Contextual Supports and Barriers to Career Choice: A Social Cognitive Analysis

Page created by Darryl Cross
 
CONTINUE READING
Journal of Counseling Psychology                                                             Copyright 2000 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
2000, Vol. 47, No. 1,36-49                                                                         0022-0167/00/S5.00 DO1: 10.1037//0022-0167.47.1.36

                        Contextual Supports and Barriers to Career Choice:
                                   A Social Cognitive Analysis
                           Robert W.Lent                                                           Steven D. Brown
                        University of Maryland                                                 Loyola University Chicago

                                                                 Gail Hackett
                                                            Arizona State University

                           Social cognitive career theory (SCCT; R. W. Lent, S. D. Brown, & G. Hackett, 1994)
                           emphasizes cognitive-person variables that enable people to influence their own career
                           development, as well as extra-person (e.g., contextual) variables that enhance or constrain
                           personal agency. Although the theory has yielded a steady stream of inquiry and practical
                           applications, relatively little of this work has examined SCCT's contextual variables or
                           hypotheses. In this article, several avenues for stimulating study of the contextual aspects of
                           career behavior are considered. In particular, the authors (a) examine "career barriers," a
                           conceptually relevant construct, from the perspective of SCCT; (b) advocate study of
                           contextual supports as well as barriers; and (c) propose additional context-focused research
                           and practice directions derived from SCCT.

   Social cognitive career theory (SCCT) represents a rela-                   conceptual and measurement status of the barriers construct,
tively new effort to understand the processes through which                   and have discussed ways in which SCCT may be used as a
people form interests, make choices, and achieve varying                      framework for studying and modifying barrier effects (Al-
levels of success in educational and occupational pursuits                    bert & Luzzo, 1999; Swanson, Daniels, & Tokar, 1996;
(Lent, Brown, & Hackett, 1994). Anchored in Bandura's                         Swanson & Woitke, 1997). In this article, we consider
(1986) general social cognitive theory, SCCT focuses on                       SCCT's account of environmental effects in light of research
several cognitive-person variables (e.g., self-efficacy, out-                 on career barriers, and attempt to build stronger linkages
come expectations, and goals), and on how these variables                     between these two areas of inquiry. We begin by summariz-
interact with other aspects of the person and his or her                      ing SCCT's conceptual statements about the interplay among
environment (e.g., gender, ethnicity, social supports, and                    environmental and person variables in the career choice
barriers) to help shape the course of career development.                     process. We then briefly critique the recent literature on
Although the theory has stimulated much research and                          career barriers from the vantage point of social cognitive
practical activity (e.g., see Brown & Lent, 1996; Lent,                       theory. Finally, we suggest several areas in which SCCT's
Brown, & Hackett, 1996; Swanson & Gore, in press), most                       analysis of environmental effects may be clarified and
of this work has focused on SCCT's cognitive-person                           elaborated. Our critique and conceptual refinements are
variables alone, in isolation from important environmental                    intended to stimulate further context-sensitive research and
(e.g., social, cultural, and economic) variables that are                     intervention efforts derived from SCCT.
assumed to influence both the cognitive-person variables
and other aspects of career behavior.
   Recent research on barriers to career development, or                               SCCT View of Environmental Effects
career barriers, is clearly relevant to SCCT's environmental
hypotheses. Reviews of this literature have examined the                         Lent et al. (1994) partitioned SCCT into two complemen-
                                                                              tary levels of theoretical analysis. The first level presented
                                                                              cognitive-person variables (self-efficacy, outcome expecta-
  Robert W. Lent, Department of Counseling and Personnel                      tions, personal goals) that enable people to exercise agency
Services, University of Maryland; Steven D. Brown, Department of              (i.e., personal control) within their own career development.
Leadership, Foundations, and Counseling Psychology, Loyola                    The second level of analysis considered the paths through
University Chicago; Gail Hackett, Division of Psychology in                   which several additional sets of variables—such as physical
Education, Arizona State University.                                          attributes (e.g., sex and race), features of the environment,
  An earlier version of this article was presented as part of the             and particular learning experiences—influence career-
symposium, "Social Cognitive Theory's Extension to Counseling                 related interests and choice behavior. Drawing on general
Psychology: Three Domains" (L. M. Larson, Chair), at the 106th                social cognitive theory assumptions (Bandura, 1986), Lent
Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association,
San Francisco, August 18,1998.                                                et al. (1994) hypothesized that person, environment, and
  Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to               behavior variables affect one another through complex,
Robert W. Lent, Department of Counseling and Personnel Services,              reciprocal linkages. In this section, we briefly revisit SCCT's
University of Maryland, College Park, Maryland 20742. Electronic              conceptualization of environmental variables, highlighting
mail may be sent to RL95@umail.umd.edu.                                       their subjective versus objective features, their temporal

