Predicting School-Aged Cognitive Impairment in Children Born Very Preterm
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
Predicting School-Aged Cognitive Impairment in Children Born Very Preterm Carmina Erdei, MD,a,b Nicola C. Austin, DM,c,d Sara Cherkerzian, ScD,a,b Alyssa R. Morris, MA,e Lianne J. Woodward, PhDf Children born very preterm (VPT) are at high risk of cognitive BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVES: abstract impairment that impacts their educational and social opportunities. This study examined the predictive accuracy of assessments at 2, 4, 6, and 9 years in identifying preterm children with cognitive impairment by 12 years. METHODS: We prospectively studied a regional cohort of 103 children born VPT (#32 weeks’ gestation) and 109 children born term from birth to corrected age 12 years. Cognitive functioning was assessed by using age-appropriate, standardized measures: Bayley Scales of Infant Development, Second Edition (age 2); Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence (ages 4 and 6); and Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Fourth Edition (ages 9 and 12). RESULTS:By 12 years, children born VPT were more likely to have severe (odds ratio 3.9; 95% confidence interval 1.1–13.5) or any (odds ratio 3.2; 95% confidence interval 1.8–5.6) cognitive impairment compared with children born term. Adopting a severe cognitive impairment criterion at age 2 under-identified 44% of children born VPT with later severe impairment, whereas a more inclusive earlier criterion identified all severely affected children at 12 years. Prediction improved with age, with any delay at age 6 having the highest sensitivity (85%) and positive predictive value (66%) relative to earlier age assessments. Inclusion of family-social circumstances further improved diagnostic accuracy. CONCLUSIONS: Cognitive risk prediction improves with age, with assessments at 6 years offering optimal diagnostic accuracy. Intervention for children with early mild delay may be beneficial, especially for those raised in socially disadvantaged family contexts. a Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Boston, Massachusetts; bDepartment of Pediatrics, Harvard Medical School, WHAT’S KNOWN ON THIS SUBJECT: Approximately half Harvard University, Boston, Massachusetts; cChristchurch Women’s Hospital, Christchurch, New Zealand; of children born very preterm have mild to moderate d Department of Pediatrics, University of Otago, Christchurch, New Zealand; eDepartment of Psychology, University cognitive impairments by school age. Early of Southern California, Los Angeles, California; and fSchool of Health Sciences, University of Canterbury, identification is challenging because cognition is Christchurch, New Zealand difficult to assess at younger ages. It is unclear when Study enrollment and patient monitoring and follow-up occurred at Christchurch Women’s Hospital school-aged cognitive risk is best predicted. in New Zealand. WHAT THIS STUDY ADDS: Cognitive risk prediction Dr Erdei conceptualized the research questions of this study, performed the analysis and improves with age during early childhood, with interpretation of the data in conjunction with the coauthors, drafted the initial manuscript, and edited the manuscript; Dr Cherkerzian contributed to the statistical methods and analysis and assessments at age 6 years offering the best critically reviewed and helped revise the present manuscript; Ms Morris conducted the initial diagnostic accuracy. Both mild and severe early delay analysis and critically reviewed the present manuscript; Prof Woodward and Associate Prof Austin place children at risk later, with risk being further conceptualized the design of this study in addition to the larger study on which this analysis is exacerbated by family-social disadvantage. based, coordinated and supervised data collection, contributed to the data analysis plan, and critically reviewed and helped revise the present manuscript; and all authors approved the final To cite: Erdei C, Austin NC, Cherkerzian S, et al. Predicting manuscript as submitted and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work. School-Aged Cognitive Impairment in Children Born Very Preterm. Pediatrics. 2020;145(4):e20191982 Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news by guest on September 23, 2021 PEDIATRICS Volume 145, number 4, April 2020:e20191982 ARTICLE
Children born very preterm followed children born VPT or classified as showing severe (VPT) remain at high risk for extremely preterm prospectively to impairment if their IQ score was neurodevelopmental impairments examine the relations between early .2 SDs below the term group despite advances in neonatal care.1–3 ability and middle childhood mean and any impairment (mild or These impairments span multiple cognitive functioning, with variable severe) if their IQ was .1 SD domains, with cognitive difficulties results.21–31 Some studies report below the term mean at each affecting approximately half of good concordance between Bayley assessment point.1,6 surviving children.1,4–7 Between 40% assessments and later cognitive 2. Examine the predictive accuracy of and 50% of children born VPT meet function