Graphs with logarithmic axes distort lay judgments

Page created by Marshall Johnston
 
CONTINUE READING
Graphs with logarithmic axes distort lay judgments
finding

Graphs with logarithmic
axes distort lay judgments
William H. Ryan & Ellen R. K. Evers

                                                   abstract           *

                                                   Graphs that depict numbers of COVID-19 cases often use a linear or
                                                   logarithmic scale on the y-axis. To examine the effect of scale on how the
                                                   general public interprets the curves and uses that understanding to infer
                                                   the urgency of the need for protective actions, we conducted a series of
                                                   experiments that presented laypeople with the same data plotted on one
                                                   scale or the other. We found that graphs with a logarithmic, as opposed
                                                   to a linear, scale resulted in laypeople making less accurate predictions of
                                                   how fast cases would increase, viewing COVID-19 as less dangerous, and
                                                   expressing both less support for policy interventions and less intention
                                                   to take personal actions to combat the disease. Education about the
                                                   differences between linear and logarithmic graphs reduces but does not
                                                   eliminate these effects. These results suggest that communications to the
                                                   general public should mostly use linear graphs. When logarithmic graphs
                                                   must be used, they should be presented alongside linear graphs of the
                                                   same data and with guidance on how to interpret the plots.

                                                   Ryan, William H., & Evers, Ellen R. K. (2020). Graphs with logarithmic axes distort lay
                                                   judgments. Behavioral Science & Policy. Retrieved from https://behavioralpolicy.org/
                                                   journal_issue/covid-19/

a publication of the behavioral science & policy association                                                                            1
I
      n responding to the public’s hunger for infor-                  When presenting data on disease growth, scien-
      mation about the global COVID-19 epidemic,                      tists generally use either a linear or a logarithmic
      policymakers, public health experts, and jour-                  y-axis. A linear axis is divided into equal incre-
    nalists need to decide how to present data on                     ments, going, say, from 100,000 cases at the
    the growth rate of infections in a way that lay                   first tick mark to 200,000, then 300,000, and
    audiences can understand easily. Even though                      so on. A logarithmic y-axis tracks the logarithms
    the objective reality described by the data may                   of values—which means that each interval on
    be identical in different presentations, the choice               the graph corresponds to an order of magni-
    of the scale used on the y-axis can greatly influ-                tude increase in values rather than to a fixed
    ence interpretations of how quickly the number                    increment. Thus, a jump from 100 (102) to 1,000
    of cases is changing. Being aware of the way                      (103) COVID-19 cases near the bottom of an axis
    that scale influences these interpretations is                    and the jump from 1,000 cases to 10,000 (104)
    critical, because incorrect understandings can                    cases a little higher up will be represented by
    affect the public’s beliefs about the urgency of                  the same vertical rise on a curve, even though
    the need to take precautions to protect them-                     the numbers of cases represented by that spatial
    selves and others.1                                               range differs enormously. See Figure 1 for an

    Figure 1. Linear & logarithmic graphs of COVID-19 cases in the United States as
    of May 1, 2020

                      Confirmed cases (linear scale)
                      1,200,000
                      1,100,000
                      1,000,000
                        900,000
                        800,000
                        700,000
                        600,000
                        500,000
                        400,000
                        300,000
                        200,000
                        100,000
                              0

                      Confirmed cases (logarithmic scale)
                      1,000,000
                        500,000