                                                                         36
SOCIAL COGNITIVE CAREER THEORY                                                            37
nature, and their presumed causal paths relative to career                        A focus on the perceived environment presents several
behavior.                                                                      theoretical and practical challenges. For instance, on the one
                                                                               hand, such a focus seems necessary to account for individual
                                                                               differences in response to similar environmental conditions.
Objective and Perceived Aspects of the Environment                             Yet, on the other hand, if taken too far, assumptions about
    According to SCCT, career development is influenced                        contextual effects existing (only) in people's minds can lead
both by objective and perceived environmental factors.                         to attributions that blame the victim (or, conversely, credit
Examples of objective factors include the quality of the                       the beneficiary) of received environmental conditions. Many
educational experiences to which one has been exposed and                      people have encountered persons who achieved great career
the financial support available to one for pursuing particular                 and life successes despite the environmental odds against
training options. Such objective factors can potently affect                   them; similarly, there are many stories of people who have
one's career development, whether or not one specifically                      failed in life's pursuits despite having every seeming environ-
apprehends their influence. However, the effect of a particu-                  mental advantage. If environmental conditions like material
lar objective factor often depends at least partly on the                      wealth were the only important consideration, all poor kids
manner in which the individual appraises and responds to it                    would fail and all rich ones would succeed. Yet obviously
(Vondracek, Lerner, & Schulenberg, 1986). Hence, in SCCT                       life is not so simple. Career development theorists, there-
people are not seen as mere passive repositories of past or                    fore, need to consider multiple, potentially compensatory
present environmental influences. They can certainly be                        aspects of the objective environment—such as economic
affected adversely or beneficially by events that are beyond                   conditions, parental behaviors, and peer influences (cf.
their control or awareness; yet, how individuals construe the                  Arbona, in press)—as well as how individuals make sense
environment and themselves also affords the potential for                      of, and respond to, what their environment provides.
personal agency in one's career development.
    SCCTs conceptualization of the subjective psychological                    Distal and Contemporary Environmental Influences
environment is adapted from Astin's (1984) notion of the
perceived "opportunity structure" and from Vondracek et                           In addition to the objective and perceived aspects of the
al.'s (1986) "contextual affordance" construct. Both the                       environment, SCCT highlights the temporal period during
Astin and Vondracek et al. positions emphasize that the                        which particular environmental influences occur. For concep-
opportunities, resources, barriers, or affordances presented                   tual convenience, environmental variables are divided into
by a particular environmental variable may be subject to                       two basic categories according to their relative proximity to
individual interpretation. Thus, these positions suggest that                  the career choice-making process. The first category (shown
it is important to attend to the person's active phenomenologi-                at the lower left part of Figure 1) contains distal, background
cal role in processing both positive and negative environmen-                  contextual factors that affect the learning experiences through
tal influences. (We will hereafter use the terms environmen-                   which career-relevant self-efficacy and outcome expecta-
tal and contextual interchangeably to refer to career-relevant                 tions develop. Examples include the types of career role
influences that exist, or are perceived to exist, in the milieu                models to which one is exposed and the sort of support or
surrounding the person.)                                                       discouragement one receives for engaging in particular

                                                                                                      Contextual Influences
                                                                                                   Proximal to Choice Behavior

                 Person Inputs
                                                                       Self-efficacy                 I                  I
                - Predispositions                                      Expectations    f ^          I                   1
                - Gender                                                                                                1
                - Race/ethnicity
                - Disability/                                      /                                                               i

                   Health status
                                            Learning
                                                                  /                      1
                                                                                                    i                  •
                                            Experiences
                                                              \
                                                                                       Interests            Goals
                                                                                                                      -**        Actions

               Background                                              \   .
               Contextual                                               Outcome
               Affordances                                             Expectations

Figure I. Model of social cognitive influences on career choice behavior. Note that dotted paths indicate moderator effects on interest-goal
and goal-action relations. From "Toward a Unifying Social Cognitive Theory of Career and Academic Interest, Choice, and Performance"
[Monograph], by R. W. Lent, S. D. Brown, and G. Hackett, 1994, Journal of Vocational Behavior, 45, p. 93. Copyright 1994 by R. W. Lent,
S. D. Brown, and G. Hackett. Reprinted with permission.
38                                                       LENT, BROWN, AND HACKETT

academic or extracurricular activities. The second, proximal,         influential others may hold sway over the individual's own
category of contextual influences is particularly important           personal career preferences. In a recent test of this hypoth-
during active phases of educational or career decision                esis within a sample of Asian American college students,
making (see the upper right part of Figure 1). Examples of            two contextual variables (family involvement and accultura-
proximal influences include the adequacy of one's informal            tion) and self-efficacy were stronger predictors of an index
career contacts or exposure to discriminatory hiring prac-            of career choice than were personal interests (Tang, Fouad,
tices. Figure 2 summarizes the primary ways in which distal           & Smith, 1999). In individualistic cultures as well, career
versus proximal contextual factors (or affordances) are               interests or goals often need to be subjugated to economic or
hypothesized to affect the career choice process.                     other environmental presses. Thus, SCCT posits that, when
                                                                      confronted by such presses, an individual's choice behavior
Direct and Moderating Effects                                         may be guided less by personal interests than by other
of Environmental Variables                                            environmental and person factors (e.g., availability of accept-
                                                                      able if nonideal options, coupled with self-efficacy and
   According to SCCT, proximal environmental variables                outcome expectations related to these options).
can moderate and directly affect the processes by which
people make and implement career-relevant choices. Specifi-                Career Barriers: A Complementary Construct
cally, proximal (and anticipated) contextual factors may
moderate the relations of (a) interests to choice goals, and             Unfortunately, owing partly to a lack of theory-derived
(b) goals to actions. As shown in Figure 1, one's primary             measures for assessing contextual factors, SCCT's environ-
interests are likely to prompt corresponding goals (e.g.,             mental hypotheses have generally received limited inquiry
social interests lead to intentions to pursue a social-type           to this point. However, career barriers, a construct that is
career); goals, in turn, promote choice-relevant actions (e.g.,       conceptually related to SCCT, has received a surge of
applying for a training program related to one's goal).               renewed interest among researchers in recent years (see
However, contextual influences help determine how these               Albert & Luzzo, 1999; McWhirter, Torres, & Rasheed,
processes unfold. It is posited that people are less likely to        1998; Swanson et al., 1996; Swanson & Woitke, 1997). In
translate their career interests into goals, and their goals into     this section, we examine the recent career barriers literature
actions, when they perceive their efforts to be impeded by            from the perspective of SCCT, considering ways in which
adverse environmental factors (e.g., insurmountable barriers          these two lines of inquiry may be strengthened through their
or inadequate support systems). Conversely, the perception            closer linkage.
of beneficial environmental factors (e.g., ample support, few            Swanson and her colleagues have documented the evolu-
barriers) is predicted to facilitate the process of translating       tion of research on career-related barriers, noting that this
one's interests into goals and goals into actions.                    construct emerged largely from the literature on women's
   In addition to their moderating effect on the choice               career development (Swanson et al., 1996; Swanson &
process, contextual factors may assert a direct influence on          Woitke, 1997). In particular, barriers were seen as a mecha-
choice making or implementation. For example, particularly            nism for explaining the restriction of women's career
in collectivist cultures and subcultures, the wishes of               aspirations and the oft-noted gaps between their abilities and

                           Choice-Distal
                           Contextual Affordances                               Career-relevant
                                                                                Learning Experiences
                           e.g., opportunity for skill
                           development, range of
                           available role models

