Longitudinal Associations Between Screen Use and Reading in Preschool-Aged Children
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
Longitudinal Associations Between Screen Use and Reading in Preschool-Aged Children Brae Anne McArthur, PhD,a,b Dillon Browne, PhD,c Sheila McDonald, PhD,a,d Suzanne Tough, PhD,a,b,* Sheri Madigan, PhDa,b,* BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVES: The home literacy environment has been identified as a key predictor abstract of children’s language, school readiness, academic achievement, and behavioral outcomes. With the increased accessibility and consumption of digital media, it is important to understand whether screen use impacts off-line enrichment activities such as reading or whether reading activities offset screen use. Using a prospective birth cohort, we examined reading and screen use at 24, 36, and 60 months to elucidate the directional association between screen use and reading over time. METHODS: Thisstudy included data from 2440 mothers and children in Calgary, Alberta, drawn from the All Our Families cohort. Children’s screen use and reading activities were assessed via maternal report at age 24, 36, and 60 months. Sociodemographic covariates were also collected. RESULTS: Using a random-intercepts cross-lagged panel model, which statistically controls for individual-level confounds, this study revealed that greater screen use at 24 months was associated with lower reading at 36 months (b = 2.08; 95% confidence interval: 20.13 to 20.02). In turn, lower reading at 36 months was associated with greater screen use at 60 months (b = 2.11; 95% confidence interval: 20.19 to 20.02). Covariates did not modify the associations. CONCLUSIONS: A reciprocal relationship between screen use and reading was identified. Early screen use was associated with lower reading activities, resulting in greater screen use at later ages. Findings emphasize the need for practitioners and educators to discuss screen use guidelines and encourage families to engage in device-free activities to foster early literacy exposure. a University of Calgary, Calgary Alberta, Canada; bAlberta Children’s Hospital Research Institute, Calgary, Alberta, WHAT’S KNOWN ON THIS SUBJECT: Book reading is a critical Canada; cUniversity of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada; and dAlberta Health Services, Calgary, element of the home environment that promotes school Alberta, Canada readiness and academic achievement. With increasing use of media devices, longitudinal research is needed to determine *Contributed equally as joint senior authors if screen use is interfering with off-line activities such as reading. Drs McArthur and Madigan conceptualized and designed the study, conducted data analyses, drafted the manuscript, and reviewed and revised the manuscript; Dr Browne assisted with data WHAT THIS STUDY ADDS: Findings support a dynamic analysis and reviewed the manuscript for important intellectual content; Drs McDonald and Tough relationship whereby screen use at 24 months leads to lower conceptualized the cohort study, designed the data collection instruments and study methodology, reading at 36 months, which in turn leads to greater screen secured funding for data collection, and reviewed the manuscript for important intellectual content; use at 60 months. Families should be encouraged to engage and all authors approved the final manuscript as submitted and agree to be accountable for all in device-free time. aspects of the work. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2020-011429 To cite: McArthur BA, Browne D, McDonald S, et al. Longitudinal Associations Between Screen Use and Accepted for publication Feb 26, 2021 Reading in Preschool-Aged Children. Pediatrics. 2021; 147(6):e2020011429 Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news by guest on October 13, 2021 PEDIATRICS Volume 147, number 6, June 2021:e2020011429 ARTICLE
Children enter school with varying considered to be the most robust movies, videos, or stories on literacy skills, and these differences method for addressing directionality a videocassette recorder or digital tend to get larger over time without in observational studies by video disk player; and using intervention.1,2 The home statistically controlling for individual- a computer, gaming system, or other environment, including parent-child level confounds, such as stable screen-based device) on a typical shared print book reading and family-level stressors.13 The weekday and typical weekend day. A language exposure, has been shown secondary aim of this study was to weighted average across week and to have a large impact on children’s explore the extent to which the weekend days and electronic devices later academic achievment.3 In longitudinal associations between was calculated to yield screen use in addition, shared book reading screen use and reading varied on the hours per week. At each time point, promotes important parent-child basis of sociodemographic