                        100,000
                         50,000

                         10,000
                          5,000

                           1,000
                             500

                             100

    Note. These graphs, from the data-visualization website https://91-divoc.com, demonstrate the difference in the appearance
    of curves when identical data is plotted using a linear versus a logarithmic y-axis. Titles have been added above plots and the
    x-axis labels have been revised for clarity. Data displays from the 91-DIVOC website have been shared virally online and used in
    briefings by the governors of Kentucky13 and Washington.14 The linear graph is adapted from “An Interactive Visualization of the
    Exponential Spread of COVID-19,” by W. Fagen-Ulmschneider, May 1, 2020 (https://91-divoc.com/pages/covid-visualization
    /?chart=countries&highlight=United%20States&show=highlight-only&y=both&scale=linear&data=cases&data-source=jhu&xaxis
    =right#countries). CC BY 4.0. The logarithmic graph is adapted from “An Interactive Visualization of the Exponential Spread of
    COVID-19,” by W. Fagen-Ulmschneider, May 1, 2020 (https://91-divoc.com/pages/covid-visualization/?chart=countries&highlight
    =United%20States&show=highlight-only&y=both&scale=log&data=cases&data-source=jhu&xaxis=right#countries). CC BY 4.0.

2                                                                                               behavioral science & policy
illustration of the difference between a linear       on active computation, people often use intu-
and a logarithmic graph.                              itive, so-called system 1–like, judgments, which
                                                      are more likely to be biased (that is, systematically
Logarithmic axes can be useful because they           incorrect in one direction or another).8 Because of
make it easier to compare exponential growth          this intuitive processing, their judgments can be
rates and to see whether case rates in different      influenced by design choices.9 For example, iden-
places are accelerating or decelerating simi-         tical growth data in the same type of graph can
larly over time. They are also helpful when           yield different inferences regarding growth rates if
comparing countries with cases that are an            the aspect ratio of the graph is changed to make
order of magnitude different from one another.        a curve’s slope appear more or less steep.10,11 Even
If one country, for example, has 10,000 cases         many experts can misunderstand graphs that use
and a second country has only 100 cases, the          nonstandard design approaches.12 Such findings
curve for the second country would be so tiny         suggest that consumers of COVID-19-related
on a linear graph as to be unreadable. Linear         data may tend to misjudge the severity of the
graphs present case counts more directly,             pandemic when those data are presented using
however, which can make them easier to grasp.         a logarithmic scale.

In the studies presented in this article, we asked    In spite of the challenges posed by logarithmic
whether the general public is more likely to          graphs, government leaders and media outlets
misconstrue data presented on a logarithmic           use both types of graphs in communicating with
scale than data presented on a linear scale.          the public about COVID-19, as seen in presen-
We explored this possibility in part because we       tations given by the governors of Kentucky13
know of at least two cognitive processes that         and Washington.14 In a further example, when
could contribute to such misunderstandings.           we reviewed graphs in three major newspa-
                                                      pers—the Financial Times, The New York Times,
First, individuals are notoriously bad at numer-      and The Wall Street Journal—we found that
ical thinking in general—that is, at interpreting     although linear scales were most common in
probabilities and other mathematical infor-           articles, all three publication also used loga-
mation. Numerical thinking is often measured          rithmic scales. Notably, the Financial Times
using a test known as an objective numeracy           presented its primary COVID-19 tracker in a
scale, which includes math problems such as           logarithmic format. In addition, logarithmic
the question, “Which of the following numbers         graphs are omnipresent in online scientific
represents the biggest risk of getting a disease:     communications, which laypeople are increas-
1 in 100, 1 in 1,000, or 1 in 10?” In one study,      ingly accessing directly via social media and
16%–20% of participants failed to answer the          preprint services 15—such as the medical site
simplest questions correctly. 2 This difficulty can   medRxiv, which saw its number of visitors
carry over to the interpretation of logarithmic       increase 100-fold between December and April.
scales, because people tend to be less familiar
with logarithmic than linear scales and less able     In this article, we present four studies investi-
to extrapolate from the slopes they see, partic-      gating the accuracy of predictions made by
ularly when confronted with the exponential           participants on the basis of each type of graph.
growth often found in data describing the             Collectively, the studies assessed participants’
numbers of people afflicted by a disease.3,4          perceptions, after viewing the graphs, of the
                                                      danger posed by COVID-19 and the importance
Second, even if individuals are able to correctly     of governments’ and individuals’ taking action
interpret logarithmic scales, they may not be         against the spread of the disease. Overall, we
motivated to do so, instead preferring to avoid       found that people are reliably less accurate in
numeric computations—a tendency demon-                their predictions of the growth rate and believe
strated by an experimental tool known as the          that COVID-19 is less dangerous when data
subjective numeracy scale.5–7 Studies using this      are presented using a logarithmic rather than
instrument have found that rather than relying        a linear scale. Additionally, this belief correlates