                           Choice-Proximal                                     Moderates Interest-Goal Relation
                           Contextual Affordances
                                                                               Moderates Goal-Action Relation
                           e.g., career network
                           contacts, external                                  Direct Effect on Goals and/or
                           barriers                                            Choice Actions

                  Figure 2, Hypothesized effects of distal versus proximal contextual factors (or affordances) on the
                  career choice process. Copyright 1998 by R. W. Lent, S. D. Brown, and G. Hackett. Printed with
                  permission.
SOCIAL COGNITIVE CAREER THEORY                                                   39
their achievements. The barrier construct has subsequently          conceptually, between the person (e.g., low self-efficacy)
been extended to the study of men's and racial-ethnic               and contextual (e.g., disapproval of significant others)
minority group members' career development. Although                factors that hamper career progress. Although person and
several theorists have considered the role of barriers in           contextual variables are seen as being in continual, recipro-
career development (e.g., Farmer, 1976) and research on             cal interplay over the course of an individual's career
career barriers has increased noticeably in recent years,           development (e.g., environmental conditions help to shape
Swanson et al. (1996) have suggested that inquiry on                self-efficacy beliefs, which, in turn, affect one's response to
barriers has been beset by two major problems:                      environmental challenges), this does not mean that person
                                                                    and contextual variables represent a single, monolithic
     (a) The barriers construct has lacked a firm theoretical
     framework into which research findings could be incorporated   source of influence.
     and from which subsequent research hypotheses could be            Efforts to distinguish person and contextual factors that
     derived, and (b) most of the empirical research has been       constrain career development may have several theoretical
     conducted with measures that have been idiosyncratic to the    and practical benefits, such as helping to (a) clarify the
     investigators' particular studies, (p. 220)
                                                                    processes through which contextual barriers, such as bias in
Our reading of this literature led us to concur with Swanson        opportunities for skill development, become internalized;
et al.'s assessment, and we attempt to build on their               (b) suggest novel counseling and developmental strategies
thoughtful critique of barriers research by highlighting a          for coping with, or compensating for, environmentally
select group of topics that may engender refinements in the         imposed barriers; and (c) identify differing intervention
conceptualization and assessment of career barriers.                targets and roles for counselors (e.g., social advocacy,
                                                                    system-level change), depending on the person or contextual
                                                                    location at which a given negative influence is seen as
Barriers as Intrapersonal Versus                                    occurring. In SCCT, barriers generally refers to negative
Environmental Impediments                                           contextual influences, with the understanding that contextual
   Swanson and Woitke (1997) defined barriers as "events or         barriers are often functionally related to, yet conceptually
conditions, either within the person or in his or her               distinct from, detrimental person factors (e.g., adverse
environment, that make career progress difficult" (p. 434).         learning conditions can diminish self-efficacy).
This definition derives from the intuitively appealing notion
that barriers represent both intrapersonal (e.g., self-concept)     Barriers as Generalized Versus
and environmental (e.g., workplace discrimination) factors
that hinder career development (Crites, 1969). Most career
                                                                    Task-Specific Variables
barrier researchers have implicitly adopted this omnibus               Research on career barriers has frequently sidestepped the
definition and, consequently, have often treated intraper-          somewhat subtle, but crucial question: Barriers to what?
sonal and contextual barriers as conceptually equivalent            That is, what is a given barrier deterring an individual from
(e.g., Luzzo, 1993; McWhirter, 1997). In addition, career           doing? Obviously, barriers may impede virtually any aspect
development researchers have typically studied the effects          of career progress (the term used in Swanson & Woitke's,
of perceived, as opposed to objectively defined, barriers (cf.      1997, definition of career barriers). Yet, for theoretical and
Swanson & Woitke, 1997).                                            practical reasons, it seems important to consider barriers in
   One justification for classifying all types of adverse           relation to the more specific developmental tasks that
events and conditions—intrapersonal and contextual—as               comprise career progress, such as career choice formulation,
career barriers is that such influences often occur together        choice implementation, or career advancement.
and can be intimately intertwined (Swanson & Tokar, 1991a;             Although certain barriers may be generalized or pervasive
Swanson & Woitke, 1997). For example, research partici-             (e.g., negative family influences), the presence and effects of
pants often list a combination of person and contextual             most barriers are likely to depend both on the developmental
hindrances when asked to indicate the career barriers they          task facing the individual and on the specific choice options
have encountered (e.g., Lent et al., 1998), and researchers'        he or she is entertaining. Consider a hypothetical college
efforts to classify particular participant-generated barriers       student who chooses to major in art history, and promptly
into internal versus external categories can require a bit of       discovers that his or her parents vehemently oppose this
interpretation (Swanson & Tokar, 1991a; Swanson & Woitke,           choice, threatening to remove their monetary support. The
1997). Swanson and Woitke (1997) have noted that external           social and financial barriers with which this student must
or environmental barriers are particularly difficult to classify    contend are not generalized to all aspects of his or her career
"primarily because the locus of the barrier often could reside      progress, but rather are linked to a particular developmental
either in the person or in the environment" (p. 433).               task (major choice) and choice option (art history).
   Although the decision to combine both person and                    Swanson and Tokar (1991a) found that college students
contextual hindrances under the larger rubric of career             listed somewhat different barriers, depending on the nature
barriers is understandable, we believe that such a nondiffer-       of the developmental task (e.g., "choosing a major or
entiated view of barriers may muddy the conceptual waters,          career" versus "getting the first job"). Nevertheless, most
obscuring the potentially differing paths through which             barriers research, using either open-ended questions (e.g.,
different factors impede career development. From the               Luzzo, 1993) or structured questionnaires (Swanson &
perspective of SCCT, it is advantageous to distinguish,             Tokar, 1991b), has not specified either the developmental
40                                                      LENT, BROWN, AND HACKETT

