Processing Fluency and Aesthetic Pleasure: Is Beauty in the Perceiver's Processing Experience?
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
Personality and Social Psychology Review Copyright © 2004 by 2004, Vol. 8, No. 4, 364-382 Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Processing Fluency and Aesthetic Pleasure: Is Beauty in the Perceiver's Processing Experience? RolfReber Department of Psychosocial Science University of Bergen, Norway Norbert Schwarz Department of Psychology and Institute for Social Research University of Michigan Piotr Winkielman Department of Psychology University of California, San Diego We propose that aesthetic pleasure is a function of the perceiver's processing dynam- ics: The more fluently perceivers can process an object, the more positive their aes- thetic response. We review variables known to influence aesthetic judgments, such as figural goodness, figure-ground contrast, stimulus repetition, symmetry, and pro- totypicality, and trace their effects to changes in processing fluency. Other variables that influence processing fluency, like visual or semantic priming, similarly increase judgments of aesthetic pleasure. Our proposal provides an integrative framework for the study of aesthetic pleasure and sheds light on the interplay between early prefer- ences versus cultural infiuences on taste, preferences for both prototypical and ab- stracted forms, and the relation between beauty and truth. In contrast to theories that trace aesthetic pleasure to objective stimulus features per se, we propose that beauty is grounded in the processing experiences of the perceiver, which are in part a func- tion of stimulus properties. What is beauty? What makes for a beautiful face, kiewicz, 1970). This objectivist view inspired many appealing painting, pleasing design, or charming scen- psychological attempts to identify the critical contrib- ery? This question has been debated for at least 2,500 utors to beauty. Among the identified features were bal- years and has been given a wide variety of answers ance and proportion (Arnheim, 1974; Birkhoff, 1933; (Feagin, 1995; Tatarkiewicz, 1970). However, one can Fechner, 1876; Gombrich, 1995), symmetry (Arnheim, broadly distinguish three main positions. 1974; Birkhoff, 1933; Gombrich, 1984; Humphrey, Many theorists, dating back at least to Plato, saw 1997), informational content and complexity (Berlyne, beauty as a property of an object that produces a plea- 1971, 1974; Eysenck, 1941; Gamer, 1974), as well as surable experience in any suitable perceiver (Tatar- contrast and clarity (Gombrich, 1984,1995; St. Thom- as of Aquinas, see Maritain, 1966; Solso, 1997). The objectivist view of beauty was so dominant in the The reported research was supported by the Swiss National Sci- 16th century that artists introduced pattern books, of- ence Foundation (Grant 61-57881.99 to Rolf Reber) and the Na- fering pictorial elements that artists could copy and tional Science Foundation (Grant BCS-0217294 to Piotr Winkiel- combine with each other to create beauty (see Gom- man). Preparation of this article was supported through a fellowship from the Center for Advanced Study in the Behavioral Sciences to brich, 1995). Norbert Schwarz. We thank Marilyn Brewer, Tedra Fazendeiro, Other theorists, dating back at least to the Sophists, Greg Feist, Jamin Halberstadt, Kent Harber. Christian Jakob, Jack proposed that anything could be beautiful if it pleases Katz, Bruce Morton, John Skowronski, Bruce Whittlesea, Bernd Wittenbrink, and an anonymous reviewer for comments on the arti- the senses (Tatarkiewicz, 1970). From this perspective, cle and helpful discussions. beauty is a function of idiosyncratic qualities of the Requests for reprint.s should be sent to Rolf Reber, University perceiver and all efforts to identify the laws of beauty of Bergen, Department of Psychosocial Science, Christiesgate 12, are futile. This subjectivist view, reflected in expres- N-5015 Bergen, Norway. E-mail; rolf.reber@psysp.uib.no; Norbert sions like "beauty is in the eye of the beholder" or "de Schwarz, Institute for Social Reseach, 426 Thompson Street, Ann gustibus non est disputandum" (taste cannot be de- Arbor, MI 48106-1248. E-mail: nschwarz@umich.edu; Piotr Win- kielman, Department of Psychology, University of California at San bated), underlies the social constructivist emphasis on Diego, 9500 Gilman Drive, La Jolla, CA 92093-0109. E-mail: the historically changing and culturally relative nature pwinkiel@ucsd.edu of beauty (see Kubovy, 2000). 364
FLUENCY AND AESTHETIC PLEASURE Most modern philosophical analyses, however, re- Beauty and Judgment ject the objective versus subjective distinction. Instead, Research participants in experimental aesthetics they suggest that a sense of beauty emerges from pat- and psychology studies are rarely asked to judge terns in the way people and objects relate (e.g.. In- "beauty" per se. Instead, most studies have focused on garden, 1985; Merleau-Ponty, 1964). In this article, we judgments like figural goodness, pleasantness, liking, adopt this interactionist perspective and seek to iden- and preference. One may wonder whether these more tify those patterns. As detailed later, we propose that modest judgments capture the grand realm of beauty. beauty is grounded in the processing experiences of Most researchers, including us, believe that by study- the perceiver that emerge from the interaction of stimu- ing such simple judgments, one can identify basic pro- lus properties and perceivers' cognitive and affective cesses underlying the aesthetic experience. Further, processes. several lines of research, for example on the mere ex- posure effect (addressed later in this article), demon- strate that different kinds of evaluative judgments op- Beauty, Experience, and Judgment erate via similar processes (see Bomstein, 1989). Thus, there are reasons to believe that judgments of prefer- Before we present our proposal, a few clarifications ence, liking, and beauty are closely related. are in order. We first introduce our use of the concept of Another issue raised by the focus on basic processes beauty and relate it to the concept of aesthetic pleasure. and the requirements of experimental control is that Subsequently, we discuss the relation between beauty most psychological studies explore fairly mild aes- and various aesthetic judgments, such as judgments of thetic experiences, such as those likely to accompany preference and judgments of aesthetic value. the perception of a vase, a simple melody, an abstract shape, or a human face. Clearly, much of what most hu- mans call "beautiful" on a daily basis falls into the cat- Beauty and Aesthetic Pleasure egory of such mild experiences. However, we hope that future research will also explore stronger subjective The philosopher George Santayana described three experiences and examine to what extent our analysis defining features of beauty: "beauty ... is value posi- can inform such research. tive, intrinsic, and objectified" (Santayana, 1896/1955, Finally, we note that beauty as explored in this arti- p. 31). By value positive and intrinsic, Santayana cle is unrelated to aesthetic value (see, e.g., Beardsley, meant that beauty provides pleasure without any rea- 1981). Since the emergence of modern art, a piece of soning about expected utility. This is similar to Thom- art can have aesthetic value without being beautiful as of Aquinas' definition of beauty as what gives plea- (i.e., without producing an experience of aesthetic sure at sight ("id quod visum