Change in Juvenile Offending Versatility Predicted by Individual, Familial, and Environmental Risks

Page created by Lester Davidson
 
CONTINUE READING
DOI: 10.11576/ijcv-4559
                                                                                                  IJCV: Vol. 15/2021

Change in Juvenile Offending Versatility Predicted by
Individual, Familial, and Environmental Risks
Susanne Wallner
Institute of Psychology, Friedrich-Alexander University Erlangen-Nuremberg, Germany
susanne.wallner@fau.de
Hoben Thomas
Department of Psychology, Pennsylvania State University, USA
hxt@psu.edu
Mark Stemmler
Institute of Psychology, Friedrich-Alexander University Erlangen-Nuremberg, Germany
mark.stemmler@fau.de

Vol. 15/2021
The IJCV             provides a forum for scientific exchange and public dissemination of up-to-date scien-
                     tific knowledge on conflict and violence. The IJCV is independent, peer reviewed, open
                     access, and included in the Social Sciences Citation Index (SSCI) as well as other rele-
                     vant databases (e.g., SCOPUS, EBSCO, ProQuest, DNB).
                     The topics on which we concentrate—conflict and violence—have always been central
                     to various disciplines. Consequently, the journal encompasses contributions from a
                     wide range of disciplines, including criminology, economics, education, ethnology, his-
                     tory, political science, psychology, social anthropology, sociology, the study of reli-
                     gions, and urban studies.
                     All articles are gathered in yearly volumes, identified by a DOI with article-wise pagi-
                     nation.
                     For more information please visit www.ijcv.org
Suggested Citation: APA: Wallner, S., Thomas, H., Stemmler, M. (2021). Change in Juvenile Offending Ver-
                    satility Predicted by Individual, Familial, and Environmental Risks. International Jour-
                    nal of Conflict and Violence, 15, 1-16. doi:10.11576/ijcv-4559
                    Harvard: Wallner, Susanne, Thomas, Hoben, Stemmler, Mark. 2021. Change in Juvenile
                    Offending Versatility Predicted by Individual, Familial, and Environmental Risks. Inter-
                    national Journal of Conflict and Violence 15: 1-16. doi: 10.11576/ijcv-4559

                This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution—NoDerivatives License.
                ISSN: 1864–1385
IJCV: Vol. 15/2021
Wallner, Thomas, Stemmler: Change in Juvenile Offending Versatility Predicted by Individual, Familial, and En-          1
vironmental Risks

Change in Juvenile Offending Versatility Predicted by
Individual, Familial, and Environmental Risks
Susanne Wallner
Institute of Psychology, Friedrich-Alexander University Erlangen-Nuremberg, Germany
Hoben Thomas
Department of Psychology, Pennsylvania State University, USA
Mark Stemmler
Institute of Psychology, Friedrich-Alexander University Erlangen-Nuremberg, Germany

Developmental and life-course criminology elucidate the developmental course and change of antisociality over
time, considering that longitudinal trajectories differ. Specific relations between risks and different antisociality
outcomes are emphasized. We assume that adolescents have different longitudinal trajectories considering the
change of offending over time and that risks contribute variably to offending pathways. The current study is
based on a German research project in which adolescents (N = 577) were interviewed in two German cities.
Based on self-reported crime data, we utilized the slope values of offending versatility (OV) over time as out -
come values in regression mixture models capturing the trends for participants over age and exhibiting two com -
ponents of offending adolescents. We explored the contribution of different risks to OV, defining specific risk
patterns: Acceptance of violence and peer delinquency have significant negative effects on the emergence of OV
within the group of adolescents with decreasing OV. Acceptance of violence has a significant negative effect, and
corporal punishment has a significant positive effect on the emergence of OV within the group of adolescents
with increasing or rather stable OV. The results underline the relevance of the violence-related risk factor cor-
poral punishment for the emergence of OV within the last-mentioned group.

Keywords: juvenile delinquency, developmental risks, longitudinal research, developmental criminology, regres-
sion mixture models

Acknowledgement: This paper includes results from the study “Chances and Risks in the Life Course” (CURL;
“The Emergence and Development of Deviant and Delinquent Behavior over the Life Course and its Significance
for Processes of Social Inequality”; for example, Reinecke et al. 2013; Reinecke, Stemmler, and Wittenberg 2016;
Wallner et al. 2019). The study (research project A2) was part of the Collaborative Research Center (SFB) 882
“From Heterogeneities to Inequalities”, which was established at Bielefeld University, Germany, in 2011 and
which was funded by the German Research Foundation (DFG) until 2016. The authors are grateful to Jost Rei -
necke, Maria Arnis, Nihad El-Kayed, Eva Link, Julia Meinert, Andreas Pöge, Debbie Schepers, Burcu Uysal,
Maren Weiss, and Jochen Wittenberg for their valuable work on the project. Additionally, the authors gratefully
acknowledge the cooperation of the schools and students participating in the CURL study

Developmental and life-course criminology considers          course persistent vs. adolescence limited antisociality
the change of antisociality over time relating to the        (Moffitt 1993, 2006) has been developed to integrate
developmental course (for example, see Farrington, Pi-       sociological and psycho-biological theories and to ex-
quero, and Jennings 2013; Sampson and Laub 2016).            plain the age-crime curve: “The taxonomy achieved its
Accordingly, Moffitt’s well-known taxonomy of life-          original goal, to account for the age-crime curve. Its

                                                                                                                 ijcv.org
IJCV: Vol. 15/2021
Wallner, Thomas, Stemmler: Change in Juvenile Offending Versatility Predicted by Individual, Familial, and En-            2
vironmental Risks