task or the content of the option that is being thwarted (e.g.,              Career barriers research has generally explored barriers at
students are asked to list barriers they have overcome or                 differing points along the temporal continuum. For example,
those they expect to encounter in the future in relation to               Swanson and Tokar's (1991a) thought-listing study pre-
their career development generally). This omission may be                 sented participants with several different scenarios (e.g.,
based on implicit uniformity and trait assumptions about                  barriers to "choosing a major or career," to "getting the first
perceived barriers—namely, that barriers are generic and                  job," and to "advancing in career"), which, for their
ever-present, transcending particular choice domains and                  undergraduate participants, represented present and future
developmental considerations (e.g., an individual would                   career tasks. Luzzo's (1993, 1995, 1996; Luzzo & Hutche-
experience the same barrier regardless of the developmental               son, 1996) thought-listing task asked students to list sepa-
tasks or choice options he or she faces).                                 rately the past barriers they have encountered and the future
   In its current form, SCCT deals primarily with contextual              barriers they expect to face. McWhirter (1997) and her
variables related to making (formulating) and implementing                colleagues (McWhirter, Hackett, & Bandalos, 1998;
(pursuing) career choices (Lent et al., 1994). These phases of            McWhirter & Luzzo, 1996) assessed the barriers that high
the choice process are distinguished, in part, because                    school students anticipated in relation to future jobs and
different environmental conditions may become salient in                  college attendance.
formulating a career goal than in actually pursuing the goal.
For example, goal pursuit may expose the individual to                       These studies have assessed barriers within somewhat
monetary problems or discrimination that he or she had not                differing time frames, although the emphasis has been on
anticipated when setting the goal. Moreover, barriers are                 relatively nonspecific beliefs about future barriers. For
assumed to be domain and context specific. Because most                   example, the Career Barriers Inventory (CBI) has partici-
adults either must work or choose to do so, barriers do not               pants rate the degree to which a number of potential barriers
usually prevent their pursuit of all options. Rather, certain              "would hinder your career progress," without specifying
barriers typically constrain pursuit of some courses of action            when the barriers might be encountered (although the
but allow for the pursuit of other options or back-up plans. It           approximate time frame is implicit in some items; Swanson
may, therefore, be useful to consider such questions as, what             & Daniels, 1995). By contrast, there has been relatively little
types of barriers, encountered by which persons and at                    effort to study either the distal contextual barriers that
which stage of the choice process, will have what kinds of                precede and help shape interests and initial career choices, or
impact? Although admittedly complex, this view reflects the               the effects of proximal barriers on the pursuit of chosen
considerable variation that exists with respect to whether,               options (e.g., Schaefers, Epperson, & Nauta, 1997)—yet,
how, and when particular barriers are experienced, and with               both of these temporal locations are central to SCCTs
what effects.                                                             environmental hypotheses. The study of distal and proximal
                                                                          barriers might usefully be approached from several method-
                                                                          ological directions, such as qualitative interviews and longi-
Locating Barriers Along a Temporal Dimension                              tudinal research that tracks the emergence of particular
   Along with an effort to "contextualize" barriers (e.g., to             barriers, and people's response to them, over time. In a later
consider the specific phase of the choice process and the                 section, we provide some examples of recent studies of
nature of the options facing the individual), a social cogni-             proximal barriers.
tive analysis would also "temporalize" barriers by differen-
tiating between barriers encountered in the past, those
hampering the person in the present, and those anticipated in             Barrier Perceptions and Related Constructs
the future. As we noted previously, Lent et al.'s (1994) initial             Just what is being measured by the various structured
theoretical statement divided contextual factors into two                 inventories (McWhirter, 1997; Swanson etal., 1996), thought-
broad categories, based on their relative proximity to active             listing tasks (Luzzo, 1993; Swanson & Tokar, 1991a), and
career choice-making:                                                     interviews (Lent et al., 1998) that have been used to assess
     (a) more distal, background influences that precede and help         career barriers? The answer to this question bears impor-
     shape interests and self-cognitions (e.g., differential opportuni-   tantly on the construct validity of career barriers and has
     ties for task and role model exposure; emotional and financial       important implications for theories, such as SCCT, within
     support for engaging in particular activities; cultural and
     gender role socialization processes), and (b) proximal influ-        which the barrier construct resides.
     ences that come into play at critical choice junctures (e.g.,           At first blush, the above question may appear naive.
     personal career network contacts; structural barriers, such as       Barriers measures, regardless of their specific format, seem
     discriminatory hiring practices.) (p. 107)
                                                                          to have obvious face validity. They generally ask research
Within this scheme, historically distal versus contemporary               participants to indicate the barriers they have experienced
(or proximal) contextual factors serve somewhat differing                 (Luzzo, 1993) or to rate the potential impact of various
functions. The distal factors affect the learning experiences             adverse conditions on their future career behavior (Swanson
through which personal interests and other influences on                  et al., 1996). In responding to such tasks, one might suspect
career choice are forged, whereas choice-proximal factors                 that research participants simply supply their perceptions of
help form the opportunity structure within which career                   past or future barriers. However, given that barrier percep-
plans are made and implemented.                                           tions represent, at least in part, people's phenomenological
SOCIAL COGNITIVE CAREER THEORY                                                  41
constructions of reality, there is potential for such percep-         The emergence of separate coping efficacy measures is a
