Graphs with logarithmic axes distort lay judgments
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
finding Graphs with logarithmic axes distort lay judgments William H. Ryan & Ellen R. K. Evers abstract * Graphs that depict numbers of COVID-19 cases often use a linear or logarithmic scale on the y-axis. To examine the effect of scale on how the general public interprets the curves and uses that understanding to infer the urgency of the need for protective actions, we conducted a series of experiments that presented laypeople with the same data plotted on one scale or the other. We found that graphs with a logarithmic, as opposed to a linear, scale resulted in laypeople making less accurate predictions of how fast cases would increase, viewing COVID-19 as less dangerous, and expressing both less support for policy interventions and less intention to take personal actions to combat the disease. Education about the differences between linear and logarithmic graphs reduces but does not eliminate these effects. These results suggest that communications to the general public should mostly use linear graphs. When logarithmic graphs must be used, they should be presented alongside linear graphs of the same data and with guidance on how to interpret the plots. Ryan, William H., & Evers, Ellen R. K. (2020). Graphs with logarithmic axes distort lay judgments. Behavioral Science & Policy. Retrieved from https://behavioralpolicy.org/ journal_issue/covid-19/ a publication of the behavioral science & policy association 1
I n responding to the public’s hunger for infor- When presenting data on disease growth, scien- mation about the global COVID-19 epidemic, tists generally use either a linear or a logarithmic policymakers, public health experts, and jour- y-axis. A linear axis is divided into equal incre- nalists need to decide how to present data on ments, going, say, from 100,000 cases at the the growth rate of infections in a way that lay first tick mark to 200,000, then 300,000, and audiences can understand easily. Even though so on. A logarithmic y-axis tracks the logarithms the objective reality described by the data may of values—which means that each interval on be identical in different presentations, the choice the graph corresponds to an order of magni- of the scale used on the y-axis can greatly influ- tude increase in values rather than to a fixed ence interpretations of how quickly the number increment. Thus, a jump from 100 (102) to 1,000 of cases is changing. Being aware of the way (103) COVID-19 cases near the bottom of an axis that scale influences these interpretations is and the jump from 1,000 cases to 10,000 (104) critical, because incorrect understandings can cases a little higher up will be represented by affect the public’s beliefs about the urgency of the same vertical rise on a curve, even though the need to take precautions to protect them- the numbers of cases represented by that spatial selves and others.1 range differs enormously. See Figure 1 for an Figure 1. Linear & logarithmic graphs of COVID-19 cases in the United States as of May 1, 2020 Confirmed cases (linear scale) 1,200,000 1,100,000 1,000,000 900,000 800,000 700,000 600,000 500,000 400,000 300,000 200,000 100,000 0 Confirmed cases (logarithmic scale) 1,000,000 500,000 100,000 50,000 10,000 5,000 1,000 500 100 Note. These graphs, from the data-visualization website https://91-divoc.com, demonstrate the difference in the appearance of curves when identical data is plotted using a linear versus a logarithmic y-axis. Titles have been added above plots and the x-axis labels have been revised for clarity. Data displays from the 91-DIVOC website have been shared virally online and used in briefings by the governors of Kentucky13 and Washington.14 The linear graph is adapted from “An Interactive Visualization of the Exponential Spread of COVID-19,” by W. Fagen-Ulmschneider, May 1, 2020 (https://91-divoc.com/pages/covid-visualization /?chart=countries&highlight=United%20States&show=highlight-only&y=both&scale=linear&data=cases&data-source=jhu&xaxis =right#countries). CC BY 4.0. The logarithmic graph is adapted from “An Interactive Visualization of the Exponential Spread of COVID-19,” by W. Fagen-Ulmschneider, May 1, 2020 (https://91-divoc.com/pages/covid-visualization/?chart=countries&highlight =United%20States&show=highlight-only&y=both&scale=log&data=cases&data-source=jhu&xaxis=right#countries). CC BY 4.0. 2 behavioral science & policy