dual theories of LCP [life-course persistent] and AL           According to Jennings and Reingle (2012), trajectory
[adolescence limited] development met their original         research should also focus on the identification of risk
goal of drawing from biological, psychological and so-       and protective factors for differentiating developmen-
ciological theories to explain antisocial behaviour”         tal trajectories, raising, for example, the following
(Moffitt 2018, 184). The age-crime curve represents          question: “[D]o the same risk and protective factors
“one of the brute facts of criminology” (Hirschi and         that distinguish non-offenders from life-course persis-
Gottfredson 1983, p. 552) displaying the relations be-       tent offenders also distinguish adolescent-limited of-
tween crime and age and the change of delinquent             fenders from life-course persistent offenders, or does
behavior over time (concerning the lifetime of individ-      this same set of risk and protective factors have a dif-
uals). According to this, the prevalence of criminal be-     ferential effect across trajectory groups?” (486). Over-
havior clearly increases from late childhood to adoles-      all, empirical studies in developmental and life-course
cence and finally decreases in early adulthood (see          criminology show the importance of numerous risk
Moffitt 1993; Farrington, Piquero, and Jennings 2013).       factors – such as individual characteristics, familial
Incidentally, the peak occurs earlier in self-report of-     characteristics, environmental characteristics – in the
fending data (including dark figures) than in regis-         explanation of antisocial behavior in general (for ex-
tered data (official records/reported crime in official      ample, see Corrado 2002, 2012; Craig et al. 2017; Far-
crime statistics; for example, see Boers and Reinecke        rington, Ttofi, and Piquero 2016; Stemmler, Wallner,
2007; Loeber et al. 2015). The age-crime curve also          and Link 2018; Thornberry et al. 2012; Wallner et al.
shows that many children and adolescents remain              2018). Related bio-psycho-social risk models corre-
trouble-free. Generally, the relations between age and       spond with the life-course-persistent path proposed
crime in youth and early adulthood can be explained          by Moffitt (see above). The bio-psycho-social risk
by co-occurring sociological, psychological, and bio-        model of Lösel and Bender (2003) focuses on the rela-
logical changes (for example, see Sweeten, Piquero,          tions between different risks, for example, poor child-
and Steinberg 2013). Longitudinal trajectories for anti-     rearing and low self-control in childhood, deviant
social outcomes are quite different, especially in child-    peers and beliefs in adolescence, and serious and vio-
hood and adolescence. Empirical criminological litera-       lent criminality in early adulthood. Of course, other
ture reveals no consistent number of groups of antiso-       risks are also considered and many other risk combi-
ciality. For example, according to Broidy et al. (2003),     nations are possible. Concentrating on risk/needs
three to four different physical aggression trajectories     management issues, Corrado (2002, 2012) summarizes
in childhood were most prevalent. Similarly, Jennings        the risks for serious and violent antisocial behavior,
and Reingle (2012) found that studies examining vio-         considering environmental, individual, family, inter-
lence, aggression, and delinquency trajectories mostly       vention, and externalizing behavior contents and risk
reported three to four trajectory groups, where the          domains, respectively (see also Stemmler, Wallner, and
number of groups generally ranged from two to seven.         Link 2018; Wallner et al. 2018). Specifically, concerning
The number and shape of trajectory groups seem to            different types or trajectories of delinquent behavior,
be quite variable (for example, depending on the sam-        the relevance of risks varies, defining specific risk
ple composition, length of follow-up, and geographi-         patterns (in other words, risks are different for differ-
cal location; ibid.). The studies, however, are in accor-    ent developmental pathways): individual and family
dance with Moffitt’s taxonomy and comprise a                 risks (for example, concerning personality, child-rear-
chronic or life-course persistent group (chronics), a        ing practices) play a particularly important role in life-
group of escalators or adolescent-limited offenders          course persistent antisociality, whereas predictors of
(desistors), and a group without problem behavior            adolescence limited antisociality (for example, prob-
(ibid.). The vast majority of empirical studies examin-      lematic peer contacts, peer delinquency) are mainly
ing longitudinal data on antisocial trajectories apply       related to the contemporary maturity gap in adoles-
General Growth Mixture Models (GGMM; for exam-               cence (for example, Moffitt 1993; see also Odgers et al.
ple, see Nagin 1999; Stemmler and Lösel 2015).               2008). Further, life-course persistent antisociality

                                                                                                                 ijcv.org
IJCV: Vol. 15/2021
Wallner, Thomas, Stemmler: Change in Juvenile Offending Versatility Predicted by Individual, Familial, and En-           3
vironmental Risks

seems to be associated with versatile and more seri-          measure. Generally, antisociality captures deviant and
ous offending, whereas adolescence limited antisocial-        delinquent behaviors (for example, Wittenberg and
ity is associated with less serious, more youth-typical       Wallner 2016). However, studies suggest that diverse
offending (for example, see Moffitt 1993 and Odgers et        antisocial trajectories are mostly defined by different
al. 2008; see also Elliott et al. 2017). Concerning antiso-   risks; correspondingly, “[r]esearch has begun to
cial conduct problems, Odgers et al. (2008) approve           demonstrate that there are a number of correlates of
the use of variety scores, which are “highly correlated       offending … and other adverse outcomes … that can
with frequency scores … and [that] are commonly               be considered alongside violence, aggression, and
used in population-based studies” (ibid., 676). Antiso-       delinquency as manifesting a similar developmental
cial behavior in adolescence is often influenced by ad-       process that can largely be explained by a shared sim-
verse peer contacts (for example, see Battin et al. 1998;     ilarity in risk and protective factors” (Jennings and
Lösel and Bender 2003). A high rate of peer delin-            Reingle 2012, 486). More recently, recapitulating previ-
quency (offending in groups) and a high rate of co-of-        ous evidence, Gutman, Joshi, and Schoon (2019)
fending are typical characteristics of juvenile delin-        stated that “different influences and processes may
quency (Wallner and Weiss 2019). Côté et al. (2006)           explain diverse pathways of conduct problems” (ibid.,
examined the development of physical aggression in            p. 181).
childhood and identified three different developmen-            In the research presented here, we focus on the rela-
tal trajectories (low desisting, moderate desisting,          tions between different risk variables and antisocial
high stable), and found that, for example, (1) specific       outcomes in adolescence over time. Generally, refer-
risks distinguished between children with physical ag-        ring to the research described above, the age-crime
gression trajectories and other children, and (2) hos-        curve illustrates that the prevalence of crime increases
tile/ineffective parenting was predictively related to        from late childhood to adolescence and decreases in
the high level group trajectory. Consistent with previ-       early adulthood. In the current work, we focus on the
ous results, many studies defined specific factors pre-       critical developmental stage of adolescence. As al-
dicting several distinct trajectories of different antiso-    ready outlined, the associations between age and
cial outcomes (for example, see Corovic et al. 2017;          crime in adolescence and early adulthood can com-
Gutman, Joshi, and Schoon 2019; Lacourse, Dupéré,             monly be explained by co-occurring sociological, psy-
and Loeber 2008; Odgers et al. 2008; Seddig and Rei-          chological, and biological changes (see above). The
necke 2017, see also Reinecke and Seddig 2011). For           present work is primarily rooted in risk-factor re-
example, Lacourse, Dupéré, and Loeber (2008) defined          search. Specifically referring to the empirical findings
specific factors predicting distinct trajectories of anti-    outlined above, the following core hypotheses were
social outcomes (violence and theft) in a sample of           derived. First, adolescents have different longitudinal
young men. Seddig and Reinecke (2017) explored self-          trajectories considering the change in delinquent be-
reported delinquency trajectories, presented a solu-          havior over time (in other words, adolescents’ trends
tion with seven classes, and explained membership in          of offending versatility over age are varying in the
a special class of offenders utilizing patterns of spe-       course of adolescence). Prior trajectory research com-
cific variables (for example, peer group, attitudes; see      monly relied on GGMM (see above). In the present
also Reinecke and Seddig 2011). Corovic and col-              study (on the basis of our longitudinal crime data),
leagues (2017) recapitulated various risk factors in          however, we utilize the slope values of offending ver-
childhood and adolescence and adulthood adjustment            satility (see methods section for a definition) as out-
outcomes differentiating between specific pathways            come values in a mixture of regression routine (for ex-
of crime. However, these studies differ concerning the        ample, see Benaglia et al. 2009) capturing the trends
outcome measure, specific sample characteristics (for         for adolescents over time and, therefore, applying a
example, age), the number of antisocial pathways              method which is not common in current (criminologi-
found and other issues. For example, different facets         cal) trajectory research. Second, individual, familial,
of antisocial behavior are considered as the outcome          and environmental risks in adolescence have distinct