tions to be affected by certain qualities of the perceiver. In     welcome sign, from the perspective of SCCT. Such mea-
essence, barrier perception measures can engage beliefs            sures can be used to address research questions that are both
about the self or environment that extend beyond the mere          theoretically and practically important. For example, are
presence or absence of particular barriers. We, therefore,         barriers less likely to be perceived—or, when perceived, are
briefly consider career barriers vis-a-vis a few other poten-      they less likely to be harmful to choice behavior—under
tially relevant theoretical constructs: coping efficacy, dispo-    conditions of high versus low coping efficacy? Later, we
sitional affect, and outcome expectations.                         suggest some additional theory-derived research questions
   Coping efficacy. In general, career barrier assessment          involving coping efficacy that warrant empirical scrutiny.
devices may confound two conceptually distinct constructs:            Dispositional affect. Another variable that needs to be
barrier perceptions and coping efficacy. Barrier perceptions       distinguished from barrier perceptions is dispositional affect.
have been operationally defined by inventories that ask            In outlining SCCT, Lent et al. (1994) suggested that
participants to rate the likelihood that they will encounter
                                                                   tendencies to experience negative or positive affect might
certain conditions that most people would find as aversive
                                                                   influence the way in which people process efficacy-relevant
(McWhirter, 1997) or by having them rate how much these
                                                                   information. For example, those inclined toward high nega-
conditions would hinder their career progress, should the
                                                                   tive affect may tend to discount their success experiences
conditions actually materialize (Swanson et al., 1996). It is
possible that people's responses to both of these rating tasks,    and, thereby, fail to profit from what would ordinarily be an
and especially the latter, are influenced by their sense of        efficacy-enhancing event. Likewise, it is possible that affec-
coping efficacy, that is, beliefs about their ability to manage    tive tendencies may color perceptions of environmental
or negotiate the obstacles that appear on the inventories (cf.     conditions. For example, high negative-affect experiences
Bandura, 1986, 1997; Hackett & Byars, 1996; Lent et al.,           may be likely to perceive more barriers and fewer supports
1994). Swanson et al. (1996) have also considered this             than do those who experience low negative affect or high
conceptual overlap between coping efficacy and barrier             positive affect. Dispositional affect could also conceivably
hindrance ratings.                                                 influence appraisals of coping efficacy, for example, with
   To clarify, it is unlikely that people have "immaculate         high negative affect leading to diminished beliefs about
conceptions" about barriers in their environments. Instead,        one's ability to cope with particular barriers. Although
their prospective expectations about encountering particular       speculative, such possibilities suggest the value of studying
hurdles are likely to reflect, in part, barriers they have         dispositional affect both in relation to barrier perceptions
personally experienced, those they have learned about              and coping efficacy.
vicariously, and beliefs about whether they could cope                Outcome expectations. In their theoretical discussion of
successfully with these hurdles. Presumably, if one per-           career barriers, Swanson et al. (1996) astutely noted that
ceives oneself as being able to cope effectively with a given      SCCT is unclear about how barriers differ from outcome
event or environmental condition, one would be less likely         expectations. The latter refers to personal beliefs about the
to define it as a barrier. Hence, asking how much a particular     consequences of performing particular behaviors ("if I do
barrier would hinder or disrupt one's career progress may be       this, what will happen?"). In an example intended to
confounding perceptions of the barrier with the individual's       illustrate the differing effects of self-efficacy and outcome
confidence in his or her ability to cope with it. Although this    expectations, Lent et al. (1994) had suggested that a person
confound is apparent in the CBFs "hindrance" format                with high self-efficacy for mathematics might choose to
(Swanson et al., 1996), it is also likely to be a consideration,   avoid science-intensive career fields "if she or he anticipates
albeit more subtle, in barrier "likelihood" formats                negative outcomes (e.g., non-support of significant others,
(McWhirter, 1997) or, for that matter, in thought-listing or       work/family conflict) to attend such options" (p. 84).
interview assessments (e.g., Luzzo, 1993; Lent et al., 1998).      Swanson et al. (1996) observed that the examples of
   This critique suggests that it may be difficult to disen-       negative outcome expectations we had used resembled
tangle barrier perceptions from coping efficacy beliefs. One
                                                                   certain barrier scales on the CBI. They further suggested that
possible solution is to develop separate measures of coping
                                                                   our example implies that "barriers either influence one's
efficacy, for use along with barrier perception measures.
                                                                   outcome expectations or may be considered as synonymous
Examining the effects of barrier perceptions on career choice
                                                                   with outcome expectations" (p. 239).
or implementation, while controlling for coping efficacy,
might offer a clearer understanding of barrier perception-            We think that Swanson et al.'s (1996) argument is very
criterion relations. It is noteworthy that McWhirter (1997)        well-taken. In essence, there is a need to clarify the nature of
developed a generalized measure of coping efficacy as part         the relationship between barriers and outcome expectations.
of her Perception of Barriers instrument, finding that this        Are they merely different names for the same latent con-
measure was negatively related to barrier perceptions.             struct? Or are they different, with one influencing the other?
McWhirter and Luzzo (1996) also developed a Coping With            We return to these questions in a later section, but offer a
Barriers (coping efficacy) scale. However, research has yet        brief preview of our response here: Barriers may usefully be
to examine the potentially complex relations among barrier         conceived and operationalized as a particular form of
perceptions and coping efficacy beliefs relative to particular     outcome expectation related to one's perception of the
choice outcomes (cf. Albert & Luzzo, 1999).                        environment.
42                                               LENT, BROWN, AND HACKETT