illustration of the difference between a linear on active computation, people often use intu- and a logarithmic graph. itive, so-called system 1–like, judgments, which are more likely to be biased (that is, systematically Logarithmic axes can be useful because they incorrect in one direction or another).8 Because of make it easier to compare exponential growth this intuitive processing, their judgments can be rates and to see whether case rates in different influenced by design choices.9 For example, iden- places are accelerating or decelerating simi- tical growth data in the same type of graph can larly over time. They are also helpful when yield different inferences regarding growth rates if comparing countries with cases that are an the aspect ratio of the graph is changed to make order of magnitude different from one another. a curve’s slope appear more or less steep.10,11 Even If one country, for example, has 10,000 cases many experts can misunderstand graphs that use and a second country has only 100 cases, the nonstandard design approaches.12 Such findings curve for the second country would be so tiny suggest that consumers of COVID-19-related on a linear graph as to be unreadable. Linear data may tend to misjudge the severity of the graphs present case counts more directly, pandemic when those data are presented using however, which can make them easier to grasp. a logarithmic scale. In the studies presented in this article, we asked In spite of the challenges posed by logarithmic whether the general public is more likely to graphs, government leaders and media outlets misconstrue data presented on a logarithmic use both types of graphs in communicating with scale than data presented on a linear scale. the public about COVID-19, as seen in presen- We explored this possibility in part because we tations given by the governors of Kentucky13 know of at least two cognitive processes that and Washington.14 In a further example, when could contribute to such misunderstandings. we reviewed graphs in three major newspa- pers—the Financial Times, The New York Times, First, individuals are notoriously bad at numer- and The Wall Street Journal—we found that ical thinking in general—that is, at interpreting although linear scales were most common in probabilities and other mathematical infor- articles, all three publication also used loga- mation. Numerical thinking is often measured rithmic scales. Notably, the Financial Times using a test known as an objective numeracy presented its primary COVID-19 tracker in a scale, which includes math problems such as logarithmic format. In addition, logarithmic the question, “Which of the following numbers graphs are omnipresent in online scientific represents the biggest risk of getting a disease: communications, which laypeople are increas- 1 in 100, 1 in 1,000, or 1 in 10?” In one study, ingly accessing directly via social media and 16%–20% of participants failed to answer the preprint services 15—such as the medical site simplest questions correctly. 2 This difficulty can medRxiv, which saw its number of visitors carry over to the interpretation of logarithmic increase 100-fold between December and April. scales, because people tend to be less familiar with logarithmic than linear scales and less able In this article, we present four studies investi- to extrapolate from the slopes they see, partic- gating the accuracy of predictions made by ularly when confronted with the exponential participants on the basis of each type of graph. growth often found in data describing the Collectively, the studies assessed participants’ numbers of people afflicted by a disease.3,4 perceptions, after viewing the graphs, of the danger posed by COVID-19 and the importance Second, even if individuals are able to correctly of governments’ and individuals’ taking action interpret logarithmic scales, they may not be against the spread of the disease. Overall, we motivated to do so, instead preferring to avoid found that people are reliably less accurate in numeric computations—a tendency demon- their predictions of the growth rate and believe strated by an experimental tool known as the that COVID-19 is less dangerous when data subjective numeracy scale.5–7 Studies using this are presented using a logarithmic rather than instrument have found that rather than relying a linear scale. Additionally, this belief correlates a publication of the behavioral science & policy association 3