                                                                                                                 ijcv.org
IJCV: Vol. 15/2021
Wallner, Thomas, Stemmler: Change in Juvenile Offending Versatility Predicted by Individual, Familial, and En-          4
vironmental Risks

influences on specific delinquent behavior (different        ables the examination of participants’ development in
risks contribute variably to offending versatility path-     childhood and adolescence. One important aim of the
ways defining specific risk patterns). Therefore, we         current study is to investigate the relations among de-
predict different trajectories of offending versatility      viant and delinquent behavior and various meaningful
(the different slope values of offending versatility; see    precursors longitudinally. The project data is based on
methods section for a definition) based on different         self-reports of male and female students who com-
risks for antisociality, contributing more specific and      pleted several questionnaires. Participating students
detailed analyses on the prediction of divergent devel-      were interviewed once a year as part of school-based
opmental trajectories and, thus, estimating the rela-        and postal surveys (Weiss and Wallner 2019).
tions between different predictor effects and various
offending versatility trajectories (see results section).    1.2 Sample
Specifically, we assume that the developmental risks –       Participants were surveyed in the German cities of
acceptance of violence, impulsivity, peer delinquency,       Nuremberg and Dortmund with three annual follow-
and corporal punishment – are differently associated         up assessments since 2012, providing a large sample.
with distinct pathways of offending versatility. This        The Nuremberg sample comprises only students from
research therefore focuses on violence-related risks         lower-track schools, whereas the Dortmund sample is
(corporal punishment, acceptance of violence; see            composed of a broader range of school types. Impor-
methods section for justification). It should also be        tantly, we use the longitudinal sample (three-wave
noted that several violent offenses are considered for       panel) of male and female students of the older cohort
the outcome offending versatility (see also methods          (9th to 11th grade); the sample comprises only stu-
section for further details). Again, in a nutshell, this     dents participating in our study at the first, second,
work differs from prior work in trajectory research in       and third assessment points (t1, t2, t3; N = 577 adoles-
that it explores the application of a longitudinal           cents: n = 350 female and n = 227 male). The mean age
method for identifying different developmental path-         of the ninth-graders was about 15 years at t1. At t1,
ways that is unusual in this context and applies this        41.9 percent (n = 242) of the students attended a
method to validate these pathways, using juvenile            lower-track school. Overall, 53.6 percent (n = 309) of
risks relating to diverse developmental domains,             the sample have a migration background (t1). In the
hence, far exceeding the information provided by             current work, we apply to the broad definition of “mi-
mere description.                                            gration background“ used by the Federal Statistical
                                                             Office (Destatis, Germany; Statistisches Bundesamt
1 Methods                                                    2012): Accordingly, those persons have a migration
1.1 Research Project                                         background who immigrated to Germany after 1949
In line with the research project “Chances and Risks         as well as all persons born in Germany without Ger-
in the Life Course” (CURL; research project A2 “The          man citizenship and all persons born in Germany with
Emergence and Development of Deviant and Delin-              German citizenship with at least one parent who is an
quent Behavior over the Life Course and its Signifi-         immigrant or born in Germany without a German
cance for Processes of Social Inequality”; e.g., Rei-        passport. Generally, the definitions of migration back-
necke et al., 2013; Reinecke, Stemmler, and Wittenberg       ground differ. In any case, regarding the heterogeneity
2016; Wallner et al. 2019) the development of deviant        of the present sample, limitations regarding the inter-
and delinquent behavior is studied. This project is          pretability of the results have to be considered (see
part of the Collaborative Research Center (“Sonder-          discussion section). For further details of the sample
forschungsbereich”, SFB) “From Heterogeneities to In-        see Weiss and Wallner (2019). The mean t1 offending
equalities” (SFB 882), which was established at Biele-       versatility score of the remaining three-wave panel (M
feld University, Germany, in 2011 and was funded by          = 1.09, SD = 2.21) was only marginally lower than the
the German Research Foundation (DFG) until 2016.             mean t1 offending versatility score for the whole sam-
The study uses a cohort-sequential design that en-           ple (M = 1.27, SD = 2.38, N = 1417), providing some in-

                                                                                                                 ijcv.org
IJCV: Vol. 15/2021
Wallner, Thomas, Stemmler: Change in Juvenile Offending Versatility Predicted by Individual, Familial, and En-          5
vironmental Risks