Career Supports: A Missing Environmental Ingredient              (e.g., identifying role models or funding sources in one's
                                                                 existing support system), or (b) to shift from, or alter, their
   From an SCCT perspective, one class of variables has          environments in order to access currently unavailable re-
received far less study than it deserves within research on      sources (e.g., developing new peer support systems). This
contextual influences on career behavior: environmentally        change in perspective, from deficits (barriers) to assets
supportive conditions or resources. Supports or support          (supports), is consistent with the long-held intervention
systems are conceived within SCCT as environmental               philosophy of counseling psychology (Super, 1955).
variables that can facilitate the formation and pursuit of          Study of contextual supports would add a few new
individuals' career choices. Such supportive or enabling         theoretical conundrums, such as the question of how sup-
environmental conditions have long been recognized in the        ports and barriers interrelate. Betz's (1989) important trea-
career development literature (e.g., Tinsley & Faunce,           tise on the "null environment" implies that support is not
1980), but have not often captured sustained research            simply the absence of barriers, or leaving the individual
attention.                                                       alone. In other words, support is not a neutral condition;
   Several recent studies have examined the role of per-         rather, it involves factors that actively promote career
ceived support variables relative to a variety of career and     behavior. Thus, one might infer that supports and barriers
academic outcomes. For example, rural adolescents' percep-       represent largely unique constructs. For example, an indi-
tions of parental support for pursuing certain Holland theme     vidual may recognize distinct barriers (e.g., family disap-
fields were predictive of their interest, self-efficacy, and     proval, limited savings) and supports (e.g., peer approval,
valuing of these fields (Lapan, Hinkelman, Adams, &              access to scholarship funding) relative to a given choice
Turner, in press). Perceived support from fathers was found      option. Alternatively, one might contend that support and
to relate to the educational plans and career expectations of    barrier perceptions are inversely related or reflect opposite
Mexican American high school girls (McWhirter, Hackett,          poles on a positive-negative continuum.
et al., 1998). Faculty support or encouragement has been
                                                                    Although there is little research in the career literature
associated with engineering students' academic performance
                                                                 addressing this issue, one recent interview study found that
(Hackett, Betz, Casas, & Rocha-Singh, 1992) and persis-
                                                                 college students listed somewhat different, yet overlapping
tence (Schaefers et al., 1997). Supportive influences from
                                                                 categories of supportive and hindering influences on their
teachers, parents, and friends were found to relate to
                                                                 choice behavior (Lent et al., 1998). For example, 68% of
self-reported positive academic experiences in high school
                                                                 respondents listed financial concerns as a barrier, whereas
(Fisher & Stafford, 1999). Several qualitative-interview
                                                                 20% listed financial resources as a support. Access to social
studies have also explored career support dimensions in
                                                                 support and to role models were reported as supportive
adult workers (Blustein, Phillips, Jobin-Davis, Finkelberg,
                                                                 influences by 87% and 33%, respectively, of the sample.
& Roarke, 1997; Richie et al., 1997).
                                                                 Absence of support or role models were not specifically
   This important group of studies notwithstanding, it is        mentioned as barriers, but 42% of participants cited negative
understandable that contextual career support mechanisms         social or family influences as a hindering factor. Clearly,
have been underexplored relative to barriers. Interest in        more research is needed on the dimensionality of, and nature
barriers stemmed largely from a desire to explain factors that   of the relationship between, perceived supports and barriers.
stymie women's career development. Because the issues
were framed in terms of roadblocks to the pursuit of
particular career options or to the realization of personal      Prevalence and Impact of Barrier Perceptions
potential, it made sense to focus on barriers. Yet such a
barrier-focused purview may have also constricted research          The study of career barriers raises at least two fundamen-
on contextual effects. If one is interested in restoring         tal questions: Do people, in fact, perceive particular barriers
previously blocked or discarded options, it also seems           to their career progress? Assuming that they do, to what
essential to study those aspects of the environment—and of       extent does the presence of these barriers relate to, or afreet,
the individual's appraisal of, and response to, the environ-     their choice behavior or other important outcomes?
ment—that can facilitate career choice and development.             Our reading of this small but growing literature leads us to
Hence, complementing the focus on barriers, one might pose       concur with the assertion that "college students clearly do
such questions as, what contextual conditions support            perceive the existence of barriers" (Swanson & Woitke,
women's choice of nontraditional careers? What conditions        1997, p. 441). Likewise, high school students (males and
enable members of particular racial-ethnic minority groups       females of different racial-ethnic groups) have been found
to pursue certain major or career options in the face of         to perceive barriers to their future college attendance and
deterring conditions?                                            work lives (McWhirter, 1997; McWhirter, Hackett, et al.,
   Answers to such questions may have useful implications        1998), young adults have reported barriers in making the
for counseling and preventive interventions. For example,        transition from school to work (Blustein et al., 1997), and
although a focus on barriers suggests the design of barrier-     highly achieving women have cited barriers to their career
coping strategies (Brown & Lent, 1996), a spotlight on           progress (Richie et al., 1997). Such barrier perceptions are
facilitative conditions would suggest complementary, sup-        apparent across diverse assessment formats, such as thought-
port-enhancing efforts, such as helping individuals (a) to       listing (Luzzo, 1993, 1995, 1996; Swanson & Tokar, 1991a),
marshall the contextual assets that are available to them        interviews (Lent et al., 1998), and ratings on structured
SOCIAL COGNITIVE CAREER THEORY                                                   43