placet"), suggesting pleasure). Conversely, a painting that "pleases the immediate joy without intermediate reasoning (see eyes" may be without any artistic merit. The judgment Maritain, 1966). Similarly, the art historian Read of aesthetic value, in contrast to beauty or aesthetic (1972) did "not believe that a person with real sensibil- pleasure as defined here, often involves substantial rea- ity ever stands before a picture and, after a long process soning about the piece of art under consideration. We of analysis, pronounces himself pleased. We either like return to these issues in the discussion. at first sight, or not at all" (p. 38). Finally, beauty is objectified. For example, the experience of having a cold drink on a hot day is both value positive and intrin- sic, but this immediate pleasure lies exclusively in a Overview positive sensation of the body and has little to do with aesthetic appreciation of the object. In contrast, per- Our core proposal is straightforward. We suggest ceivers look at a painting not to please their body, but to that aesthetic experience is a function of the perceiver's enjoy the painting's beauty. Hence, people experience processing dynamics: The more fiuently the perceiver beauty as something that lies in the object. Therefore, can process an object, the more positive is his or her beauty is not an "objective," but an "objectified" prop- aesthetic response. This proposal entails four specific erty (see also Feagin, 1995). assumptions. First, objects differ in the fiuency with In our analyses we follow this philosophical tradi- which they can be processed. Features that facilitate tion and define beauty as a pleasurable subjective expe- fluent processing include all the core features identi- rience that is directed toward an object and not medi- fied in the objectivist tradition, like goodness of form, ated by intervening reasoning. This definition closely symmetry, figure-ground contrast, as well as variables resembles the definition of aesthetic experience used that have not received attention in traditional theories in empirical aesthetics (e.g., Kubovy, 2000; Martindale of aesthetic pleasure, like perceptual and conceptual & Moore, 1988). Accordingly, we use the words priming procedures. Second, processing fluency is it- "beauty" and "aesthetic pleasure" interchangeably. self hedonically marked and high fluency is subjec- 365
REBER, SCHWARZ, WINKIELMAN tively experienced as positive, as indicated by psy- tlesea, 1993), and even truth (e.g., Begg, Anas, & chophysiological findings. Third, processing fluency Farinacci, 1992; R. Reber & Schwarz, 1999). feeds into judgments of aesthetic appreciation because Although fiuency may characterize mental process- people draw on their subjective experience in making es occurring at various levels, our discussion is primar- evaluative judgments, unless the informational value ily concerned with perceptual fluency (i.e., the ease of of the experience is called into question. Finally, the identifying the physical identity of the stimulus). Per- impact of fluency is moderated hy expectations and ceptual fluency is influenced by variables such as per- attribution. On one hand, fluency has a particularly ceptual priming, clarification, presentation duration, strong impact on affective experience if its source is repetition, or figure-ground contrast, as discussed unknown and fluent processing comes as a surprise. shortly. However, our arguments apply as well to con- On the other hand, the fluency-based affective experi- ceptual fluency, or the ease of mental operations con- ence is discounted as a source of relevant information cerned with stimulus meaning and its relation to se- when the perceiver attributes the experience to an irrel- mantic knowledge structures (e.g., Whittlesea, 1993; evant source. Winkielman, Schwarz, Fazendeiro, & Reber, 2003). In the remainder of this article, we elaborate on this We use the more general term processing fluency to proposal and discuss supporting evidence from re- capture these commonalities. search in social and cognitive psychology as well as empirical aesthetics. We first introduce the concept of Hedonic Marking of Fluency processing fluency, discuss its relation to evaluative Multiple theoretical notions converge on the as- processes, and review relevant experimental findings. sumption that high fluency is positively marked. The Next, we provide a selective review of research that at- basic idea in all these notions is that high fluency says tempted to identify key contributors to heauty, drawn something about a positive state of affairs, either from different research traditions. We propose that all within the cognitive system or in the world (see of the previously identified variables share a common Winkielman et al., 2003, for a more comprehensive underlying characteristic, namely the ability to facili- treatment). Specifically, high fluency may elicit posi- tate stimulus processing. Following this review, we tive affect because it is associated with progress toward highlight how attrihutional processes and processing successful recognition of the stimulus, error-free pro- expectations moderate fluency effects on perceived cessing, or the availability of appropriate knowledge beauty, and discuss challenges to our proposal. We fi- structures to interpret the stimulus (Carver & Scheier, nally show that a perceptual fluency theory of beauty 1990; Derryberry & Tucker, 1994; Femandez-Duque, can account for phenomena that are difficult to concep- Baird, & Posner, 2000; Schwarz, 1990; Simon, 1967; tualize in the context of other theories. Specifically, a Ramachandran & Hirstein, 1999; Vallacher & Nowak, perceptual fluency theory helps explain the interplay 1999). High fiuency may also feel good because it sig- between developmentally early preferences and social- nals that an external stimulus is familiar, and thus un- ization, the apparent contradiction between prefer- likely to be harmful (Zajonc, 1968, 1998). ences for average versus exaggerated forms, and why artists and scientists have often considered truth and beauty as two sides of the same coin. Fluency and Evaluations: Empirical Evidence As our review of the empirical literature will indi- Processing Fluency cate, high fluency is reliably associated with more pos- itive evaluations. Historically, this possibility has re- The Concept of Processing Fluency ceived most attention in the context of debates on the The processing of any stimulus can be characterized nature of the mere-exposure effect (i.e., the observa- by a variety of parameters that are nonspecific to its tion that repetition enhances liking for an initially neu- content, such as speed and accuracy of stimulus pro- tral stimulus; for reviews, see Bomstein, 1989; Zajonc, cessing (see R. Reber, Wurtz, & Zimmermann, 2004). 2000). Several authors proposed that the mere-expo- These parameters tend to lead to a common experience sure effect might reflect increases in perceptual flu- of processing ease or "fluency" (for reviews see Clore, ency (e.g., Bornstein & D'Agostino, 1994; Jacoby, 1992; Jacoby, Kelley, & Dywan, 1989; Whittlesea, Kelley, et al., 1989; Seamon, Brody, & Kauff, 1983, Jacoby, & Girard, 1990). A large number of studies Whittlesea, 1993). If so, we may expect that any vari- show that people draw on fluency to make a variety of able that facilitates fiuent processing similarly results nonaesthetic judgments, including judgments of loud- in increased liking, even under conditions of a single ness (e.g., Jacoby, Allan, Collins, & Larwill, 1988), exposure. Studies from our lab support this possibility. clarity (e.g., Whittlesea et al., 1990), duration (e.g., Some of these studies relied on variables that have a Witherspoon & Allen, 1985), familiarity (e.g., Whit- long tradition in theories of aesthetics, like figure- 366
