A Transdisciplinary Framework for SLA in a Multilingual World - NTNU

Page created by Mario Estrada
 
CONTINUE READING
A Transdisciplinary Framework for SLA in a Multilingual World - NTNU
A Transdisciplinary Framework
for SLA in a Multilingual World
THE DOUGLAS FIR GROUP1

THE PHENOMENON OF MULTILINGUALISM                        indigenous, minority, or heritage languages, (b)
is as old as humanity, but multilingualism has           to explain the linguistic processes and outcomes
been catapulted to a new world order in the              of such learning, and (c) to characterize the lin-
21st century. Social relations, knowledge struc-         guistic and nonlinguistic forces that create and
tures, and webs of power are experienced by many         shape both the processes and the outcomes. One
people as highly mobile and interconnected—for           of many contributors of knowledge into the learn-
good and for bad—as a result of broad socio-             ing and teaching of languages in the wider field
political events and global markets. As a con-           of applied linguistics, SLA remains focused on
sequence, today’s multilingualism is enmeshed            understanding linguistic development in an addi-
in globalization, technologization, and mobility.        tional language. Begun as an interdisciplinary en-
Communication and meaning-making are often               deavor over half a century ago (e.g., Corder, 1967;
felt as deterritorialized, that is, lived as something   Selinker, 1972), SLA’s early research efforts drew
“which does not belong to one locality but which         on scholarly developments from the fields of lin-
organizes translocal trajectories and wider spaces”      guistics and psychology and drew on practical con-
(Blommaert, 2010, p. 46), while language use and         cerns for language pedagogy in the post-World
learning are seen as emergent, dynamic, unpre-           War II era (see Huebner, 1998). In the early 1980s,
dictable, open ended, and intersubjectively nego-        Hymes’s (1974) work in sociolinguistics and his
tiated. In this context, increasingly numerous and       notion of communicative competence were in-
more diverse populations of adults and youth be-         strumental in the reconceptualization of profi-
come multilingual and transcultural later in life,       ciency in a second language (Canale & Swain,
either by elective choice or by forced circum-           1980) and thus in expanding SLA constructs (see
stances, or for a mixture of reasons. They must          Hornberger, 2009). However, the legacy of lin-
learn to negotiate complex demands and oppor-            guistics and psychology meant that most the-
tunities for varied, emergent competencies across        ories and insights remained strongly cognitive
their languages. Understanding such learning re-         in orientation and generally ignored other re-
quires the integrative consideration of learners’        search, such as Labov’s (1970, 1972) in variation-
mental and neurobiological processing, remem-            ist sociolinguistics (Tarone, 1979, 1988). A pro-
bering and categorizing patterns, and moment-            cess of epistemological expansion was initiated in
to-moment use of language in conjunction with a          the late 1980s and reached momentum by the
variety of socioemotional, sociocultural, sociopo-       late 1990s (Block, 2003; Firth & Wagner, 1997;
litical, and ideological factors.                        Lantolf, 1996), resulting in a field that has un-
   The field of second language acquisition (SLA)        dergone enormous interdisciplinary growth in
seeks (a) to understand the processes by which           the last 25 years or so (Atkinson, 2011; Swain &
school-aged children, adolescents, and adults            Deters, 2007).
learn and use, at any point in life, an ad-                 In part, the expansion has been driven by an
ditional language, including second, foreign,            increase in the number of researchers from a
                                                         wider range of intellectual traditions and disci-
The Modern Language Journal, 100 (Supplement 2016)       plinary roots who are interested in the study of
DOI: 10.1111/modl.12301                                  language learning by adults and youth. Inform-
0026-7902/16/19–47 $1.50/0                               ing their research efforts are concepts, theories,

C 2016 The Modern Language Journal                       and methodologies from fields that are more
20                                                     The Modern Language Journal, 100, Supplement 2016
socially attuned, including anthropology, cogni-         SLA in the 21st century, (c) to serve as a plat-
tive science (particularly in its variants of cog-       form for the development of practical, innovative,
nitive integration, situated cognition, and niche        and sustainable solutions that are responsive to
construction), education, and sociology. Various         the challenges of language teaching and learn-
areas that are considered subfields of linguis-          ing in our increasingly networked, technologized,
tics and/or psychology entered the SLA scene             and mobile worlds, and (d) to improve commu-
thereafter and have contributed to this expan-           nication with a wider range of audiences, espe-
sion as well, such as anthropological linguistics,       cially any and all stakeholders that SLA investi-
cognitive linguistics, corpus linguistics, cultural      gates or whom it hopes to benefit, so they can
psychology, developmental psychology, neurolin-          use SLA work to improve their material and social
guistics, bi/multilingualism, sociolinguistics, and      conditions.
systemic-functional linguistics.                            The document presents the framework using
   Beyond the enrichment brought on by this in-          the following progression: We first position our-
terdisciplinary expansion, our present collective        selves as authors in relation to the field of SLA.
text is motivated by the conviction that SLA must        We then explore the changing nature of lan-
now be particularly responsive to the pressing           guage learning and teaching in a multilingual
needs of people who learn to live—and in fact            world. Those considerations usher in our bid for
do live—with more than one language at various           transdisciplinarity. We describe the framework it-
points in their lives, with regard to their educa-       self in terms of 10 closely interrelated themes.
tion, their multilingual and multiliterate develop-      After briefly recapitulating them we sketch out
ment, social integration, and performance across         some forward directions for language learning
diverse contexts. A new SLA must be imagined,            and teaching that it implies and conclude with an
one that can investigate the learning and teaching       invitation to vigorous and fruitful professional de-
of additional languages across private and pub-          bate of our proposal.