covariates. outliers .4 SDs from the mean were engagement during sensitive periods Implications of these findings could winsorized16 (n = 8 at 24 months, n = of development.4 As a result, there inform pediatricians, health care 16 at 36 months, and n = 7 at 60 have been long-standing efforts to practitioners, child care providers, months). identify factors that may influence the educators and policymakers seeking home literacy environment.5–7 to guide parents on appropriate recommendations for screen Reading Activities With the increased use and exposure and off-line activities such When children were aged 24, 36, and accessibility of media devices,8 screen as reading during the sensitive period 60 months, mothers reported the use is becoming a consistent part of of early childhood. range of time their child spent in children’s day-to-day lives. According reading activities using a 4-point to the displacement hypothesis,9 response scale. At 24 months, when children are watching screens, METHODS mothers were asked, “Do you or they are less likely to spend time Study Design and Population another adult of the household read practicing skills important for to your child or show him/her picture learning and development.10 As such, Participants were from All Our books?” with response options screen use may be influencing the Families, a pregnancy cohort of 3388 ranging from (1) never to (4) daily. At home learning environment, mothers and children from Calgary, 36 months, mothers were asked, specifically engagement in off-line Canada.14,15 Women were recruited “How many minutes each day do you enrichment activities such as reading between August 2008 and December spend sharing books with your child? print books,11 and displacement may 2010 through primary health care ” with response options ranging from be one mechanism to explain the offices, community advertising, and (1) 0 to 10 minutes to (4) relation between screen time and laboratories. Inclusion criteria were $30 minutes. At 60 months, mothers delays in developmental skill (1) age $18 years, (2) fluent in were asked, “How many hours per acquisition. Although it is possible English, (3) gestational age ,25 day does your child spend doing the that screen use interrupts enriching weeks, and (4) receiving community- following activities outside of child off-line activities such as print book based prenatal care. Mothers were care, preschool, or school: Read or reading,9,12 it is also possible that followed-up at ,25 weeks’ gestation look at books?” on a typical weekday early reading activities may offset and at 4, 12, 24, 36, and 60 months’ and weekend day. Response options later screen use. However, to test postpartum. The 24-, 36-, and 60- ranged from (1) none or 0 minutes to this hypothesis, longitudinal data month time points were the focus of (4) $3 hours. At 60 months, with repeated measurement are this analysis because screen use and a weighted average across week and needed to examine directional reading variables were both collected. weekend days was calculated to yield associations between screen use A detailed description of the study reading in hours per day, with a range and reading. sample can be found in Table 1. All from (1) none or 0 minutes to (4) $3 procedures were approved by the The primary aim of this study was to hours. The reading items were institutional ethics board. explicitly test what comes first: designed to reflect the natural higher screen use or lower reading progression of reading activities Measures activities? In a sample of 2440 across early childhood. Results from families, using a 3-wave (24, 36, and Screen Use this study suggest consistency in this 60 months) random intercept cross- When children were aged 24, 36, and measurement method over time lagged panel model (RI-CLPM),13 we 60 months, mothers reported the (24–36 months [b = .23; 95% predict that higher screen use will range of time their child spent using confidence interval (CI): .18 to .29]; relate to lower reading activities at electronic devices (ie, watching 36–60 months [b = .24; 95% CI: .18 later time points. The RI-CLPM is television programs; watching to .29]). Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news by guest on October 13, 2021 2 MCARTHUR et al