FLUENCY AND AESTHETIC PLEASURE ground contrast, and will be reviewed in a later section. stead, the same pattern of effects was obtained when Other studies relied on manipulations that are uniquely participants studied a list of concept primes before they suggested by a fluency account of aesthetic pleasure, were exposed to the pictures. namely visual and conceptual priming procedures. Affective Response Priming Procedures Importantly, the influence of processing fluency is R. Reber, Winkielman, and Schwarz (1998, Study not limited to explicit judgments of preference and 1) presented participants with pictures of everyday ob- beauty, but can also be captured with psychophysi- jects, such as a desk, bird, or plane (taken from Snod- ological measures. This was demonstrated by Win- grass & Vanderwart, 1980). The quality of the pictures kielman and Cacioppo (2001), who assessed partici- was slightly degraded and processing fluency was pants' affective responses to fluent stimuli with facial manipulated through a visual priming procedure. De- electromyography (EMG). EMG relies on the observa- pending on conditions, the target picture was preceded tion that positive affective responses increase activity by a subliminally presented, highly degraded contour over the region of the zygomaticus major ("smiling of either the target picture or a different picture. We ex- muscle"), whereas negative affective responses in- pected that a matching contour would facilitate pro- crease activity over the region of the corrugator su- cessing (high fluency), consistent with research show- percilli ("frowning muscle;" e.g., Cacioppo, Petty, ing that subliminal visual primes enhance the accuracy Losch, & Kim, 1986; Lang, Greenwald, Bradley, & with which a stimulus can be identified (Bar & Bieder- Hamm, 1993). As expected, high fluency was associ- man, 1998). Some participants were asked to indicate ated with stronger activity over the zygomaticus region how much they liked the target pictures. Other partici- (indicative of positive affect), but was not associated pants were asked to press a key as soon as they could with the activity of the corrugator region (indicative of recognize the object in the picture, thus providing a negative affect). Moreover, the observed differences measure of recognition speed, an indicator of fluency. occurred in the first 3 sec after stimulus presentation The data were consistent with predictions: Pictures and several seconds before participants made overt primed by matched contours were recognized faster, judgments, indicating a spontaneous affective response indicating higher fluency, and were liked more than to processing fluency. A subsequent study replicated pictures preceded by mismatched contours. Moreover, this effect, using presentation duration as a manipula- participants were unaware of the fluency manipulation, tion of fluency. thus eliminating the possibility of strategic responding to pictures preceded by various primes. The Mediating Role of Affect Extending this work with a cross-modal priming Theoretically, we assume that the spontaneous af- task, Winkielman and Fazendeiro (2003) showed par- fective response observed in the Winkielman and ticipants a series of unambiguous pictures of common Cacioppo (2001) studies mediates the impact of flu- objects and animals. Each picture was preceded by a ency on evaluative judgments. Presumably, perceivers letter string consisting either of a word or a nonword. interpret the positive affect elicited by processing flu- Participants first indicated, as fast as possible, if the let- ency as their response to the target, resulting in more ter string was an actual English word. Subsequently, positive evaluations. This assumption is consistent they reported their liking for the picture. The letter with the feelings-as-information model, which holds strings served as the fluency manipulation. Some pic- that feelings serve as a source of information in their tures were preceded by matching words (e.g., word own right, unless their perceived informational value "dog"—picture of a dog), introducing the highest level for the judgment at hand is undermined through of fluency. Other pictures were preceded by associa- (mis)attribution manipulations (e.g., Schwarz & Clore, tively related words (e.g., word "key"—picture of a 1983; for a review see Schwarz & Clore, 1996). lock), introducing a medium level of fluency. Yet other Winkielman and Fazendeiro (2003) tested this pre- pictures were preceded by an unrelated word (e.g., diction by replicating the conceptual priming study de- word "snow"—picture of a desk), introducing the low- scribed earlier with a misattribution manipulation. est level of fluency. The results showed a robust effect Before they made their liking judgments, they specifi- of concept priming on participants' evaluation of the cally told participants that their reactions to the stimuli target pictures. As expected, pictures preceded by might be influenced by background music played to matching words were liked significantly more than them. The music was an ambiguous new-age piece re- pictures preceded by related words, which, in turn, corded at half-speed (see Schwarz et al., 1991, for de- were liked significantly more than pictures preceded tails on this manipulation). Some participants were by unrelated words. Follow-up studies indicated that told that the music might bias how easily stimuli come these fluency effects do not require that the concept to mind (i.e., their fluency experience), whereas other primes immediately precede the target pictures. In- participants were told the music might influence how 367
REBER, SCHWARZ, WINKIELMAN they feel ahout the various stimuli (i.e., their affective ing on two classes of factors that influence perceived experience). The results were highly informative. Spe- beauty. We first discuss "objective" features of stimuli, cifically, attributing subjective fiuency to music did not such as amount of information, symmetry, figure- eliminate the effect of processing facilitation on liking. ground contrast, and clarity. Subsequently, we address That is, participants who were informed that the music the role of perceivers' previous experience with stim- might influence how easily things come to mind still uli, such as repeated exposure to the stimulus, implicit judged pictures as more likeable when they were pre- learning of rules that underlie stimuli, and the pro- ceded by related rather than unrelated words, replicat- totypicality of the stimulus. We propose that the influ- ing the previous findings. In contrast, attributing the af- ence of all of these variables derives from their ability fecfive response to the music did eliminate the effect of to facilitate fluent stimulus processing. processing facilitation on liking. That is, participants who were informed that the music might infiuence their feelings toward various stimuli no longer judged Objective Features of Stimuli new pictures as more likeable when they were pre- Amount of Information ceded hy related rather than by unrelated words. The idea that the amount of information is an im- This pattern of results suggests that it is the fiu- portant determinant of beauty has a long history in aes- ency-based affective reaction, and not the