with less inclination to support protective Study 1A was completed by 266 people (mean behaviors recommended or mandated by age = 38 years; 40% were female). Study 1B was governments and less inclination to adopt such completed by 403 people (mean age = 36.7 behavior oneself. The Supplemental Material years; 37% were female). provides additional details on these studies’ methods and results, supplementary analyses, Procedure. Both studies involved two condi- and information about three additional studies tions: one using a linear scale and one using a we conducted; also see note A. logarithmic scale. The graphs were similar to those in Figure 1. In Study 1A, each participant was shown a graph indicating case numbers up Studies 1A & 1B: Effects on to the present for a succession of four countries Predictions of Future Cases (the United States first and then three others in Overview random order). In Study 1B, participants saw a In Studies 1A and 1B, we tested whether the use graph for one hypothetical country. In both of logarithmic or linear scales affected the accu- studies, after participants viewed the graphs, racy of individual’s predictions of the future rate they predicted the number of cases that would of growth of a disease. In Study 1A, participants be seen one, three, five, and 10 days from the saw graphs displaying total COVID-19 cases in present for each country shown. real countries and extrapolated to predict case counts in the future (see note B). Because it was In Study 1B, we also measured judgments of the possible that a country could change the criteria danger posed by COVID-19. Studies 2 and 3 also for counting cases during the course of a study, address this judgment; details of the measures we also ran Study 1B: a conceptual replication used are included later in this article and in the in which we presented participants with hypo- Supplemental Material. thetical growth data for an imaginary disease and compared their predictions of future case Results counts with the number of cases that the equa- Overall, the mean absolute error in participants’ tion behind the graph would predict. predictions was higher in the logarithmic condi- tion than in the linear condition in Studies 1A Method and 1B (see Figure 2). Participants. Participants were U.S.-based Mechanical Turk workers, who are paid to The results of Studies 1A and 1B suggest that participate in research or do other online tasks. logarithmic scales tend to make laypeople’s Figure 2. Errors in predicting cases one, three, five, & 10 days ahead, in Studies 1A & 1B Scale Condition: Linear Scale Logarithmic Scale A Study 1A: Mean Absolute Prediction Error by Condition B Study 1B: Mean Absolute Prediction Error by Condition Note. In both studies, for each prediction, the error is greater in the logarithmic condition. Here and in the following figures, error bars are calculated as the population-level standard error for the measure. 4 behavioral science & policy
judgments about growth more variable than the the basis of people’s predictions of future cases. judgments induced by linear graphs and thus For instance, some individuals’ judgments may less accurate. The greater variance could stem be more affected by the total number of cases from the lower granularity in the logarithmic they foresee in the near future, whereas others scale. At values above 10 cases, a given phys- may be more affected by anticipated growth ical distance on the curve often corresponds rates. In addition, if some people overestimate to a greater absolute change in cases when future numbers considerably but most people the y-axis is logarithmic rather than linear. This underestimate the numbers slightly, the group feature may make it more difficult to accurately average may suggest that people are making infer the number of cases higher up on the accurate judgments when in fact most people logarithmic scale. are underestimating the future trend and are therefore inclined to underreact to the threat of Average variance is not the only way to measure COVID-19. the accuracy of participants’ interpretations. One could also ask whether presenting the data Method in a logarithmic format creates systematic over- Participants. The study was completed by 891 or underestimation in the logarithmic group U.S.-based Mechanical Turk workers (mean age by biasing most people’s interpretations in one = 37.9 years; 48% were female). direction or the other. We tested this possibility but did not find a consistent pattern. We also did Procedure. The participants were presented with not find that the extent of people’s inaccuracy true disease data for the United States and then in predicting future case counts correlated well disease data for three other countries in succes- with their judgments of danger. It is possible that sion in either linear or logarithmic formats such a correlation exists but that we lacked the and were asked questions about future case statistical power to detect it. Additional details numbers in each country. Because it is possible on the accuracy analyses for these