in children,21,23,25,30 whereas standardized cognitive measures criteria for either mild or severe others suggest poor correlation at ages 2, 4, 6, and 9 in identifying cognitive or intellectual impairment, with preschool-26,31 and school- cognitive impairment at age 12 in which is defined as an IQ .1 SD aged22,24,27 cognitive outcomes. One children born VPT. below the normative mean. These study of extremely preterm survivors 3. Assess whether cognitive risk cognitive difficulties are in turn examined the accuracy of the Bayley prediction for children born VPT associated with high rates of special scales in predicting cognitive function could be further improved by education service use,8,9 longer-term at school age using multiple age considering the family-social educational underachievement,10 assessments.23 Results suggested that context. social1,5,6 and mental health11 early assessments were relatively difficulties, as well as reduced earning good predictors of later cognitive and employment potential in function, with accuracy of risk METHODS adulthood.12,13 This suggests that prediction improving with age. The even milder cognitive impairments generalizability of these observations Sample may have significant impacts on to VPT survivors remains uncertain. Two groups of children were functioning over the life course. An additional and important included. The VPT group comprised Early neurodevelopmental consideration is the social context in 103 children born at #32 weeks’ intervention is therefore critical to which children are raised given that gestational age (GA) consecutively mitigate these adverse long-term family socioeconomic factors also admitted into a level III NICU at effects. Not only is the brain impact cognitive development.4,5,32–34 Christchurch Women’s Hospital in undergoing rapid development Mangin et al35 found that family- New Zealand from 1998 to 2000 during early childhood but it is also social adversity contributed (92% recruitment) and followed characterized by a high degree of additively to preterm children’s through age 12 years. Exclusion neural plasticity and sensitivity to cognitive risk in middle childhood. criteria included congenital positive and negative environmental Other data suggest that over time, abnormalities and non–English- influences.14,15 Yet, a major challenge environmental factors may play an speaking parents. Recruited infants for early identification of cognitive increasingly important role in did not differ from nonrecruited impairment is that some deficits do shaping VPT children’s cognitive infants on clinical or family-social not manifest until older ages, when development than earlier perinatal factors. Excluding post–NICU- the demands of the environment exposures.33 This highlights the discharge deaths (n = 3), retention at exceed the developmental capabilities importance of considering not only ages 4, 6, 9, and 12 was 98%, 97%, of the child. This raises important the timing of earlier assessments 96%, and 97%, respectively. questions regarding the optimal but also the extent of family-social The term group comprised 109 duration of developmental disadvantage when assessing children born at 37 to 41 weeks’ monitoring to ensure accurate and need for ongoing monitoring and gestation identified from hospital timely identification and intervention intervention for children born VPT. birth records, recruited at age 2, and for children born VPT with clinically followed through age 12 years. significant cognitive and learning Therefore, our aims in this study were Children were matched for sex and needs.16,17 as follows: pregnancy due date, with 62% (n = The Bayley Scales of Infant 1. Examine the extent of severe and 113) of regionally representative Development18 represent the most any (mild or severe) cognitive eligible infants being included. commonly used measure of cognitive impairment in children born VPT Nonparticipation reasons included ability before age 3. After this time, compared with those born term at primarily family circumstances.6 cognition is typically assessed by corrected ages 2, 4, 6, 9, and Retention rates at ages 4, 6, 9, and using standardized intelligence 12 years. For consistency with 12 were 96%, 96%, 97%, and measures.19,20 Multiple studies have other studies, children were 96%, respectively. Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news by guest on September 23, 2021 2 ERDEI et al
Procedures TABLE 1 Study Sample: Clinical and Demographic Characteristics (N = 212) Procedures and measures were VPT Term Test Statistica P (N = 103) (N = 109) approved by the Canterbury Regional Ethics Committee with written Child clinical characteristics GA, mean (SD) 28 (2) 39 (1) 245.1 ,.001b informed consent obtained from Birth wt, mean (SD) 1061 (314) 3585 (411) 250.4 ,.001b all parents and/or guardians. All Male sex, n (%) 52 (50) 60 (55) 0.4 .51 children underwent comprehensive Twin birth, n (%) 34 (33) 4 (4) 31.0 ,.001b neurodevelopmental assessments Small for GA, n (%) 11 (11) 1 (1) 9.5 .002b close to their second, fourth, sixth, Culture-proven sepsis, n (%) 30 (29) 0 (0) na — Oxygen use at 36 wk, n (%) 36 (35) 0 (0) na — ninth, and 12th birthdays (corrected ROP stage 3 or 4, n (%) 4 (4) 0 (0) na — for GA). Cognitive assessments were IVH grade 3 or 4, n (%) 6 (6) 0 (0) na — completed by a blinded registered Moderate to severe white matter abnormality, 19 (18) 0 (0) na — psychologist. Although GA correction n (%) through age 12 is not routinely Postnatal dexamethasone, n (%) 6 (6) 0 (0) na — Early intervention and learning support performed clinically, this approach Early intervention services by 4 y, n (%) 63 (61) 1 (1) — ,.001b was adopted to reduce the likelihood Remedial support at 6 y, n (%) 44 (43) 22 (20) — ,.001b of overestimation of cognitive Family and social characteristics impartment among children born Maternal age, mean (SD) 31 (5) 31 (4) 20.4 .69 VPT.36 At ages 2, 4, and 6, families Ethnic minority, n (%) 14 (14) 13 (12) 0.1 .74 Single-parent family, n (%) 20 (19) 13 (12) 2.2 .14 were surveyed about their children’s Mother did not complete high school, n (%) 42 (41) 20 (18) 12.6 ,.001b participation in early intervention Low family SES, n (%) 31 (30) 11 (10) 13.1 ,.001b and/or special education services. Family-social risk index Teachers were also questioned None, n (%) 38 (37) 66 (61) 11.9 ,.001b regarding remedial support at age 6, Low, 1 family-social risk factor, n (%) 31 (30) 28 (26) 0.5 .47 High, 2 or more family-social risk factors, n (%) 34 (33) 14 (13) 12.3 ,.001b and clinical agencies were contacted to confirm the nature and duration of IVH, intraventricular hemorrhage; na, not applicable; ROP, retinopathy of prematurity; —, not available. a Continuous distributions (normally distributed) compared by group status by using a t test and binary variables services children received. compared by using a x2 test. b Comparison statistically significant at a , .05 based on a 2-sided test. Measures At corrected age 2 years, cognitive encompass the indices of the full years), maternal education (did not function was estimated by using the form: Verbal Comprehension, graduate high school), single-parent Bayley Scales of Infant Development, Perceptual Reasoning, Working family, and family socioeconomic Second Edition.18 At 4 and 6 years’ Memory, and Processing Speed. Three status (SES) (semiskilled, unskilled, corrected age, the short form of the preterm children were assigned or unemployed). Each variable was Wechsler Preschool and Primary a score of 40, and 1 preterm child had coded as either present or absent and Scale of Intelligence, Revised19 was an IQ estimated from 2 subtests then summed to form a composite administered, consisting of 2 verbal (Vocabulary and Matrix Reasoning) at family-social risk index. Because few (Comprehension and Arithmetic) and 9 years because of inability to children were exposed to .2 risk 2 performance (Picture Completion complete the assessment. At 12 years, factors, the index was operationalized and Block Design) subtests that 2 preterm children were assigned an to reflect 0, 1, or $2 family-social risk correlate highly with full-scale IQ (r = IQ of 40 (severe disability). factors. 0.89–0.92).37 One preterm child at age 4 and 2 children at age 6 were Across all assessments, the average Statistical Analysis assigned the minimum IQ score of 1-SD cutoff point ranged from 91.5 We calculated means, SDs, and 40 because of severe disability. (age 2) to 95.3 (age 6), with the percentages of relevant demographic One term child was excluded at age 4 average 2-SD cutoff score ranging and clinical characteristics and (incomplete assessment), and one from 70.6 (age 2) to 83.7 (age 6). compared them by term status using preterm child was excluded at age 6 the t test and x2 test, respectively. (administration error). Family-Social Risk The magnitude of between-group At corrected ages 9 and 12 years, We collected 5 measures of family- differences in cognitive performance a short form of the Wechsler social risk during the first 2 years of by age was assessed by using Cohen’s Intelligence Scale for Children, Fourth life: maternal minority ethnicity d or x2 and/or Fisher’s exact tests. Edition was administered,20 (non–New Zealand European), For any (IQ .1 SD) cognitive consisting of 5 subtests that maternal age at child birth (,21 impairment measures, the odds ratios Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news by guest on September 23, 2021 PEDIATRICS Volume 145, number 4, April 2020 3
FIGURE 1 Comparison of the distributions of cognitive performance by group status across ages of assessment: 2, 4, 6, 9, and 12 years. The smoothed distribution of scores among children born VPT and term are represented on kernel density estimate curves. The histograms represent the distribution of IQ scores by group status. The kernel density estimate curves represent nonparametric estimates of the probability functions for IQ by VPT status. For the kernel density estimate curves, the parameter that determines the degree of smoothness in the estimated density function (ie, the bandwidth) is based on an approximation of the mean integrated square error calculated as the sum of the integrated squared bias and the variance. IQ_4, IQ at age 4; IQ_6, IQ at age 6; IQ_9, IQ at age 9; IQ_12, IQ at age 12. Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news by guest on September 23, 2021 4 ERDEI et al