fluency ex- thetics (e.g., Arnheim, 1974; Gombrich, 1984). In psy- perience itself, that serves as a basis of judgment, chology, relevant research has mostly been conducted resulting in enhanced liking. This interpretation is con- in the Gestalt tradition. Early researchers focused on sistent with research into the use of moods (e.g., stimulus organization and proposed that perceived Schwarz & Clore, 1983) and other phenomenal experi- "goodness" of the stimulus depends on the relation be- ences (e.g., ease of recall; Schwarz, 1998) as experien- tween stimulus organization and psychological mecha- tial sources of information. It suggests that the affec- nisms (e.g., Koffka, 1935). In their view, stimuli iso- tive reactions to fluency, captured in EMG studies morphic to physiological mechanisms are easy to (Harmon-Jones & Allen, 2001; Winkielman & Caciop- process and are rated as "good figures." This work's po, 2001) as well as self-reports of mood (Monahan, emphasis on processing ease anticipated later interest Murphy, & Zajonc, 2000), serve a crucial mediating in perceptual fluency by several decades.' role. Subsequent research in the Gestalt tradition has pri- marily focused on the amount of information rep- Summary resented in a stimulus, consistent with the traditional In combination, the reviewed studies demonstrate assumption that beauty resides in the object (e.g., Att- that variables that facilitate the processing of a stimu- neave, 1954; Hochberg & McAlister, 1953). Garner lus result in more positive affective reactions, as cap- (1974) expressed the amount of information extracted tured by psychophysiological measures (Winkielman from a stimulus in terms of inferred R x R subsets, & Cacioppo, 2001), as well as more favorable judg- which represent the number of different shapes after ments of preference (R. Reber etal., 1998). The impact Reflecting and Rotating a shape. He found that judg- of fluency on evaluative judgments is apparently medi- ments of figural goodness were higher the less infor- ated by the elicited affective reactions and hence elimi- mation people had to extract from a stimulus to per- nated when the informational value of the affective re- ceive it (i.e., when the stimulus had high redundancy). action is called into question through misattribution This finding is consistent with people's preference for manipulations (Winkielman & Fazendeiro, 2003). symmetric shapes, because they contain less informa- Not surprisingly, the key variables used in these ex- tion than asymmetric, but otherwise identical shapes periments—visual and conceptual priming—have not (Garner, 1974). Further research has demonstrated that received attention in traditional theories of aesthetic this principle extends beyond judgments of figural judgment. Yet, they share with the variables commonly goodness to evaluative judgments. For example, Nicki, considered in these theories—like goodness of form, Lee, and Moss (1981) found that ambiguity in cubist symmetry, figure-ground contrast or prototypicality— paintings, defined in informational terms, was nega- that they facilitate fiuent processing of the target stimu- tively related to pleasantness judgments. lus. We propose that this shared characteristic is at the Important for our proposal, stimuli with less in- heart of aesthetic pleasure. formation are not only more pleasing, but also easier to process, as measured, for example, by recognition Determinants of Beauty: A Review 'Another historical precursor is Eysenck's observation that "the pleasure derived from a percept as such is directly proportional to the decrease of energy capable of doing work in the total nervous sys- Next, we review research from social psychology, tem, as compared with the original state of the whole system" cognitive psychology, and experimental aesthetics bear- (Eysenck, 1942, p. 358). 368
FLUENCY AND AESTHETIC PLEASURE speed (Checkosky & Whitlock, 1973). Hence, our pro- preferred because symmetry facilitates fluent process- posal suggests that controlling for the amount of infor- ing (see Reber, 2002). mation should reveal a preference for easy-to-process stimuli. Several studies bear on this issue. Some of Contrast and Clarity these studies explored processing ease of different Theorists of aesthetics have long considered con- forms of symmetry, such as symmetry involving re- trast and clarity as "objective" determinants of beauty flection around a vertical axis (as in capital "A" or (Gombrich, 1984, 1995;Maritain, 1966; Solso, 1997). "V"), symmetry involving reflection around a horizon- Again, empirical research suggests that the influence tal axis (as in capital "E" or "D"), or symmetry involv- of these features may be mediated by their effect on ing reflection around a diagonal axis (as in the Nordic processing fluency. character "0"). Palmer and Hemenway (1978), as well Studies show that recognition speed, a standard as Royer (1981), used reaction times to show that verti- measure of fluency, is faster for stimuli high in fig- cal symmetry is easier to detect than horizontal sym- ure-ground contrast (e.g., Checkosky & Whitlock, metry, which in turn is easier to detect than diagonal 1973). Similarly, Whittlesea et al. (1990) showed that a symmetry. Building on this work, Palmer (1991) pre- related variable, visual clarity, influences perceptual sented dot patterns in vertically, horizontally, or diag- fluency as measured by memory misattribution. In onally symmetrical arrangements, manipulating the their study, participants saw short and rapidly pre- ease of processing while controlling for the amount of sented lists of words. After each list, a target word was information presented (in terms of R x R subsets, as presented within a visual noise mask, resulting in discussed earlier). Consistent with our perceptual flu- higher or lower visual clarity of the target word. As ex- ency analysis, the same stimuli received the highest pected, higher clarity increased the likelihood that an ratings of figural goodness when presented in a vertical item was erroneously recognized as having been pre- arrangement and the lowest ratings when presented in a sented earlier. This finding indicates a misattribution diagonal arrangement, with horizontal arrangements of high perceptual fluency, induced by visual clarity, to falling in between. Given that the amount of presented previous exposure. Consistent with this interpretation, information was identical in all conditions, these find- the effect disappeared when participants knew that vi- ings strongly support the hypothesis that figural good- sual clarity was manipulated. ness is a function of perceptual fluency rather than amount of information per se. Building on this work, we explored the influence of figure-ground contrast on liking (R. Reber et al., 1998). In one study we manipulated perceptual fluency Symmetry by varying the figure-ground contrast of circles pre- sented for 1 sec. As expected, circles with high fig- The aforementioned findings also bear on proposals ure-ground contrast were judged as prettier (or less that try to locate beauty in objective symmetry. Sym- ugly, depending on the framing of the judgment task) metry has been found to influence the perceived attrac- than circles with low figure-ground contrast. More- tiveness of human faces (Gangestad, Thornhill, & Yeo, over, the impact of processing fluency did not depend 1994; Rhodes, Proffitt, Grady, & Sumich, 1998; on the framing of the judgment task. Specifically, we Rhodes, Sumich, & Byatt, 1999). Moreover, symmet- asked some participants