuso et al., 2009). Such proce- Reports from such studies that have come to my dures hold the promise of allowing direct tests of attention are listed in Table 1. hypotheses about the evolution of color vision as well as Space does not permit extended discussion of these more searching examinations of various aspects of the investigations of mammalian color vision. Instead, a neural underpinnings of color vision. summary comment is offered for each in Table 1. A number of these studies sought to establish the dimen- Acknowledgements sionality of color vision in some target species. These (indicated as ÔDichromacyÕ or ÔTrichromacyÕ depending I thank the officers and members of the International on the results claimed) involved tests using either spectral Colour Vision Society for providing the opportunity to lights or calibrated colored papers as test stimuli. The present the Verriest lecture at their 2009 meeting in trichromatic color vision found in the marsupial Sminth- Braga, Portugal. opsis (the dunnart) is particularly noteworthy because that species expresses only two different cone opsins, the References third pigment required to support its trichromacy being, possibly, a rod pigment expressed in a cone (Cowing Ahmadinejad, M., Pishkar, J., Asadi, M. R., Aravisani, A., et al., 2008). Similar kinds of color vision tests were Mahadavi, A. and Bafarani, A. R. H. (2008) Color conducted on several species of platyrrhine monkeys and discrimination in caspian pony. Ippologia 19, 27–37. these had the general goal of documenting individual Ahnelt, P. K. and Kolb, H. (2000) The mammalian photore- ceptor mosaic-adaptive design. Prog. Retin. Eye Res. 19, differences in color vision for correlation with L/M cone 711–770. photopigment variations (ÔPolymorphismÕ in Table 1). Araujo, A. C. Jr, Didonet, J. J., Araujo, C. S., Saletti, P. G., Other experimenters either sought to establish the pres- Borges, T. R. J. and Pessoa, V. F. (2008) Color vision in the ence of color vision or to examine a more complex feature black howler monkey (Alouatta caraya). Vis. Neurosci. 25, of color perception (color categorization, color classifi- 243–248. cation or the distinctiveness of color). Finally, thresholds Arrese, C. A., Oddy, A. Y., Runham, P. B., Hart, N. S., for color vision were determined for representatives of Shand, J., Hunt, D. M. and Beazley, L. D. (2005) Cone four different taxa. The horse was the hands-down topography and spectral sensitivity in two potentially winner as the most popular subject during this recent trichromatic marsupials, the quokka (Setonix brachyurus) period, attracting attention from seven different groups and quenda (Isodon obesulus). Proc. Biol. Sci. 272, 791– of investigators. Based on earlier measurements of the 796. Arrese, C. A., Beazley, L. D. and Neumeyer, C. (2006) cone pigments in this species (Carroll et al., 2001) these Behavioural evidence of marsupial trichromacy. Curr. Biol. animals were predicted to have dichromatic color vision 16, R193–R194. and the experiments listed in Table 1 all compellingly van Arsdel, R. E. and Loop, M. S. (2004) Color vision establish that fact, while also demonstrating the close sensitivity in normally dichromatic species and humans. Vis. linkage that exists between cone pigments and color Neurosci. 21, 685–692. vision in this species. ª 2010 The Author, Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics ª 2010 The College of Optometrists