a publication of the behavioral science & policy association                                                  3
with less inclination to support protective                           Study 1A was completed by 266 people (mean
                                  behaviors recommended or mandated by                                  age = 38 years; 40% were female). Study 1B was
                                  governments and less inclination to adopt such                        completed by 403 people (mean age = 36.7
                                  behavior oneself. The Supplemental Material                           years; 37% were female).
                                  provides additional details on these studies’
                                  methods and results, supplementary analyses,                          Procedure. Both studies involved two condi-
                                  and information about three additional studies                        tions: one using a linear scale and one using a
                                  we conducted; also see note A.                                        logarithmic scale. The graphs were similar to
                                                                                                        those in Figure 1. In Study 1A, each participant
                                                                                                        was shown a graph indicating case numbers up
                                  Studies 1A & 1B: Effects on                                           to the present for a succession of four countries
                                  Predictions of Future Cases                                           (the United States first and then three others in
                                  Overview                                                              random order). In Study 1B, participants saw a
                                  In Studies 1A and 1B, we tested whether the use                       graph for one hypothetical country. In both
                                  of logarithmic or linear scales affected the accu-                    studies, after participants viewed the graphs,
                                  racy of individual’s predictions of the future rate                   they predicted the number of cases that would
                                  of growth of a disease. In Study 1A, participants                     be seen one, three, five, and 10 days from the
                                  saw graphs displaying total COVID-19 cases in                         present for each country shown.
                                  real countries and extrapolated to predict case
                                  counts in the future (see note B). Because it was                     In Study 1B, we also measured judgments of the
                                  possible that a country could change the criteria                     danger posed by COVID-19. Studies 2 and 3 also
                                  for counting cases during the course of a study,                      address this judgment; details of the measures
                                  we also ran Study 1B: a conceptual replication                        used are included later in this article and in the
                                  in which we presented participants with hypo-                         Supplemental Material.
                                  thetical growth data for an imaginary disease
                                  and compared their predictions of future case                         Results
                                  counts with the number of cases that the equa-                        Overall, the mean absolute error in participants’
                                  tion behind the graph would predict.                                  predictions was higher in the logarithmic condi-
                                                                                                        tion than in the linear condition in Studies 1A
                                  Method                                                                and 1B (see Figure 2).
                                  Participants. Participants were U.S.-based
                                  Mechanical Turk workers, who are paid to                              The results of Studies 1A and 1B suggest that
                                  participate in research or do other online tasks.                     logarithmic scales tend to make laypeople’s

Figure 2. Errors in predicting cases one, three, five, & 10 days ahead, in Studies 1A & 1B

                                                Scale Condition:            Linear Scale           Logarithmic Scale

A Study 1A: Mean Absolute Prediction Error by Condition                                   B Study 1B: Mean Absolute Prediction Error by Condition

Note. In both studies, for each prediction, the error is greater in the logarithmic condition. Here and in the following figures, error bars are calculated as the
population-level standard error for the measure.