formation on dropout issues. See Weiss and Link              cording to Arnis (2015), Cronbach’s alpha was satis-
(2019) for further information on panel mortality in         factory (α = .75; 9th grade).
the longitudinal study.                                        Acceptance of violence. We also assessed acceptance
                                                             of violence using a scale from the CrimoC study
1.3 Measures                                                 (Boers and Reinecke 2007, see also Boers and Rei-
The data basis of the present study is derived from          necke 2019 for further details on the CrimoC study;
the three-wave panel of the older cohort. First, several     see Dünkel and Geng 2003). According to Dünkel and
risk variables (i.e., individual, familial, and environ-     Geng (2003), self-reported violent behavior is substan-
mental risks; independent variables, predictors, t1),        tially positively related to acceptance of violence in
were used, also including violence-related risks. To         adolescence. The measure comprises nine items in a
justify the selection of risk variables, we refer to risk-   five-point rating format ranging from does not apply
factor research described above, particularly to our         at all through does definitely apply (for example,
own previous work on these topics (for example,              “When another person attacks me physically, then I
Stemmler, Wallner, and Link 2018; Wallner et al. 2018;       fight back”). High scale values represent high levels of
Wallner and Stemmler 2019). The selection applied            acceptance of violence. Further information is pro-
were relevance concerning serious and violent antiso-        vided by Meinert, Kaiser, and Guzy (2014). The corre-
cial behavior (for example, see Corrado 2002, 2012),         sponding alpha coefficient was satisfactory at α = .76
age-appropriateness, broad content (to include differ-       (9th grade; Arnis 2015).
ent risk domains, ibid.), and availability within our re-      Corporal punishment. To operationalize parenting
search project. Second, delinquency (offending versa-        practices including parenting deficits we utilized a
tility; dark figures, outcome measure, t1 – t3; see sec-     scale from the modified version of the Alabama Par-
tion “Offending versatility” below for justification),       enting Questionnaire (APQ; see Essau, Sasagawa, and
was utilized, also comprising several violent offences.      Frick 2006; Shelton, Frick, and Wootton 1996). In our
Further information on relevant measures, items, and         research project we utilized a modified self-report ver-
scales can be obtained from Arnis (2015) and Meinert,        sion for children and adolescents (see Meinert, Kaiser,
Kaiser, and Guzy (2014). Overall, we also utilized sev-      and Guzy 2014) which is based on a German parent
eral (partly modified) items and scales from the             version of Lösel et al. (2003). We used four items from
CrimoC study (Boers and Reinecke 2007, see also              the subscale Corporal Punishment. The items are an-
Boers and Reinecke 2019). Additional information on          swered in a five-point rating format ranging from
that study, particularly information related to trajec-      never through always. One example: “My parents hit
tory research, can be obtained from, for example, Rei-       me.” High scale values indicate high levels of corporal
necke and Seddig (2011) and Seddig and Reinecke              punishment by parents. The alpha coefficient was ex-
(2017).                                                      cellent (α = .89; 9th grade; Arnis 2015).
  Impulsivity. Self-ratings concerning low self-control        Peer delinquency. We assessed peer delinquency (i.e.,
were derived from the German version of the Gras-            precisely, the deviant and delinquent behavior of
mick Scale (Grasmick et al. 1993; German version: Ei-        peers) following the CrimoC study (for example,
fler and Seipel 2001). At t1, the scale consisted of ten     Boers and Reinecke 2007, see also Boers and Reinecke
items. Overall, items from the five-point subscales          2019 for more information on the CrimoC study) and
Risk Behavior, Impulsivity, Temper, and Simple Tasks         PADS+ (Peterborough Adolescent and Young Adult
were utilized ranging from strongly disagree through         Development Study; for example, Wikström et al.
strongly agree (see Meinert, Kaiser, and Guzy 2014).         2012). According to the PADS+ scale Peer Crime In-
One example: “When I am really angry, other people           volvement and specific CrimoC delinquency items, our
better stay away from me”. High scale values indicate        measure incorporates different deviant (drug use) and
low levels of self-control and high levels of impulsiv-      delinquent acts (for example, theft, break-in), respec-
ity. In the context of the present work, we use the          tively, committed by peers (see Meinert, Kaiser, and
short label “impulsivity” for this aggregate scale. Ac-      Guzy 2014). Participating students were asked how

                                                                                                                 ijcv.org
IJCV: Vol. 15/2021
Wallner, Thomas, Stemmler: Change in Juvenile Offending Versatility Predicted by Individual, Familial, and En-             6
vironmental Risks

often friends committed particular acts, for example,        criminal behavior – in contrast to, for example, the
“Does it often happen that some of your friends press        frequency of crime (for example, see Boman, Mowen,
somebody for money?”. Seven items capture the fre-           and Higgins 2019) or crime specialization (for exam-
quencies in a five-point rating format ranging from          ple, see Lussier et al. 2017; Tzoumakis et al. 2012).
never through very often. High scale values represent        Crime frequencies are widely utilized. However,
high levels of peer delinquency. The alpha coefficient       Bendixen, Endresen, and Olweus (2003, 135) suggest
was quite acceptable (i.e., α = .85; 9th grade; Arnis        that “using a scale including the (raw) frequencies of
2015).                                                       antisocial acts committed instead of a variety scale
   Offending versatility. Delinquency items were based       would result in reduced internal consistency, lower
on a modified version of the Self-Report on Delinquent       stability over time, smaller group differences and
(and Deviant) Behavior (“Delinquenzbelastungsskala”,         weaker associations with conceptually related vari-
DBS; Lösel 1975; available in Weiss, Runkel, and Lösel       ables. Similarly, in regression analyses the (raw) fre-
2012; for example, theft) and on the Delinquency Scale       quency scale contributed little to the explained vari-
(“Delinquenzskala”; CrimoC; for example, Boers and           ance in conceptually related variables over and be-
Reinecke 2007; for example, scratching; see also Boers       yond that contributed by the variety scale”. Therefore,
and Reinecke 2019 for further details on the CrimoC          we utilized a variety index which captures the diver-
study). The adolescents’ self-reports on criminal be-        sity of offending at each assessment (t1 – t3) and – in
havior include dark figures referring to unrecorded          some aspects – reveals the intensity of offending over
crime and providing other information than registered        time. Thus, offending versatility represents the num-
data. As mentioned above (see introduction), the peak        ber of different delinquent acts committed by an indi-
occurs earlier in self-report data on offending (includ-     vidual in the last year at each of the three time points;
ing dark figures) than in registered data (for example,      therefore, high scale values indicate high levels of of-
see Boers and Reinecke 2007; Loeber et al. 2015). We         fending versatility.
applied the epidemiological definitions of prevalence
and incidence: “Basically, prevalence (or prevalence         1.4 Strategy of Analyses: Capturing Trends in
rate) refers to the number of diseases or spells of dis-         Longitudinally Assessed Offending Versatility
ease existing at a particular point in time or within a          Responses
specified time period related to the total number of         Slope values of offending versatility over time. Initially,
persons exposed to risk (a population or a defined           self-reported crime data are considered utilizing the
group of people). Incidence (or incidence rate) on the       slope values of offending versatility over time. The of-
other hand measures the rate of appearance of new            fending versatility response is a count of the number
cases in the group or population, i.e., the number of        of adolescent-reported offenses during the past year
diseases/spells of disease beginning within a specified      (see methods section). It is easily interpreted, and less
period of time related to the total number of persons        likely influenced by memory or guessing. These re-
exposed to risk during that period” (Olweus 1989,            sponses take integer values from zero to fifteen in the
187). In the current work, delinquent acts refer to van-     first assessment and zero to nine in the third. Figure 1
dalism, property crime, violent crime, and drug deal-        displays the roughly L-shaped histograms of the of-
ing (Wallner and Weiss 2019; Wittenberg and Wallner          fending versatility measures at each of the three as-
2016). Nineteen different criminal acts were consid-         sessment points.
ered (for example, scratching, bicycle theft, robbery).        One central question is how to model these offend-
Partially, several delinquent acts refer to the same         ing versatility responses in a way that reflects
crime type, so that some categories are overrepre-           changes in offending versatility response trajectory
sented. The considered delinquency items varied con-         over the three assessment time points within some re-
cerning severity. We employed the versatility of crime       gression model. An answer is not obvious, partly be-
(i.e., [offending] versatility) in the past twelve months    cause repeated measurements data models often re-
focusing on the range, heterogeneity, and diversity of       quire full specification of the joint distribution of re-