TABLE 1 Sample Demographics and Study Characteristics Covariates Characteristic Value Child sex (1 [female]; 0 [male]), Maternal education, n (%) household income (reported in Less than high school 40 (1.6) increments of $10 000 Canadian Graduated high school 104 (4.3) dollars [CAD]: 1 [#$29 999]; 11 Some college or trade school or university 233 (9.6) Graduated college or trade school or university 1265 (51.9) [$$150 000]), and maternal Some graduate school 42 (1.7) education (1 [less than a high school Completed graduate school 307 (12.6) education]; 6 [completed graduate Missing 449 (18.3) school]) were maternal self-report. At Household income, CAD $, n (%) 24 months, maternal screen use and #29 999 35 (1.4) 30 000–39 999 43 (1.8) maternal reading were measured 40 000–49 999 50 (2.1) with single self-report items asking 50 000–59 999 98 (4.0) the amount of time mothers spend 60 000–69 999 76 (3.1) watching television or reading, 70 000–79 999 122 (5.0) respectively, on a typical weekday (1 80 000–89 999 140 (5.7) 90 000–99 999 147 (6.0) [none]; 6 [$7 hours per day]). 100 000–124 999 358 (14.7) Attending the library (eg, story time, 125 000–149 999 258 (10.6) borrowing books or videos, etc) in the $150 000 644 (26.4) past year (yes [1]; no [0]) was also Missing 469 (19.1) measured with a single self-report Maternal race and/or ethnicity, n (%) White 1993 (81.7) item. Mothers completed the Brief Black and/or African American 29 (1.2) Infant‐Toddler Social and Emotional Indigenous 12 (0.5) Assessment (BITSEA) to identify child Asian 254 (10.4) behavior problems (eg, aggression, Latin American 37 (1.5) defiance, over‐activity, negative Multiracial or other 100 (4.1) Missing 15 (0.6) emotionality, anxiety, and Child sex, n (%) withdrawal). By using the BITSEA Female 937 (38.4) standardized scoring cutoffs, children Male 1018 (41.7) were categorized with possible Missing 485 (19.9) behavioral problems if they scored in Nonparental child care or day care before 60 mo, n (%) Yes 1433 (58.8) the $75th percentile on the scale.17 No 533 (21.9) At 60 months, mothers responded to Missing 474 (19.4) “has your child been in nonparental Maternal screen use at 24 mo, n (%) child care or day care on a regular None 111 (4.6) basis before this year?” (0 [no]; 1 ,1 h 441 (18.1) 1–,3 h 859 (35.3) [yes]). 3–,5 h 149 (6.1) 5–,7 h 24 (1.0) Statistical Analyses $7 h 10 (0.4) The longitudinal associations Missing 846 (34.6) between hours of screen use and Maternal reading at 24 mo, n (%) None 199 (8.2) reading activities were examined by ,1 h 879 (36.1) using an RI-CLPM.13 The RI-CLPM 1–,3 h 440 (18.1) statistically distinguishes variance at 3–,5 h 57 (2.3) the temporal level (ie, within-person 5–,7 h 13 (0.5) or time-varying) from variance at the $7 h 6 (0.2) Missing 846 (34.6) individual level (ie, between-person Attended the library at 24 mo, n (%) or stable) and, therefore, constitutes Yes 961 (39.5) a multilevel approach accounting for No 635 (26.0) repeated measurements that are Missing 844 (34.5) nested within individuals. An Problem behavior (BITSEA) at 24 mo, n (%) At risk 236 (9.7) important advantage of the RI-CLPM Normative 1344 (55.1) over the common cross-lagged panel Missing 860 (35.2) model is that RI-CLPM controls for Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news by guest on October 13, 2021 PEDIATRICS Volume 147, number 6, June 2021 3
TABLE 1 Continued standardized root mean square Characteristic Value residual = 0.002). Weekly hours of screen use at 24 mo, mean (SD) 17.07 (11.82) In the time-variant component of the Weekly hours of screen use at 36 mo, mean (SD) 24.90 (12.50) model, statistically significant Weekly hours of screen use at 60 mo, mean (SD) 10.84 (5.29) Reading activities at 24 mo, mean (SD) 3.92 (0.29) autocorrelations for every estimated Reading activities at 36 mo, mean (SD) 2.61 (0.94) lag indicate substantial within-person Reading activities at 60 mo, mean (SD) 2.48 (0.52) stability in constructs over time. That is, on average, children’s screen use and reading activities were stable across adjacent time points. As stable individuals’ differences (ie, at the between-person level). detailed in Fig 1 and Table 2, after between-person and time-invariant Statistical significance was set at the accounting for this temporal stability, effects, such as stable family-level P , .05, 2-tailed level; 95% CIs are there was a significant and negative stressors) in reading activities and reported. All analyses were cross lag linking higher levels of screen use, allowing for greater conducted in Mplus version 8.1.21 screen use at 24 months of age with insight into how the two central lower levels of reading activities at constructs in the model (ie, screen Missing Data 36 months of age (b = 2.08; 95% CI: use and reading activities) are linked From the initial pregnancy cohort 2.13 to 2.02). The obverse direction at an intraindividual (ie, within- (N = 3388), 95% (n = 3223) agreed to of higher levels of reading activities at person and time-varying) level. This be contacted for follow-up research. 