The important role of private wildlife organisations in facilitating scientific research: A case study of the Australian Dingo Foundation - Allen ...

Page created by Lewis Sullivan
 
CONTINUE READING
The important role of private wildlife organisations in facilitating scientific research: A case study of the Australian Dingo Foundation - Allen ...
Theme edition. The dingo dilemma: cull, contain or conserve, edited by Thomas Newsome, Chris Dickman and Daniel
                                                           Lunney.

The important role of private wildlife
organisations in facilitating scientific research:
A case study of the Australian Dingo
Foundation
Bradley P. Smith1,2, Shennai G. Palermo2, and Lyn Watson2*
1
   Smith Human-Wildlife Coexistence Lab, School of Human Health and Social Sciences, Central

                                                                                                                                         Downloaded from http://meridian.allenpress.com/australian-zoologist/article-pdf/doi/10.7882/AZ.2020.023/2645716/10.7882_az.2020.023.pdf by guest on 12 December 2020
   Queensland University Australia, Adelaide Campus, 44 Greenhill Road, Wayville, South
   Australia, Australia.
2
  Australian Dingo Foundation, Toolern Vale, Victoria, Australia.
 *
   To whom correspondence should be addressed:
Mrs Lyn Watson. Australian Dingo Foundation. P.O. Box 502, Gisborne, Victoria 3437, Australia.
   Tel: +61 03 5428 1245. E-mail: dingodiscovery@yahoo.com.au

                  As we enter an era of global mass extinctions, it is important to tackle wildlife research and
                  conservation from multiple fronts, including those made available by wildlife organisations, zoos
                  and sanctuaries. Captive studies are particularly useful when studying free-ranging populations is
                  difficult, and/or when controlled conditions are required. Yet, despite the significant role that they
                  play in supporting research and conservation of species and ecosystems, they are rarely recognised
       ABSTRACT

                  in the scientific literature. Here we present a case study of the Australian Dingo Foundation (ADF),
                  a private organisation and captive breeding facility that actively supports research and conservation
                  efforts relating to the dingo (Canis dingo). Over the past decade (2010 to 2020), the ADF has
                  facilitated research across eight research disciplines that include archaeology, behaviour, biology,
                  cognition, evolutionary psychology, non-lethal management, reproduction and parental behaviour, and
                  vocalisations. This has resulted in at least 21 published scientific studies which are summarised in
                  this paper. As this case study demonstrates, captive facilities have the potential to contribute to the
                  understanding and conservation of dingoes by providing practical alternatives to, and/or supplement
                  studies of free-ranging populations. We conclude by outlining some of the implications and limitations
                  of conducting research using captive dingo populations.

                  Key words: Science; research; conservation; captivity; zoos; sanctuaries; organisations; not-for-profit.

                                                                                              DOI: https://doi.org/10.7882/AZ.2020.023

Introduction
Not-for-profit wildlife organisations, zoos and sanctuaries            behaviour. Work was directly carried out at their custom
provide a fundamental role in the conservation of                      research facilities in Alice Springs, NT (Division of Wildlife
many of Australia’s native species such as the dingo                   Research; Figure 1) and Canberra, ACT (Arid Zone
(Canis dingo). Not only do captive populations provide                 Research Institute). Funding for much of the early research
opportunities to observe and engage with wildlife,                     with dingoes, including those carried out by the CSIRO,
they are actively involved in conservation education,                  was provided by the Australian Meat Research Committee
wildlife tourism, breeding programs, and the facilitation              and the Australian Meat and Livestock Corporation, in an
and financial support of research (Tribe 2001; Tribe                   effort to “know thine enemy” (Newsome et al. 1972, p.1),
and Booth 2003). The contribution to the latter is                     and thus assist with eradication attempts. Contemporary
particularly important, because research investigating                 funding for dingo research comes from a range of sources,
the biology and behaviour of many free-ranging species                 and is conducted by scientists from around the world. These
is rarely possible, or at least very difficult.                        researchers represent a number of scientific disciplines,
                                                                       and assert that the dingo is a scientifically and culturally
There is a long history of utilising captive dingoes for               valuable species (Smith 2015). Research and the number
research, the first of which were carried out by CSIRO                 of scientific publications focusing on the dingo has also
in the 1970s, 80s and early 90’s (Corbett 2001). These                 increased dramatically over the past few decades (see Smith
captive studies, led by ecologists Alan Newsome, Laurie                and Appleby 2015), including research involving captive
Corbett, and Peter Catling, were used to complement                    dingo populations.
their field studies on several aspects of dingo biology and

2020
                                                      Australian
                                                            Zoologist                                                              A
The important role of private wildlife organisations in facilitating scientific research: A case study of the Australian Dingo Foundation - Allen ...
Smith et al.