to report how "pretty" the ric patterns are preferred even if they do not serve any stimuli were and others how "ugly" the stimuli were. biologically relevant function, both in humans (e.g., Increased fluency resulted in judgments of higher Humphrey, 1997; R. Reber & Schwarz, in press) and in "prettiness" as well as judgments of lower "ugliness," animals (Rensch, 1957, 1958). These observations are indicating that fluency did not simply facilitate ex- often explained by postulating an innate preference for treme judgments per se.2 symmetry (e.g., Etcoff, 1999; Pinker, 1997). Given that symmetry is indicative of mate value in several species Of course, one may argue that in the R. Reber et al. (e.g., Thornhill & Gangstead, 1993, 1999), this is a (1998) study, it is the high figure-ground contrast per plausible hypothesis, although the evidence for hu- se (i.e., the objective feature), not perceptual fluency, mans is mixed (see Kalick, Zebrowitz, Langlois, & that contributes to more positive evaluations. If so, Johnson, 1998, for a discussion). More important for high-contrast stimuli should be judged more favorably our purposes, symmetrical patterns also have less in- formation and are hence easier to process (Gamer, ^This result, related findings by Seamon, McKenna, and Binder 1974). In addition, computer-modeling work suggests (1998), as well as studies using psychophysiological measures (Har- that a perceptual system designed to recognize objects mon-Jones & Allen, 2001; Winkielman & Cacioppo, 2001) speak from different viewpoints will process symmetrical against the possibility that the influence of fluency on preferences is reducible to misattribution of nonspecific activation to a salient judg- patterns more efficiently than any other pattern (En- ment dimension (Mandler, Nakamura, & Van Zandt, 1987). Instead, quist & Arak, 1994; Johnstone, 1994). Based on these they suggest that the affective reaction induced by fluency is hedoni- findings, we propose that symmetrical patterns may be cally positive. 369
REBER, SCHWARZ, WINKIELMAN than low-contrast stimuli, regardless of stimulus pre- Perceiver's History With the Stimulus sentation time. Conversely, if perceptual fluency is the Psychological research on factors underlying beau- key to preference, figure-ground contrast should be ty has also investigated the history of a perceiver's ex- most influential during short presentation times, when perience with the stimulus. This research points to the high contrast can facilitate processing, and least influ- role of repeated exposure, implicit learning of stimulus ential during long presentation times, when stimuli can structure, and prototypicality. Again, the influence of be easily processed regardless of their contrast level. these variables can be traced to their influence on pro- R. Reber and Schwarz (2001) tested this reasoning in a cessing fluency. study where figure-ground contrast was manipulated for circles presented for 0.3, 1, 3, and 10 sec. As pre- dicted by the fluency hypothesis, figure-ground con- Repeated Exposure trast influenced aesthetic judgments only at short expo- Repeated exposure to a stimulus results in more sure durations, but not at the duration of 10 sec. Note favorable evaluations, a phenomenon known as the that a theory that assumes that some objective feature mere exposure effect (Zajonc, 1968, 1998). Mere ex- of the stimulus per se, such as figure-ground contrast, posure effects have been obtained with a variety of determines preference cannot account for this fmding. stimuli (faces, ideographs, words, melodies) and a va- After all, different exposure times do not change the riety of measures (judgments of preference, behavioral objective features of the stimulus. If anything, the ob- choices, physiological responses), indicating the ro- jective features of the stimulus are most clearly percep- bustness of the phenomenon (for a meta-analysis see tible at long exposure times—yet, they fail to influence Bornstein, 1989; for mere-exposure effects with works judgments under this condition. ofart, see Leder, 2001). Previously seen stimuli differ from novel stimuli Summary with regard to at least three fluency-related parameters. First, familiar stimuli are processed faster than novel In sum, a diverse body of research shows that objec- stimuli (e.g., Haber & Hershenson, 1965; Jacoby & tive features of stimuli—like amount of information, Dallas, 1981). Second, familiar stimuli elicit less at- symmetry, or figure-ground contrast^—influence per- tentional orienting than novel stimuli (Desimone, Mil- ceptual fluency as well as preference judgments. More ler, Chelazzi, & Lueschow, 1995). Third, familiar stim- important, knowledge of the objective features per se is uli have more organized processing dynamics than not sufficient to predict an object's evaluation. Instead, novel stimuli (Lewenstein & Nowak, 1989; Norman & the crucial variable appears to be processing fluency. O'Reilly, 2001; E. R. Smith, 2000). Based on such Although fluency typically covaries with objective fea- findings, several researchers suggested that perceptual tures, fluency is the predictive variable when both are fluency is central to the mere exposure effect and pro- juxtaposed. Thus, identical patterns are rated more fa- vided evidence consistent with this account (e.g., vorably when presented with vertical rather than hori- Bornstein & D'Agostino, 1994; Jacoby, Kelley, & zontal symmetry (Palmer, 1991), consistent with the Dywan, 1989; Seamon, Brody, & Kauff, 1983; observation that vertical symmetry facilitates process- Whittlesea, 1993; Whittlesea & Price, 2001). ing (e.g., Royer, 1981). Similarly, high contrast en- hances liking for patterns shown briefly, but not for In our reading, a perceptual fluency account of mere identical patterns shown longer (R. Reber & Schwarz, exposure effects is compatible with Zajonc's (1968, 2001). Moreover, objectively identical stimuli are eval- 1998) original account. Specifically, Zajonc proposed uated more favorably when their processing is facili- that the mere exposure effect reflects a precognitive tated through priming procedures (R. Reber et al., mechanism that ensures caution in encounters with 1998; Winkielman & Fazendeiro, 2003). In light of novel, and potentially harmful, stimuli and the gradual these results, we propose that the influence of objective extinction of this caution over repeated encounters, features is mediated by their influence on processing consistent with the observation of a "fear of the un- fluency. known" in a variety of species (for a review see Hill, 1978). Given that processing fluency can often be as- sessed before a stimulus is identified with certainty, 'One well-known objective feature that we did not address is the fluency may indeed serve as a very early indicator golden section. The possibility that people have a preference for the golden section is one of the oldest and most studied phenomena in of stimulus novelty (see Curran, 2000, and Rugg & empirical aesthetics (e.g., Fechner, 1876; for a review, see Green, Yonelinas, 2003, for a discussion of specific mecha- 1995). For example, rectangles are assumed to be most pleasing if nisms). Moreover, the spontaneous affective responses the ratio of the short side to the long side of the rectangle equals the that accompany fluency may serve as a biologically ratio of the long side to the sum of short and long side, which is about functional signal (see Winkielman et al., 2003, for a 1:1.618. However, evidence for preference of the golden section is mixed (Green, 1995; Kubovy, 2000). Further, we are not aware of discussion). We therefore consider both accounts com- any work testing how golden section influences processing fluency. patible, but note that a fluency account provides a more Accordingly, we do not address the phenomenon. general conceptualization that can accommodate the 370