The Verriest Lecture: G. H. Jacobs 431 Asenjo, A. B., Rim, J. and Oprian, D. D. (1994) Molecular platypus: a novel route to mammalian colour vision. Curr. determinants of human red/green color discrimination. Biol. 17, R161–R163. Neuron 12, 1131–1138. Deegan, J. F. II and Jacobs, G. H. (1996) Spectral Bearder, S. K., Nekaris, K. A. I. and Curtis, D. J. (2006) A re- sensitivity and photopigments of a nocturnal prosimian, evaluation of the role of vision in the activity and commu- the bushbaby (Otolemur crassicaudatus). Am. J. Primatol. nication of nocturnal primates. Folia Primatol. 77, 50–71. 40, 55–66. Birgersson, B., Alm, U. and Forkman, B. (2001) Colour vision Derrington, A. M., Parker, A. R., Barraclough, N. E., Easton, in fallow deer: a behavioural study. Anim. Behav. 61, 367– A., Goodson, G. R., Parker, K. S., Tinsley, C. J. and Webb, 371. B. S. (2002) The uses of colour vision: behavioural and Blakeslee, B. and Jacobs, G. H. (1985) Color vision in the ring- physiological distinctiveness of colour stimuli. Philos. Trans. tailed lemur (Lemur catta). Brain Behav. Evol. 26, 154–166. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 357, 975–985. Bowmaker, J. K. (1991) Visual pigments, oil droplets and Dominy, N. J. and Lucas, P. W. (2001) Ecological importance photoreceptors. In: The Perception of Colour (ed. P. Gou- of trichromatic colour vision to primates. Nature 410, 363– ras), CRC Press, Boca Raton, pp. 108–127. 365. Bowmaker, J. K. (2008) Evolution of vertebrate visual Dominy, N. J., Garber, P. A., Bicca-Marques, J. C. and pigments. Vision Res. 48, 2022–2041. Azevedo-Lopes, M. A. D. (2003) Do female tamarins use Briscoe, A. D. and Chittka, L. (2001) The evolution of color visual cues to detect fruit rewards more successfully than do vision in insects. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 46, 471–510. males? Anim. Behav. 66, 829–837. Buck, S. L. (2004) Rod-cone interactions in human vision. In: Dulai, K. S., von Dornum, M., Mollon, J. D. and Hunt, D. M. The Visual Neurosciences, vol. 1 (eds L. M. Chalupa and J. S. (1999) The evolution of trichromatic color vision by opsin Werner), MIT Press, Cambridge, pp. 863–878. gene duplication in New World and Old World primates. Bunce, J. A. (2009) Ecology and genetics of color vision in Genome Res. 9, 629–638. Callicebus brunneus, a neotropical monkey. Doctoral Dis- Endler, J. A. (1992) Signals, signal conditions and the direction sertation, Department of Anthropology, University of of evolution. Am. Nat. 139, S125–S153. California, Davis CA, pp. 185. Fagot, J., Goldstein, J., Davidoff, J. and Pickering, A. (2006) Caine, N. G. and Mundy, N. I. (2000) Demonstration of a Cross-species differences in color categorization. Psychon. foraging advantage for trichromatic marmosets (Callthrix Bull. Rev. 14, 275–280. geoffroyi) dependent on food colour. Proc. Biol. Sci. 267, Fernandez-Duque, E. (2003) Influences of moonlight, ambient 439–444. temperature, and food availability on the diurnal and Calkins, D. J. (2001) Seeing with S cones. Prog. Retin. Eye Res. nocturnal activity of owl monkeys (Aotus azarai). Behav. 20, 255–287. Ecol. Sociobiol. 54, 431–440. Carroll, J. and Jacobs, G. H. (2008) Mammalian photopig- Finlay, B. L. (2008) The developing and evolving retina: using ments. In: The Senses: A Comprehensive Reference, vol. 1 time to organize form. Brain Res. 1192, 5–16. (eds R. H. Masland and T. D. Albright), New York, Geisbauer, G., Griebel, U., Schmid, A. and Timney, B. (2004) Elsevier, pp. 247–266. Brightness discrimination and neutral point testing in the Carroll, J., Murphy, C. J., Neitz, M., Ver Hoeve, J. N. and horse. Can. J. Zool. 82, 660–670. Neitz, J. (2001) Photopigment basis for dichromatic color Gilad, Y., Wiebe, V., Przeworski, M., Lancet, D. and Paabo, vision in the horse. J. Vis. 1, 80–87. S. (2004) Loss of olfactory receptor genes coincides with the Chavez, A. E., Bozinovic, F., Peichl, L. and Palacios, A. G. acquisition of full trichromataic vision in primates. PLOS (2003) Retinal spectral sensitivity, fur coloration, and urine Biol. 2, 1–6. reflectance in the genus octodon (rodentia): implications Go, Y., Satta, Y., Takenaka, O. and Takahata, N. (2005) for visual ecology. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 44, 2290– Lineage-specific loss of function of bitter taste receptor 2296. genes in humans and nonhuman primates. Genetics 176, Chiao, C.