TABLE 2 Rates of Severe (IQ .2 SDs) and Any (Mild or Severe: IQ .1 SD) Cognitive Impairment at Each Time Point by Group Status Age of Cognitive Impairment VPT Term Effect Size, Comparisons by Group Assessment Threshold (N = 103), (N = 109), d Test Pb Unadjusted 95% Adjusted 95% CI % % Statistica ORc CI ORc 2 20.5 Severe 13 5 — .04b 2.7 (1.0–7.2) 2.3 (0.8–6.5) Any 40 15 15.3 ,.001b 2.7 (1.6–4.5) 2.5 (1.4––4.2) 4 20.7 Severe 10 2 — .02b 5.1 (1.1–22.7) 3.4 (0.7–15.7) Any 35 12 14.7 ,.001b 2.8 (1.6–5.0) 2.4 (1.3–4.4) 6 20.8 Severe 16 4 — .004b 4.2 (1.4–12.0) 3.4 (1.1–10.2) Any 49 16 25.7 ,.001b 3.1 (1.9–4.9) 2.8 (1.7–4.7) 9 20.6 Severe 13 3 — .008b 4.5 (1.3–15.5) 3.2 (0.9–11.4) Any 34 12 14.5 ,.001b 2.8 (1.6–5.0) 2.4 (1.3–4.3) 12 20.6 Severe 11 3 — .03b 3.9 (1.1–13.5) 2.8 (0.8–10.2) Any 38 12 19.2 ,.001b 3.2 (1.8–5.6) 2.8 (1.5–5.1) —, not available. a Proportions by group status were compared by using the x2 test and Fisher’s exact test (when cell sizes were small [,5]). b a , .05 based on a 2-sided test. c Odds of any (.1 SD) cognitive impairment among children (VPT versus term); adjusted analyses for family-social risk status (0, 1, or 2). (ORs) and 95% confidence intervals to less educated women of lower SES. By age 12, 14 children (VPT, n = 11 (CIs) were assessed by using logistic Reflecting these increased levels of [11%]; term, n = 3 [3%]; P = .03) met regression models, both unadjusted social disadvantage, children born criteria for severe impairment, and 52 and adjusted for family-social risk. The VPT were 2.6 times more likely to be (VPT, n = 39 [38%]; term, n = 13 [12%]; classification accuracy of identifying raised in families with $2 family- P , .001) met criteria for any cognitive cognitive impairment at age 12 by social risk factors (33% VPT; 13% impairment. using severe (IQ .2 SDs) and any (IQ term). With regard to support .1 SD) criteria at ages 2, 4, 6, and services, 63 children (61%) born VPT Given the higher rates of family 9 years was evaluated on the basis of received intervention by 4 years: disadvantage among the VPT group sensitivity, specificity, positive 48 received 1 service, 12 received 2 (ORs; Table 2), we examined to what predictive value (PPV), negative services, and 3 received 3 services. extent between-group differences may predictive value (NPV), and receiver In addition, more children born VPT partly reflect the effects of social risk. operating curve (ROC). Effect received remedial support at 6 years Accounting for family-social risk in modification by family-social risk was (43% VPT; 20% term). regression models (adjusted ORs; examined by comparing the Table 2) attenuated effect estimates by classification accuracy of the best roughly 10%. Nonetheless, the odds of Extent of Cognitive Impairment cognitive impairment predictor by cognitive impairment remained higher Examination of scores at each age for children born VPT even after family-social risk strata. All tests of indicated that children born VPT adjustment for family-social risk. statistical significance were 2 tailed performed significantly worse than with a , .05. Analyses were those born term across all cognitive conducted by using SAS 9.4 (SAS Predictive Utility of Earlier measures (Fig 1). Correspondingly, Assessments in Identifying 12-Year Institute, Inc, Cary, NC). significantly more children born VPT Cognitive Risk were subject to severe (10%–16%) or Figure 2 examines the predictive any (34%–49%) cognitive RESULTS accuracy of cognitive impairment impairment compared with children classification at 2, 4, 6, and 9 years in Sample Characteristics born term. identifying VPT children at risk for The clinical and family characteristics Table 2 shows the extent of severe and cognitive impairment at 12 years. of the 2 study groups are shown in any cognitive impairment among all Results show that adopting a severe Table 1. Notably, infants in the VPT children, with children in the VPT group impairment predictor criterion had group were more likely to be of obtaining significantly lower cognitive poorer sensitivity in identifying cases of multiple gestation and small for GA; and/or IQ scores than children born severe cognitive impairment at age 12 they were also more likely to be born term across all assessments (P , .05). than any impairment criterion. Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news by guest on September 23, 2021 PEDIATRICS Volume 145, number 4, April 2020 5
family-social circumstances (21 family-social risk factors) improved the diagnostic accuracy of any cognitive impairment at age 6 predicting any later cognitive impairment during middle childhood. Diagnostic accuracy was better among children in the high versus low family-social risk group with superior PPV (79% vs 58%), sensitivity (88% vs 82%), and specificity (75% vs 72%). DISCUSSION This study is the first to evaluate the predictive accuracy of early standardized assessments in identifying VPT children at risk for cognitive delay through middle school. Study strengths included the recruitment of representative cohorts of children, high sample retention, and availability of cognitive FIGURE 2 assessments throughout early and Classification accuracy of cognitive impairment at age 12 (severe: IQ .2 SD; any: IQ .1 