lic, material and digital social contexts in a mul-
tilingual world. We propose that it begin with the       POSITIONING OURSELVES IN RELATION TO
social-local worlds of L2 learners and then pose         THE FIELD OF SLA
the full range of relevant questions—from the
neurobiological and cognitive micro levels to the           In order to provide an interpretive context
macro levels of the sociocultural, educational, ide-     for the rest of the document we would like to
ological, and socioemotional.                            explain who we are and how the present text
   To meet this challenge, we offer here a               came about. The framework proposed here is
framework for SLA that is transdisciplinary. In          the result of intensive collaboration over an ex-
agreement with scholars who have called for              tended period of time2 among a group of 15
transdisciplinarity in other domains of applied          scholars with different theoretical roots, includ-
linguistics (e.g., Hornberger & Hult, 2006),             ing in no particular order: sociocultural theory
we characterize such a framework as problem-             (Johnson, Lantolf, Negueruela, Swain), language
oriented, rising above disciplines and particular        socialization theory (Duff), social identity theory
strands within them with their oftentimes strong         (Norton), complexity and dynamic systems the-
theoretical allegiances. It treats disciplinary          ory (Larsen–Freeman), usage-based approaches
perspectives as valid and distinct but in dialogue       (Ellis, Ortega), the biocultural perspective (Schu-
with one another in order to address real-world          mann), ecological and sociocognitive approaches
issues. Specifically, it seeks to integrate the many     (Atkinson), variationist sociolinguistics (Tarone),
layers of existing knowledge about the processes         systemic functional linguistics (Byrnes, Doran),
and outcomes of additional language learning by          and conversation analysis (Hall). Many but per-
deriving coherent patterns and configurations of         haps not all of us would consider SLA as one of
findings across domains and “over many different         the main research communities in which we par-
levels of granularity and timescale” (N. C. Ellis,       ticipate actively. We find it a strength that our
2014, p. 399).                                           disciplinary and theoretical allegiances with SLA
   In making this proposal we have four aims:            should be so varied. Our views are also enriched
(a) to advance fundamental understandings of             by the diverse parts of the world in which each of
language learning and teaching, including un-            us has worked, done research, and collaborated
derstandings of linguistic development in an             with others. Nevertheless, we must recognize that
additional language, taking into account forces          our affiliation with institutions in only two parts of
beyond individual learners, (b) to promote the           the world, the United States and Canada, bound
development of innovative research agendas for           our intellectual views.
The Douglas Fir Group                                                                                   21
   We also make explicit four fundamental              when new languages are being learned later in
choices of wording and substance with regard to        life (N. C. Ellis, 2015; Lee et al., 2009; MacWhin-
the discipline of SLA, because they have conse-        ney, 2012). Consequently, we define the object
quences for positions taken in this document.          of inquiry of SLA as additional language learn-
First, in negotiating our successive drafts, we        ing at any point in the life span after the learn-
felt uneasy about certain labels. All labels come      ing of one or more languages has taken place in
with a disciplinary history, but in SLA many are       the context of primary socialization in the fam-
encumbered by deficit ideologies that have come        ily; in most societies this means prior to formal
to be contested (Block, 2003; Cook, 2002; Firth        schooling and sometimes in the absence of liter-
& Wagner, 1997; Kubota, 2009; Larsen–Freeman,          acy mediation. Thus, not only the timing but also
2014a; May, 2011; Norton & Toohey, 2011; Or-           instruction and literacy development constitute
tega, 2014b). For example, the language that is        three sites of difference that distinguish the ob-
learned is often referred to as a ‘second language’    ject of study in SLA from that in two neighboring
(L2), at times an ‘additional language.’ The peo-      fields which, like SLA, are primarily concerned
ple who do the learning are called ‘L2 learners,’      with language development, namely monolingual
but they can also be referred to as ‘L2 users’ or as   first language acquisition (Ambridge & Lieven,
‘(late) bi/multilinguals.’ In the particular case of   2011) and bilingual first language acquisition (De
learning English in the United States, they have       Houwer, 2009). In both, the focus of interest is
recently been designated as ‘long-term English         primary socialization inside the family, in other
learners’ and, even more pointedly, as ‘English        words, the period from birth to right before for-
learners at risk of becoming long-term                 mal schooling and literacy enter children’s lives.
English learners’ (cf. Olsen, 2010). What is              Third, through the prolonged and open inter-
being learned is denoted with the nouns ‘acqui-        actions that yielded this document all the authors
sition,’ ‘learning,’ and ‘development,’ sometimes      came to see our ontologies and with them our the-
used synonymously as alternative options, some-        ories of language and learning as broadly com-
times in strong opposition to each other. Our          patible in important ways, despite their different
own attempt to navigate and resist facile yet          optics. When explaining what language is, our var-
consequential labels has been to choose less           ious theoretical understandings emphasize three
deficiency-oriented options where this was pos-        attributes as central: meaning, embodiment, and
sible, though some nonsignificant alternation          self-adaptive local emergence of patterning. Fur-
among terms occasionally seemed unavoidable.           ther, when it comes to explaining what learn-
   Second, the timing of learning also posed un-       ing is, at least conceptually and often empirically,
comfortable challenges. On the one hand, it is         our various theories stipulate the mutual entail-
crucial in the definition of SLA’s object of in-       ment of the cognitive, the social, and the emo-
quiry (as the traditionally used adjective ‘second’    tional. This broad ontological agreement is not
indicates). On the other hand, the disciplinary        shared among all theories of SLA and, indeed, the
understanding of what constitutes ‘a late(r) tim-      group authoring this text did not include schol-
ing’ is itself a matter of unresolved theoretical      ars representing theories that define language
debate. For SLA researchers who interpret the ex-      as a bounded system of formal rules and con-
tant empirical evidence to be in support of a crit-    ceptualize learning as a solely or primarily cog-
ical period for the learning of human language         nitive phenomenon. These other theories have
(e.g., Hyltenstam & Abrahamsson, 2003), the            certainly shaped SLA as a field and contribute
purview of SLA should be postpubescent learners.       valuable knowledge about the research questions
We distance ourselves from that position and in-       they pursue. However, we believe that the alter-
stead side with those who find the empirical evi-      native ontologies we espouse are needed if re-
dence about critical periods thus far inconclusive     searchers are to be able to shed a stronger empir-
and therefore remain agnostic about them (e.g.,        ical light on how multilingualism unfolds in the
Birdsong, 2014; Muñoz & Singleton, 2011). More-        lives of people across their private, public, mate-
over, although we acknowledge competing theo-          rial, and digital social contexts.