                                                                                                                  ijcv.org
IJCV: Vol. 15/2021
Wallner, Thomas, Stemmler: Change in Juvenile Offending Versatility Predicted by Individual, Familial, and En-               7
vironmental Risks

Figure 1: Histograms of offending versatility responses

                                                                                Frequency
Frequency

                                       Frequency

              Versatility Time 1                        Versatility Time 2                       Versatility Time 3
Notes: Ordinates have been truncated; heights at zero are 354, 458, 484; vectors of mean and variance are (1.09,
4.36), (.48, 1.97), (.26, .67), times one to three respectively; n = 571; Versatility: offending versatility.

sponses and typically under distribution normality.          treated as continuous outcomes. A positive slope, yi >
These empirical facts preclude the plausible use of a        0, indicates increasing offending versatility over the
very large class of possible models including those          three years; a negative slope, yi < 0, indicates a de-
based on normal or multivariate normality.                   creasing offending versatility, and yi = 0 indicates no
  The approach selected is the following: Define a lin-      change.
ear slope response for each i by linearly regressing           The distribution of the yi slopes, the response vari-
each i’s three offending versatility integer scores          able, reveals large individual differences. Among the
against the time points designated as 1, 2, 3. For indi-     577 individuals, half (287) reported zero offenses at all
vidual i, a slope yi indicates i’s offending versatility     three times. Because of their consistency, these is are
trajectory over the three offending versatility re-          viewed as special and treated as such in the analyses
sponses. These slope values take on 19 discrete values       below. An i is an element in the set τ: i ∈ τ if and only
from -7 to 2.5, a sufficient number to hopefully be          if its offending versatility scores are (0; 0; 0), or “triple
considered continuous outcomes. Still, they will be          zeros” (with τ denoting triple or three; read ∈ as “in”

                                                                                                                    ijcv.org
IJCV: Vol. 15/2021
Wallner, Thomas, Stemmler: Change in Juvenile Offending Versatility Predicted by Individual, Familial, and En-               8
vironmental Risks

and ∉ as “not in”; τ is a set with elements individuals i    lowing, we refer to the corresponding Regression Mix-
with triple zeros, in other words, zero offending versa-     ture Model.
tility responses at all three assessment points). These i      There are i = 1, 2,…, n individuals, each with re-
∈ τ are regarded as following a zero variance mass           sponse yi arranged in a column vector y1,…, yn of n in-
point distribution (see below).                              dependent observations. Recall, yi is i’s slope. The as-
  There were 28 additional is with estimated slopes of       sociated covariates for each i are x1,…, xn. Each xi has
zero but not in τ. 174 individuals displayed negative        p predictors in a row vector p + 1 long, xi = (1, xi,1,
estimated slopes; only 26 had positive estimated             …,xi,p) with the first entry coded one for the intercept.
slopes, signaling an increase in offenses over time. The     The design matrix X has n rows and p + 1 columns.
remaining 62 = 577 – 287 – 28 – 174 – 26 individuals         Thus, there are n pairs of the form (yi, xi) with each i
had missing offending versatility values, denoted NA         assumed to belong to only one of m = 3 three regres-
for “not available” (R Core Team 2017). R software was       sion mixture components. The first component j = 0
used for all analyses (see below). Of the 62 individuals     denotes those i ∈ τ, while i ∉ τ were admissible for ei-
with offending versatility NA values, six had NA val-        ther of components j = 1, 2 in the normal mixture of
ues on two times. For all but one i, these NAs oc-           regressions to be defined explicitly momentarily (see
curred on the second or third time. Of 56 is with NA         below). To anticipate, using BIC (Schwarz 1978) the
on one time, most had identical offending versatility        data support no more than two normal components.
values on the other two times or a smaller value on          Following Schwarz, the model with the largest BIC is
the second time than on the first. This finding sug-         taken as the best model.
gested that it would be reasonable to impute for those         βj is a column vector of coefficients associated with
is with a single missing NA their smallest observed          components j = 1, 2. βj = (βj0, βj1, …, βjp), so there are p
values. Most of the imputed values were zero. Follow-        + 1 values of β for each βj, j = 1, 2.
ing imputation, contained 307 individuals or 54 per-           Let N (·|µ,σ²) denote a conditionally normal density
cent of the usable sample (.54 = 307/571) with 38 zero       function with mean µ and variance σ². (Let N (µ,σ²)
slopes i ∉ τ and a total of 264 i ∉ τ. In summary, fol-      denote a normal density unconditionally.) The regres-
lowing imputation 571 = 307 + 264 individuals had us-        sion mixture model is:
able slope response values. Consequently, just six of                                                                  (1)
577 individuals were excluded from all subsequent
                                                               I(i) = 1 if i ∉ τ, and zero otherwise. σj² are variances
analyses. Of course, some additional individuals were
                                                             of components j = 1,2; λj ≥ 0,
excluded if they had NA values on a predictive covari-
ate because NA responses are inadmissible in the mix-
ture of regressions in the R package mixtools (Be-           Thus, like conventional regression, model (1) is condi-
naglia et al. 2009), the source of all mixture routines      tioned on the observed predictors.
employed below.                                                                1, if i 
                                                              f ( yi | x i )                                         (2)
  So far our initial empirical results addressed the                           0, otherwise
common gap in research relating to the availability of       f (yi | xi) is a zero variance mass point at zero with
different methods in the identification of specific de-      height one. Note that f (yi | xi) does not depend on xi.
velopmental pathways (see introduction). The follow-         The proportion of the observations following f (yi | xi)
ing analyses employ the slope values of offending ver-       is λ0. Those i ∈ τ and consequently their yis receive no
satility to explain distinct juvenile pathways over          further analysis. It is worth noting that handling the
time.                                                        zeros in (2) is similar to a zero-inflated Poisson in pa-
  The Regression Mixture Model. Next, we use the             rameterization, a model often used in studies with
slope values of offending versatility (see above) as         many observed zero values such as Tzoumakis et al.
outcome values in a mixture of regression routine en-        (2012); in both, zeros are viewed as components of
compassing the trends over time. Therefore, in the fol-      mixture distributions.