and the that individuals make more accurate projections following studies can be found in the Supple- when data are embedded in a larger context,16 mental Material. we also varied whether participants saw data of the case prevalence of only a single country or saw a country’s data on one graph that included Study 2: Influences on the data for 10 additional countries. Beliefs & Attitudes Overview To assess participants’ inferences about the Of course, laypeople are not usually asked to growth rate of COVID-19 cases, we asked them predict the spread of a disease. What is most to indicate how much they expected the rate important for policy and for communicating to change using a scale ranging from Decrease with the public about COVID-19 is how the significantly to Increase significantly. The scale of linear and logarithmic graphs affects perceived threat was measured by having partic- people’s perceptions of threat and need for a ipants rate COVID-19 on a scale ranging from response. In Study 2, we looked more directly Not at all dangerous to Extremely dangerous. at how the choice of scale in data displays influ- To measure views of what governments should enced participants’ judgments of not only the do, we had participants use a scale ranging from COVID-19 growth rate but also the threat posed Disagree strongly to Agree strongly to indicate by the disease and whether these judgments agreement with the statement that the country influenced attitudes regarding how individuals depicted in the graph “should ban public gath- and governments should respond. erings, close non-essential businesses, and ask all citizens to stay at home unless they are going We deemed these direct measurements of views to work or carrying out necessary errands.” We of the threat posed by COVID-19 and of the also asked participants to consider all the efforts need for action to be critical because, as Study people were taking to combat the coronavirus in 1B suggested, it is not safe to infer such views on each depicted country and to indicate whether a publication of the behavioral science & policy association 5
the people were exerting the right amount of adherence to social distancing. (See Figure 3 for effort; participants used a scale ranging from the numerical ranges of the scales.) Significantly decrease to Significantly increase to indicate how they thought people should Results adjust their efforts. In addition to that item, we Judgments of growth rate, danger, appropriate had participants indicate whether, on the basis policy response, and required individual effort of what they saw on the graphs, they would to combat COVID-19 were all lower when increase or decrease their own mask use and logarithmic scales were used, regardless of Figure 3. Differences in data interpretations, attitudes, & behavioral intentions, Study 2 Scale Condition: Linear Scale Logarithmic Scale Note. The plots show mean responses to questions answered on the basis of viewing graphs of COVID-19 cases in multiple or single countries. Regardless of the number of countries represented on the graphs, use of a logarithmic versus a linear scale led to lower judgments of the future growth in COVID-19 cases (A), the danger posed by the disease (B), the need for policy response by governments (C), and the need for individual effort (D); it also resulted in lower intentions to use masks (E) and socially distance (F). The questions and range of replies follow: Except for B, all answers used a 7-point scale ranging from −3 (at the bottom of the axis) to 3 (at the top). A. “How do you expect the growth rate of cases, i.e. the number of new cases per day, to change in [COUNTRY]?” Range: from −3 = Significantly fewer new cases/day to 3 = Significantly more new cases/day. B. “How dangerous do you believe Coronavirus is to [COUNTRY] and its citizens?” Range: from 1 = Not at all dangerous to 7 = Extremely dangerous. C. “How much do you agree with the policy: [COUNTRY] should ban public gatherings, close non-essential businesses, and ask all citizens to stay at home unless they are going to work or carrying out necessary errands?” Range: from −3 = Disagree strongly to 3 = Agree strongly. D. “Think of all the efforts the people in [COUNTRY] may be doing to try to stop the disease, such as social distancing, wearing face masks, and avoiding non-essential travel. Based on this graph, how do you think people in [COUNTRY] should change the amount of effort they put into these actions?” Range: from −3 = Significantly decrease effort to 3 = Significantly increase effort. E and F (relating to U.S. data): “Based solely on this graph, do you see yourself wearing a mask more or less often than you do now?” (E) and “Based solely on this graph, do you see yourself being significantly more or less careful about social distancing relative to now?” (F). Range: −3 = Significantly less often to 3 =Significantly more often. 6 behavioral science & policy