ries that posit a marked difference in processes          Fourth and finally, we embrace explicit edu-
and mechanisms before and after a certain age          cational goals for the field (e.g., Byrnes, Weger–
(e.g., Bley–Vroman, 2009; Paradis, 2009; Ullman,       Guntharp, & Sprang, 2006; Duff & Li, 2009;
2005), we favor a fundamental continuity hypoth-       Johnson, 2009; Lantolf & Poehner, 2014; Larsen–
esis: To us, there is good reason to consider the      Freeman & Anderson, 2011; Norton & Toohey,
processes involved in the learning of first lan-       2004; see Swain & Johnson, 1997, for bilingual
guages to be largely the processes also at work        and immersion education, particularly for
22                                                      The Modern Language Journal, 100, Supplement 2016
younger learners). In this, we side with many (but        Heller, 2012); efforts to differentiate and disem-
not all) SLA researchers. The National Federa-            power social groups ethnically, culturally, and reli-
tion of Modern Language Teachers Associations             giously; and the continued influence and reentex-
(NFMLTA), a major activity of which is the pub-           tualization of the nation-state, a market economy,
lication of The Modern Language Journal, declares         and social inequality (Appadurai, 1996). Global-
as its main mission “the expansion, promotion,            ization, technologization, and mobility, however,
and improvement of the teaching of languages,             are forces that exert especially profound and con-
literatures, and cultures” (NFMLTA, undated,              tinuous pressure on what it means to learn and
see: http://nfmlta.org/). We share this aspira-           use more than one language. As such, they com-
tion to impact language education and offer our           pel the research community to train its eyes with
position here as one that is relevant not only to         utmost scrutiny to how it investigates and comes
language theory and additional language learn-            to know its object of study (Reyes, 2013, p. 374).
ing but also—and crucially—to the teaching of                New mobile technologies that increasingly in-
languages. In our estimation, then, SLA, precisely        tegrate in complex ways diverse data sources and
because of its unmistakable focus on language             networks have reached even seemingly remote
development, ought to contribute useful knowl-            corners of the globe and are changing L2 users’
edge for the improvement of education and                 worlds. We have come to understand that they are
instruction of any and all languages, including           neither neutral nor innocent but, in oftentimes
English with its special status as a global language.     subtle ways, reproduce social, economic, and cul-
As we assert and affirm this link (see also Bygate,       tural inequalities (e.g., van Deursen & van Dijk,
2004; R. Ellis, 2010; Ortega, 2005), we readily           2014). At the same time, they have also trans-
acknowledge that in this document we draw little          formed the ways in which language learners in-
on the extensive language teaching scholarship            terpret and make meaning, and thus the ways in
that exists (Borg, 2015; Burns, 2010; Johnson,            which they need and want to use language. For
2009; Kubanyiova, 2014; Kubanyiova & Feryok,              example, although meaning and communication
2015) or say little about the teachers who do this        were always multimodal, using the many technolo-
work. Instead, we focus on research into language         gies of the body (Mauss, 1973), with new tech-
learning and language learners/users. We are              nologies multimodality has reached a qualitatively
also aware that we run the risk of positioning SLA        new level. Graphic, pictorial, audio, physical, and
researchers as ‘telling’ language teachers what to        spatial patterns of meaning are integrated within,
do or how to think about who, what, and how they          and even supplant, traditional spoken and written
are to teach, thereby potentially leaving out their       texts (The New London Group, 1996). Notions of
voices, their worlds, or their work. Even so, we          space and time collapse online, and boundaries
wish to affirm, both as a statement of belief and as      between private and public, real and virtual be-
a statement of aspiration, a strong commitment            come blurred (Thorne, 2013). New technologies
on the part of SLA to language teaching and ed-           have also created new forms of leisure and new
ucation and express the hope that this document           opportunities not only for exchanging and inter-
might, in time, foster more collaborative forms of        preting information but also for authoring knowl-
engagement between teachers and researchers.              edge and art and for building social networks “in
                                                          the digital wilds” (Thorne, Sauro, & Smith, 2015,
THE CHANGING NATURE OF LANGUAGE                           p. 215). As a result, the very scope and constitu-
LEARNING AND TEACHING IN A                                tion of communication practices between individ-
MULTILINGUAL WORLD                                        uals and within and across social groups and com-
                                                          munities worldwide have also changed: They have
   In today’s multilingual world, the rising tide         created new needs for new language and new real
of globalization has penetrated all aspects of L2         and imagined discourse communities, and they
learners’ lifeworlds. Amidst globally felt changes        have also created new desires for new products,
that seem to occur in breathtaking succession, two        commodities, and processes, such as online learn-
closely related phenomena of particular durabil-          ing. The future is a moving target, and in coming
ity have been technologization and mobility. We           years the emerging new technologies that people
have chosen to emphasize globalization, technol-          will want to use in their multiple languages in-
ogization, and mobility for their potential to fa-        clude mobile devices, game-based learning, and
cilitate grass-roots agency and action. At the same       (further on the horizon) gesture-based comput-
time, we do not wish to naïvely deny the contin-          ing and learning analytics (e.g., Spector, 2013).
ued existence of traditional power dynamics, such         In turn, educators will want to exploit them for
as the commodification of language (Duchêne &             transforming and expanding opportunities for
The Douglas Fir Group                                                                                    23
the learning and teaching of languages (Kern,          plicity of languages, discourses, literacy practices,
2014; Thorne, 2013).                                   and interlocutors. It is thus not surprising that
   The fabric of L2 learners’ social groups and        in these superdiverse environments, transformed
communities has also been altered by mobility, a       as they are by digital means for communicating
term which denotes global movements not only           across geographical boundaries and by expand-
of people, but also of objects, capital, and in-       ing opportunities for learning and using addi-
formation across the globe. The movement of            tional languages, the once normative dichotomies
people is of great consequence for understand-         in SLA of the ‘second’ and the ‘foreign’ (more
ing today’s multilingualism, especially the form       recently applied as well to the ‘heritage’ and
of human mobility related to migration known           the ‘indigenous’) language context or the ‘real
as transnationalism, or “the crossing of cultural,     world’ and the ‘classroom’ setting become in-
ideological, linguistic, and geopolitical borders      creasingly questionable. Affordances for language
and boundaries of all types but especially those       learning and use arise in multilingual and mul-
of nation-states” (Duff, 2015, p. 57; see also         timodal encounters with different interlocutors
Appadurai, 1996). The patterns of such crossing        for diverse purposes, across space and time, and
or movement, as Duff notes, are further com-           in face-to-face and virtual contexts. Moreover, the
plicated by virtual and multigenerational expe-        diversity of contemporary life outside of class-
riences as well as by temporary mobility pat-          rooms is transforming language classrooms, mak-
terns, for example, involving short-term sojourn-      ing them into “complex communicative space[s]
ers for tourism, study abroad, or work—and also        criss-crossed with the traces of other communica-
by the multiple boundary crossing experiences          tive encounters and discourses both institutional
of returnees (Kanno, 2003; Kubota, 2013a). The         and everyday” (Baynham, 2006, p. 25). It is then
large-scale movement (including migration) of          not surprising that the expanded potential for
individuals, families, and larger social groups        meaning-making also harbors enormous poten-
around the world, along with the movement of           tial for miscommunication, as attested in major
information and various forms of capital, creates      social tensions at all levels of communication in
communities that are linguistically, socially, and     our world—among individuals and groups, within
culturally extraordinarily diverse. To be sure, mul-   countries, across countries and regions, and
tilingual communities have long existed in tra-        globally.