                                                                                                                    ijcv.org
IJCV: Vol. 15/2021
Wallner, Thomas, Stemmler: Change in Juvenile Offending Versatility Predicted by Individual, Familial, and En-            9
vironmental Risks

  Intuitively, consider only those i ∉ τ, and consider         varying. The corresponding slope model described
fitting an ordinary multiple regression equation, as in        above models the growth trajectory of slopes assess-
conventional settings, except that it is hypothesized          ing three occasions over a time span of two years. In
that there is more than one regression equation ap-            addition to the component of non-offending adoles-
propriate for the data. The task is to decide with             cents, two components of offending adolescents were
which regression, j = 1, 2, for i ∉ τ the pair (yi, xi) is a   defined. Perhaps there are appropriate alternative ap-
partner. The solution “matches-up” the observations            proaches to the data modeling perspective employed
with their most probable regression equations via an           here. Growth mixture models might seem an obvious
iterative EM algorithm. Matching is achieved using a           candidate, however, these models make strong as-
Bayes Rule classifier. Rather than least squares, as in        sumptions, including distributional multivariate nor-
conventional regression, the approach is maximum               mality, assumptions often ignored. Consequently, the
likelihood under normality. If further details are re-         results can be misleading (Bauer and Curran 2003).
quired, there are general discussions of finite mixtures       Even univariate normality fails dramatically here as
and mixtures of regressions available in many sources          the discrete Figure 1 distributions reveal. The ap-
(for example, Benaglia et al. 2009; McLachlan and Peel         proach taken here is less susceptible to such concerns.
2000).
  The approach in the current setting is not quite             2 Results: Contribution of Risks to Offending
standard however, as there is one large component                   Versatility
modeling those i ∈ τ that must be considered. The              Finally, the contribution of different developmental
zero variance mass point distribution f (yi | xi), is quite    risks to offending versatility trajectories is examined,
non-normal in distribution. Consequently, this com-            displaying the predictive importance of these risks for
ponent is treated in an ancillary fashion. Thus, only          distinct components of offending versatility. The con-
those i ∉ τ pairs (yi, xi) are analyzed under the mix-         tribution of longitudinally assessed individual, famil-
tools call regmixEM. The λ1 and λ2 component weights           ial, and environmental juvenile risks to explain differ-
are then adjusted at the end, so that the three λ esti-        ent pathways is brought into focus – going beyond
mates sum to one.                                              the mere description of pathways. For brevity, we con-
  The distribution of point mass component at zero             sider the analysis under model (1).
f (yi | xi) seems easily defended. The assumed condi-            Let p = 4, with predictor variables acceptance of vio-
tional normality of components j = 1 and j = 2 in (1) is       lence, impulsivity, peer delinquency, and corporal
more difficult to defend rigorously, but it is hoped a         punishment, with values taken at the first time point
mixture of two normal distributions will hold at least         (see Table 1 for summary statistics for the variables).
approximately or at least not lead one astray. If the            A one-component (conventional) regression model
normal regression model holds, the slope distribution          and two-component mixture of regressions were ap-
is normal (for example, Freedman 2005), so this result         plied to the data. Model selection employed Schwarz’s
would give some hope for the appropriateness of the            (1978) Bayesian Information Criterion denoted as
component conditional normality assumption in (1).             BIC(LL; k; n), where LL is the loglikelihood, k the num-
  The regression mixture procedure provides esti-              ber of estimated parameters, and n sample size. The
mates for three λ, two σ² and 2(p+1) estimates of β.           model with the largest BIC is taken as the best-suited
The number of covariates p varies, which means the             model (taking the largest BIC following Schwarz). For
sample size n varies as well because as already noted,         the one- and two-component regression models BIC
only complete data are admissible under the reg-               (–346.21, 6, 235) = –362.39 and BIC(–316.86, 13, 235) =
mixEM routine.                                                 –351.46. Thus, the mixture model was selected. See
  Collectively, the adolescents participating in our           Table 2 for the corresponding parameter estimates.
study show different longitudinal trajectories consid-         The standard errors in parentheses were obtained us-
ering change of offending versatility, hence, adoles-          ing standard bootstrap observed data resampling pro-
cents’ trends of offending versatility over time are

                                                                                                                 ijcv.org
IJCV: Vol. 15/2021
Wallner, Thomas, Stemmler: Change in Juvenile Offending Versatility Predicted by Individual, Familial, and En-             10
vironmental Risks

cedures (Efron and Tibshirani 1993). In addition, λ0 =       functions are nearly identical. Acceptance of violence,
0.540(.020).                                                 for component j = 1, β11,acc is negative, and significantly

Table 1: Summary statistics for the predictor variables acceptance of violence, impulsivity, peer delin-
quency, and corporal punishment (first time point, t1)
                               y              acc             imp               pee             pun

variable range             -7 to 2.5            0 to 29              0 to 35           0 to 28             0 to 16

mean                            -.87              11.47               17.50               5.02                1.34

var                             1.80              38.29               44.90              26.57                9.24

Notes: Predictor variables: acc: acceptance of violence, imp: impulsivity, pee: peer delinquency, pun: corporal pun-
ishment. These statistics are all based on n = 237, representing complete data for all five variables.

Table 2: Two-component multivariate mixture of regressions estimates with four predictors: accep-
tance of violence, impulsivity, peer delinquency, and corporal punishment (standard errors in paren-
theses)
                                                                          Components
                                                Component 1 (j = 1):                 Component 2 (j = 2):
Predictors
                                           Decreasing Offending Versatility Increasing/Stable Offending Versatility
                                ^
                               j                    0.228(0.094)                           0.232(0.094)

                                ^
                              j                     1.391(0.258)                           0.282(0.180)

                                   ^
                                j0                  0.881(0.264)                          –0.495(0.057)

                               ^
Acceptance of violence          j1,acc             –0.099(0.017)                         –0.013(0.004)

                               ^
Impulsivity                     j 2,imp             –0.021(0.015)                          0.005(0.003)

                               ^
Peer delinquency                j 3,pee            –0.077(0.018)                           0.007(0.005)

                               ^
Corporal punishment             j 4,pun             0.002(0.032)                          0.024(0.007)

Notes: Parameter estimates relating to the predictors: Values marked in bold are statistically significant (at least
at the 0.05 significance level).