the number of countries presented, as Figure Results 3 shows. Alarmingly, relative to the reports The results are depicted in Figures 4 and 5. of participants in the linear condition, those Informing participants about the pitfalls of who saw logarithmic graphs indicated that logarithmic graphs reduced the difference in they would wear masks less often and be less perceptions of growth and danger; however, committed to social distancing. Presenting those who saw logarithmic graphs still perceived additional countries for context reduced the lower growth and less danger than did those perception of the danger posed to the target who saw linear graphs. That is, education helped country in both conditions, probably because the additional countries shown all had high case Figure 4. Effects of education in graph reading, Study 3 counts, making the target country’s case count seem smaller in comparison. Scale Condition: Linear Scale Logarithmic Scale A Intervention Effect on Growth Ratings by Graph Scale Study 3: Education’s Ability to Reduce Prediction Errors Some media outlets have begun educating their audience about how to correctly interpret loga- rithmic graphs. In Study 3, we evaluated whether this instruction brings interpretations of disease growth rates and the threat posed by COVID-19 made on the basis of logarithmic graphs in line with the interpretations made on the basis of linear graphs. Method Participants. This study was completed by 739 Mechanical Turk workers (mean age = 40.3 years; 50.2% were female). Procedure. We divided the participants into B Intervention Effect on Danger Ratings by Graph Scale two groups. One group saw linear graphs and the other group saw logarithmic graphs showing COVID-19 case data for four coun- tries in succession. Before viewing the graphs, half of the participants in each group watched a “debiasing” video explaining how to correctly interpret linear and logarithmic graphs, and the other half of the participants—those in the control condition—watched an unrelated video of equal length about painting. The debiasing video was a 1 minute 45 second clip from the second section of a Vox Media video (available at https://youtu.be/O-3Mlj3MQ_Q?t=65.) After viewing the videos and the graphs, participants used the same scales as in Study 2 to indi- cate how much they expected growth rates to Note. A and B show mean responses to the same questions asked about growth and danger in change and how dangerous COVID-19 seemed Study 2. Education about how to interpret logarithmic and linear graphs reduced differences in to them. growth and danger ratings by those who viewed logarithmic graphs but did not eliminate the gaps. a publication of the behavioral science & policy association 7
Figure 5. Summary results from Studies 1B, 2, & 3 situation when data were presented using a logarithmic scale as compared with a linear Scale Condition: Linear Scale Logarithmic Scale scale. Logarithmic scales also led participants to believe the pandemic was less severe and A Growth Ratings by Condition and Study less dangerous than linear scales did. Figure 5 summarizes the growth ratings and danger perceptions reported in Studies 1B, 2, and 3. Individuals presented with logarithmic graphs were less supportive of government policies aimed at reducing the growth in cases and less likely to take individual actions such as mask wearing and social distancing. We also explored whether a couple of interven- tions would minimize the misleading influences of seeing logarithmic graphs. Providing data on multiple countries did not reduce the differ- ences between the effects of the two types of graphs, and educating participants about how to read the graphs reduced but did not eliminate the differences. We did not establish that inac- curacy in predicting future cases on the basis of B Danger Ratings by Condition and Study seeing logarithmic plots led directly to behavior change meant to limit COVID’s spread. These two effects of reading the graphs might be independent, but researchers conducting future work should examine this issue in more detail. We did not extensively test whether the actual growth of the outbreak could influence differ- ence in interpretations of the two types of graphs, but we conducted one test of the possibility (as is described in Appendix Study 1 in the Supplemental Material.). To under- stand how the actual numbers might