FLUENCY AND AESTHETIC PLEASURE influence of priming procedures as well as the influ- Buchner (1994) first exposed participants to grammati- ence of repeated exposure. In addition, a fluency ac- cal letter strings and subsequently presented strings count predicts under which conditions mere exposure with a perceptual clarification procedure, asking par- effects should not be observed, namely when partici- ticipants to identify the strings. Specifically, the test pants are aware of the source of fluency (cf. Bomstein, strings were presented in a black mask, from which 1989). We return to this issue in a later section, when pixels were gradually removed at random until the par- we discuss the moderating role of attributions and ticipant could identify the letter string. Participants re- expectations. acted faster to grammatical rather than ungrammatical letter strings, indicating that grammatical letter strings are easier to process. Implicit Learning In combination, the available findings again indi- of Stimulus Structure cate that increased preference is associated with in- Research into the effects of repeated exposure is creased fluency: "Regular" or "grammatical" stimuli closely related to research into the relation between are not only preferred over "irregular" or "ungrammat- preferences and the implicit learning of stimulus struc- ical" ones, but also easier to process. ture. In one experiment, Gordon and Holyoak (1983) used letter strings constructed in accordance with an Prototypicality artificial finite state grammar (see A. S. Reber, 1967, Martindale (1984) proposed that prototypical forms 1993). In a learning phase, participants were exposed are preferred over nonprototypical forms—a proposal to grammatical letter strings. In a subsequent test that is closely related to the idea that people prefer "av- phase, participants received novel grammatical and un- erage" stimuli (Rhodes & Tremewan, 1996).'* Numer- grammatical strings and indicated their liking for these ous studies confirm that prototypical and "average" strings. As predicted, grammatical letter strings were forms are preferred over nonprototypical ones. For liked more than ungrammatical ones (see also Manza, example, several studies found a positive relation Zizak, & Reber, 1998). In a second experiment, between prototypicality and aesthetic evaluations in Gordon and Holyoak (1983) were able to show that color patches (e.g., Martindale & Moore, 1988), paint- participants' liking of complex visual patterns was neg- ings (e.g., Hekkert & van Wieringen, 1990), and furni- atively related to pattern distortion from an acquired ture (e.g., Whitfield & Slatter, 1979). Preference for standard; the less distorted the pattern was in compari- prototypicality has also been found in music. For ex- son to the standard, the more it was liked. ample, J. D. Smith and Melara (1990) showed that pro- In a series of experiments, Newell and Bright totypical chord progressions were preferred by novices (2001) presented grammatical letter strings in an en- (though not by experts, as we discuss later). Similarly, coding phase and then assessed grammaticality and Repp (1997) mixed music performances of different liking ratings for both grammatical and ungrammatical performers into an averaged performance, and found items at test. In all three experiments, there was a con- that such performance was highly rated. sistent effect of grammaticality on liking, but only if A number of studies show that prototypical faces encoding and test conditions stayed the same, ensuring are preferred over nonprototypical faces (e.g., fluent processing. In contrast, grammaticality judg- Langlois & Roggman, 1990; Rhodes & Tremewan, ments were not influenced by inconsistencies between 1996). Theoretical explanations for this robust effect encoding and test. Newell and Bright concluded that have typically focused on the idea that organisms are grammaticality judgments reflected attempts to explic- biologically predisposed to interpret prototypicality itly recall information about training items, whereas as a cue to mate value (Symons, 1979). For example, the effect on liking was based on an (mis)attribution of Thornhill and Gangestad (1993, 1999) suggested that fluency. facial prototypicality signals health, lending individu- Sollberger and Reber (2004) demonstrated effects als with a preference for facial prototypicality in mate of implicit learning on liking in the domain of music. They constructed tonal music sequences in accordance with a finite state grammar. During a learning phase, ''Although the concept of "averageness" can refer to an objective participants were exposed to grammatical tone se- feature (i.e., the stimulus represents the arithmetical mean of all ex- emplars in the population), in most studies averageness is closely re- quences. During the test phase, they were given old lated to prototypicality (i.e., perceiver's mental representation of the grammatical, novel grammatical, and novel ungram- best or most typical instance of a category). That is, when partici- matical sequences. Participants liked old and novel pants are given stimuli that are average (e.g., they were created by grammatical sequences more than ungrammatical se- morphing multiple exemplars together), usually those stimuli are prototypical (e.g., participants also see the contributing exemplars quences, supporting the notion that grammatical stim- and form a mental representation of the category). Though it is theo- uli are pleasant. retically possible to distinguish the "objective" population average Importantly, research shows that grammatical stim- from a more idiosyncratic psychological prototype, few studies ex- uli are processed with higher fluency. For example. amine distinct contributions of these two factors. 371
REBER, SCHWARZ, WINKIELMAN selection a selective advantage in the evolutionary and Vrana (1998) observed that repeated exposure past. However, recent research casts doubt on this as- increased liking up to nine exposures. After 27 expo- sumption. Some studies failed to document a relation sures, liking increased if the salience of the repetition between facial prototypicality and health (Kalick et scheme was low, but decreased if repetition salience al., 1998). More important, innate preferences for was high. Other studies found that mere-exposure prototypical faces should not necessarily lead to pref- effects were more easily obtained for complex than for erence for other prototypical objects. Yet, several simple visual stimuli (Bornstein, Kale, & Cornell, studies using different methodologies show that peo- 1990), although the latter are easier to process. ple also prefer prototypical dogs, watches, and birds To account for such findings within a perceptual (Halberstadt & Rhodes, 2000, 2003). Thus, there are fluency account, it is useful to consider factors that de- reasons to look for another explanation of the pro- termine (a) the elicitation of fluency-related subjective totypicality preference. Again, perceptual fluency is a experiences and (b) the use of these experiences in promising candidate. judgment. A large body of literature in cognitive psychology indicates that prototypical stimuli are processed more