24 months being associated with approach has been shown to reduce Of those who agreed to follow-up and lower exposure to screens at bias in directional estimates and were eligible at the time of 36 months was not observed (b = more closely approximate causal questionnaire completion, 76% 2.05; 95% CI: 2.11 to .01). At relationships.18 completed the 24-month 36 months of age, lower levels of questionnaire (n = 1595), 69% reading activities predicted higher First, the standard RI-CLPM was completed the 36-month exposure to screen use at 60 months estimated. In the RI-CLPM, between- questionnaire (n = 1994), and 71% (b = 2.11; 95% CI: 2.19 to 2.02). person (stable) factors were completed the 60-month The obverse association was not extracted from the repeated measures questionnaire (n = 1992). Attrition observed (b = .01; 95% CI: 2.04 to of screen use and reading, and these rates observed in the current study .06). Also, within-time covariances factors were permitted to covary. The are similar to other prospective birth were significant at 24 and 36 months within-person component comprises cohorts.22–24 Predictors of dropout but not at 60 months, suggesting that, 3 types of estimates: (1) are reported elsewhere (younger on average, at the 24- and 36-month autoregressions (ie, lags) capture the mothers and lower income).10 study waves, children’s screen use within-person, rank-order stability in Consistent with other pediatric RI- was significantly related to children’s constructs over time; (2) within-time CLPMs,10 participants were included reading activities (b = 2.10 [95% CI: covariances capture the strength and (n = 2440) if they completed 2.17 to 2.04] and b = 2.08 [95% CI: direction of associations between questionnaires for at least 1 time 2.13 to 2.03], respectively). screen use and reading within persons at each time point; and (3) point at either 24, 36, or 60 months. Taken together, these findings suggest the cross lags capture the longitudinal To adjust for missing data, models that higher levels of screen use at and directional associations between were run with full-information 24 months of age, relative to a child’s screen use and reading within maximum likelihood estimation.25,26 average level of screen use (ie, the persons and are comparable to the child’s stable mean), was associated proportion of unique variance with significantly lower levels of explained in the outcome that is not RESULTS reading activities at the next study shared with any other predictor (ie, wave, relative to a child’s average a squared semipartial Primary Analyses level of reading. In addition, lower correlation19,20; Fig 1). After fitting The standard RI-CLPM (Fig 1) levels of reading activities at the standard RI-CLPM, pairwise revealed that the model was a good fit 36 months of age, relative to a child’s comparisons were conducted by to the observed data on the basis of average level of reading, was using post hoc t tests to identify the fit indices (x21 = 0.09; P = .768; root associated with significantly higher extent to which the cross-lag mean square error of approximation levels of screen use at 60 months of estimates varied between different = 0.00; 95% CI: 0.00 to 0.04; age, relative to a child’s average level levels of the covariates (measured comparative fit index = 1.00; of screen use. Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news by guest on October 13, 2021 4 MCARTHUR et al
FIGURE 1 The standard RI-CLPM revealing within-person association between screen use and reading from ages 24 to 60 months, controlling for between-person differences. Standardized estimates (b) and 95% CIs are presented. Solid lines represent estimates in which 95% CIs do not include 0. The central, blue- tinted part of the model is the within-person (dynamic) part, and the outer, gray-tinted part of the model is the between-person (stable) component. a Pathways constrained to 1.00 to extract between-person factor (n = 2440). Secondary Analyses use is a common household activity activities at 36 months is associated To determine the extent to which the for young children.8 With this change with greater screen use at 60 months. longitudinal associations between comes growing concern about the The obverse associations (ie, greater screen use and reading varied on the role of screen use on the home reading at 24 months leading to basis of covariates, the differences in learning environment, specifically lower screen time at 36 months and, the cross-lagged associations engagement in off-line enrichment in turn, greater reading at 60 months) between levels of each study activities such as print book reading. were not observed. covariate were examined (Table 3). This longitudinal, 3-wave study uses repeated measures and a rigorous A robust body of literature Cross-lagged parameters did not statistical model that more closely underscores the importance of the significantly differ on the basis of approximate causality to clarify early home learning environment to different levels of the study whether screen use interferes with encourage the development of school covariates. later print book reading or if early readiness and literacy skills.5,23 reading activities may offset later Consistent with the displacement screen use. Results suggest that hypothesis,9 this study provides DISCUSSION higher screen use at 24 months is support for the notion that screen use With expanding media options and related to lower reading activities at may be interfering with reading a dynamic digital landscape, screen 36 months, and in turn, lower reading activities. Indeed, at 24 months, it Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news by guest on October 13, 2021 PEDIATRICS Volume 147, number 6, June 2021 5