SD) among middle childhood using well- VPT children using measures of cognitive impairment at 2, 4, 6, and 9 years. Prediction of severe validated measures. Study findings cognitive impairment at 12 years using earlier (A) severe and (B) any impairment. Prediction of any and implications are discussed below. cognitive impairment at 12 years using earlier (C) severe and (D) any impairment criteria. Similar to others,4,25–27 we found that children born VPT had higher rates of Specifically, the severe cognitive predictors was moderate (56% [age cognitive impairment relative to their impairment predictor criterion at ages 2, 2] to 66% [age 6]), whereas the term-born peers, with odds of any 4, and 6 missed between 18% and 44% specificity (73% [ages 2 and 6] to impairment ranging from 3.7 (95% CI of severe cases (Fig 2A), whereas the 81% [age 9]) and NPV (76% [age 4] 1.9–7.4) at age 2 to 5.0 (95% CI more inclusive any impairment criterion to 89% [age 6]) were high. Results 2.6–9.6) at age 6. Notably, most successfully predicted all severe suggest that any delay at age 6 was children born VPT experience mild impairment cases at 12 years (Fig 2B). particularly sensitive in detecting risk (27.2%) rather than severe (10.7%) of any cognitive impairment at age cognitive impairment at 12 years. Further examination of diagnostic 12 years among children born VPT. This finding has potential clinical accuracy showed that the diagnostic Risk prediction was further assessed implications because children with precision of early severe impairment by plotting the age-specific ROCs for mild impairment often do not qualify in identifying any cognitive the prediction of any cognitive for school assistance programs. impairments at age 12 was modest (Fig 2C). In contrast, employing impairment at age 12 using any In keeping with other longitudinal a criterion of early Mental impairment criterion at earlier ages studies,21,23,25 we found that early Developmental Index (MDI) and/or (Fig 3). Results from this analysis severe cognitive impairment was IQ .1 SD to identify any cognitive confirmed the above observation that a relatively good indicator of impairment cases at age 12 years had any cognitive impairment at age 6 continued problems into middle generally good diagnostic properties offers the best prediction of any school. However, a high proportion (Fig 2D). That is, 100% of the severe cognitive impairment at 12 years. (38%) of children born VPT were cases and 43% (age 4) to 79% (age 6) subject to either mild or severe of the any impairment cases at Effect Modification by Family-Social cognitive impairment at 12 years. 12 years were identified by using the Risk Importantly, 44% of these children earlier any impairment predictors. Figure 4 shows the extent to which would have been missed at their 2- PPV for the any impairment additional consideration of a child’s year assessment if only those with Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news by guest on September 23, 2021 6 ERDEI et al
general developmental level rather than accurately predict higher-order cognitive functions. We recognize that rates of any cognitive impairment were particularly elevated at age 6 in both groups. This finding may represent a measurement artifact. Alternatively, it could reflect the increased cognitive expectations experienced by children at age 6 relative to their earlier preschool years. All study children started elementary school around their fifth birthday, with this transition and its accompanying increased cognitive demands and expectations potentially resulting in milder deficits becoming more apparent. Furthermore, the high rates of cognitive difficulties in the VPT cohort at 6 years might also reflect the additive effect of family-social risk. Findings suggest that monitoring cognitive functioning of children born VPT until age 6 might be beneficial to create a safety net for this high-risk population during the challenging transition to school. Risk-stratification findings suggest that children born VPT who experienced FIGURE 3 ROCs: diagnostic accuracy in predicting any cognitive impairment (IQ .1 SD) at age 12 by using any $2 family-social risk factors were at impairment at ages 2, 4, 6, and 9 years. Any cognitive impairment cutoffs are marked with black additional risk of persistent cognitive circles. Among earlier assessments, the model with any IQ delay at age 6 has optimal discriminatory impairment above and beyond the risk ability. AUC, area under the curve. conferred by earlier delay, with prediction of impairment at 12 years severe impairment were deemed of cognitive measures improved with being superior in the high– versus eligible for ongoing monitoring. age. This was further confirmed by low–social-risk subgroup. This Examination of the diagnostic ROC analyses, which indicated that IQ reaffirms the additive effects of accuracy of early childhood delay at 12 years was optimally prematurity and social disadvantage on assessments further suggested that predicted by any IQ delay at age 6. cognitive function and emphasizes the adopting a more inclusive (MDI Notably, IQ performance at age 9 was importance of not only considering and/or IQ .1 SD) criterion offered also a good