Processing Expectations easily than nonprototypical ones (see Posner & Keele, 1968; Rumelhart, McClelland, & the PDP Research According to the discrepancy-attribution hypothe- Group, 1986; E. E. Smith, Shoben, & Rips, 1974). Fol- sis (Whittlesea & Williams, 1998,2000), fluency asso- lowing up on this literature, we conducted a study in ciated with processing a certain event is more likely to which participants first studied several visual dot pat- elicit a subjective experience (pleasure, familiarity, terns. The patterns were random, but were constructed etc.) if the fluency is unexpected in light of the person's to converge on a prototype. In a later test phase, partici- processing expectations, which constitute a "norm" for pants (a) preferred the prototype over other previously the event (Kahneman & Miller, 1986). For example, unseen patterns; (b) showed stronger EMG responses seeing your dentist (a fiuent stimulus) at the dental of- from the zygomaticus region, indicating more positive fice does not generate a strong feeling of familiarity, affect; and (c) falsely recognized the prototype as hav- whereas unexpectedly seeing the same dentist at the ing been shown previously, indicating high fluency airport does.^ According to the discrepancy-attribu- (Fazendeiro & Winkielman, 2003). Moreover, P. J. tion hypothesis, salient causes of fluency (e.g., an obvi- Reber, Stark, and Squire (1998) showed that exposure ous repetition scheme, a very simple pattern, predictive to several exemplars of dot patterns from the same cat- context) allow participants to formulate accurate ex- egory resulted in more fluent visual processing of the pectations regarding the processing fluency of the tar- prototype, as reflected in decreased activity in the pos- get stimuli and thus reduce the likelihood that fluency terior occipital cortex (see Aizenstein et al., 2000, for a will elicit a subjective experience (Whittlesea & Wil- related finding). liams, 2000). Nevertheless, many fluent stimuli may In sum, we propose that the robust relation between continue to elicit a pleasant experience even when flu- prototypicality and attractiveness may be traced to pro- ency of processing is expected. For example, we sus- cessing fluency: Prototypical stimuli are easier to pro- pect that people continue to enjoy prototypical faces, cess and hence evaluated more positively, consistent symmetrical patterns, harmonious chords, and high with the observation that any other variable that in- clarity drawings even after they formed fairly accurate creases processing fluency also increases liking. processing expectations for these stimuli. Schwarz (2004a, 2004b) suggested that these di- verging intuitions reflect that expectations have a dual Moderating Variables: influence: On the one hand, unexpected fluency is Expectations and Attributions more likely to capture attention, resulting in a con- scious experience as suggested by the discrepancy-at- Next, we turn to variables that may moderate this re- tribution hypothesis. On the other hand, expectations lation and address a number of potential complica- entail an attribution to the variable that gave rise to the tions. To date, moderating variables have received expectation in the first place. When this variable is ir- most attention in research on the mere exposure effect relevant to the evaluation of the stimulus (as is the case (Zajonc, 1968). As reviewed earlier, this research showed that repetition elicits a positive mood (Mona- han et al., 2000) and increases preference for stimuli ^This possibility is elegantly illustrated by experiments showing (Bornstein, 1989). However, this research has also that pseudohomophones (letter strings that sound like real words, identified important qualifications. Some studies such as "phraug") and orthographically regular nonwords (e.g., "hension") elicit illusions of familiarity and are rated as pleasant found that liking initially increased with the number of (Whittlesea & Shimizu, 2001). Presumably, these stimuli elicit cog- presentations, followed by a decline in liking (e.g.. nitive and affective reactions because they are processed faster than Kail & Freeman, 1973). For example. Van den Bergh participants expect. 372
FLUENCY AND AESTHETIC PLEASURE for repetition schemes or predictive contexts), the ex- Stimulus Complexity perience is considered uninformative and not used An important challenge for our fiuency account is informing a judgment, as discussed later. When the why people sometimes prefer complex over simple variable is relevant to the evaluation (as is the case for stimuli. After all, a simple stimulus (e.g., a single line symmetry, prototypicality, and so on, which are char- or one musical note) should be easier to process than a acteristics of the stimulus itself), the experience is con- complex stimulus (e.g., a pattern of lines or a chord), sidered informative and used as a basis of judgment. but the latter are commonly preferred. We propose that This conjecture, derived from the feelings-as-informa- several processes may contribute to a relative prefer- tion model (Schwarz & Clore, 1996), awaits empirical ence for complexity. testing. Expectations and Attributions Attributional Processes As discussed in the preceding section, the ability of fiuency to elicit an experience, and the perceiver's will- Once an affective experience is elicited by a fluent ingness to use that experience in forming a judgment, stimulus, its impact on preference judgments is moder- depends on expectations and attributions. Simple stim- ated by attributional processes (e.g., Bornstein & uli allow the perceiver to form more accurate process- D'Agostino, 1994; Van den Bergh & Vrana, 1998). In ing expectations, reducing the strength of the fluency- general, individuals only rely on experiential informa- based experience. Further, simple stimuli provide the tion when it seems to bear on the target of judgment, perceiver with an obvious attribution for the experi- but not when they are aware that it may reflect the in- ence. Consistent with these ideas, a common view in fiuence of an unrelated variable (Schwarz & Clore, aesthetics holds that highest beauty is attained by "uni- 1996). Thus, people should only rely on fluency-based formity in variety," or "simplicity in complexity," as experiences when their informational value is not when a complex theme is presented in an accessible called into question. When it becomes apparent that way (see Dickie, 1997). This phenomenon can be un- their experience may merely reflect the influence of derstood by conceptualizing uniformity or simplicity high repetition or a preceding prime, the experience is as a source of fiuency, and variety or complexity as a discounted and perceivers switch to alternative inputs source of processing expectations. That is, when pro- to form a judgment (e.g., Bornstein & D'Agostino, cessing is expected to be difficult, yet turns out to be 1994; Van den Bergh & Vrana, 1998). This assumption easy, it creates a particularly strong experience of aes- is consistent with (mis)attribufion effects observed in thetic pleasure (see Gombrich, 1984, for a related the exploration of other sources of experiential infor- discussion).