4                                                                                                                                  behavioral science & policy
judgments about growth more variable than the         the basis of people’s predictions of future cases.
judgments induced by linear graphs and thus           For instance, some individuals’ judgments may
less accurate. The greater variance could stem        be more affected by the total number of cases
from the lower granularity in the logarithmic         they foresee in the near future, whereas others
scale. At values above 10 cases, a given phys-        may be more affected by anticipated growth
ical distance on the curve often corresponds          rates. In addition, if some people overestimate
to a greater absolute change in cases when            future numbers considerably but most people
the y-axis is logarithmic rather than linear. This    underestimate the numbers slightly, the group
feature may make it more difficult to accurately      average may suggest that people are making
infer the number of cases higher up on the            accurate judgments when in fact most people
logarithmic scale.                                    are underestimating the future trend and are
                                                      therefore inclined to underreact to the threat of
Average variance is not the only way to measure       COVID-19.
the accuracy of participants’ interpretations.
One could also ask whether presenting the data        Method
in a logarithmic format creates systematic over-      Participants. The study was completed by 891
or underestimation in the logarithmic group           U.S.-based Mechanical Turk workers (mean age
by biasing most people’s interpretations in one       = 37.9 years; 48% were female).
direction or the other. We tested this possibility
but did not find a consistent pattern. We also did    Procedure. The participants were presented with
not find that the extent of people’s inaccuracy       true disease data for the United States and then
in predicting future case counts correlated well      disease data for three other countries in succes-
with their judgments of danger. It is possible that   sion in either linear or logarithmic formats
such a correlation exists but that we lacked the      and were asked questions about future case
statistical power to detect it. Additional details    numbers in each country. Because it is possible
on the accuracy analyses for these and the            that individuals make more accurate projections
following studies can be found in the Supple-         when data are embedded in a larger context,16
mental Material.                                      we also varied whether participants saw data of
                                                      the case prevalence of only a single country or
                                                      saw a country’s data on one graph that included
Study 2: Influences on                                the data for 10 additional countries.
Beliefs & Attitudes
Overview                                              To assess participants’ inferences about the
Of course, laypeople are not usually asked to         growth rate of COVID-19 cases, we asked them
predict the spread of a disease. What is most         to indicate how much they expected the rate
important for policy and for communicating            to change using a scale ranging from Decrease
with the public about COVID-19 is how the             significantly to Increase significantly. The
scale of linear and logarithmic graphs affects        perceived threat was measured by having partic-
people’s perceptions of threat and need for a         ipants rate COVID-19 on a scale ranging from
response. In Study 2, we looked more directly         Not at all dangerous to Extremely dangerous.
at how the choice of scale in data displays influ-    To measure views of what governments should
enced participants’ judgments of not only the         do, we had participants use a scale ranging from
COVID-19 growth rate but also the threat posed        Disagree strongly to Agree strongly to indicate
by the disease and whether these judgments            agreement with the statement that the country
influenced attitudes regarding how individuals        depicted in the graph “should ban public gath-
and governments should respond.                       erings, close non-essential businesses, and ask
                                                      all citizens to stay at home unless they are going
We deemed these direct measurements of views          to work or carrying out necessary errands.” We
of the threat posed by COVID-19 and of the            also asked participants to consider all the efforts
need for action to be critical because, as Study      people were taking to combat the coronavirus in
1B suggested, it is not safe to infer such views on   each depicted country and to indicate whether

a publication of the behavioral science & policy association                                                5
the people were exerting the right amount of                        adherence to social distancing. (See Figure 3 for
                                  effort; participants used a scale ranging from                      the numerical ranges of the scales.)
                                  Significantly decrease to Significantly increase
                                  to indicate how they thought people should                          Results
                                  adjust their efforts. In addition to that item, we                  Judgments of growth rate, danger, appropriate
                                  had participants indicate whether, on the basis                     policy response, and required individual effort
                                  of what they saw on the graphs, they would                          to combat COVID-19 were all lower when
                                  increase or decrease their own mask use and                         logarithmic scales were used, regardless of

Figure 3. Differences in data interpretations, attitudes, & behavioral intentions, Study 2