                                                                                                                                Downloaded from http://meridian.allenpress.com/australian-zoologist/article-pdf/doi/10.7882/AZ.2020.023/2645716/10.7882_az.2020.023.pdf by guest on 12 December 2020
Figure 1. CSIRO experimental officer with captive dingoes held at their research center in the Northern Territory.
Photo taken in 1967. Source: National Archives of Australia, A1200, L64195.
The benefits of captive research                                 In captivity, not only can housing conditions be
                                                                 manipulated and replicated, but the identification and
Like most wild canids, dingoes are notoriously difficult to      selection of individuals is easier (including sex, age, body
study in the wild because they are highly mobile, occupy         mass, ancestry, genetics). Scientists have greater power
large territories, are difficult to see in many environments,    to take medical, physiological or observational samples
and are generally reclusive (Klinghammer and Goodman             from individuals with known life histories. To give an
1987; Morrant 2015). As such, the study of many aspects          example, to estimate the food consumption of dingoes,
of biology and behaviour in the field become impractical         Green (1978) required carefully controlled experiments
and are better suited to captive situations where                with tame dingoes in laboratory settings. In the CSIRO
conditions can be controlled. In fact, a more complete           facilities, researchers were able to extensively study dingo
understanding of behaviour can often be achieved when            reproductive biology, behaviour and seasonality (Newsome
field observations are supplemented by, or aided by,             et al. 1973; Catling 1979; Catling, Corbett, and Newsome
observations conducted in captivity. To give an example,         1992); to look at the impact and identification of dingo-
Allen (2015) used dingo paw prints left in sand plots to         dog hybrids, including genetics and coat colour (Newsome
determine the impact of poison baiting programs on the           and Corbett 1982); and to develop age determination
abundance and age distribution of the resident dingoes.          techniques and measure growth rates (Catling, Corbett,
To facilitate this research, measurements of dingo paws          and Westcott 1991).
from captive dingoes of known ages were collected and
used as a reference sample. Much of our understanding            Captive environments also offer a valuable and cost-
of the social behaviour of wild canids was gained through        effective opportunity to test the behavioural responses
captive wolves (Schenkel 1947) and later supported by            of dingoes to non-lethal tools or strategies before large
field studies by Mech (1970). Similarly, a lot of early          scale field testing. For instance, the effectiveness of an
knowledge of dingo social behaviour was based on studies         ultrasonic device as a deterrent (Edgar et al. 2007).
of captive populations and substituted with observations         Captive dingo populations have been used to test the
of free-ranging dingoes (Corbett and Newsome 1975;               behavioural responses of dingoes to dingo urine (Robley
Corbett 1988). Captive research continues to add value           et al. 2015), and the anti-predator reactions of kangaroos
to our understanding of wild canids (e.g. Kershenbaum et         to dingo urine (Parsons and Blumstein 2010). The
al. 2016; Gese et al. 2018).                                     collection of urine (including from known individuals)
                                                                 for such purposes would be virtually impossible in the

B
                                                  Australian
                                                        Zoologist                                                      2020
The contribution of captive dingo populations to scientific research

wild from living animals, as would a controlled setting for      as government watchdogs and lobbyists. 5) Facilitate
determining its effect on behaviour.                             scientific research. The significance of these roles is
                                                                 largely under-acknowledged, particularly by the scientific
There are many questions regarding ethology and                  community. With this in mind, we wish to draw attention
psychology that require well-socialised animals that can         to some of the positive and valuable roles that sanctuaries
only be found in captive situations. Some argue that             can play in aiding scientific studies and conservation efforts.
for certain behaviours (such as following social cues),          One organisation, the Australian Dingo Foundation (and
wolves must be intensely socialised and kept separate            its affiliated Dingo Discovery Sanctuary and Research
from conspecifics for most of their first 4-6 months in          Centre), has provided an invaluable contribution to the
order to get accurate results (suggested by Klinghammer          scientific understanding of the dingo across multiple fields