predictor of any cognitive children’s early developmental improved prediction for both severe delay at age 12 given its more functioning but also the extent of social and any cognitive impairment at proximal assessment (Fig 3). adversity when determining a child’s 12 years in children born VPT. Given However, such a late assessment is eligibility for developmental the long-term ramifications that even likely of limited use if the goal is early monitoring and/or intervention. milder cognitive impairments may detection and proactive intervention for the child and family. Our finding Limitations have on functional outcomes, adopting a more inclusive approach that 2- and 4-year assessments were Our data indicate that having any when identifying at-risk children may not as good predictors of school-aged cognitive impairment at age 6 be warranted. functioning as the 6- and 9-year appears to be the best predictor of assessments is consistent with other cognitive impairment at age 12 from Consistent with an earlier study of reports22,26,27 and may reflect the the models evaluated. Given the children born extremely preterm,23 limitations of early evaluations that relatively high false-positive rate we found that the predictive accuracy are designed to gauge a child’s (34%), this model has its limitations; Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news by guest on September 23, 2021 PEDIATRICS Volume 145, number 4, April 2020 7
neurodevelopmental outcomes is warranted. Implications Our findings highlight the potential benefit of monitoring children at high risk with early delay until elementary school. We acknowledge that this would result in a higher number of referrals and potentially increased short-term costs. Developmental follow-up is costly,38 yet early developmental services are valuable and positively impact preterm children’s cognitive39 and preacademic skills.40 Future work should examine which specific strategies and interventions have the greatest potential to positively impact cognitive outcomes in children born VPT. CONCLUSIONS Cognitive impairment in middle school is poorly predicted by early severe delay. Monitoring children born VPT until age 6, intervening for FIGURE 4 children with early mild cognitive ROCs: effect modification by family-social risk of the diagnostic accuracy of any cognitive impairment delay, and assisting families with at age 6 predicting any cognitive impairment at 12 years. Any cognitive impairment cutoffs are social disadvantages are factors that marked with black circles. Any IQ delay at 6 years in high–social-risk strata has better discrimi- natory ability. AUC, area under the curve. warrant consideration in supporting preterm children in achieving their however, this can be improved by early family-social risk was best potential long-term. using risk stratification. a relatively simple composite of Furthermore, it is likely that in factors extracted from clinical data. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS addition to family-social risk, other However, we acknowledge that other We give special thanks to the study factors associated with the child- factors, including medical risk, families for their time and support of rearing environment may contribute parental mental health, family this project. to children’s cognitive functioning stability, and parenting, are likely to and risk of delay. Intervention play a role in shaping cognitive support services may have also outcomes. Future research is ABBREVIATIONS impacted later cognitive functioning; important to better understand the CI: confidence interval yet, taking this into account is developmental pathways that modify GA: gestational age challenging given that the children cognitive risk for children born VPT MDI: Mental Developmental Index who received support are those and assess whether the inclusion of NPV: negative predictive value identified with early impairment. additional factors in risk-prediction OR: odds ratio The current study focuses on models improves the diagnostic PPV: positive predictive value cognitive risk prediction from early accuracy of early assessments. In ROC: receiver operating curve childhood measures often employed addition, examination of the SES: socioeconomic status by developmental monitoring predictive accuracy of these VPT: very preterm programs. Our additional measure of approaches for other Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news by guest on September 23, 2021 8 ERDEI et al