                                                             so; thus increasing acceptance of violence is associ-
  Recapitulating, the slopes are modelled as out-            ated with decreasing offending versatility. Similarly
comes, whereas the chosen risk factors are predictors        for peer delinquency, β13,pee < 0 and thus has a similar
within the regression mixture model. The first compo-        interpretation (increasing peer delinquency scores as-
nent standard deviation is again much larger than            sociated with decreasing offending versatility). Collec-
that of the second component, although the weight            tively, the risk factors acceptance of violence and peer

                                                                                                                  ijcv.org
IJCV: Vol. 15/2021
Wallner, Thomas, Stemmler: Change in Juvenile Offending Versatility Predicted by Individual, Familial, and En-            11
vironmental Risks

delinquency both have a significant negative effect on       different trajectories of offending versatility constitut-
the emergence of offending versatility within the            ing diverse patterns of risk in adolescence, so that the
group of adolescents with decreasing offending versa-        divergent developmental pathways are differentially
tility, or in other words, the higher the acceptance of      described and externally validated. Comparing our re-
violence in this group of adolescents, the smaller the       sults with the results of other studies, certain similari-
offending versatility slope in this group. This means        ties emerge. Referring to the fundamental work of
the higher the acceptance of violence, the more nega-        Moffitt (1993), risks mostly related to adolescence lim-
tive the slope, thus the more decreasing adolescents’        ited antisociality (for example, delinquent peers) are
offending versatility over t2 and t3 in this group. Re-      associated with the contemporary maturity gap in
latedly, the results concerning peer delinquency have        adolescence, whereas individual risks (for example,
a similar interpretation. Corporal punishment is not         concerning personality) and family risks (for example,
different from zero in component one. Impulsivity is         concerning child-rearing practices) are particularly
not different from zero in either component, suggest-        important in life-course persistent antisociality. In our
ing that this variable does not reflect any positive or      study, we do not have self-report data before the age
negative trend when combined with other variables,           of about 15 years and beyond the age of about 17
over the two-year period (t1 – t3). Collectively, com-       years, so childhood and (young) adulthood have to be
ponent two seems to reflect small changes about zero,        neglected and, consequently, certain statements be-
with corporal punishment significantly positive, and         yond the very limited age period of adolescence are
acceptance of violence significantly negative. Accord-       not possible here. Accordingly, the length of follow-up
ingly, corporal punishment has a significant positive        is not adequate to allow extensive conclusions. It has
effect and acceptance of violence has a significant          become apparent that peer delinquency has a signifi-
negative effect on the emergence of offending versatil-      cant negative effect on the emergence of offending
ity within the group of adolescents with slightly in-        versatility within the group of adolescents with de-
creasing or rather stable offending versatility. Peer        creasing offending versatility – which assumedly
delinquency is not different from zero in component          might be limited to adolescence and, hence, related to
two. Overall, our findings illustrate the importance of      the late period of the adolescence limited pathway.
different developmental risks for diverging develop-         Results suggest that with increasing peer delinquency
mental processes explicating risk-related variations in      there is decreasing offending versatility. As outlined in
juvenile offending versatility trajectories.                 the introduction, the bio-psycho-social risk model of
                                                             Lösel and Bender (2003) describes different predictors
3 Discussion                                                 of serious and violent criminality. Delinquent peers
Evaluating the hypotheses set forth above, it becomes        are a developmental risk mainly related to adoles-
apparent, as expected, that adolescents have different       cence (for example, see Lösel and Bender 2003; Moffitt
longitudinal trajectories considering the change of          1993, 2006). As already sketched above (see introduc-
offending versatility. In addition to a non-delinquent       tion), predictors of adolescence limited antisociality
group, our solution identified two diverging sub-            (for example, peer delinquency), are mainly associated
groups of offending adolescents: Individuals with de-        with the contemporary maturity gap in adolescence
creasing versus individuals with slightly increasing or      (for example, Moffitt 1993; see also Odgers et al. 2008),
rather stable offending versatility over time. Gener-        adolescent antisociality is often influenced by adverse
ally, regarding different antisocial outcomes, most          peer contacts (for example, see Battin et al. 1998; Lösel
studies report three to four developmental trajectories      and Bender 2003), and offending in adolescence is fre-
(for example, see Jennings and Reingle 2012). In our         quently characterized by a high rate of peer delin-
study, comprising a quite limited two-year span of           quency (offending in groups) and a high rate of co-of-
adolescence, three groups of adolescents emerged, in-        fending (for example, Wallner and Weiss 2019). Re-
cluding the non-delinquents. Furthermore, also as ex-        strictively, in the current work, we did not take into
pected, different risks contribute variably to these         account whether the committed offenses were typical

                                                                                                                 ijcv.org
IJCV: Vol. 15/2021
Wallner, Thomas, Stemmler: Change in Juvenile Offending Versatility Predicted by Individual, Familial, and En-           12
vironmental Risks

juvenile offenses, which are often committed in              regard to persistent antisociality. Generally, associa-
groups. Relatedly, however, our empirical findings in-       tions between offending and risk might vary accord-
dicate that having delinquent peers might be associ-         ing to the selected dependent variable: As described
ated with offending versatility which is likely to fade      above (see measures section), we used offending ver-
in the course of adolescence. Further longer-term and        satility, as a variety index that tends to cover the di-
more detailed analyses are required in this context          versity of offending rather than the severity of the in-
(see above). Corporal punishment (as a familial risk         dividual offenses, as may be the case with violent
factor relating to parental violence), has a significant     crime, for example. It could be expected that the rela-
positive effect on the emergence of offending versatil-      tionship between acceptance of violence and violence
ity within the group of adolescents with slightly in-        in adolescence would possibly turn out more as ex-
creasing or rather stable offending versatility. Corrado     pected. Following Dünkel and Geng (2003), the level
(2002, 2012) identifies different risks for serious and      of acceptance of violence in youth is positively related
violent antisocial behavior, taking into account differ-     to self-reported violent behavior (see measures sec-
ent risk domains (environmental, individual, family,         tion). Overall, more specific analyses might be useful
intervention, and externalizing behavior; see also           to enhance clarity.
Stemmler, Wallner, and Link 2018; Wallner et al. 2018).        Additionally, general limitations of the present
Generally, parental corporal punishment and adverse          study should be mentioned. First of all, the analyses
child-rearing practices are considered important fam-        primarily focused on offending adolescents. A large
ily-based risks for long-term, persistent, and/or severe     number of individuals with triple zeros on offending
antisociality (for example, see Corrado 2002, 2012;          versatility measures were considered as a separate
Farrington, Ttofi, and Piquero 2016; Lösel and Bender        component, as explicitly noted in equation 1 (see
2003; Moffitt 1993, 2006, 2018). In the context of physi-    methods section). They may not be as interesting to
cal aggression in childhood, Côté and colleagues             consider from a covariance perspective. Another way
(2006) identified three different trajectory groups (low     to think about the triple zeros is that they follow a
desisting, moderate desisting, high stable) and              different probability distribution (i.e., equation 2; see
showed that hostile/ineffective parenting predicted          methods section). One general limitation relates to
the high-level group trajectory. With respect to both        the attrition in the sample of our longitudinal study.
components as acceptance of violence increases, of-          We assume that our results are tolerably robust con-
fending versatility decreases (as can be seen from the       cerning dropout issues. However, we have to consider
significantly negative βs in Table 2). Therefore, unex-      these issues, even though their influence seems to be
pectedly, acceptance of violence has a significant neg-      minor (see methods section). Another important issue
ative effect on the emergence of offending versatility       pertains to the sample which contains a high propor-
within the two groups of adolescents. Indeed, crimi-         tion of students from lower-track schools, so that gen-
nological literature commonly suggests that individ-         eral conclusions concerning the population are not
ual risk concerning deviant beliefs and attitudes to-        possible on the basis of these unweighted data (see
wards delinquency is meaningful in the prediction of         methods section; Wallner and Weiss 2019). Further re-
persistent antisocial outcomes (for example, see Lösel       strictions are associated with the heterogeneity of our
and Bender 2003; Seddig and Reinecke 2017). How-             sample: We did not conduct gender-specific analyses,
ever, a recent meta-analytic review on risk factors for      partly because of the small numbers of individuals in
persistent delinquent behavior among adolescents             single groups of offending adolescents. Moreover, the
(Assink 2017) revealed relatively small effects for,         analyses do not consider other sociodemographic
amongst others, the attitude domain. Thus, substan-          variables (such as migration background, school type,
tial and clear effects should not necessarily be as-         socio-economic status). Additional analyses might in-
sumed in any case. Concerning our own results, the           clude (at least) data concerning late childhood and
relatively short time span examined should also be           early adolescence to enable more precise, complete
noted; for example, no statements can be made with           findings relating to the developmental course of anti-