TABLE 2 Standardized and Unstandardized Autoregressive and Cross-Lagged Coefficients From the no more than 1 hour of screen time Standard RI-CLPM daily.31 Family media plans32 can be Paths b (95% CI)a B (95% CI)b devised to help families develop Autoregressive parameters healthy media habits. Early Screen time, mo discussions with family may be 24 → 36 .48 (.42 to .53)c .51 (.43 to .59)c critical because research reveals that 36 → 60 .42 (.33 to .51)c .12 (.07 to .17)c once problematic screen use habits Reading, mo are developed, they tend to persist 24 → 36 .23 (.18 to .29)c .79 (.61 to .97)c 36 → 60 .24 (.18 to .29)c .13 (.09 to .16)c over the early childhood period.33 On Cross-lagged parameters the basis of the within-person Screen time → reading, mo stability of shared reading and screen 24 → 36 2.08 (2.13 to 2.02)c 2.01 (2.01 to 2.002)c media habits starting at 24 months of 36 → 60 .01 (2.04 to .06) .00 (2.002 to .003) age, this study also emphasizes the Reading → screen time, mo 24 → 36 2.05 (2.11 to .01) 22.24 (24.80 to .32) importance of establishing early 36 → 60 2.11 (2.19 to 2.02)c 2.39 (2.67 to 2.11)c reading routines known to be B, unstandardized b coefficient; b, standardized b coefficient; →, predicting. foundational for child development a Standardized b coefficients represent the SD change in an outcome variable (eg, reading at 36 mo) associated with a 1 and learning and reaffirms the need SD change in the predictor (eg, screen time at 24 mo). b Unstandardized B coefficients represent the unit change in an outcome variable (eg, 1 level of reading at 36 mo) for early discussion of reading in associated with a unit change in the predictor (eg,1 hour of screen time at 24 mo). pediatric offices. These discussions c Estimates in which 95% CIs do not include 0. can focus on the 5 R’s34 of early learning: reading together every day; was observed that greater screen use activities that involve print books for rhyme and play; developing per week relates to a lower level of young children may be advised. consistent sleep, eating, reading and reading activities at 36 months. In play routines; reward with praise; Although past research supports that addition, through interpretation of and nurture relationships rich in many factors in the home the unstandardized coefficients, a 10- serve and return interactions. At environment influence screen use29 minute decrease in reading per day at a policy level, increased access to and reading activities,4,5 results from 36 months of age relates to a ∼25- books, programs designed to help the post hoc analysis of covariates minute increase in screen use per connect at risk-families with literary reveal that the sociodemographic week at 60 months of age. These resources (eg, reach out and read35), variables included in this study did findings highlight a reciprocal process broader dissemination of screen use not significantly modify the between screen use and reading that guidelines for children aged ,5 years, magnitude of the associations unfolds over time, in which screen and a combination of early between screen use and reading over use negatively influences reading interventions targeted at both time. This finding suggests that activities and then lowered reading reading and screen use habits are sociodemographic factors may be activities lead to greater screen use. needed. more influential at a between-person level (eg, when predicting overall With the increased use and Using a large cohort and a longitudinal screen use or reading activities for accessibility of media devices, research design, as well as a robust different children) but may be less families may turn to electronics to statistical method, this study sheds impactful at a within-person level (eg, promote reading. Although reading light on the direction of the association impacting the associations between electronic books was not examined between screen use and reading reading and screen use over time for herein, researchers have recently activities across early childhood. a specific child). found that, for preschool-aged However, the findings must be children, parents and children tend to A number of practice and policy interpreted with the following collaborate and verbalize less when implications arise from this study. limitations in mind. First, this reading electronic books in Most importantly, this study study included a predominantly comparison with reading print highlights the need for practitioners, high-income, highly educated sample books.27,28 Overall, there appears to health care workers, parents, of participants, which may limit be less reciprocity and conversational policymakers, and educators to generalizability to other populations. turns (specific elements of the early promote adherence to screen use Second, the method of measurement reading environment known to guidelines. This is especially used for screen use did not capture promote language learning and important because up to 95% of the content (eg, educational literacy skills) when using electronic preschoolers are exceeding the programing) or context (eg, solitary books,4 and thus encouraging reading current screen use guidelines30 of versus coviewing) of screen use. Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news by guest on October 13, 2021 6 MCARTHUR et al