                                               Scale Condition:            Linear Scale           Logarithmic Scale

Note. The plots show mean responses to questions answered on the basis of viewing graphs of COVID-19 cases in multiple or single countries. Regardless of the
number of countries represented on the graphs, use of a logarithmic versus a linear scale led to lower judgments of the future growth in COVID-19 cases (A), the
danger posed by the disease (B), the need for policy response by governments (C), and the need for individual effort (D); it also resulted in lower intentions to use
masks (E) and socially distance (F). The questions and range of replies follow: Except for B, all answers used a 7-point scale ranging from −3 (at the bottom of the
axis) to 3 (at the top). A. “How do you expect the growth rate of cases, i.e. the number of new cases per day, to change in [COUNTRY]?” Range: from −3 =
Significantly fewer new cases/day to 3 = Significantly more new cases/day. B. “How dangerous do you believe Coronavirus is to [COUNTRY] and its citizens?”
Range: from 1 = Not at all dangerous to 7 = Extremely dangerous. C. “How much do you agree with the policy: [COUNTRY] should ban public gatherings, close
non-essential businesses, and ask all citizens to stay at home unless they are going to work or carrying out necessary errands?” Range: from −3 = Disagree
strongly to 3 = Agree strongly. D. “Think of all the efforts the people in [COUNTRY] may be doing to try to stop the disease, such as social distancing, wearing face
masks, and avoiding non-essential travel. Based on this graph, how do you think people in [COUNTRY] should change the amount of effort they put into these
actions?” Range: from −3 = Significantly decrease effort to 3 = Significantly increase effort. E and F (relating to U.S. data): “Based solely on this graph, do you see
yourself wearing a mask more or less often than you do now?” (E) and “Based solely on this graph, do you see yourself being significantly more or less careful
about social distancing relative to now?” (F). Range: −3 = Significantly less often to 3 =Significantly more often.

6                                                                                                                                behavioral science & policy
the number of countries presented, as Figure         Results
3 shows. Alarmingly, relative to the reports         The results are depicted in Figures 4 and 5.
of participants in the linear condition, those       Informing participants about the pitfalls of
who saw logarithmic graphs indicated that            logarithmic graphs reduced the difference in
they would wear masks less often and be less         perceptions of growth and danger; however,
committed to social distancing. Presenting           those who saw logarithmic graphs still perceived
additional countries for context reduced the         lower growth and less danger than did those
perception of the danger posed to the target         who saw linear graphs. That is, education helped
country in both conditions, probably because
the additional countries shown all had high case     Figure 4. Effects of education in graph reading, Study 3
counts, making the target country’s case count
seem smaller in comparison.                                       Scale Condition:           Linear Scale         Logarithmic Scale

                                                         A Intervention Effect on Growth Ratings by Graph Scale
Study 3: Education’s Ability
to Reduce Prediction Errors
Some media outlets have begun educating their
audience about how to correctly interpret loga-
rithmic graphs. In Study 3, we evaluated whether
this instruction brings interpretations of disease
growth rates and the threat posed by COVID-19
made on the basis of logarithmic graphs in line
with the interpretations made on the basis of
linear graphs.

Method
Participants. This study was completed by 739
Mechanical Turk workers (mean age = 40.3
years; 50.2% were female).

Procedure. We divided the participants into              B Intervention Effect on Danger Ratings by Graph Scale
two groups. One group saw linear graphs
and the other group saw logarithmic graphs
showing COVID-19 case data for four coun-
tries in succession. Before viewing the graphs,
half of the participants in each group watched
a “debiasing” video explaining how to correctly
interpret linear and logarithmic graphs, and
the other half of the participants—those in the
control condition—watched an unrelated video
of equal length about painting. The debiasing
video was a 1 minute 45 second clip from the
second section of a Vox Media video (available
at https://youtu.be/O-3Mlj3MQ_Q?t=65.) After
viewing the videos and the graphs, participants
used the same scales as in Study 2 to indi-
cate how much they expected growth rates to
                                                     Note. A and B show mean responses to the same questions asked about growth and danger in
change and how dangerous COVID-19 seemed             Study 2. Education about how to interpret logarithmic and linear graphs reduced differences in
to them.                                             growth and danger ratings by those who viewed logarithmic graphs but did not eliminate the gaps.