-C., Vorobyev, M., Cronin, T. W. and Osorio, D. 313–326. (2000) Spectral tuning of dichromats to natural scenes. Goldsmith, T. H. (1990) Optimization, constraint, and history Vision Res. 40, 3257–3271. in the evolution of eyes. Q. Rev. Biol. 65, 281–322. Church, S. C., Merrison, A. S. L. and Chamberlain, T. M. M. Goldsmith, T. H. (1994) Ultraviolet receptors and color vision: (2001) Avian ultraviolet vision and frequency-dependent evolutionary implications and a dissonance of paradigms. seed preferences. J. Exp. Biol. 204, 2491–2498. Vision Res. 34, 1479–1487. Collin, S., Davies, W. and Hunt, D. (2009) The evolution of Gomes, U. R., Pessoa, D. M. A., Suganuma, E., Tomaz, C. early vertebrate photoreceptors. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. and Pessoa, V. F. (2005) Influence of stimulus size on color B Biol. Sci. 364, 2925–2940. discriminatino in Capuchin monkeys. Am. J. Primatol. 67, Cowing, J. A., Arrese, C. A., Davies, W. L., Beazley, L. D. and 437–446. Hunt, D. M. (2008) Cone visual pigments in two marsupial Gosset, D. and Roeder, J. J. (2000) Colour and shape species: the fat-tailed dunnart (Sminthopsis crassicaudata) discrimination in black lemurs (Eulemur macaco). Folia and the honey possum (Tarsipes rostratus). Proc. Biol. Sci. Primatol. 71, 173–176. 275, 1491–1499. Griebel, U. and Schmid, A. (1992) Color vision in the Davies, W. L., Carvalho, L. S., Cowing, J. A., Beazley, L. D., California sea lion (Zalophus californianus). Vision Res. 32, Hunt, D. M. and Arrese, C. (2007) Visual pigments of the 477–482. ª 2010 The Author, Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics ª 2010 The College of Optometrists
432 Ophthal. Physiol. Opt. 2010 30: No. 5 Griebel, U. and Schmid, A. (1996) Color vision in the manatee Jacobs, G. H. and Deegan, J. F. II (1994a) Sensitivity to (Trichechus manatus). Vision Res. 36, 2747–2757. ultraviolet lights in the gerbil (Meriones unguiculatus): Griebel, U. and Schmid, A. (2002) Spectral sensitivity and characteristics and mechanisms. Vision Res. 34, 1433–1441. color vision in the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus). Jacobs, G. H. and Deegan, J. F. II (1994b) Spectral sensitivity, Mar. Freshwater Beh. Physiol. 35, 129–137. photopigments and color vision of the guinea pig (Cavia Hanggi, E. B., Ingersoll, J. F. and Waggoner, T. L. (2007) porcellus). Behav. Neurosci. 108, 993–1004. Color vision in horses (Equus caballus): deficiencies identi- Jacobs, G. H. and Deegan, J. F. II (2003) Diurnality and cone fied using a psuedochromatic plate test. J. Comp. Psychol. pigment polymorphism in strepsirrhines: examination of 121, 65–72. linkage in Lemur catta. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 122, 66– Hart, N. S. and Hunt, D. M. (2007) Avian visual pigments: 72. characteristics, spectral tuning, and evolution. Am.Nat. 169, Jacobs, G. H., Neitz, J. and Deegan, J. F. II (1991) Retinal SupplS7–S26. receptors in rodents maximally sensitive to ultraviolet light. Hemmi, J. M. (1999) Dichromatic colour vision in an Nature 353, 655–656. Australian marsupial, the tammar wallaby. J. Comp. Phys- Jacobs, G. H., Deegan, J. F. II, Neitz, J. A., Crognale, M. A. iol. A. 185, 509–515. and Neitz, M. (1993) Photopigments and color vision in the Hiramatsu, C., Melin, A. D., Aureli, F., Schaffner, C. M., nocturnal monkey, Aotus. Vision Res. 33, 1773–1783. Vorobyev, M., Matsumoto, Y. and Kawamura, S. (2008) Jacobs, G. H., Neitz, M., Deegan, J. F. and Neitz, J. (1996a) Importance of achromatic contrast in short-range fruit Trichromatic colour vision in New World monkeys. Nature foraging of primates. PLoS one 3, e3356. 382, 156–158. Honkavaara, J., Aberg, H. and Viitala, J. (2008) Do house Jacobs, G. H., Neitz, M. and Neitz, J. (1996b) Mutations in S- mice use UV cues when foraging? J. Ethol. 