have an effect, consider Figure 6, which illustrates the differences between the looks of the curves depicting the first 20 days versus the first 50 days of the outbreak in the United States. Row Note. The bars show mean responses to the growth and danger survey items. The logarithmic A shows the first 20 days of the outbreak in scale led to lower estimates of growth and danger in all three studies. The growth and danger logarithmic and linear formats, and row B adds questions were the same in all three studies. in the next 30 days. The logarithmic and linear graphs look more similar in row A, when growth to decrease the effects of logarithmic scales on rates are still climbing, than they do in row B, judgment but did not eliminate them (see Note C). when the rates have stabilized or decreased. To test whether the actual pattern of the outbreak could moderate the effects documented in this General Discussion article, we ran a simple conceptual replication of In the studies reported in this article, we found the growth and danger components of Study 2, consistent evidence that the public formed less only this time we chose countries whose growth accurate impressions of the current COVID-19 rates fit one of the two patterns in Figure 6. We 8 behavioral science & policy
Figure 6. Total confirmed cases of COVID-19 in the United States by the number of days since 100 cases were confirmed Note. These graphs illustrate why using a logarithmic scale to plot the growth of cases can lead the public to make lower predictions of growth than a linear-scale plot would. In row A, from early in the pandemic, both graphs give the visual impression that cases are rising, but the linear scale (right) more intuitively conveys that the rise is becoming steeper at day 15. In row B, from later in the pandemic, the linear scale (right) again gives the impression of a rise, but the logarithmic scale now appears to be starting to curve downward—which could lead some people to mistakenly assume that the number of cases is about to decline. In both cases, the logarithmic scale may lead to lower judgments of growth. found that the logarithmic axes led to underes- should depend on the needs of the audience. timation of growth and threat regardless of the Comparing growth rates between countries slope of the actual growth. is certainly important for epidemiologists and politicians who are in a position to implement In two of our studies, we conducted added and evaluate high-level policies. For them, analyses to examine whether various indi- logarithmic graphs may be most useful. For vidual differences could influence the degree to the general public, however, graphs should be which people struggle with logarithmic graphs. designed to enable viewers to accurately esti- Surprisingly, we found that greater objective mate risk and respond accordingly. Indeed, facility for working with numbers did not result when we ran a test in which we expected loga- in increased accuracy in interpreting logarithmic rithmic graphs might result in laypeople making scales relative to linear scales. In fact, some- more accurate interpretations of growth rates, times more numerate individuals fared worse we found that the claimed advantages of loga- than others who were less numerate. (See the rithmic graphs did not appear: for a lay audience, Supplemental Material for a fuller discussion of logarithmic graphs did not improve accuracy of the individual differences we measured.) comparative growth judgments. (See Appendix Study 2 in the Supplemental Material.) The skeptical reader may argue that we stacked the deck against logarithmic scales by concen- trating on their influence on impressions of Recommendations risk and danger rather than on their value for Overall, our findings make it clear that officials’ highlighting differences in the growth rates of and media’s decision to use either logarithmic cases. We argue that the choice of graph type or linear axes in graphs can influence public a publication of the behavioral science & policy association 9