ditional cultures around the globe. In parts of
Africa, for example, it is common and, indeed,         A BID FOR TRANSDISCIPLINARITY
expected, for communities to function through
multiple languages, so much so that the languages         To make sense of the varying processes and
themselves become ‘invisible’ in many commu-           outcomes of additional language learning arising
nities. One might say, then, that what globaliza-      from contemporary conditions, SLA and other
tion has accomplished is a heightened awareness        applied linguistics researchers have looked to
of the reality of multilingualism in Western soci-     other disciplines for insights and research di-
eties, which had accepted the monolingualism of        rections. These explorations have resulted in a
the nation-state as the ‘real norm.’ Indeed, diver-    wealth of approaches to the study of L2 learning
sity is now being felt on an unprecedented scale,      and teaching that coexist nowadays in addition
prompting anthropologists and, subsequently, so-       to the historically dominant cognitive and linguis-
ciolinguists and scholars in many other social         tic approaches (Atkinson, 2011). Among others,
science fields to use the term superdiversity          these include non-mainstream approaches rep-
(Vertovec, 2007, 2015).                                resented by the present authors. These newer
   Mobility and migration have triggered transna-      approaches to SLA have had a marked impact
tionalism and superdiversity and spawned an on-        on the breadth and complexity of studies ex-
going process of deterritorialization of meaning-      amining second, foreign, indigenous, and her-
making. As a result, communication now well-           itage language learning. However, they “have led,
nigh requires the expansion of creative strate-        with a few exceptions, independent and even
gies from language users as they negotiate so-         isolated existences” (Atkinson, 2011, p. xi). Cit-
cial and linguistic action in the face of mini-        ing the dangers of such isolation for advanc-
mal common ground and maximal semiotic de-             ing knowledge, some scholars have argued for
mands (Canagarajah, 2013; Kramsch, 2009): Het-         engagement across perspectives and, where possi-
erogeneous forms of social activity and options        ble, the construction of bridges or broader frames
for participating in them emerge from mobility         of reference in which the complementarities (and
and transnationalism, by way of involving a multi-     differences) are visible (Hulstijn et al., 2014).
24                                                      The Modern Language Journal, 100, Supplement 2016
Others have argued that such bridge building              ing language, and moment-to-moment language
may have limited usefulness in that “no matter            use.
how much traffic crosses the bridges, the abyss
[to be bridged] is still there” (Lantolf, 2014,           THE FRAMEWORK
p. 370).
   As a group, we have come to appreciate sev-               Our framework encompasses a growing body
eral important strengths of transdisciplinarity           of theories and research, although we can
(Larsen–Freeman, 2012). Indeed, we see an in-             do no more than refer to citations that are
teresting parallel between the mobility of people         representative, rather than inclusive or ex-
and transnationalism and the multidirectional,            haustive, of the relevant research. Inspired by
rhizomatic information flows enabled by tech-             Bronfenbrenner’s ecological framework for
nology and transdisciplinarity. Epistemologically,        human development (Bronfenbrenner, 1979;
transdisciplinarity aspires to transcend the bound-       Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 2007), our integrated
aries of disciplines and generate knowledge that          representation of the multilayered complex-
is more than the sum of a discipline-specific col-        ity of L2 learning distinguishes three levels of
lection of findings (Halliday, 1990/2001). Rather         mutually dependent influence (cf. Ricento &
than privileging the disciplines “as the locus of in-     Hornberger, 1996). As shown in Figure 1, we see
tellectual activity, while building bridges between       L2 learning as an ongoing process that begins
them, or assembling them into a collection,” Hal-         at the micro level of social activity (the smallest
liday advocates creating “new forms of activity           concentric circle), with individuals recruiting
which are thematic rather than disciplinary in            their neurological mechanisms and cognitive and
their orientation” (1990/2001, p. 176). As Hult           emotional capacities and engaging with others
(2011) notes, a transdisciplinary approach “lends         in specific multilingual contexts of action and
itself to a certain intellectual freedom but also         interaction, resulting in recurring contexts of use
to practical and conceptual challenges to be con-         that contribute to the development of multilin-
sidered along all phases of the research process”         gual repertoires (Rymes, 2010). The engagement
(p. 19). Closer to the ground, in its methodolog-         in these contexts uses all available semiotic
ical orientation, transdisciplinarity seeks to help       resources, including linguistic, prosodic, interac-
solve problems in socially useful and participant-        tional, nonverbal, graphic, pictorial, auditory, and
relevant or emic ways with whatever theoretical-          artifactual resources. These contexts are situated
analytical tools are required (e.g., Bigelow, 2014).      within and shaped at a meso level (the middle
Mixed methods research that carefully considers           concentric circle) by particular sociocultural
the contexts of language teaching and learning            institutions and particular sociocultural commu-
seems to be particularly well suited to this task         nities, such as those found in the family, school,
(cf. J. D. Brown, 2014; Hashemi & Babaii, 2013;           neighborhood, places of work, places of worship,
Mackey & Gass, 2015). In both the sciences and            social organizations like clubs, community sports
the humanities, the movement to transdisciplinar-         leagues, political parties, online forums of various
ity can also aspire to become a transgressive cri-        kinds, and so on. Importantly, the institutions
tique of normal science and normative knowledge           and communities at the meso level are power-
(J. T. Klein, 2014), inviting individual researchers      fully characterized by pervasive social conditions
to turn critical moments of recognizing differ-           (e.g., economic, cultural, religious, political),
ence into opportunities for trusting communica-           which affect the possibility and nature of persons
tion and enrichment across epistemic boundaries           creating social identities in terms of investment,
(Holbrook, 2013).                                         agency, and power. Together, these institutions,
   We thus offer a transdisciplinary framework            communities, conditions, and possible identities
that assumes the embedding, at all levels, of so-         provide or restrict access to particular types of
cial, sociocultural, sociocognitive, sociomaterial,       social experiences. Finally, at the macro level (the
ecosocial, ideological, and emotional dimensions.         largest concentric circle) there are large-scale,
Its goal is to meet the challenge of responding to        society-wide ideological structures with particular
the pressing needs of additional language users,          orientations toward language use and language
their education, their multilingual and multiliter-       learning (including belief systems and cultural,
ate development, social integration, and perfor-          political, religious, and economic values) that
mance across diverse globalized, technologized,           both shape and are shaped by sociocultural
and transnational contexts. It does so by pursu-          institutions and communities (middle circle)
ing an integrative consideration of learners’ men-        as well as by the agency of individual members
tal and neurobiological processing, remember-             within their locally situated contexts of action
The Douglas Fir Group                                                                                   25
FIGURE 1
The Multifaceted Nature of Language Learning and Teaching

                                         MACRO LEVEL
             Belief Systems
             Cultural Values
                                              OF
             Political Values       IDEOLOGICAL STRUCTURES