                                                                                                                                   Downloaded from http://meridian.allenpress.com/australian-zoologist/article-pdf/doi/10.7882/AZ.2020.023/2645716/10.7882_az.2020.023.pdf by guest on 12 December 2020
and Goodman 1987, and adopted in the study by Miklósi            of research.
et al. 2003). Research investigating dingo cognition and
behaviour conducted by Smith and Litchfield (2010a;              The Australian Dingo Foundation (ADF), established
2010b; 2013) and parental behaviour (Hudson et al.               in 2006, is a not-for-profit organisation and Federally
2016; Hudson et al. 2019) were possible because captive          registered conservation charity. The ADF supports a
populations provided a sample of individuals who were            captive population of dingoes at the Dingo Discovery
known, who were comfortable being in test situations             Sanctuary and Research Centre (hereafter Dingo
and handled by staff, and a controlled environment for           Discovery Centre). The Dingo Discovery Centre,
the experimental setup. In addition, captivity presents          established in 1990, is located on 40 acres in the foothills
opportunities for ad hoc observations to occur, such as the      of the Macedon Ranges, Victoria (Figure 2). At any one
use of tools (Smith et al. 2012).                                time, the sanctuary houses around 40 genetically ‘pure’
                                                                 and entire adult dingoes in custom-built enclosures. This
Socialised dingoes also aid the process of taking biological     includes a population of captive and wild born individuals
samples in a stress-free manner, often avoiding the need for     from all regions of Australia. For a comprehensive summary
sedation. An example of this was the longitudinal study by       of the housing and management of this population see
Smith, Flavel, and Simpson (2016) who measured seasonal          Smith and Watson (2015).
variation in cortisol. Not only does interacting with
captive animals provide insights not otherwise practical         The ADF and its volunteers play a significant role in
in the wild (e.g., comparative studies of problem solving        dingo conservation through advocacy (e.g., organising
behaviour - Smith and Litchfield 2010a, 2010b, 2013),            petitions, lobbying government, social media presence),
but failure to socialise captive wolves and dingoes for          education (e.g., running sanctuary tours, puppy open days,
certain experiments may lead to erroneous results because        fundraising events, community and school education
the presence of humans, particularly in close proximity,         sessions), and an active breeding program (e.g., maintaining
might disrupt the animal’s behaviour (Klinghammer and            several breeding lines, supplying zoos and wildlife parks
Goodman 1987).                                                   with dingoes for exhibition). Through these activities
                                                                 they also facilitate and support non-invasive scientific
Through wild rescue and captive breeding programs,               research by making their captive dingo population
populations held in private sanctuaries may provide a            available to national and international scientists. The
reservation of genetically ‘pure’ dingoes from regions           Dingo Discovery Centre has been directly supporting
around the country, should dingoes become endangered             genetic studies since its inception, and behavioural studies
or absent. They may also be useful for reintroduction            since the late 1990’s (Wilton et al. 1999; Wilton 2001),
programs involving the release of dingoes into national          and the ADF has supported dingo research by providing
parks (Ritchie et al. 2012; Newsome et al. 2015). Private        financial support through scholarships such as the ‘NWG
sanctuaries with active breeding colonies, typically have        and Anne Macintosh Student Scholarship’.
skilled dingo handlers, who can likely offer valuable services
and consultation for reintroduction and rewilding programs.      The sanctuary maintains a healthy, entire, and diverse
                                                                 population of dingoes which enables them to suit the
                                                                 requirements of researchers from a plethora of research
The Australian Dingo Foundation
                                                                 disciplines (for examples, see Figure 3). These include (but
There are a handful of private not-for-profit organisations      not limited to): animal behaviour (e.g., social behaviour
scattered across the country that maintain captive dingoes       and parental care), cognition (e.g., problem solving,
and are dedicated to dingo conservation. As outlined             higher order behaviour), evolutionary and comparative
by Smith and Watson (2015) these organisations make              psychology, biology (e.g., microbiome, stress responses,
at least five key contributions. 1) The preservation of          urine chemistry), non-lethal management (e.g., testing
genetically ‘pure’ dingoes from regions across Australia         efficacy of non-lethal biological deterrents), and genetic
(i.e., dingoes with no modern dog ancestry). 2) Provide          testing. To demonstrate this, in Table 1 we present
a source of animals for displays at wildlife parks, and for      a number of published scientific studies that resulted
future rewilding programs. 3) Raise community knowledge          from research that took place or based on dingoes held
and awareness of dingoes and their conservation. 4) Act          at the sanctuary between 2010 and 2020. The list of

2020
                                                   Australian
                                                         Zoologist                                                           C
Smith et al.