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2019-1982 Accepted for publication Jan 15, 2020 Address correspondence to Carmina Erdei, MD, Department of Pediatric Newborn Medicine, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, 75 Francis St, Boston, MA 02115. E-mail: cerdei@bwh.harvard.edu PEDIATRICS (ISSN Numbers: Print, 0031-4005; Online, 1098-4275). Copyright © 2020 by the American Academy of Pediatrics FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose. FUNDING: Funded by the Neurological Foundation, Lottery Grants Board, Canterbury Medical Research Foundation, and Health Research Council of New Zealand. POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: The authors have indicated they have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose. REFERENCES 1. Woodward LJ, Moor S, Hood KM, 8. Litt JS, Gerry Taylor H, Margevicius S, 15. Bick J, Nelson CA. Early adverse et al. Very preterm children show Schluchter M, Andreias L, Hack M. experiences and the developing brain. impairments across multiple Academic achievement of adolescents Neuropsychopharmacology. 2016;41(1): neurodevelopmental domains by age born with extremely low birth weight. 177–196 4 years. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Acta Paediatr. 2012;101(12):1240–1245 16. Hintz SR, Newman JE, Vohr BR. Ed. 2009;94(5):F339–F344 9. Pritchard VE, Bora S, Austin NC, Levin Changing definitions of long-term 2. Joseph RM, O’Shea TM, Allred EN, KJ, Woodward LJ. Identifying very follow-up: should “long term” be even et al; ELGAN Study Investigators. preterm children at educational risk longer? Semin Perinatol. 2016;40(6): Neurocognitive and academic outcomes using a school readiness framework. 398–409 at age 10 years of extremely preterm Pediatrics. 2014;134(3). Available at: 17. Doyle LW, Anderson PJ, Battin M, et al. newborns. Pediatrics. 2016;137(4): www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/ Long term follow up of high risk e20154343 134/3/e825 children: who, why and how? BMC 3. Linsell L, Johnson S, Wolke D, et al. 10. Strenze T. Intelligence and Pediatr. 2014;14:279 Cognitive trajectories from infancy to socioeconomic success: a meta-analytic 18. Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infant early adulthood following birth before review of longitudinal research. Development, 2nd ed. San Antonio, TX: 26 weeks of gestation: a prospective, Intelligence. 2007;35(5):401–426 Psychological Corporation; 1993 population-based cohort study. Arch Dis Child. 2018;103(4):363–370 11. Koenen KC, Moffitt TE, Roberts AL, et al. 19. Wechsler D; WPPSI-R WD. Wechsler Childhood IQ and adult mental Preschool and Primary Scale of 4. Eryigit Madzwamuse S, Baumann N, disorders: a test of the cognitive Intelligence-Revised. San Antonio, TX: Jaekel J, Bartmann P, Wolke D. Neuro- reserve hypothesis. Am J Psychiatry. Psychological Corporation Harcourt cognitive performance of very preterm 2009;166(1):50–57 Brace Jovanovich; 1989 or very low birth weight adults at 26 years. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 12. Darlow BA, Horwood LJ, Pere-Bracken 20. Wechsler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale 2015;56(8):857–864 HM, Woodward LJ. Psychosocial for Children- Fourth Edition (WISC-IV). outcomes of young adults born very San Antonio, TX: Psychological 5. Anderson PJ, Doyle LW. Cognitive and low birth weight. Pediatrics. 2013; Corporation; 2003 educational deficits in children born extremely preterm. Semin Perinatol. 132(6). Available at: www.pediatrics. 21. Bode MM, D’Eugenio DB, Mettelman BB, 2008;32(1):51–58 org/cgi/content/full/132/6/e1521 Gross SJ. Predictive Validity of the 13. Dobson KG, Ferro MA, Boyle MH, Bayley, Third Edition at 2 Years for 6. Woodward LJ, Clark CA, Bora S, Schmidt LA, Saigal S, Van Lieshout RJ. Intelligence Quotient at 4 Years in Inder TE. Neonatal white matter How do childhood intelligence and early Preterm Infants. In: J Dev Behav Pediatr, abnormalities an important predictor psychosocial adversity influence vol. 35. 2014:570–575 of neurocognitive outcome for very preterm children. PLoS One. 2012;7(12): income attainment among adult 22. O’Shea TM, Joseph RM, Allred EN, et al; e51879 extremely low birth weight survivors? A ELGAN Study Investigators. Accuracy of test of the cognitive reserve hypothesis. the Bayley-II mental development index 7. Cheong JLY, Anderson PJ, Burnett AC, Dev Psychopathol. 2018;30(4):1421–1434 at 2 years as a predictor of cognitive et al; Victorian Infant Collaborative 14. Fox SE, Levitt P, Nelson CA III. How the impairment at school age among Study Group. Changing neurodevelopment at 8 years in timing and quality of early experiences children born extremely preterm. influence the development of brain J Perinatol. 2018;38(7):908–916 children born extremely preterm since the 1990s. Pediatrics. 2017;139(6): architecture. Child Dev. 2010;81(1): 23. Doyle LW, Casalaz D; Victorian Infant e20164086 28–40 Collaborative Study Group. Outcome at Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news by guest on September 23, 2021 PEDIATRICS Volume 145, number 4, April 2020 9
14 years of extremely low birthweight 29. Victorian Infant Collaborative Study 35. Mangin KS, Horwood LJ, Woodward LJ. infants: a regional study. Arch Dis Child Group. Eight-year outcome in infants Cognitive development trajectories of Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2001;85(3):F159–F164 with birth weight of 500 to 999 grams: very preterm and typically developing 24. Hack M, Taylor HG, Drotar D, et al. Poor continuing regional study of 1979 and children. Child Dev. 2017;88(1):282–298 predictive validity of the Bayley Scales 1980 births. J Pediatr. 1991;118(5): 36. Wilson-Ching M, Pascoe L, Doyle LW, of Infant Development for cognitive 761–767 Anderson PJ. Effects of correcting for function of extremely low birth weight 30. Breeman LD, Jaekel J, Baumann N, prematurity on cognitive test scores in children at school age. Pediatrics. 