             Religious Values
             Economic Values

                                           MESO LEVEL
                                               OF                           Social Identities
                                    SOCIOCULTURAL INSTITUTIONS
                                         AND COMMUNITIES

                                                                               Families
                                              MICRO LEVEL                      Schools
               Semiotic Resources                 OF                           Neighborhood
                   Linguistic               SOCIAL ACTIVITY                    Places of Work
                   Prosodic                                                    Places of Worship
                   Interactional                                               Social Organizations
                   Nonverbal
                   Graphic
                   Pictorial
                   Auditory
                   Artifactual

and interaction (smallest circle). While each of       communication and learning in their multilin-
the three levels represented in Figure 1 has its       gual lifeworlds. Another goal is to foster in learn-
distinctive characteristics, no level exists on its    ers a profound awareness not only of the cultural,
own; each exists only through constant interac-        historical, and institutional meanings that their
tion with the others, such that each gives shape to    language-mediated social actions have, but also,
and is shaped by the next, and all are considered      and just as importantly, of the dynamic and evolv-
essential to understanding SLA. They persist only      ing role their actions play in shaping their own
through constant interaction with each other           and others’ worlds. Learners as language users
and so exist in a state of continuous change (cf.      have this power via the semiotic resources they
Fairclough, 1996; Larsen–Freeman & Cameron,            choose to use and respond to in their interactions
2008).                                                 with others. In short, the framework is intended to
   The framework is built on an understanding          help multilingual users to thrive with and through
that, ideally, should foster two goals of additional   their very multilinguality by the kind of research
language learning and teaching. One goal is to         and practice it advocates.
expand the perspectives of researchers and teach-         Pursuit of these goals crucially necessitates
ers of L2 learners with regard to learners’ diverse    several constructs. One is the construct of commu-
multilingual repertoires of meaning-making re-         nity, including speech communities (Gumperz,
sources and identities so as to enable their par-      1968), discourse communities (Swales, 1990,
ticipation in a wide range of social, cognitive,       pp. 21–32), and communities of practice
and emotional activities, networks, and forms of       (Lave & Wenger, 1991). These notions have
26                                                    The Modern Language Journal, 100, Supplement 2016
contributed substantially to capturing the social       dynamic, and holistic (Larsen–Freeman, 1997;
nature of language learning. Most recently, the         Tarone, 1983).
construct has become contested among other                 The totality of a speaker’s semiotic resources
reasons because of its inability to capture ade-        must be considered her or his communicative
quately powerful social relationships outside the       and interactional competence. It goes without say-
community, with individual networks of practice         ing that our invoking the term ‘competence’ is
being suggested instead in order to describe            markedly different from its use by Chomsky, per-
people’s engagement with other users and learn-         haps even its use by Hymes. Multilingual speakers
ers of language (cf. Zappa–Hollman & Duff,              will deploy their semiotic resources by choosing
2015). Norm and choice, identity and agency are         across their languages and/or varieties and regis-
other important constructs. It is communities           ters in response to local demands for social action.
or, as appropriate, social networks that give rise      Multilinguals are well documented as handling
to always-changing but nevertheless operational         this rich semiotic repertoire flexibly, sometimes
norms of language use, form, and function,              keeping the languages separate, at other times al-
together with exploitable potentials for novel          ternating them, mixing them, or meshing them.
meaning-making through language choice. Both            The competence of multilingual speakers is the
language norms and language choice must be              holistic sum of their multiple-language capacities
developed through experience and both must              (Blackledge & Creese, 2010; Cook & Li Wei, 2016;
be recognizable as such by a given community of         Grosjean, 1989; He, 2013). Their multilingualism
users and more locally by a given co-interlocutor,      “is fluid, not fixed: difficult to measure, but real”
if learners are to participate in particular types      (Gorter, 2015, p. 86).
of discourse as legitimate speakers with the right         Learners’ developmental trajectories, medi-
to be interpreted favorably and to impose mean-         ated by the opportunities and struggles of their
ing and position themselves in a desirable light        multilingual lifeworlds, vary in and outside of
(Norton, 2013). In other words, flexible com-           the classroom. Some people develop compre-
petencies over both norm and choice allow the           hensive and elaborate repertoires of multilin-
speakers/writers to present themselves and their        gual semiotic resources, while others develop
views in a particular way, not only accomplishing       more specialized resources linked to particu-
successful referential communication goals but          lar contexts (e.g., technical L2 vocabulary for
also reflecting the person’s fashioned identity in      academic, specific, or vocational purposes). Yet
relation to the topic and audience members.             others craft minimal, transitory competences
Thus, learning such discursive norms and choices        based on snippets of additional languages (e.g.,
further enables new language users not only             isolated greeting/leave-taking patterns like hola
to participate in discourse but also to exercise        from Spanish or sayonara from Japanese; see
agency, that is, to negotiate some impact on their      Blommaert & Backus, 2011), or bricolage and
local contexts and on the improvement of their          mesh resources from multiple languages and va-
material and social worlds (Byrnes, 2014b; Miller,      rieties (e.g., hip hop varieties; Alim, Ibrahim,
2014).                                                  & Pennycook, 2009). Still others appropriate
   Ensuing from the framework are 10 funda-             limited linguistic repertoires for purposes of
mental themes. They obtain from the characteris-        identity performance, play, and styling (Broner
tics of the three levels, their interconnectedness,     & Tarone, 2001; Li Wei & Zhu, 2013; Ramp-
and their potential as affordances (Gibson, 1979),      ton, 2013). Other language users may imag-
that is, their potential to offer action possibili-     ine themselves to remain steadfastly monolin-
ties that can be appropriated, negotiated, trans-       gual, discounting their multilectal and multireg-
formed, and made into means or constraints for          ister competencies. And despite increased and
L2 researching, learning, and teaching. In the re-      varied social encounters marked by extensive
mainder of the article we present each theme in         use of multilingual resources, some may insu-
turn.                                                   late themselves from other languages by choice
                                                        or circumstance. Further shaping what it means
1. Language Competencies Are Complex, Dynamic,          to develop multilingual repertoires is the con-
   and Holistic                                         tested and ambivalent role of English as a global
                                                        lingua franca, which affects the worlds of L2
   A new, reimagined SLA that addresses the re-         learners and users in the realms of educa-
alities of L2 learning in a multilingual world          tion, diplomacy, science, popular cultural me-
necessitates a reconceptualized understanding           dia (e.g., movies, music, Twitter, dance), and
of linguistic competence: One that is complex,          technology.