A)                                                                       B)

                                                                                                                                 Downloaded from http://meridian.allenpress.com/australian-zoologist/article-pdf/doi/10.7882/AZ.2020.023/2645716/10.7882_az.2020.023.pdf by guest on 12 December 2020
C)

 Figure 2. The Dingo Discovery Centre. (a) A section of the custom dingo housing (Photo: Bradley Smith). (b) Closeup
 of the enclosures (Photo: Bradley Smith). (c) A dingo in one of the exercise yards (Photo: Shari Trimble).

 publications was sourced from the records maintained by         physiological studies of dingoes that are born and raised
 the ADF, and the methodology and acknowledgements of            in captivity (O’Regan and Kitchener 2005; Hartstone-
 each publication cross-checked to ensure the sanctuary          Rose et al. 2014), particularly where selective breeding
 or foundation was acknowledged as a contributor. It is          and multiple generations of captive-bred animals are used.
 likely that additional studies have been conducted at           Although in saying this, the appropriateness is entirely
 the sanctuary, but the results not published, and it is also    dependent on the purpose of the study. For example,
 possible that not all published outcomes are presented in       captive-bred animals can provide a unique opportunity
 the table (although none were deliberately omitted).            to assess the relative importance of environmental and
                                                                 genetic influences on morphology (O’Regan and Kitchener
                                                                 2005); for examining human-animal relationships and
 Cautions when using captive dingo
                                                                 interactions (e.g. Ward and Melfi 2015); and of course
 populations for scientific studies                              for studying captive welfare, behaviour and management
 It is important when studying the behaviour of wild             (Hill and Broom 2009).
 animals in captivity that they be kept in a way that
 maximises their opportunities for the expression of normal      There are also some behaviours observable in captivity
 behaviour. One concern that has been raised about using         that may not translate to wild populations. For example, in
 captive populations for research, and their value to dingo      wild canids, tool use has been observed in captivity (Smith
 conservation more broadly, is that captivity somehow            and Litchfield 2012) but not in free-ranging populations.
 changes the individual so it no longer truly represents         Although this provides insight into the cognitive potential
 the species. Corbett (2001) for instance, argued that a         of the species, the behaviour observed in captivity is likely
 captive dingo ceases to be a dingo and is “just another         to be a reflection of increased opportunity and energy to
 dog” because selection pressure for certain traits can          explore and manipulate objects within the environment.
 easily take place, both deliberately and inadvertently,         More realistic and ecologically valid examples of tool-use
 and therefore could override the natural selection of wild      and higher order behaviours have been observed in the
 characteristics. The impact of captivity, which include         field in other canids (see Smith, Appleby, and Litchfield
 unnatural diets (even when foods are mimicked to provide        2012 and references within), but such observations remain
 adequate nutrition) and restriction of movement, is likely      rare and difficult to capture.
 to be most significant in relation to morphological and

     D
                                                  Australian
                                                        Zoologist                                                       2020
The contribution of captive dingo populations to scientific research

    Table 1. A list of publications grouped across eight discipline areas, that has resulted from research carried out at
    the Dingo Discovery Sanctuary and Research Centre (DDC) between 2010 and 2020. A brief summary of the
    background and/or aims, methodology, and main findings from each publication are provided.

Discipline     Title                        Background/Aims             Methods                      Findings                            Reference
Archaeology    Dingo scat-bone              To address the lack         Scats from 25 captive        Wild and captive dingo skeletal     Reynolds,
               ‘signature patterns’:        of data on carnivore        dingoes from the DDC         modifications were similar but      Dortch,
               An actualistic study         modifications to skeletal   and 31 wild dingo            dependent on prey consumed.         and Balme
               and comparison of            fragments with focus on     scats were analysed          Captive dingo scat contained        (2016)
               wild and captive scat-       the dingo.                  for skeletal fragments.      fewer and smaller skeletal
               bone assemblages             Useful as a potential       Tooth ‘pits’ and ‘scores’    fragments than wild dingo
               and interpretation of                                    were also measured on        scats. Both tooth-mark and