barriers inventories (McWhirter, 1997; Swanson et ai.,              development. This interpretation, derived from attribution
1996).                                                              theory (Weiner, 1986), recalls our earlier discussion of
   The evidence suggests that various groups do perceive            coping efficacy. Indeed, there appear to be some conceptual
career barriers; however, it also raises some questions about       similarities between the internal-controllable attributional
the prevalence and magnitude of barrier effects, at least in        style and coping efficacy—though a key difference is that
high school and college students. Swanson and Woitke                the former is viewed as traitlike, whereas the latter is seen as
(1997) noted that there are substantial individual differences      a context-specific construct.
in how barriers are perceived. Indeed, qualitative assessment
methods reveal that certain barriers (e.g., financial) are
reported by many persons, whereas others are perceived by           Summary
relatively few (e.g.. Lent et al., 1998; Luzzo, 1993; Swanson
& Tokar, 1991a). Such methods also suggest that there is               The foregoing review of the career barriers literature
considerable variation in how many total barriers partici-          presents a mixed picture. On the one hand, students and
pants list, although some findings suggest that most partici-       workers do perceive barriers to their career progress; but on
pants report relatively few barriers. For example, Luzzo and        the other hand, barrier ratings are often found to be
Hutcheson (1996) found that most of their participants listed       somewhat modest in size and have not been shown to be
fewer than two past or anticipated barriers.                        consistently related to important career outcome or process
                                                                    variables in the mostly student samples in which they have
   Research employing structured barriers questionnaires            been studied. This is not to say that barriers are not
suggests that although some students use the full scale range,      consequential for many Individuals. Indeed, they are likely
obtained mean ratings are generally fairly modest, suggest-         to be especially salient for those who have been victimized
ing that most participants view most potential barriers as          by various forms of oppression. However, several concep-
either not terribly likely to be encountered or to pose a threat,   tual and methodological issues—such as the manner in
if encountered (McWhirter, 1997; Swanson etal., 1996). Not          which barriers have been defined, failure to consider the
surprisingly, racial-ethnic and male-female differences have        context and temporal specificity of barriers, noncorrespon-
emerged with respect to the rating of certain barriers (e.g.,       dence between barriers and outcome criteria (in terms of
sex and racial discrimination; see Swanson et al., 1996) or in      content and developmental task), infrequent attention to
total numbers of barriers perceived (McWhirter & Luzzo,             mediating and moderating variables, understudy of nonstu-
1996). However, the magnitude of intergroup differences             dent samples, and use of designs that do not explore
has frequently been smaller than expected, and differences          potential causal effects of barriers—may have partly ob-
have not always been in the expected direction (McWhirter,          scured the impact of perceived barriers and the mechanisms
 1997; McWhirter & Luzzo, 1996; Swanson et al., 1996).              through which they affect career behavior.
   Studies exploring the relations of perceived barriers to            Despite these considerations, recent career barriers re-
other career variables have produced somewhat mixed                 search has constructed an important conceptual and method-
findings. For example, Swanson et al. (1996) reported that          ological foundation for further inquiry, and the program-
CBI scales were relatively unrelated to variables such as           matic research of Swanson, McWhirter, and Luzzo and their
career indecision and vocational identity in college students,      colleagues has been particularly pivotal. In the remainder of
although there was some evidence that gender moderated              this article, we attempt to build on this foundation, offering a
CBI-criterion relations (e.g., higher CBI-indecision correla-       modest set of suggestions for future research on career
tions were found for men than for women). McWhirter,                barriers and their conceptual partner, career supports. These
Hackett, et al. (1998) found that barrier perceptions did not       suggestions are premised on the need to clarify or elaborate
explain significant variance in high school students' educa-        certain aspects of SCCT having relevance for the conceptu-
tional plans or career expectations. Other studies have found       alization and assessment of contextual variables.
few or minimal statistical relations between career barriers
and measures of career attitudes (Luzzo, 1996; Luzzo &
Hutcheson, 1996) or job satisfaction (Blustein et al., 1997).
However, Luzzo (1996, 1998) found significant, negative                  Extending SCCT's View of the Environment
relations between future barrier perceptions and career                Citing a "lack of clarity and cohesiveness" in previous
decision-making self-efficacy.                                      conceptual and empirical approaches to career barriers,
   Luzzo and Hutcheson (1996) also reported that barrier-           Swanson et al. (1996) asserted that future barrier research
criterion relations may be moderated by certain attributional       might profit from "the application of an appropriate theoreti-
style variables: students who saw career decision-making as         cal framework" (p. 221). In particular, they suggested that a
an externally caused and uncontrollable process exhibited           theory-based approach might help to (a) identify "back-
significant, negative correlations between future barrier           ground factors and mechanisms" affecting the development
perceptions and career maturity; however, those with an             of barrier perceptions, (b) offer hypotheses about barrier
internal, controllable attributional style did not show such a      effects, and (c) organize and integrate research findings.
pattern. The authors suggested that persons who feel in             Swanson and Woitke (1997) also noted that a theoretical
control of career decisional tasks are more likely to believe       framework could assist in the design of barrier-coping
they can surmount occupational barriers; hence, their percep-       interventions. SCCT was cited as a "particularly promising
tion of barriers is less likely to be disruptive to their career    model for understanding career-related barriers" by Swan-
44                                                LENT, BROWN, AND HACKETT