The Douglas Fir Group                                                                                    27
2.   Language Learning Is Semiotic Learning             which are reinstantiated with each new use in a
                                                        slightly different context.
   Semiotic resources include a wide array of con-         The greater the number and diversity of con-
ventionalized form–meaning constructions that           texts of interaction within and across social in-
vary in degree of analytic specificity, ranging         stitutions that L2 learners gain and are given
from minimal meaningful units, such as mor-             access to and are motivated to participate in,
phemes and words, to collocations of units and          the richer and more linguistically diverse their
other groupings comprising idioms and rou-              evolving semiotic resources will be. Likewise, the
tines, as well as the more conventionally recog-        more extended the learners’ opportunities are
nized linguistic units such as sentences (Boyd &        for deriving form–meaning patterns from these
Goldberg, 2009; N. C. Ellis & Robinson, 2008;           meaning-making resources (e.g., through trans-
Pawley & Syder, 1983). Semiotic resources also in-      parency of connections in their use and guided
clude larger, more holistic types of meanings, for      support from others to notice and remember the
example, at the level of discourse and rhetoric.        connections), the more robust their multilingual
In the case of oral language use, they also in-         repertoires are likely to be. Importantly, however,
clude patterns for taking turns, and paralinguis-       access is neither easy nor assured and, in some
tic resources such as intonation, stress, tempo,        cases, is in fact blocked, whether intentionally or
pausing, and other such features that accom-            unintentionally.
pany talk as well as the full array of nonverbal
signs—gestures, facial expressions, body position-
ing, accompanying action, head movement, etc.           3.   Language Learning Is Situated and Attentionally
In the case of written language, resources also in-          and Socially Gated
clude orthographic and typographic representa-
tions. Semiotic resources further include visual,          Language learning begins at the micro level of
graphic, and auditory modes of meaning-making           social activity (see the inner concentric circle in
(Kress, 2009).                                          Figure 1) through L2 learners’ repeated experi-
   All semiotic resources, individually and in com-     ences in regularly occurring and recurring con-
bination, have meaning potentials, that is, con-        texts of use, often characterized by interpersonal
ventionalized form–meaning combinations that            (oral, signed, or written) interaction with other
develop from their past uses in contexts of action      social actors. From these situated, local iterative
in the world that, in turn, are shaped by larger so-    contexts, language use and language learning can
cial institutions (e.g., the family, schools, places    emerge—though they do not always do so. The
of work and worship, civic organizations, etc.).        scope of these contexts can be wide-ranging and
These resources offer particular visions of the         includes everyday, informal contexts of interac-
world, that is, they create “specific complexes of      tion, such as ad hoc conversations, text messag-
values, definitions of the situation, and meanings      ing, online game-playing, as well as more formal
of possible actions” (Morson & Emerson, 1990,           contexts such as those comprising L2 classrooms
p. 22) that bind their users, to some degree, to par-   where students instruct and are instructed, in-
ticular ways of construing the world (Hall, 2011).      form, discuss, problem solve, and so on. These en-
This is where the macro level has a powerful influ-     counters can be very brief or longer lasting; their
ence through the politico-economic system that          purposes can be varied, and the means—the semi-
impacts schools, work, civic, and religious institu-    otic resources—by which they are accomplished
tions, etc. The meaning potentials of all semiotic      can vary as well (see, e.g., Tarone, 1979, 1985;
resources are considered affordances in that in         Tarone & Liu, 1995).
their local, emergent contexts of use they enable          In the field of usage-based developmental lin-
certain possible construals of experience by their      guistics, a well-known principle is that regularly
users and certain possible interpretations by the       occurring and recurring social interactions are
recipients (e.g., hearers, readers) (Byrnes, 2006;      characterized by joint actions that are depen-
N. C. Ellis & Robinson, 2008).                          dent on intersubjective or shared cognition, that
   Meaning potentials of semiotic resources, then,      is, a human being’s recognition that she can
are not neutral, value-free, systems. Rather, each      share beliefs and intentions with other humans
resource “tastes of the context and contexts            (Clark, 1996). Shared attention develops in the
in which it has lived its socially charged life”        first 2 years of life, when infants develop their
(Bakhtin, 1981, p. 293). In this way all semiotic re-   capabilities of attention detection (gaze follow-
sources function as the “carriers of sociocultural      ing), attention manipulation (directive pointing),
patterns and knowledge” (Wertsch, 1994, p. 204),        intention understanding (“theory of mind” or
28                                                      The Modern Language Journal, 100, Supplement 2016
the realization that others are goal-directed), and       support adaptive behavior” (Okon–Singer et al.,
social coordination with shared intentionality            2015, p. 6). For L2 learners this may mean that
(engaging in joint activities with shared interest,       the more they experience emotionally and moti-
negotiating meanings) (Tomasello, 2003). Shared           vationally positive evaluations of their anticipated
attention, shared cooperative activity, and shared        and real interactions, the more effort they will
cognition are key to the emergence of language            make to participate in them and affiliate with
in infants through socially contingent, meaning-          others.