                                                                                                                                                       Downloaded from http://meridian.allenpress.com/australian-zoologist/article-pdf/doi/10.7882/AZ.2020.023/2645716/10.7882_az.2020.023.pdf by guest on 12 December 2020
                                            investigative tool
               bone fragments from          at Holocene                 skeletal fragments.          scat techniques are best used
               Witchcliffe Rock Shelter,    archaeological sites.                                    in combination. Dingoes have
               south Western Australia.                                                              characteristic tooth-marks,
                                                                                                     of which were identified on
                                                                                                     some skeletal fragments found
                                                                                                     in Witchcliffe rock shelter.
               Analysis of pit and          Comparative approach        DDC was used to              Devils and tiger quolls             Koungoulos,
               score tooth-mark sizes       for archaeological          pilot the methodology        produced very large tooth           Faulkner,
               from bones modified          material, using dingoes,    used in the larger study.    marks in relation to their          and
               by Holocene Australian       devils, quolls, monitor     Experimental feeding         body size. Aussie fauna does        Asmussen
               terrestrial fauna in         lizards, dogs and human     trials of captive animals,   not conform to international        (2018)
               relation to body size.       pit marks and scores        including analysis of        trends, that is, body weight
                                            left on bone.               2895 tooth marks.            does not correlate with tooth
                                                                                                     mark size. Carnivores that
                                                                                                     consume bone in their diet
                                                                                                     will have specific adaptations
                                                                                                     that will determine pit mark
                                                                                                     and score sizes left on bone
                                                                                                     fragments.
Behaviour      The function of play         To investigate the role     Camera footage was           The behavioural function            Byosiere et
               bows in Canis lupus          of domestication on the     taken of 5 litters           of play bows (i.e. as a             al. (2018)
               and its variants: A          function of behavioural     (10 puppies total)           visual signal) is similar for
               comparison of dingo          play bows in canids.        interacting at the DDC.      dingoes, domestic dogs
               (Canis lupus dingo), dog                                 Play behaviour was           and wolves. Plays bows are
               (Canis lupus familiaris)                                 observed and coded           more exaggerated, longer,
               and wolf puppies (Canis                                  according to five criteria   and involve vocalisations in
               lupus).                                                  and compared with            domestic dogs compared with
                                                                        those of dogs and            dingoes and wolves.
                                                                        wolves.
Biology        The effect of age,           To analyse the canid        Scat samples from 101        Canid microbiome differences        Flavel
               gender, and antibiotics      microbiome at different     domestic dogs and            can be attributed to                (2012)
               on the canine gut            life stages, and how it     from 28 dingoes at the       various factors, such as age.
               microbiome.                  is affected by factors      DDC were analysed            Bacteroidetes/ prevotella group
                                            like age and gender. To     for two main bacterial       and clostridia coccoides were
                                            compare the microbe         classes, bacteroidetes       found in Labradors and
                                            of domestic dogs and        and clostridia.              dingoes at different life stages.
                                            dingoes.
               Quantification of            To better understand        Saliva samples were          Cubs had higher cortisol            Smith,
               salivary cortisol from       ‘stress’ and its            collected from 13            levels than adults. Adults did      Flavel, and
               captive dingoes (Canis       implications on captive     adult dingoes, and 8         not differ, but males during        Simpson
               dingo) in relation to age,   and wild dingoes. To        dingo cubs from the          breeding season had higher          (2016)
               gender, and breeding         obtain a reference          DDC across several           cortisol levels than females.
               season.                      range of dingo salivary     time points (across a
                                            cortisol levels.            day, development and
                                                                        seasonality).
Cognition      How well do dingoes          To test dingoes on a        A v-shaped version of        Dingoes were proficient at          Smith and
               (Canis dingo) perform        spatial problem-solving     the ‘detour task’ using      completing the task with fewer      Litchfield
               on the detour task.          task. Previously tested     four experimental            errors and shorter delays than      (2010a)
                                            wolves performed            conditions was tested        dogs. Wolves and dingoes are
                                            better than domestic        on 20 dingoes at the         more capable at non-social
                                            dogs - a potential result   DDC. Success, latency        spatial problem solving than
                                            of domestication.           and errors detouring         domestic dogs.
                                                                        fence were measured.

    2020
                                                          Australian
                                                                Zoologist                                                                      E
Smith et al.

Discipline     Title                      Background/Aims              Methods                     Findings                           Reference
               Dingoes (Canis dingo)      To test the                  The ‘object-choice          Dingoes passed 6/9                 Smith and
               can use human social       responsiveness of            task’ was tested on 7       human social cues. Dingo           Litchfield
               cues to locate hidden      dingoes to human social      dingoes at the DDC to       performance was more               (2010b)
               food.                      cues, as they have a         see how they respond        akin to wolves than dogs,
                                          unique evolutionary          to 9 different human        suggesting domestication may
                                          history that may shed        social cues. Success        influence the comprehension
                                          light on canid evolution.    resulted in approaching     of human gestures.
                                                                       container being referred
                                                                       to by human.
               Spontaneous tool-use:      To document the              Two male dingoes            Observations of tool-use           Smith,