26, 339–345. cone pigment genes and the absence of colour vision in two Hunt, D. M., Wilkie, S. E., Bowmaker, J. K. and Poopalas- species of nocturnal primate. Proc. Biol. Sci. 263, 705–710. undaram, S. (2001) Vision in the ultraviolet. Cell. Mol. Life Jacobs, G. H., Fenwick, J. A. and Williams, G. A. (2001) Sci. 58, 1583–1598. Cone-based vision of rats for ultraviolet and visible lights. Hunt, D. M., Cowing, J. A., Wilkie, S. E., Parry, J. W. L., J. Exp. Biol. 204, 2439–2446. Poopalasundaram, S. and Bowmaker, J. K. (2004) Diver- Jacobs, G. H., Deegan, J. F. II, Tan, Y. and Li, W.-H. (2002) gent mechanisms for the tuning of shortwave sensitive visual Opsin gene and photopigment polymorphism in a prosimian pigments in vertebrates. Photochem. Photobiol. Sci. 3, 713– primate. Vision Res. 42, 11–18. 720. Jacobs, G. H., Calderone, J. B., Fenwick, J. A., Krogh, K. and Hunt, D. M., Carvalho, L. S., Cowing, J. A. and Davies, W. Williams, G. A. (2003) Visual adaptations in a diurnal (2009a) Evolution and spectral tuning of visual pigments in rodent, Octodon degus. J. Comp. Physiol. A. 189, 347–361. birds and mammals. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. Jacobs, G. H., Williams, G. A. and Fenwick, J. A. (2004) 364, 2941–2955. Influence of cone pigment coexpression on spectral sensi- Hunt, D. M., Chan, J., Carvalho, L. S., Hokoc, J. N., tivity and color vision in the mouse. Vision Res. 44, 1615– Ferguson, M. C., Arrese, C. A. and Beazley, L. D. (2009b) 1622. Cone visual pigments in two species of South American Jacobs, G. H., Williams, G. A., Cahill, H. and Nathans, marsupials. Gene 433, 50–55. J. (2007) Emergence of novel color vision in mice engineered Hut, R. A., Scheper, A. and Daan, S. (2000) Can the circadian to express a human cone photopigment. Science 315, 1723– system of a diurnal and a nocturnal rodent entrain to 1725. ultraviolet light? J. Comp. Physiol. A. 186, 707–715. Johnsen, S., Kelber, A., Warrant, E., Sweeney, A. M., Widder, Jacobs, G. H. (1992) Ultraviolet vision in vertebrates. Am. E. A., Lee., R. L. Jr and Hernandez-Andres, J. (2004) Zool. 32, 544–554. Crespuscular and nocturnal illumination and its effects on Jacobs, G. H. (1993) The distribution and nature of colour color perception by the nocturnal hawkmoth Deilephila vision among the mammals. Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. elpenor. J. Exp. Biol. 209, 789–800. 68, 413–471. Kainz, P. M., Neitz, J. and Neitz, M. (1998) Recent evolution Jacobs, G. H. (2007) New World monkeys and color. Int. J. of uniform trichromacy in a New World monkey. Vision Primatol. 28, 729–759. Res. 38, 3315–3320. Jacobs, G. H. (2008) Primate color vision: a comparative Kaskan, P. M., Franco, E. C. S., Yamada, E. S., Silveira, L. perspective. Vis. Neurosci., 25, 619–633. C., Darlington, R. B. and Finlay, B. L. (2005) Peripheral Jacobs, G. H. (2009) Evolution of colour vision in mammals. variability and central constancy in mammalian visual Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci., 364, 2957–2967. system evolution. Proc. Biol. Sci. 272, 91–100. Jacobs, G. H. and Deegan, J. F. II (1992) Cone photopigments Kawamura, S. and Kubotera, N. (2004) Ancestral loss of short in nocturnal and diurnal procyonids. J. Comp. Physiol. A. wave-sensitive cone visual pigment in lorsiform prosiminans, 171, 351–358. contrasting with its strict conservation in other prosimians. Jacobs, G. H. and Deegan, J. F. II (1993) Photopigments J. Mol. Evol. 58, 14–21. underlying color vision in ringtail lemurs (Lemur catta) and Kellie, A., Dain, S. J. and Banks, P. B. (2004) Ultraviolet brown lemurs (Eulemur fulvus). Am. J. Primatol. 30, 243– properties of Australian mammal urine. J. Comp. Physiol. A. 256. 190, 429–435. ª 2010 The Author, Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics ª 2010 The College of Optometrists
You can also read