ful usage. Furthermore, this crucially important             As Schumann (2010) and Lee and colleagues
activity of joint attention is a process into which       (2009) note, infants in normal situations acquire
novices are socialized in their particular culture        their primary languages through bonding rela-
(e.g., P. Brown, 2011). In these usage events un-         tionships with their caregivers that are almost un-
folding at the micro level of social activity, the        conditionally offered to them; by comparison, the
semiotic resources that more mature communica-            older the learner, the more complicated interper-
tors tool and retool to accomplish social actions         sonal and social relations become. This means
are afforded for the infants, as novice commu-            that older learners are likely to experience re-
nicators, to appropriate, recycle, and expand in          duced intensity of the brain reward system from
contextually adaptive ways, as they co-construct          such affiliations, although these can occur un-
meaning. Such contextually adaptive ways ideally          der certain circumstances. Consequently, their
serve language development, and positive out-             interest or motivation to seek out and sustain
comes can be expected given average conditions            affiliative interactions within L2 contexts of ac-
of health and social and emotional well-being. In         tion (i.e., their emotional investment) and, con-
sum, infants’ language learning is gated by both          comitantly, their opportunities for learning, are
attention and sociality at the same time (N. C.           likely to be also reduced. By the same token,
Ellis, 2014, 2015). Within the fundamental con-           extraordinarily high, and highly emotional, mo-
tinuity hypothesis we espouse, these processes are        tivation can occur with adult learners. For ex-
equally relevant to infants learning their first lan-     ample, Henry, Davydenko, and Dörnyei (2015)
guage(s) and to youth or adults learning an addi-         found what they call ‘directed motivational cur-
tional language (N. C. Ellis, 2015; Lee et al., 2009;     rents’ in their study of unusually successful immi-
MacWhinney, 2012). The development of an ad-              grants learning Swedish. Dewaele and MacIntyre
ditional language is thus also attentionally and so-      (2014) also found that many foreign language
cially gated, as learners’ multilingual repertoires       learners report intense feelings of enjoyment, as
in their varied micro contexts likewise depend in         well as anxiety, in L2 classrooms. Moreover, in an-
part on neurobiological mechanisms with which             other study Denies, Yashima, and Janssen (2015)
all human beings are endowed.                             showed that the behavioral manifestation of the
   Socially meaningful interaction is partly de-          interactional instinct that SLA research has re-
pendent on an interactional instinct, that is,            ferred to as ‘willingness to communicate,’ can be
a biologically specified attentional and moti-            realized differently in one and the same learner
vational brain networked system that pushes               group, depending on whether the interaction
the infant to seek out emotionally rewarding,             takes place in a classroom setting or in the larger
affiliative relationships with others, and to bond        society. It turns out that perceived competence
emotionally and affectively (Lee et al., 2009;            of self in both the classroom and larger society,
Schumann, 2010). As with all learning, for young          more so than motivation in and of itself, helped
children, adolescents, and adults, too, L2 learn-         predict users’ willingness to communicate. Such
ing is an emotionally driven process, one that re-        differences explain in part the great variability of
quires minimally that they be motivated to par-           outcomes that is observed in L2 learning.
ticipate with others in particular contexts of ac-           Also playing a significant role in additional
tion, in classrooms and society at large. To deter-       language learning and use is the set of general
mine the reward potential that may be afforded by         cognitive and emotional capabilities on which
L2 contexts of action, humans evaluate them ac-           learners draw to register and catalogue their en-
cording to five dimensions: novelty, pleasantness,        counters with the various semiotic resources com-
goal or need significance, coping potential, and          prising their contexts of interaction. These in-
self- and social image (Lee et al., 2009). This is        clude the abilities to select and attend to par-
part of regular brain functioning: Human brains           ticular meaning-making components and their
“integrate ‘emotional’ (e.g., value, risk) and ‘cog-      patterns of action, to form schemas based on
nitive’ computations (e.g., prediction error, at-         their recurrences, to create mappings across
tention allocation, action selection) in ways that        units based on functional similarities, and to
The Douglas Fir Group                                                                                      29
hypothesize about and continually test their un-         (Atkinson, 2014; Eskildsen & Wagner, 2015;
derstandings of their meanings. Learning first           Goldin–Meadow & Alibali, 2013; Ibbotson,
and second languages, like learning about all            Lieven, & Tomasello, 2013). Moreover, “humans
other aspects of the world, involves the full scope      use the entire body to participate in socially orga-
of cognition and emotion: the remembering of ut-         nized processes of understanding and learning,
terances and episodes; the categorization of expe-       [a fact] which ultimately challenges a strict Carte-
rience; the determination of patterns among and          sian division between mind and body. Instead,
between stimuli; the generalization of concep-           the mind is the body” (Eskildsen & Wagner, 2015,
tual schema and prototypes from exemplars; and           p. 442; cf. also Harris, 1998, and his advocacy of
the use of cognitive models, metaphors, analo-           ‘integrationism’).
gies, and images in thinking (N. C. Ellis, 2008,            Language learning happens by mediation,
2015). Conscious and unconscious learning pro-           through cultural resources and tools that individ-
cesses similarly affect the dance of dialogue where      uals use to move through, respond to, and make
conversation partners align perspectives and             sense of their social worlds (cf. Scollon, 2001;
means of linguistic expression. Language is used         Vygotsky, 1978; Wertsch, 1994). The role of me-
to focus the listener’s attention to the world,          diation in L2 learning is seen as central in socio-
potentially relating many different perspectives         cultural theories (Lantolf & Thorne, 2006; Swain,
about the same scene or referents. What is at-           Kinnear, & Steinman, 2015) but cannot be ig-
tended to focuses learning, and so language is           nored in any attempts at understanding language
both constitutive of and constituted by attention.       learning, regardless of theoretical predilections.