a publication of the behavioral science & policy association                                                                                       7
Figure 5. Summary results from Studies 1B, 2, & 3                                               situation when data were presented using a
                                                                                                logarithmic scale as compared with a linear
            Scale Condition:           Linear Scale         Logarithmic Scale                   scale. Logarithmic scales also led participants
                                                                                                to believe the pandemic was less severe and
       A Growth Ratings by Condition and Study
                                                                                                less dangerous than linear scales did. Figure
                                                                                                5 summarizes the growth ratings and danger
                                                                                                perceptions reported in Studies 1B, 2, and 3.
                                                                                                Individuals presented with logarithmic graphs
                                                                                                were less supportive of government policies
                                                                                                aimed at reducing the growth in cases and less
                                                                                                likely to take individual actions such as mask
                                                                                                wearing and social distancing.

                                                                                                We also explored whether a couple of interven-
                                                                                                tions would minimize the misleading influences
                                                                                                of seeing logarithmic graphs. Providing data on
                                                                                                multiple countries did not reduce the differ-
                                                                                                ences between the effects of the two types of
                                                                                                graphs, and educating participants about how
                                                                                                to read the graphs reduced but did not eliminate
                                                                                                the differences. We did not establish that inac-
                                                                                                curacy in predicting future cases on the basis of
       B Danger Ratings by Condition and Study                                                  seeing logarithmic plots led directly to behavior
                                                                                                change meant to limit COVID’s spread. These
                                                                                                two effects of reading the graphs might be
                                                                                                independent, but researchers conducting future
                                                                                                work should examine this issue in more detail.

                                                                                                We did not extensively test whether the actual
                                                                                                growth of the outbreak could influence differ-
                                                                                                ence in interpretations of the two types of
                                                                                                graphs, but we conducted one test of the
                                                                                                possibility (as is described in Appendix Study
                                                                                                1 in the Supplemental Material.). To under-
                                                                                                stand how the actual numbers might have an
                                                                                                effect, consider Figure 6, which illustrates the
                                                                                                differences between the looks of the curves
                                                                                                depicting the first 20 days versus the first 50
                                                                                                days of the outbreak in the United States. Row
Note. The bars show mean responses to the growth and danger survey items. The logarithmic       A shows the first 20 days of the outbreak in
scale led to lower estimates of growth and danger in all three studies. The growth and danger   logarithmic and linear formats, and row B adds
questions were the same in all three studies.
                                                                                                in the next 30 days. The logarithmic and linear
                                                                                                graphs look more similar in row A, when growth
                                to decrease the effects of logarithmic scales on                rates are still climbing, than they do in row B,
                                judgment but did not eliminate them (see Note C).               when the rates have stabilized or decreased. To
                                                                                                test whether the actual pattern of the outbreak
                                                                                                could moderate the effects documented in this
                                General Discussion                                              article, we ran a simple conceptual replication of
                                In the studies reported in this article, we found               the growth and danger components of Study 2,
                                consistent evidence that the public formed less                 only this time we chose countries whose growth
                                accurate impressions of the current COVID-19                    rates fit one of the two patterns in Figure 6. We

8                                                                                                                    behavioral science & policy
Figure 6. Total confirmed cases of COVID-19 in the United States by the
number of days since 100 cases were confirmed

Note. These graphs illustrate why using a logarithmic scale to plot the growth of cases can lead the public to make lower
predictions of growth than a linear-scale plot would. In row A, from early in the pandemic, both graphs give the visual
impression that cases are rising, but the linear scale (right) more intuitively conveys that the rise is becoming steeper at day 15.
In row B, from later in the pandemic, the linear scale (right) again gives the impression of a rise, but the logarithmic scale now
appears to be starting to curve downward—which could lead some people to mistakenly assume that the number of cases is
about to decline. In both cases, the logarithmic scale may lead to lower judgments of growth.