                                                                                                                                                   Downloaded from http://meridian.allenpress.com/australian-zoologist/article-pdf/doi/10.7882/AZ.2020.023/2645716/10.7882_az.2020.023.pdf by guest on 12 December 2020
               An observation of a        cognitive abilities of two   at the DDC were             extend to canids. Highlights       Appleby,
               dingo (Canis dingo)        captive dingoes, and the     observed and recorded       the dingoes potential for          and
               using a table to access    first case of tool-use in    exhibiting higher order     ‘intelligent’ behaviour adds       Litchfield
               an out-of-reach food       the Canidae family.          behaviours. 1) Sterling     insight into their cognitive       (2012)
               reward.                                                 moved a table in the        abilities and potential.
                                                                       enclosure to reach an
                                                                       otherwise unobtainable
                                                                       treat.
                                                                       2) Teddy opened a gate
                                                                       latch to get to his mate
                                                                       when he was separated
                                                                       from her.
               Looking back at ‘looking   To find out whether          A ‘rope-pulling task’ was   Dingoes do not rely on             Smith and
               back’: Operationalizing    dingoes look-back            tested on 12 dingoes        humans for assistance during       Litchfield
               referential gaze for       for human assistance         at the DDC, with a          tasks in the same way as dogs.     (2013)
               dingoes in an unsolvable   when faced with              solvable trial, followed    A variety of ‘look-backs’ was
               task.                      an unsolvable task.          by an unsolvable one.       observed in dingoes but was
                                          Previously tested canids     Behaviour in response       not likely associated with
                                          included domestic dogs       to unsolvable condition     seeking human assistance. This
                                          and wolves. Domestic         coded.                      resulted in the refinement of
                                          dogs looked back to                                      the definition of a ‘look-back’.
                                          humans for assistance,
                                          wolves did not.
Evolutionary   The evolution and          To investigate the           An ‘inequity aversion       Both canid species did not         McAuliffe
psychology     development of             expression of ‘judging       task’ was tested on 72      express ‘inequity aversion’;       (2013)
               inequity aversion.         fairness’ or ‘inequity       dogs and 11 dingoes         not sensitive when only
                                          aversion’ in dogs and        at the DDC. Unfamiliar      ‘confederate partner’
                                          dingoes. To determine        human handlers and          received food. May not be
                                          whether this is a            unfamiliar dog or           a cooperative mechanism
                                          result of a cooperative      dingo ‘confederate          in social canids. Looking at
                                          mechanism found in           partners’ were used.        human handlers was more
                                          social canids, or a result   Trials involved absence/    prevalent in dogs than in
                                          of domestication.            presence of food. Video     dingoes.
                                                                       recordings were coded
                                                                       and analysed; sensitivity
                                                                       to presence/absence
                                                                       of food, response time
                                                                       and looking at human
                                                                       handlers.
               Exploring the              To investigate the           Three experimental          No evidence for overimitation      Johnston,
               evolutionary origins       origins of learning          trials used on 40 dogs      in dogs or dingoes. Dingoes        Holden,
               of overimitation: A        via ‘copying’ or             and 13 dingoes at the       may be better at finding           and Santos
               comparison across          ‘overimitating’ in           DDC. 1) Transparent         aspects of the puzzle that are     (2016)
               domesticated and non-      humans. To establish         puzzle box, lever not       irrelevant, compared to dogs.
               domesticated canids.       whether or not               needed to obtain treat.     Overimitation appears to be
                                          overimitation is a           2) Non-transparent          a unique form of learning in
                                          shared learning skill with   puzzle box, lever not       humans.
                                          dogs, which have close       needed to obtain treat.
                                          social bonding with
                                          humans.                      3) Transparent puzzle
                                                                       box, lever needed to
                                          Dingoes used as a            obtain treat.
                                          comparison to dogs.

    F
                                                         Australian
                                                               Zoologist                                                                2020
The contribution of captive dingo populations to scientific research

Discipline     Title                      Background/Aims             Methods                    Findings                              Reference
               Uncovering the origins     To explore interspecific    Duration of eye contact    Dingoes established eye               Johnston et
               of dog–human eye           communication using         made by dingoes            contact more than wolves,             al. (2017)
               contact: Dingoes           human-dingo eye             was recorded over a        but less than dogs. Human-
               establish eye contact      contact. Dingoes            5-minute period with       canid eye contact may have
               more than wolves, but      were tested to gain         23 dingoes at the          developed at the onset of
               less than dogs.            further insight into        DDC. Familiarity was       domestication. Prolonged eye
                                          canid domestication,        also tested to see if      contact may have evolved
                                          given their unique          eye contact changed        later, especially toward familiar
                                          evolutionary history.       according to whether       humans, as observed in
                                                                      or not the dingo knew      domestic dogs.
                                                                      the handler.