2005; Bartmann P, Wolke D. Preterm cognitive childhood. J Paediatr Child Health. 116(2):333–341 function into adulthood. Pediatrics. 2014;50(3):182–188 25. Munck P, Niemi P, Lapinleimu H, 2015;136(3):415–423 37. LoBello SG. A short form of the Lehtonen L, Haataja L; PIPARI Study 31. Claas MJ, de Vries LS, Bruinse HW, et al. Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale Group. Stability of cognitive outcome Neurodevelopmental outcome over of Intelligence-Revised. J Sch Psychol. from 2 to 5 years of age in very low time of preterm born children #750 g 1991;29(3):229–236 birth weight children. Pediatrics. 2012; at birth. Early Hum Dev. 2011;87(3): 129(3):503–508 38. Behrman R, Stith Butler A; Institute of 183–191 Medicine Committee on Understanding 26. Potharst ES, Houtzager BA, van 32. Wong HS, Edwards P. Nature or nurture: Premature Birth and Assuring Healthy Sonderen L, et al. Prediction of a systematic review of the effect of Outcomes Board on Health Sciences cognitive abilities at the age of 5 years socio-economic status on the Outcomes. Preterm Birth: Causes, using developmental follow-up developmental and cognitive outcomes Consequences, and Prevention. assessments at the age of 2 and 3 years in very preterm children. Dev of children born preterm. Matern Child Washington, DC: National Academies Med Child Neurol. 2012;54(3):240–246 Health J. 2013;17(9):1689–1700 Press; 2007 27. Roberts G, Anderson PJ, Doyle LW; 33. Linsell L, Malouf R, Morris J, Kurinczuk 39. Spittle A, Orton J, Anderson PJ, Boyd R, Victorian Infant Collaborative Study JJ, Marlow N. Prognostic factors for Doyle LW. Early developmental Group. The stability of the diagnosis of poor cognitive development in intervention programmes provided developmental disability between ages children born very preterm or with post hospital discharge to prevent 2 and 8 in a geographic cohort of very very low birth weight: a systematic motor and cognitive impairment in preterm children born in 1997. Arch Dis review. JAMA Pediatr. 2015;169(12): preterm infants. Cochrane Database Child. 2010;95(10):786–790 1162–1172 Syst Rev. 2015;(11):CD005495 28. Spencer-Smith MM, Spittle AJ, Lee KJ, 34. Manley BJ, Roberts RS, Doyle LW, et al. 40. Litt JS, Glymour MM, Hauser-Cram P, Doyle LW, Anderson PJ. Bayley-III Social variables predict gains in Hehir T, McCormick MC. Early cognitive and language scales in cognitive scores across the preschool intervention services improve school- preterm children. Pediatrics. 2015; years in children with birth weights 500 age functional outcome among 135(5). Available at: www.pediatrics. to 1250 grams. J Pediatr. 2015;166(4): neonatal intensive care unit graduates. org/cgi/content/full/135/5/e1258 870–876–2 Acad Pediatr. 2018;18(4):468–474 Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news by guest on September 23, 2021 10 ERDEI et al
Predicting School-Aged Cognitive Impairment in Children Born Very Preterm Carmina Erdei, Nicola C. Austin, Sara Cherkerzian, Alyssa R. Morris and Lianne J. Woodward Pediatrics originally published online March 6, 2020; Updated Information & including high resolution figures, can be found at: Services http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/early/2020/03/04/peds.2 019-1982 References This article cites 32 articles, 8 of which you can access for free at: http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/early/2020/03/04/peds.2 019-1982#BIBL Subspecialty Collections This article, along with others on similar topics, appears in the following collection(s): Developmental/Behavioral Pediatrics http://www.aappublications.org/cgi/collection/development:behavior al_issues_sub Cognition/Language/Learning Disorders http://www.aappublications.org/cgi/collection/cognition:language:lea rning_disorders_sub Fetus/Newborn Infant http://www.aappublications.org/cgi/collection/fetus:newborn_infant_ sub Neonatology http://www.aappublications.org/cgi/collection/neonatology_sub Permissions & Licensing Information about reproducing this article in parts (figures, tables) or in its entirety can be found online at: http://www.aappublications.org/site/misc/Permissions.xhtml Reprints Information about ordering reprints can be found online: http://www.aappublications.org/site/misc/reprints.xhtml Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news by guest on September 23, 2021
Predicting School-Aged Cognitive Impairment in Children Born Very Preterm Carmina Erdei, Nicola C. Austin, Sara Cherkerzian, Alyssa R. Morris and Lianne J. Woodward Pediatrics originally published online March 6, 2020; The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, is located on the World Wide Web at: http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/early/2020/03/04/peds.2019-1982 Pediatrics is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly publication, it has been published continuously since 1948. Pediatrics is owned, published, and trademarked by the American Academy of Pediatrics, 345 Park Avenue, Itasca, Illinois, 60143. Copyright © 2020 by the American Academy of Pediatrics. All rights reserved. Print ISSN: 1073-0397. Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news by guest on September 23, 2021
You can also read