found that the logarithmic axes led to underes-                     should depend on the needs of the audience.
timation of growth and threat regardless of the                     Comparing growth rates between countries
slope of the actual growth.                                         is certainly important for epidemiologists and
                                                                    politicians who are in a position to implement
In two of our studies, we conducted added                           and evaluate high-level policies. For them,
analyses to examine whether various indi-                           logarithmic graphs may be most useful. For
vidual differences could influence the degree to                    the general public, however, graphs should be
which people struggle with logarithmic graphs.                      designed to enable viewers to accurately esti-
Surprisingly, we found that greater objective                       mate risk and respond accordingly. Indeed,
facility for working with numbers did not result                    when we ran a test in which we expected loga-
in increased accuracy in interpreting logarithmic                   rithmic graphs might result in laypeople making
scales relative to linear scales. In fact, some-                    more accurate interpretations of growth rates,
times more numerate individuals fared worse                         we found that the claimed advantages of loga-
than others who were less numerate. (See the                        rithmic graphs did not appear: for a lay audience,
Supplemental Material for a fuller discussion of                    logarithmic graphs did not improve accuracy of
the individual differences we measured.)                            comparative growth judgments. (See Appendix
                                                                    Study 2 in the Supplemental Material.)
The skeptical reader may argue that we stacked
the deck against logarithmic scales by concen-
trating on their influence on impressions of                        Recommendations
risk and danger rather than on their value for                      Overall, our findings make it clear that officials’
highlighting differences in the growth rates of                     and media’s decision to use either logarithmic
cases. We argue that the choice of graph type                       or linear axes in graphs can influence public

a publication of the behavioral science & policy association                                                                           9
response to COVID-19. When people are                end notes
     presented with graphs with logarithmic instead       A. Full materials, preregistrations, and data for the
     of linear axes, they make less accurate predic-         studies described in this article and for additional
     tions of future growth; view COVID-19 as less           studies we conducted can be found at https://osf.
     of a threat; and, accordingly, are less supportive      io/zqut5/?view_only=3aa66d592dd2495ca508b4f
     of governmental and individual action against           a8729381a.

     COVID-19. Education can reduce these effects,        B. In Study 1A, actual case counts were derived from
     but it cannot eliminate them.                           the COVID-19 data repository maintained by the
                                                             Center for Systems Science and Engineering at
                                                             John Hopkins University, available from https://
     Logarithmic graphs still have significant value
                                                             github.com/CSSEGISandData/COVID-19.
     for presenting scientific data. However, on the
     basis of our research, we recommend using            C. It is conceivable that people who judge COVID-19
                                                             to pose a low degree of danger on the basis of
     them for the general public only when they are
                                                             seeing logarithmic graphs are more accurate
     truly the most reasonable option. And when
                                                             in their threat assessment than are people who
     they are used, presenters should spend signifi-
                                                             view the same data on linear graphs. We do not
     cant time explaining how to read the graphs and         think that they are more accurate, however. When
     should supplement the logarithmic graphs with           people are taught how to read logarithmic graphs,
     linear displays of the data.                            their sense of danger does not fall; rather, it rises.

                                                          supplemental material
                                                          • http://behavioralpolicy.org/publications

                                                          • Methods & Analysis

                                                          author affiliation
                                                          Ryan & Evers: University of California, Berkeley.
                                                          Corresponding author’s e-mail: wryan@
                                                          berkeley.edu.