                                                                                                                                                       Downloaded from http://meridian.allenpress.com/australian-zoologist/article-pdf/doi/10.7882/AZ.2020.023/2645716/10.7882_az.2020.023.pdf by guest on 12 December 2020
Non-lethal     How guardian dogs          To determine how            Dingo vocalisation         Dingo vocalisation playbacks          van Bommel
management     protect livestock from     maremma dogs                recordings (howling)       and scent elicited behavioural        and Johnson
               predators: Territorial     maintain territories        and urine samples were     responses in maremma                  (2015)
               enforcement by             inclusive of livestock.     taken from dingoes         sheepdogs. If ‘threat’ was
               maremma sheepdogs.         That is, whether they       housed at the DDC.         inside range, they left sheep
                                          do so directly when         Sound and scent tests      in pursuit of ‘threat’. If ‘threat’
                                          livestock are threatened,   were carried out on        was outside range, they just
                                          or indirectly (through      two sheep properties       barked, and scent marked.
                                          sound and scent).           with 4 GPS collared        Thus, protection appears to
                                                                      maremma sheepdogs          extend beyond the immediate
                                                                      on each. Cameras were      vicinity of livestock, so they do
                                                                      also set up to record      maintain a larger territory.
                                                                      maremma behavioural
                                                                      responses.
               Dingo semio-chemicals:     To assess the viability     Urine collected from 6     Identification of 27 chemicals        Robley et al.
               Towards a non-lethal       of the use of semio-        sexually mature males      were found to be unique               (2015)
               control tool for the       chemicals, extracted        at a private property.     to dingoes. Dominant male
               management of dingoes      from dominant male          Semio-chemicals            urine will cause a behavioural
               and wild dogs in           dingo urine, as a non-      specific to dingoes were   response. Utilisation of
               Australia.                 lethal tool in dingo/wild   extracted from the         extracted semio-chemicals
                                          dog management.             above urine samples        from male dingo urine appears
                                                                      and used to test the       to be a valuable non-lethal
                                                                      responses of 14 dingoes    tool.
                                                                      at the DDC.
               Fladry as a non-lethal     To determine how            Exposed 10 dingoes at      Minor behavioural reaction.           Smith et al.
               deterrent for dingoes.     captive dingoes             the DDC over three         Fladry may offer a cost               (2019)
                                          responded to fladry (a      experimental trials to a   effective, temporary barrier,
                                          rope line with brightly     6x6m exclusion zone        but aversive effects likely to
                                          coloured flags).            lined with fladry.         dissipate quickly.
               Automated shepherds:       To test whether a novel     Exposed 12 dingoes         Acoustic deterrent was not            Smith et al.
               Captive dingo              acoustic deterrent and      at the DDC to two          effective. Inflatable effigy highly   (2020)
               responses to sound and     effigy are effective non-   conditions (loud gun       effective, and remained so
               an inflatable, moving      lethal deterrents.          shots, and an inflatable   over multiple exposures (9/36
               effigy.                                                moving effigy). Success    accessed food across trials).
                                                                      determined if dingoes
                                                                      were prevented from
                                                                      eating from food bowls.
Reproduction   Variation in               To compare the              Literature review used     Highlighted the different             Lord et al.
and parental   reproductive traits        parental and                to gather parental         reproductive strategies               (2013)
behaviour      of members of the          reproductive strategies     and reproductive data      between domestic and
               genus Canis with           of domestic and wild        for all canids. Captive    wild canids across multiple
               special attention to the   canids.                     breeding records           domains. Dingoes more
               domestic dog (Canis                                    collected at the DDC       akin to wild than domestic
               familiaris).                                           over ten years were        canids. Dingoes mate during
                                                                      used as part of the        decreasing daylight (southern
                                                                      comparison between         hemisphere), and wolves
                                                                      wild and domestic dog      during increasing daylight
                                                                      reproductive timing.       (northern hemisphere).

    2020
                                                         Australian
                                                               Zoologist                                                                    G
Smith et al.