                                                                                                                 ijcv.org
IJCV: Vol. 15/2021
Wallner, Thomas, Stemmler: Change in Juvenile Offending Versatility Predicted by Individual, Familial, and En-                 13
vironmental Risks

sociality. Restrictions concerning the chosen measures       variably to offending versatility pathways as mostly
have to be mentioned: The justification for the vari-        expected, extensively more research concerning devel-
ables selected is based on theory (see introduction          opmental criminology is needed to identify underly-
and methods section). Other or additional risks could        ing risk patterns in the prediction of heterogeneous
certainly have been selected as independent variables        antisocial pathways, especially concerning, for in-
(for example, see Farrington, Ttofi, and Piquero 2016;       stance, the specific antisocial outcome, possible pro-
Lösel and Bender 2003; Wallner et al. 2018). Moreover,       tective effects, and the individual’s age.
consideration of protective factors would have en-
abled more precise statements concerning buffering           References
effects and flexibility in development which is marked       Arnis, Maria 2015. Die Entstehung und Entwicklung devianten
by certain processes of resilience and desistance (see          und delinquenten Verhaltens im Lebensverlauf und ihre Be-
                                                                deutung für soziale Ungleichheitsprozesse: Fragebogendo-
Rutter 2012; Sampson and Laub 1993). An additional
                                                                kumentation der Schülerbefragung in Dortmund und Nürn-
restrictive aspect is that the outcome measure offend-          berg. Band 2: Skalendokumentation. Erste Erhebungswelle,
ing versatility and the independent variable peer delin-        2012. (SFB 882 Technical Report Series, 18). Bielefeld: DFG
                                                                Research Center (SFB) 882 From Heterogeneities to In-
quency partially refer to similar contents (a similarly         equalities.
structured range of problematic social behaviors and         Assink, Mark 2017. Preventive Risk Assessment in Forensic
related facets of antisociality). The independent vari-         Child and Youth Care. https://pure.uva.nl/ws/files/
                                                                8012091/02.pdf [retrieved November 21, 2019]
ables acceptance of violence and impulsivity are
                                                             Battin, Sara R., Karl G. Hill, Robert D. Abbott, Richard F.
strongly behavior-related (see measures section). We            Catalano, and J. David Hawkins 1998. The Contribution
preferred offending versatility as an outcome measure           of Gang Membership to Delinquency beyond Delinquent
                                                                Friends. Criminology 36 (1): 93-115. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-
(see methods section for justification), however, other
                                                                9125.1998.tb01241.x
possible dependent measures of antisociality refer to,       Bauer, Daniel J., and Patrick J. Curran 2003. Distributional
for example, offending frequency, violence, deviant be-         Assumptions of Growth Mixture Models: Implications for
havior, or conduct problems and should be further an-           Overextraction of Latent Trajectory Classes. Psycholo-
                                                                gical Methods, 8 (3), 338-363. doi: 10.1037/1082-
alyzed in future work.                                          989X.8.3.338
  In conclusion, we should mention the strengths of          Bendixen, Mons, Inger M. Endresen, and Dan Olweus 2003.
our research. We sought associations between distinct           Variety and Frequency Scales of Antisocial Involvement:
                                                                Which one is Better? Legal and Criminological Psychology
developmental risks and different offending versatility         8: 135-150. doi: 10.1348/135532503322362924
pathways over time in adolescence. First, we empha-          Benaglia, Tatiana, Didier Chauveau, David R. Hunter, and
size the longitudinal design of our study, which pro-           Derek S. Young, D. S. 2009. mixtools: An R Package for
                                                                Analyzing Finite Mixture Models. Journal of Statistical
vides longitudinal crime data, enables the application          Software 32 (6): 1-29. doi: 10.18637/jss.v032.i06
of an unconventional longitudinal method in trajec-          Boers, Klaus, and Jost Reinecke 2007. Delinquenz im Jugend-
tory research, and, hence, allows for testing taxo-             alter. Erkenntnisse einer Münsteraner Längsschnittstudie.
nomic predictions in an unusual way. Although our               Münster: Waxmann.
                                                             Boers, Klaus, and Jost Reinecke 2019. Delinquenz im Alters-
results support the suggestion that the trends of of-           verlauf. Erkenntnisse der Langzeitstudie Kriminalität in der
fending versatility over age vary during adolescence            modernen Stadt. Münster, New York: Waxmann.
and, relatedly, the identification of different develop-     Boman, John H., Thomas J. Mowen, and George E. Higgins
mental pathways seemed to be quite successful, more             2019. Social Learning, Self-Control, and Offending Spe-
                                                                cialization and Versatility among Friends. American
methodological research is required concerning addi-            Journal of Criminal Justice 44: 3-22. doi: 10.1007/s12103-
tional, alternative methods being useful for compari-           018-9445-7
son purposes. Second, as the strength of the current         Broidy, Lisa M., Daniel S. Nagin, Richard E. Tremblay, John
                                                                E. Bates, Bobby Brame, Kenneth A. Dodge, David Fer-
study might be seen the direct (i.e., immediate) vali-          gusson, John L. Horwood, Rolf Loeber, Robert Laird, Don-
dation of the varying offending versatility trajectories,       ald R. Lynam, Terrie E. Moffitt, Gregory S. Pettit, and
                                                                Frank Vitaro 2003. Developmental Trajectories of Child-
utilizing a subset of individual, familial, and environ-
                                                                hood Disruptive Behaviors and Adolescent Delinquency:
mental risks in adolescence and, therefore, going be-           A Six-Site, Cross-National Study. Developmental Psycho-
yond description. Although these risks contributed              logy 39 (2): 222-245. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.39.2.222

                                                                                                                     ijcv.org
You can also read