10                                                                               behavioral science & policy
references
1. Chapman, G. B., & Coups, E. J.                9. Zacks, J., & Tversky, B. (1999).
   (2006). Emotions and preventive                  Bars and lines: A study of graphic
   health behavior: Worry, regret,                  communication. Memory & Cognition,
   and influenza vaccination. Health                27, 1073–1079. https://doi.org/10.3758/
   Psychology, 25, 82–90. https://doi.              bf03201236
   org/10.1037/0278-6133.25.1.82                10. Beattie, V., & Jones, M. J. (2002).
2. Lipkus, I. M., Samsa, G., & Rimer,               The impact of graph slope on rate of
   B. K. (2001). General performance                change judgments in corporate reports.
   on a numeracy scale among highly                 Abacus, 38, 177–199. https://doi.
   educated samples. Medical Decision               org/10.1111/1467-6281.00104
   Making, 21, 37–44. https://doi.              11. Cleveland, W. S., & McGill, R. (1987).
   org/10.1177/0272989x0102100105                   Graphical perception: The visual
3. Wagenaar, W. A., & Sagaria, S. D.                decoding of quantitative information
   (1975). Misperception of exponential             on graphical displays of data. Journal
   growth. Perception & Psychophysics,              of the Royal Statistical Society,
   18, 416–422. https://doi.org/10.3758/            Series A, 150, 192–229. https://doi.
   bf03204114                                       org/10.2307/2981473
4. Wagenaar, W. A., & Timmers, H.               12. Fischer, H., Schütte, S., Depoux, A.,
   (1979). The pond-and-duckweed                    Amelung, D., & Sauerborn, R. (2018).
   problem; Three experiments on the                How well do COP22 attendees
   misperception of exponential growth.             understand graphs on climate change
   Acta Psychologica, 43, 239–251. https://         health impacts from the fifth IPCC
   doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(79)90028-3             assessment report? International
                                                    Journal of Environmental Research and
5. Schwartz, L. M., Woloshin, S., Black,
                                                    Public Health, 15, Article 875. https://
   C., & Welch, H. G. (1997). The role of
                                                    doi.org/10.3390/ijerph15050875
   numeracy in understanding the benefit
   of screening mammography. Annals of          13. Beshear, A. [Governor Andy
   Internal Medicine, 127, 966–972. https://        Beshear]. (2020, April 5). Update on
   doi.org/10.7326/0003-4819-127-11-                COVID-19 in Kentucky—4.5.2020
   199712010-00003                                  [Video file]. Retrieved from
                                                    https://www.youtube.com/
6. Zikmund-Fisher, B. J., Smith, D. M.,
                                                    watch?v=gSzUuOTzGE8&feature=youtu.
   Ubel, P. A., & Fagerlin, A. (2007).
                                                    be&t=1595
   Validation of the Subjective Numeracy
   Scale: Effects of low numeracy on            14. Inslee, J. [GovInslee]. (2020, March 26).
   comprehension of risk communications             GovInslee was live [Video file]. Retrieved
   and utility elicitations. Medical Decision       from https://twitter.com/GovInslee/
   Making, 27, 663–671. https://doi.                status/1243237794895413249
   org/10.1177/0272989x07303824                 15. Yan, W. (2020, April 14). Coronavirus
7. Peters, E., Hibbard, J., Slovic, P., &           tests science’s need for speed limits.
   Dieckmann, N. (2007). Numeracy                   The New York Times. Retrieved from
   skill and the communication,                     https://www.nytimes.com/2020/04/14/
   comprehension, and use of risk-benefit           science/coronavirus-disinformation.
   information. Health Affairs, 26, 741–748.        html
   https://doi.org/10.1377/hlthaff.26.3.741     16. Wagner, S. H., & Goffin, R. D. (1997).
8. Frederick, S. (2005). Cognitive                  Differences in accuracy of absolute and
  reflection and decision making.                   comparative performance appraisal
  Journal of Economic Perspectives,                 methods. Organizational Behavior and
  19(4), 25–42. https://doi.                        Human Decision Processes, 70, 95–103.
  org/10.1257/089533005775196732                    https://doi.org/10.1006/obhd.1997.2698

a publication of the behavioral science & policy association                                     11
You can also read