son et al. (1996, p. 221), who sketched several ways in            choices, typically realize that long-term payoffs may entail
which the theory may be adapted to the study of barriers.          short-term sacrifices. Envisioning the career choice process
   We agree with Swanson et al.'s (1996; Swanson &                 as a path leading to a given destination, it may be useful to
Woitke, 1997) points about the value in taking a theory-           distinguish between (a) the distal future outcomes that
based approach to the study of barriers, and are pleased that      people expect to receive upon attaining a particular career
they saw SCCT as relevant to this challenge. At the same           (i.e., the ultimate payoffs that presumably help orient them
time, their commentary and our own critique of the barriers        toward this destination to begin with) and (b) the more
literature suggests that several aspects of SCCT are ripe for      proximal outcomes or conditions that they expect to encoun-
theoretical clarification or expansion. In this section, we        ter in their pursuit of this option (i.e., the hurdles and
offer some recent thoughts about how SCCT might be                 supports that line the path toward their destination).
further extended to the study of contextual effects on career         Beliefs about proximal outcomes comprise a specific type
choice behavior. In particular, we (a) revisit the relation of     of outcome expectation that might be termed "process
barriers and supports to outcome expectations, (b) consider        expectations" because they include the sorts of supports and
various ecological layers within which individuals' career         barriers that people envision encountering while in the
behavior is embedded, (c) suggest several possible roles for       process of pursuing a particular course of action (i.e.,
coping efficacy relative to barrier perceptions, and (d)           proximal to the career decision-making process). These
ponder the phenomenological lenses through which people            process expectations are, one might expect, related to yet
interpret particular environmental elements as barriers or         distinct from the sort of larger payoffs (or distal outcome
supports.                                                          expectations) that incline people toward a particular goal.
                                                                   For example, a person might wish to become a physician
                                                                   partly because he or she is attracted by the prestige and
Process Expectations and the Proximal Environment                  opportunity to help others that this option is perceived as
                                                                   offering. These anticipated payoffs constitute ultimate, distal
   Earlier, we discussed Swanson et al.'s (1996) observation
                                                                   outcome expectations. However, the individual's willing-
about the conceptual overlap between perceived barriers and
                                                                   ness to embark on this path will, presumably, also be
outcome expectations. In particular, they had noted that
                                                                   affected by the contextual conditions that he or she expects
barriers seem to represent negative outcome expectations
                                                                   to encounter enroute (e.g., "will I have enough financial or
(e.g., a person's beliefs about receiving adverse outcomes
                                                                   family support to make it through medical school?").
from the environment contingent on performing particular
career-related actions). Indeed, the apparent relationship            We think this proximal-process versus distal-outcome
between barriers and outcome expectations deserves further         distinction is potentially important for several reasons. In
comment, which we hope may clarify the nature of the               particular, it acknowledges that people do not simply pursue
outcome expectations construct and offer some useful impli-        a career because of its ultimately foreseen payoffs. Rather,
cations for conceptualizing and assessing barrier (and sup-        they also consider the conditions they are likely to face in its
port) perceptions.                                                 pursuit (e.g., "medical school will be impossibly hard on my
   Bandura (1986) distinguished several classes of outcome         relationships and my bank account"), among other impor-
expectations, such as the anticipation that certain physical       tant factors, like self-efficacy regarding the occupation's
(e.g., monetary), social (e.g., approval of significant others),   requisite skills. Thus, two individuals with similar distal
or self-evaluative (e.g., self-satisfaction) outcomes will         outcome expectations regarding a career in medicine may
follow particular actions. These expected positive outcomes        hold quite different process expectations about the proximal
operate as potent motivators that, along with other variables      barriers and supports they would encounter in pursuing this
(e.g., self-efficacy), help to determine whether people will       goal.
undertake certain actions. Naturally, however, not all out-           Another reason for identifying process expectations as a
come expectations are positive. People may well expect             particular form of outcome expectation is that they offer a
that pursuit of a given course of action will produce either       unique way to conceptualize and assess the psychological
neutral or negative outcomes, making them less likely to           environment surrounding the career decision-making pro-
choose such activities. Outcome expectations may thus be           cess. That is, temporally proximal supports and barriers can
classified along several dimensions, such as their valence         be recast as process expectations about what will happen
(positivity vs. negativity), locus (self-administrated versus      socially, financially, and so forth, in the near term if one
other-administered outcomes), or relative importance to the        chooses to pursue a particular career path. This recasting
individual.                                                        will, hopefully, help to clarify the conceptual relations
   For the purposes of clarifying SCCT's conception of             between perceived barriers and outcome expectations (cf.
barriers, it may also be useful to add consideration of a          Swanson et al., 1996) and also suggest some new research
temporal dimension along which outcome expectations may            questions, such as whether proximal-process expectations
vary. In particular, outcome expectations generally reflect        are empirically distinct from distal-outcome expectations.
beliefs about a future state of affairs linked to one's taking        We are currently studying these process expectations to
certain prospective actions—but some consequences may be           see if they, in fact, are different from distal outcome
expected over the short run (i.e., proximally), whereas others     expectations and how they fare as indexes of the proximal
may be anticipated in the more distal future. Adolescents and      environment in tests of SCCT's contextual hypotheses. In an
adults facing complex life decisions, such as career-related       initial qualitative study, Lent et al. (1998) found that
SOCIAL COGNITIVE CAREER THEORY                                                 45
participants often cited factors such as anticipated working
conditions and reinforcers (i.e., distal-outcome expecta-
tions) as reasons for selecting a particular career option; they
also cited the absence of such favorable conditions, or the
presence of negative ones, as bases for rejecting options they
had previously considered. However, when asked about
what had supported and hindered pursuit of their chosen                                     Immediate,
option (i.e., proximal-process supports and barriers), they                              Proximal Context
generally mentioned such factors as their current financial
conditions and the responses of persons in their immediate
social support systems.
   In another recent study, Lent et al. (1999) administered
structured measures of outcome and process expectations to
college students. These measures were linked to beliefs
about the outcomes (near term versus long term) that could
accrue from majoring in a math or science-related field. The
outcome expectations measure included positively and nega-
tively valenced distal outcomes, such as, "getting a degree
in a math or science-related field would allow me to earn a
good salary." The process expectations measure contained
positive/support and negative/barrier contextual conditions
that students expected to encounter while in college (e.g.,
"receive negative comments or discouragement about your            Figure 3. A concentric model of environmental layers that
major from family members"). Preliminary findings indi-            surround the person and form the context for his or her career
cate that the outcome and process variables represent              behavior. Copyright 1998 by R. W. Lent, S. D. Brown, and G.
relatively distinct sets of expectations. In combination with      Hackett. Printed with permission.
the findings of our qualitative study, these results also
suggest, albeit tentatively, that distal-outcome expectations
and proximal barrier/support perceptions may play some-            consisting of only two surrounding environmental layers;
what different roles in the career choice process.                 more complex models contain additional ecological struc-
                                                                   tures, such as microsystems, mesosystems, exosystems, and
   Parenthetically, it seems important to note that not all        macrosystems (Vondracek et al., 1986).
process expectations relate to the environment: Just as               Individuals are invariably affected by aspects of the
anticipated self-evaluation (e.g., self-satisfaction accruing      objective and perceived larger environment (i.e., the societal
from the ultimate achievement of one's goal) constitutes a         layer). For example, they observe the demographic features
key type of distal-outcome expectation, self-evaluative            of people who are employed in different occupations and
process expectations may also be an influential source of          learn vicariously about other people's experiences with
motivation, apart from the barriers and supports one imag-         particular barriers. However, individuals are likely to differ-
ines encountering in the environment. For instance, an             entiate beliefs about whether certain barriers exist in society
individual may expect to derive a great deal of self-              generally from their beliefs about how such barriers will
satisfaction from attaining proximal subgoals that lead to         affect the self, should they be encountered directly. It is one
one's ultimate goal (e.g., being accepted to medical school        thing to know, for example, that racism exists "out there"
enroute to becoming a physician) or from performing well at        and another to anticipate how one will deal with it personally
short-range milestones, such as particular courses. Such           (K. A. Gainor, personal communication, July 29,1998).
intermediate, self-evaluative expectations help to sustain
one along the arduous path toward one's long-term goals               Certain features of the inner layer of the environment
(e.g., career entry).                                              (e.g., one's immediate circle of significant others, interac-
                                                                   tions with mentors) may both serve as a filter that distills
                                                                   perceptions of structural barriers in the larger environment
A Concentric Model of Environmental Influences                     and a source of information about how one might cope with
                                                                   such barriers. For example, a young Black woman may
   Another potentially useful way to conceive of the environ-      perceive that racial and gender bias pose formidable ob-
ment is as a series of embedded layers, or concentric circles      stacles to Black women's pursuit of careers in engineering,
(see Figure 3). The person can be envisioned as residing in        yet her access to potent role models, adequate financial
the innermost circle, surrounded by his or her immediate           resources, and significant others who share her dream may
environment (e.g., family, friends, financial condition),          help her to persist despite the expectation of encountering
which is, in turn, encircled by the larger societal context        such bias.
(e.g., institutionalized racism and macroeconomic condi-              This potential buffering (or hampering) role of the inner
tions). This conception of people as embedded within a             environmental layer relative to career choice behavior is
multilayered environment draws on developmental-contex-            speculative but deserves empirical scrutiny. In Lent et al.'s
tualist models. Figure 3 portrays a simplified version             (1998) interview study, participants were most likely to cite
You can also read