* mation, like moods (e.g., Schwarz & Clore, 1983; for a review see Schwarz & Clore, 1996), ease of recall (e.g., A similar logic may explain why stimulus complex- Schwarz et al., 1991; Winkielman, Schwarz, & Belli, ity is often related to preference by an inverted U- 1998; for a review see Schwarz, 1998), or familiarity shaped function (e.g., Berlyne, 1971; Vitz, 1966). With (Fazendeiro, Winkielman, Luo, & Lorah, in press). low levels of complexity, the source of fluency is very Consistent with these earlier findings, we observed salient. As complexity increases, the salience of the that the impact of processing fluency on judgment is source of perceptual fluency decreases, enhancing the eliminated when participants attribute their affective misattribution of fluency to beauty. However, further reactions to an irrelevant source, such as a background increases in complexity will eventually reduce pro- music (e.g., Winkielman & Fazendeiro, 2003), as re- cessing fluency, leading to a decrease in perceived viewed earlier. beauty. These mechanisms would combine to form a U-shaped relation between complexity and beauty, as predicted and found by Berlyne (1971). Challenges and Conjectures Different Sources of Processing Facilitation In this section, we address various challenges that a Perceptual versus conceptual fluency. Complex- fluency account of aesthetic pleasure needs to handle. ity may sometimes be preferred because it facilitates We first discuss why people sometimes prefer complex access to the meaning of the stimulus. That is, a de- rather than simple stimuli, including preference for crease in perceptual fluency due to complexity may be complex stimuli by experts. We then turn to the ques- outweighed by an increase in conceptual fiuency due to tion of how stimulus valence may moderate fluency ef- fects on preference. The empirical evidence bearing on these challenges is often sparse and sometimes contra- ^Similar processes may underlie Eysenck's (1941, 1942) obser- dictory. We review what is known and offer some con- vations that order (source of fluency) and complexity (source of ex- jectures and recommendations for future research. pectations) interact in determining aesthetic pleasure. 373
REBER, SCHWARZ, WINKIELMAN meaningfulness. Consistent with this idea, Martindale, Melara, 1990). Do these observations imply that on- Moore, and Borkum (1990) found that meaningfulness ly novices' preferences are influenced by processing was a better predictor of aesthetic preference than fluency? complexity per se. In our reading, two different processes are likely to Future research may fruitfully address the relative contribute to these differential preferences. First, train- contribution of fluency to the subjective experiences of ing in the arts presumably increases the fluency with the perceiver at various stages of processing. In a first which complex art stimuli can be processed. Repeated step toward this question, R. Reber et al. (2004) exam- exposure to complex stimuli results in higher percep- ined how processing speed contributes to the subjec- tual fiuency, and training in arts gives meaning to com- tive experience of "ease" at various perceptual stages. plex structures in paintings, poems, or music, which Participants were presented with words in high and low results in an additional increase in processing ease. Ac- figure-ground contrast, printed in more and less read- cordingly, the observation that experts have higher able fonts. The participants' task was to either detect preference for complex stimuli than novices may itself the appearance of these words, or to identify them as reflect fiuency effects. Second, experts are more likely quickly as possible. As expected, high contrast sped up than novices to consider aesthetic value, the ideas be- performance on a detection task, reflecting early pro- hind the work, and the norms of "good" and "bad" taste cessing stages, but not on the identification task, re- (Bourdieu, 1979/1987; Gombrich, 1995). As a result, flecting later processing stages. On the other hand, they may evaluate simple stimuli more negatively than high font readability sped up performance on the iden- novices, despite the pleasure they receive from easy tification task, but not on the detection task. Interest- processing. ingly, both high figure-ground contrast and high font As Wilson et al. (1993) demonstrated, such an ana- readability enhanced judgments of the subjective ease lytic approach to aesthetic preference bears the risk of of perception. This finding suggests that facilitation of making poor choices. In their studies, participants who the early stimulus detection stage and the later stimulus had to explain why they liked a poster subsequently se- identification stage of processing can jointly contribute lected posters from which they derived less pleasure to the experience of ease. Future studies should address once they had it at home than participants who could how facilitation at various processing stages contrib- rely on their gut preference. Similarly, experts may run utes to liking. the risk of acquiring art that they find less enjoyable than expected, once they no longer interact with it in Objective simplicity versus cognitive ease. Fi- the thoughtful manner that led to their choice. How- nally, objectively simpler stimuli are not always easier ever, our processing fluency account provides some on the cognitive system. In the visual domain, complex hope for these experts: Extended exposure to their new shapes often have higher redundancy and thus are rec- treasure may eventually result in a preference that is ognized faster than simple shapes (Biederman, Hilton, driven by processing fluency instead of aesthetic value. & Hummel, 1991). In the auditory domain, recognition In a similar vein, in the history of western music, more of a melody is facilitated through multiple embedding complex musical intervals were gradually introduced of redundant patterns (Gentner & Hulse, 1998). Empir- and then accepted by an initially reluctant audience, ical studies and connectionist models suggest that rec- potentially refiecting increased fluency through re- ognition is often easier when a pattern is embedded in a peated exposure. In fact, Roederer (1973) suggested larger context (e.g., the letter "r" by itself or in the con- that more complex musical intervals require more text of a word), with different levels of analysis sim- complex information processing, a skill that the audi- ultaneously supporting each other (Rumelhart & ence acquires with increasing familiarity. McClelland, 1982). In short, simplicity per se does not necessarily imply ease of processing. Hence, it is al- Stimnlus Valence ways necessary to empirically examine the actual ease of processing for simple and complex stimuli. Central to our proposal is that processing fluency is experienced as positive and that this experience, in turn, results in more favorable judgments. As our re- Experts and Novices view indicates, this effect has been observed for stimuli Numerous studies demonstrated profound differ- that are largely content-free (abstract designs, novel ences in the aesthetic preferences of novices and ex- melodies, nonwords, etc.), as well as for stimuli that perts. In general, people without art training prefer are highly meaningful (everyday objects, faces, dogs, simple and symmetric visual elements, whereas people birds, watches, guns, words, sentences, etc.). This ob- with art training prefer complex and asymmetric visual servation, however, does not imply that stimulus con- elements (e.g., McWhinnie, 1968). Similarly, music tent is irrelevant, and we would certainly expect that novices prefer prototypical chord progressions, where- stimuli with positive content are evaluated more favor- as experts do not show this preference (e.g.. Smith & ably than stimuli with negative content. But how does 374
You can also read