The functions of language in use determine its us-       The semiotic resource of language is itself consid-
age and learning (N. C. Ellis, 2014). The more           ered to be a mediational tool (see earlier section);
routine, frequent, and stable the occurrences of         in addition, across various modes of communi-
particular resources are in the interactions and         cation, mediational semiotic tools can include a
the more L2 learners’ attention is drawn to their        potentially infinite set of cultural artifacts, such
form–meaning pairings, the more entrenched the           as diagrams, maps, books, computers, and even
resources become as cognitive–emotional repre-           furniture, including tables, desks, and chairs
sentations of their experiences. All else being          (Nishino & Atkinson, 2015).
equal, the more extensive, complex, and multi-              In classrooms, in addition, mediation is typi-
lingual the contexts of interaction become over          cally accomplished via a wide range of instruc-
time, and the more enduring learners’ participa-         tional actions that direct learners to perceive or
tion is in them, the more complex and enduring           notice the relevant resources and their form–
their multilingual repertoires will be.                  meaning connections and to make connections
                                                         between them and their contexts of use. For ex-
4.   Language Learning Is Multimodal, Embodied,          ample, the type of materials used in formal learn-
     and Mediated                                        ing contexts such as L2 classrooms have been
                                                         shown to play a significant role in shaping stu-
   Supporting learners’ neurobiological and cog-         dents’ contexts of interaction and participation
nitive processes are cues used by others, typi-          structures, demonstrating that they are not only
cally more experienced participants, which index         a primary source of the design of curriculum,
and at times make transparent the form–meaning           but also highly influential to the scope and types
patterns and can assist L2 learners in noticing          of instructional interactions that occur within
and remembering them. Such assistance can take           that learning community (Guerrettaz & Johnston,
many forms, such as the use of verbal and non-           2013; Toohey, 2000).
verbal actions that explicitly direct learners’ atten-
tion to the semiotic resources and their meaning-        5.   Variability and Change Are at the Heart of
making potentials, and other less explicit actions            Language Learning
including repetitions, recycling, and recasts of
one another’s words; tone, intonation, and pitch            Language learning is characterized by variabil-
changes; eye gaze and gesture; and so on.                ity and change. It is a ceaseless moving target,
   Nonlinguistic, multimodal semiotic resources          with periods of stability but never stasis, and de-
are used to make the coupling of a form and              scribable via probabilistic predictions but never
a meaning socially available during unfold-              via deterministic laws. These qualities must be ac-
ing interactions. They are not peripheral or             counted for within and across units of observa-
complementary to language learning. Instead,             tion, be it constructions, stretches of discourse,
they provide crucial social cues to grammar              learners, classrooms, or communities.
30                                                      The Modern Language Journal, 100, Supplement 2016
   First, no two people, even those in the same              [f]or the child, the construction of the grammar
classroom, will experience exactly the same social           and the construction of semantic/pragmatic con-
contexts of language use or resolve them in ex-              cepts go hand-in-hand. For the adult, construction
actly the same way. Thus, differences at the micro           of the grammar often requires a revision of seman-
                                                             tic/pragmatic concepts [available through the L1],
level of social activity and in L2 learners’ history
                                                             along with what may well be a more difficult task of
of usage across situated, local, iterative contexts
                                                             perceptual identification of the relevant morpholog-
will create differences in the learning trajecto-            ical elements. (p. 242)
ries at the individual level of observation (de Bot,
Lowie, & Verspoor, 2007; Eskildsen & Wagner,                 In other words, knowledge of the L1 results in a
2015; Larsen–Freeman, 2006; Larsen–Freeman &              ‘learned attention’ to language whereby the pro-
Cameron, 2008). This is true even when it is              cessing of the L2 proceeds in L1-tuned ways (N. C.
also possible to observe regular, more general            Ellis, 2008). The languages and cultural schemata
patterns of development at larger grain sizes             of a multilingual interact, both facilitating and
(N. C. Ellis, 2008, 2015). There is no learn-             complicating the learning of new language at the
ing without change, and thus, when a learner              level of forms, concepts, and form–meaning map-
exhibits high variability in the deployment of            pings (Jarvis & Pavlenko, 2008). The more sim-
semiotic resources, this is theoretically important       ilar, broadly speaking, these L1 forms, concepts,
and can be studied in its own right (de Bot               and form–meaning pairings are to those in the
et al., 2007; Geeslin, 2014; Larsen–Freeman &             L2, the easier it may be for L2 learners to learn
Cameron, 2008; Preston, 1989; Tarone, 1988).              them, while at the same time even slight varia-
Variability is not measurement error begging              tions and subtle differences across languages can
for better control. Acknowledging inter- as               complicate the development of apparently simi-
well as intra-individual variation helps counter          lar L2 forms, concepts, and form–meaning map-
deficit orientations in the description of lin-           pings. These cross-linguistic influences are perva-
guistic development in an L2 (W. Klein, 1998)             sive, but they are also bidirectional; and they are
and focus on what learners can do rather                  dynamic and variable, rather than deterministic
than what they cannot do (Donato & Tucker,                or constant (Jarvis & Pavlenko, 2008).
2010).
   Second, cognitive abilities involved in pattern        6. Literacy and Instruction Mediate Language
detection appear to be more variable among                   Learning
adults than among children. Differences in them
create variation in L2 development across individ-           When language is learned during primary so-
uals. The sources of such differences appear to           cialization in the family, this usually means not
be located in phonological short-term memory,             only from birth or soon after, but also without
associative memory, and implicit learning (Linck          the involvement of formal schooling or literacy
et al., 2013) as well as perhaps in pattern-detecting     mediation. In many—though certainly not all—
ability for general statistical learning of artifi-       societies and for many individuals, on the other
cial language (nonmeaningful) stimuli (Misyak &           hand, additional language learning tends to be
Christiansen, 2012). All else being equal, L2 learn-      characterized by the mediation of instruction and
ers with higher capacities for detecting patterns         literacy. Therefore, both instruction and literacy
are likely to do better than those whose capacities       need to be understood as sources of influence
are lower (MacWhinney, 2012). However, rather             on L2 learning, and disciplinary knowledge about
than accepting meager learning outcomes as bio-           them has particular potential to improve the
logically given once learners have passed a certain       learning experiences of the millions of children,
age and finding ways of theorizing them as insur-         adolescents, and adults worldwide who, by choice
mountable, our stance is that responsible educa-          or circumstance, embark on the journey of addi-
tional approaches can go a long way toward fos-           tional language learning in educational settings.
tering other learner abilities that are also known        A wealth of psycholinguistically oriented SLA re-
to affect learning success, particularly in adult         search into the development of L2 literacies has il-
learners.                                                 luminated the complexities of learning to become
   Third, in learning multiple languages an-              biliterate (Grabe & Stoller, 2011), and particularly
other factor that mediates processes and out-             in languages with different writing systems (Koda,
comes and creates variability is knowledge                2005). We now know that alphabetic print liter-
of a previous language or languages, includ-              acy shapes the way oral second languages are pro-
ing a first language (L1). As Slobin (1993)               cessed and learned (Tarone, Bigelow, & Hanson,
describes,                                                2009), so the fact that almost all SLA research on
You can also read