TABLE 3 Differences in the Cross-Lagged Associations Linking Screen Use and Reading, by measurement of the home reading Covariates environment, including parent literacy Paths Difference (95% CI)a skills and objective measures of Income b Educationc parent-child shared reading experiences (eg, conversational turns, Screen time → reading, mo 24 → 36 0.00 (20.01 to 0.01) 0.01 (20.004 to 0.02) parent engagement, etc). 36 → 60 0.00 (20.01 to 0.01) 0.00 (20.004 to 0.01) Reading → screen time, mo 24 → 36 20.55 (27.39 to 6.28) 2.33 (23.32 to 7.97) CONCLUSIONS 36 → 60 20.66 (21.64 to 0.32) 20.11 (20.83 to 0.62) With the increased exposure to digital Maternal readingd Maternal screen usee media, screen use is now a regular Screen time → reading, mo part of children’s day-to-day lives. In 24 → 36 0.00 (20.01 to 0.01) 0.00 (20.01 to 0.01) 36 → 60 20.01 (20.01 to 0.00) 0.01 (20.001 to 0.01) response to this increase in exposure, Reading → screen time, mo there is a critical need to understand 24 → 36 23.93 (29.50 to 1.65) 4.44 (21.39 to 10.27) how screen use may be influencing 36 → 60 0.14 (20.55 to 0.83) 20.20 (21.46 to 1.05) the home learning environment, Problem behaviorf Child sexg specifically engagement in off-line Screen time → reading, mo 24 → 36 0.01 (20.01 to 0.02) 20.01 (20.02 to 0.004) enrichment activities such as 36 → 60 0.00 (20.01 to 0.01) 0.00 (20.001 to 0.005) reading. This study provides Reading → screen time, mo support for a reciprocal relationship 24 → 36 1.41 (25.28 to 8.10) 0.89 (24.78 to 6.53) between screen use and reading 36 → 60 20.31 (21.21 to 0.59) 20.33 (20.88 to 0.22) activities. Higher screen use at Child careh Access to libraryi Screen time → reading, mo 24 months of age related to lower 24 → 36 0.00 (20.01 to 0.01) 0.01 (20.003 to 0.02) reading activities at 36 months of 36 → 60 0.01 (20.001 to 0.01) 0.01 (20.001 to 0.01) age, and in turn, lower levels of Reading → screen time, mo reading at 36 months of age related 24 → 36 1.13 (24.58 to 6.84) 0.17 (25.00 to 5.33) to higher levels of screen use at 36 → 60 20.26 (20.94 to 0.42) 20.17 (20.87 to 0.52) the next time point. The findings →, predicting. a Difference in the cross-lagged associations by covariate group. from this study support the need b Defined as low income (CAD$ ,60 000; 1) and high income (CAD$ $60 000; 0). for practitioners, child care c Defined as lower education (some high school, graduated high school, and some postsecondary; 1) and higher professionals, and educators to education (graduated postsecondary, some graduate school, and completed graduate school; 0). d Defined as low maternal reading (below median; 1) and high maternal reading (at or above median; 0). encourage families to engage in e Defined as low maternal screen use (below median; 1) and high maternal screen use (at or above median; 0). healthy use of screen devices (ie, f Defined as at risk (at or above the cutoff score on the BITSEA problem behavior scale; 1) and normative (below the cutoff limited duration) and to encourage score on the BITSEA problem behavior scale; 0). g Defined as male (1) and female (0). device-free time to establish early h Defined as nonparental child care or day care (1) and other (0). reading habits. i Defined as attending the library (1) and not attending the library (0). ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Presumably, families vary on the informants of child activities between We acknowledge the contributions of content and context in which screens 24 and 60 months, single-informant the All Our Families research team are used, and these elements of screen measurement introduces the potential and thank the participants who took use may have a different association for bias. With regards to reading, part in the study. with language and literacy.36 Third, a single item was used to capture the although this study reveals an frequency of reading activities at each association between screen use and time point. Although the reading items reading, further research is needed to were designed to reflect the natural ABBREVIATIONS determine the specific threshold at progression of reading activities BITSEA: Brief Infant‐Toddler So- which screen use influences reading. across early childhood, single-item cial and Emotional Fourth, because of the rapid measurement at each time point Assessment progression of technology, exposure provides fewer points of CAD: Canadian dollar and accessibility to screens may have discrimination and potentially limits CI: confidence interval changed over the course of this the sensitivity, or variation, in the RI-CLPM: random intercept cross- multiwave study.8 Additionally, measure. This study would be lagged panel model although parents are arguably the best strengthened by more detailed Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news by guest on October 13, 2021 PEDIATRICS Volume 147, number 6, June 2021 7