response to COVID-19. When people are end notes presented with graphs with logarithmic instead A. Full materials, preregistrations, and data for the of linear axes, they make less accurate predic- studies described in this article and for additional tions of future growth; view COVID-19 as less studies we conducted can be found at https://osf. of a threat; and, accordingly, are less supportive io/zqut5/?view_only=3aa66d592dd2495ca508b4f of governmental and individual action against a8729381a. COVID-19. Education can reduce these effects, B. In Study 1A, actual case counts were derived from but it cannot eliminate them. the COVID-19 data repository maintained by the Center for Systems Science and Engineering at John Hopkins University, available from https:// Logarithmic graphs still have significant value github.com/CSSEGISandData/COVID-19. for presenting scientific data. However, on the basis of our research, we recommend using C. It is conceivable that people who judge COVID-19 to pose a low degree of danger on the basis of them for the general public only when they are seeing logarithmic graphs are more accurate truly the most reasonable option. And when in their threat assessment than are people who they are used, presenters should spend signifi- view the same data on linear graphs. We do not cant time explaining how to read the graphs and think that they are more accurate, however. When should supplement the logarithmic graphs with people are taught how to read logarithmic graphs, linear displays of the data. their sense of danger does not fall; rather, it rises. supplemental material • http://behavioralpolicy.org/publications • Methods & Analysis author affiliation Ryan & Evers: University of California, Berkeley. Corresponding author’s e-mail: wryan@ berkeley.edu. 10 behavioral science & policy
references 1. Chapman, G. B., & Coups, E. J. 9. Zacks, J., & Tversky, B. (1999). (2006). Emotions and preventive Bars and lines: A study of graphic health behavior: Worry, regret, communication. Memory & Cognition, and influenza vaccination. Health 27, 1073–1079. https://doi.org/10.3758/ Psychology, 25, 82–90. https://doi. bf03201236 org/10.1037/0278-6133.25.1.82 10. Beattie, V., & Jones, M. J. (2002). 2. Lipkus, I. M., Samsa, G., & Rimer, The impact of graph slope on rate of B. K. (2001). General performance change judgments in corporate reports. on a numeracy scale among highly Abacus, 38, 177–199. https://doi. educated samples. Medical Decision org/10.1111/1467-6281.00104 Making, 21, 37–44. https://doi. 11. Cleveland, W. S., & McGill, R. (1987). org/10.1177/0272989x0102100105 Graphical perception: The visual 3. Wagenaar, W. A., & Sagaria, S. D. decoding of quantitative information (1975). Misperception of exponential on graphical displays of data. Journal growth. Perception & Psychophysics, of the Royal Statistical Society, 18, 416–422. https://doi.org/10.3758/ Series A, 150, 192–229. https://doi. bf03204114 org/10.2307/2981473 4. Wagenaar, W. A., & Timmers, H. 12. Fischer, H., Schütte, S., Depoux, A., (1979). The pond-and-duckweed Amelung, D., & Sauerborn, R. (2018). problem; Three experiments on the How well do COP22 attendees misperception of exponential growth. understand graphs on climate change Acta Psychologica, 43, 239–251. https:// health impacts from the fifth IPCC doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(79)90028-3 assessment report? International Journal of Environmental Research and 5. Schwartz, L. M., Woloshin, S., Black, Public Health, 15, Article 875. https:// C., & Welch, H. G. (1997). The role of doi.org/10.3390/ijerph15050875 numeracy in understanding the benefit of screening mammography. Annals of 13. Beshear, A. [Governor Andy Internal Medicine, 127, 966–972. https:// Beshear]. (2020, April 5). Update on doi.org/10.7326/0003-4819-127-11- COVID-19 in Kentucky—4.5.2020 199712010-00003 [Video file]. Retrieved from https://www.youtube.com/ 6. Zikmund-Fisher, B. J., Smith, D. M., watch?v=gSzUuOTzGE8&feature=youtu. Ubel, P. A., & Fagerlin, A. (2007). be&t=1595 Validation of the Subjective Numeracy Scale: Effects of low numeracy on 14. Inslee, J. [GovInslee]. (2020, March 26). comprehension of risk communications GovInslee was live [Video file]. Retrieved and utility elicitations. Medical Decision from https://twitter.com/GovInslee/ Making, 27, 663–671. https://doi. status/1243237794895413249 org/10.1177/0272989x07303824 15. Yan, W. (2020, April 14). Coronavirus 7. Peters, E., Hibbard, J., Slovic, P., & tests science’s need for speed limits. Dieckmann, N. (2007). Numeracy The New York Times. Retrieved from skill and the communication, https://www.nytimes.com/2020/04/14/ comprehension, and use of risk-benefit science/coronavirus-disinformation. information. Health Affairs, 26, 741–748. html https://doi.org/10.1377/hlthaff.26.3.741 16. Wagner, S. H., & Goffin, R. D. (1997). 8. Frederick, S. (2005). Cognitive Differences in accuracy of absolute and reflection and decision making. comparative performance appraisal Journal of Economic Perspectives, methods. Organizational Behavior and 19(4), 25–42. https://doi. Human Decision Processes, 70, 95–103. org/10.1257/089533005775196732 https://doi.org/10.1006/obhd.1997.2698 a publication of the behavioral science & policy association 11
You can also read