Address correspondence to Sheri Madigan, PhD, Department of Psychology, University of Calgary, 2500 University Ave, Calgary, AB, Canada T2N 1N4. E-mail: sheri.madigan@ucalgary.ca PEDIATRICS (ISSN Numbers: Print, 0031-4005; Online, 1098-4275). Copyright © 2021 by the American Academy of Pediatrics FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose. FUNDING: Supported by Alberta Innovates Health Solutions Interdisciplinary Team grant 200700595, the Alberta Children’s Hospital Foundation, and the Max Bell Foundation. The principal investigator of the All Our Families Study is Dr Tough. Research support was provided to Dr Madigan and Dr. Browne by the Canada Research Chairs program. Dr McArthur was supported by a fellowship from the Alberta Children’s Hospital Research Institute. POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: The authors have indicated they have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose. COMPANION PAPER: A companion to this article can be found online at www.pediatrics.org/cgi/doi/10.1542/peds.2020-047472. REFERENCES 1. Stanovich KE. Matthew effects in 8. Rideout V. The Common Sense Census: Methods: 1920–2000, and Beyond. New reading: some consequences of Media Use by Kids Zero to Eight. San York, NY: Springer; 2014 individual differences in the acquisition Francisco, CA: Common Sense Media; 17. Briggs-Gowan MJ, Carter AS, Irwin JR, of literacy. Read Res Q. 1986;21(4): 2017 Wachtel K, Cicchetti DV. The Brief Infant- 360–407 9. Christakis DA. The effects of infant Toddler Social and Emotional 2. NICHD Early Child Care Research media usage: what do we know and Assessment: screening for social- Network. Early child care and children’s what should we learn? Acta Paediatr. emotional problems and delays in development prior to school entry: 2009;98(1):8–16 competence. J Pediatr Psychol. 2004; results from the NICHD study of early 29(2):143–155 child care. Am Educ Res J. 2002;39(1): 10. Madigan S, Browne D, Racine N, Mori C, Tough S. Association between screen 18. Berry D, Willoughby MT. On the practical 133–164 time and children’s performance on interpretability of cross-lagged panel 3. Barnes E, Puccioni J. Shared book a developmental screening test. JAMA models: rethinking a developmental reading and preschool children’s Pediatr. 2019;173(3):244–250 workhorse. Child Dev. 2017;88(4): academic achievement: evidence from 1186–1206 the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study- 11. McArthur BA, Eirich R, McDonald S, Tough S, Madigan S. Screen use relates 19. Schuurman NK, Ferrer E, de Boer- Birth cohort. Infant Child Dev. 2017; to decreased offline enrichment Sonnenschein M, Hamaker EL. How to 26(6):e2035 activities. Acta Paediatr. 2021;110(3): compare cross-lagged associations in 4. Duursma E, Augustyn M, Zuckerman B. a multilevel autoregressive model. 896–898 Reading aloud to children: the evidence. Psychol Methods. 2016;21(2):206–221 Arch Dis Child. 2008;93(7):554–557 12. Radesky JS, Schumacher J, Zuckerman 20. Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS. Using B. Mobile and interactive media use by 5. Tamis-LeMonda CS, Luo R, McFadden KE, Multivariate Statistics. Harlow, United young children: the good, the bad, and Bandel ET, Vallotton C. Early home Kingdom: Pearson Education; 2014 the unknown. Pediatrics. 2015;135(1): learning environment predicts 1–3 21. Muthén L, Muthén B. Mplus Statistical children’s 5th grade academic skills. Modeling Software: Release 8.0. Los Appl Dev Sci. 2019;23(2):153–169 13. Hamaker EL, Kuiper RM, Grasman RPPP. Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén; 2017 6. Johnson AD, Martin A, Brooks-Gunn J, A critique of the cross-lagged panel model. Psychol Methods. 2015;20(1): 22. National Institute of Child Health and Petrill SA. Order in the house! 102–116 Human Development Early Child Care Associations among household chaos, Research Network. Duration and the home literacy environment, 14. Tough SC, McDonald SW, Collisson BA, developmental timing of poverty and maternal reading ability, and children’s et al. Cohort profile: the all our babies children’s cognitive and social early reading. Merrill Palmer Q (Wayne pregnancy cohort (AOB). Int development from birth through third State Univ Press). 2008;54(4):445–472 J Epidemiol. 2017;46(5):1389–1390k grade. Child Dev. 2005;76(4):795–810 7. Greenwood P, Hutton J, Dudley J, 15. McDonald SW, Lyon AW, Benzies KM, 23. Browne DT, Wade M, Prime H, Jenkins Horowitz-Kraus T. Maternal reading et al. The All Our Babies Pregnancy JM. School readiness amongst urban fluency is associated with functional Cohort: Design, Methods, and Canadian families: risk profiles and connectivity between the child’s future Participant Characteristics. In: BMC family mediation. J Educ Psychol. 2018; reading network and regions related to Pregnancy Childbirth, vol. 13. 2013:S2 110(1):133–146 executive functions and language processing in preschool-age children. 16. Berry KJ, Johnston JE, Mielke PWJ. A 24. Sontag-Padilla L, Burns RM, Shih RA, Brain Cogn. 2019;131:87–93 Chronicle of Permutation Statistical et al. The Urban Child Institue CANDLE Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news by guest on October 13, 2021 8 MCARTHUR et al
Study. Santa Monica, CA: Rand scientific responsibilities. JAMA Pediatr. 33. McArthur BA, Browne D, Tough S, Corporation; 2015 2020;174(2):111–112 Madigan S. Trajectories of screen use 25. Graham JW. Missing data analysis: during early childhood: predictors and 30. Madigan S, Racine N, Tough S. making it work in the real world. Annu associated behavior and learning Prevalence of preschoolers meeting vs Rev Psychol. 2009;60:549–576 outcomes. Comput Human Behav. 2020; exceeding screen time guidelines. JAMA 113:106501 26. Yuan KH, Bentler PM. 5. Three Pediatr. 2019;174(1):93–95 likelihood-based methods for mean and 34. American Academy of Pediatrics. Early 31. American Academy of Pediatrics. covariance structure analysis with education - The 5 R’s. Available at: American Academy of Pediatrics nonnormal missing data. Sociol https://www.aap.org/en-us/advocacy- announces new recommendations for Methodol. 2000;30(1):165–200 and-policy/aap-health-initiatives/EBCD/ children’s media use. 2016. Available at: Pages/Five.aspx. Accessed October 23, 27. Munzer TG, Miller AL, Weeks HM, https://services.aap.org/en/news-room/ 2020 Kaciroti N, Radesky J. Differences in news-releases/aap/2016/aap- parent-toddler interactions with announces-new-recommendations-for- 35. Klass P, Dreyer BP, Mendelsohn AL. electronic versus print books. media-use. Accessed September 12, Reach out and read: literacy promotion Pediatrics. 2019;143(4):e20182012 2018 in pediatric primary care. Adv Pediatr. 28. Munzer TG, Miller AL, Weeks HM, 2009;56(1):11–27 32. American Academy of Pediatrics. Family Kaciroti N, Radesky J. Parent-toddler media plan. 2019. Available at: https:// 36. Madigan S, McArthur BA, Anhorn C, social reciprocity during reading from www.healthychildren.org/English/me Eirich R, Christakis DA. Associations electronic tablets vs print books. JAMA dia/Pages/default.aspx?gclid=EAIaIQo between screen use and child language Pediatr. 2019;173(11):1–8 bChMIoq2F-eiA3QIVUFuGCh3e skills: a systematic review and meta- 29. Browne D, Thompson DA, Madigan S. 0gDnEAAYBCAAEgJqNPD_BwE. Accessed analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 2020;174(7): Digital media use in children: clinical vs July 8, 2019 665–675 Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news by guest on October 13, 2021 PEDIATRICS Volume 147, number 6, June 2021 9
Longitudinal Associations Between Screen Use and Reading in Preschool-Aged Children Brae Anne McArthur, Dillon Browne, Sheila McDonald, Suzanne Tough and Sheri Madigan Pediatrics 2021;147; DOI: 10.1542/peds.2020-011429 originally published online May 24, 2021; Updated Information & including high resolution figures, can be found at: Services http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/147/6/e2020011429 References This article cites 28 articles, 3 of which you can access for free at: http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/147/6/e2020011429#BI BL Subspecialty Collections This article, along with others on similar topics, appears in the following collection(s): Screen Time http://www.aappublications.org/cgi/collection/screen_time_sub Public Health http://www.aappublications.org/cgi/collection/public_health_sub Permissions & Licensing Information about reproducing this article in parts (figures, tables) or in its entirety can be found online at: http://www.aappublications.org/site/misc/Permissions.xhtml Reprints Information about ordering reprints can be found online: http://www.aappublications.org/site/misc/reprints.xhtml Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news by guest on October 13, 2021
Longitudinal Associations Between Screen Use and Reading in Preschool-Aged Children Brae Anne McArthur, Dillon Browne, Sheila McDonald, Suzanne Tough and Sheri Madigan Pediatrics 2021;147; DOI: 10.1542/peds.2020-011429 originally published online May 24, 2021; The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, is located on the World Wide Web at: http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/147/6/e2020011429 Pediatrics is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly publication, it has been published continuously since 1948. Pediatrics is owned, published, and trademarked by the American Academy of Pediatrics, 345 Park Avenue, Itasca, Illinois, 60143. Copyright © 2021 by the American Academy of Pediatrics. All rights reserved. Print ISSN: 1073-0397. Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news by guest on October 13, 2021
You can also read