Discipline      Title                       Background/Aims              Methods                     Findings                             Reference
                Pattern of nipple use by    In relation to suckling      Video cameras placed        No difference in suckling            Hudson et
                puppies: A comparison       behaviours in altricial      inside the den of 4         behaviour was found between          al. (2016)
                of the Australian dingo     young, previously            dingo litters (12 puppies   domestic dog puppies or
                and the domestic dog.       studied domestic dog         total) at the DDC.          dingo puppies. No indication
                                            puppies exhibited no         Suckling behaviour          that the lack of suckling order
                                            suckling pattern but         (including nipple use,      is a trait of domestication,
                                            kittens do. Aim was to       time and duration of        rather reflects a difference in
                                            determine whether the        suckling) was observed      lifestyles between canids and
                                            difference was due to        and coded according to      felids.
                                            domestication.               4 measures. Previously
                                                                         collected data on dog

                                                                                                                                                         Downloaded from http://meridian.allenpress.com/australian-zoologist/article-pdf/doi/10.7882/AZ.2020.023/2645716/10.7882_az.2020.023.pdf by guest on 12 December 2020
                                                                         litters was also used.
                Diurnal pattern of          There is a lack of           Continuous camera           Evidence of bi-parental care in      Hudson et
                pre-weaning den visits      research looking at early    monitoring inside           dingoes, and the lack thereof        al. (2019)
                and nursing in breeding     parental care in canids.     and outside of 4            in domestic dogs that may be
                pairs of captive dingoes    This paper aimed to          breeding pairs and their    attributed to a shift in reliance
                (Canis dingo).              understand the level of      litters at the DDC.         on humans. Mothers spent
                                            maternal and paternal        Study examined the          most of their time in the den
                                            care provided to pre-        behaviours (such as         nursing, especially at night.
                                            weaned cubs.                 mother/father time in       Fathers slept on top of the
                                                                         den, nursing time) from     den, seemingly to keep guard.
                                                                         birth to 3 weeks.
Vocalisations   The bark, the howl          To investigate the           Dingo ‘bark-howl’           The ‘howl’ and ‘bark’                Déaux et al.
                and the bark-howl:          acoustic structure of        recordings were             components were both                 (2016a)
                Identity cues in dingoes’   ‘bark-howls’ and the         collected from two          individually distinctive - the
                multicomponent calls.       behavioural signal ‘bark-    sanctuaries, including      former to a higher degree.
                                            howls’ represent in          the DDC. Wild               The ‘bark’ segment may
                                            dingoes.                     dingoes were tested in      function as an initial alert
                                                                         WA, with recordings         to obtain the receivers (i.e.
                                                                         played during the day/      family member) attention.
                                                                         night. ‘Bark’ and ‘howl’    Individuality of this vocalisation
                                                                         components were             may inform level of reliability
                                                                         acoustically measured       to the receiver. ‘Bark-howls’
                                                                         for individuality.          may function as alarm calls,
                                                                                                     signalling to receivers to be
                                                                                                     vigilant.
                Concatenation of ‘alert’    To further investigate       Dingo ‘bark-howl’           Dingoes were able to                 Déaux et al.
                and ‘identity’ segments     the function of ‘bark-       recordings were used        distinguish levels of familiarity    (2016b)
                in dingoes’ alarm calls.    howls’ in dingoes.           from two sanctuaries        using the ‘howl’ component;
                                            Aspects studied              in NSW. A familiar,         they were more responsive
                                            included behavioural         unfamiliar and bird         to unfamiliar ‘howls’ (potential
                                            responses to level of        recording was tested        threat) than familiar ‘howls’.
                                            familiarity, attention       on 18 dingoes at the        ‘Bark’ components served
                                            levels and vigilance         DDC. Camera footage         to initially attract receiver’s
                                            behaviours.                  was taken and coded         attention, regardless of
                                                                         according to three          familiarity. Dingoes were
                                                                         behavioural responses;      observed to always be vigilant
                                                                         no. of looks, movement      when a ‘bark-howl’ was heard,
                                                                         towards speaker and         supporting its function as an
                                                                         vigilance.                  alarm call.

    Another example is dominant female infanticide, as                       and lack of opportunity for individuals to disperse)
    reported by Corbett (1988). Corbett (1988) asserted that                 gave rise to this abnormal behaviour, or at least the
    dominant female infanticide was the primary mechanism                    commonality of it being observed. To date, dominant
    through which dingo populations were regulated. The                      female infanticide has not been reported in wild dingo
    main benefit being that mature females that no longer had                populations, and in fact, there are more examples of where
    young (after their pups had been dispatched by a dominant                it does not occur (e.g., Smith and Vague 2017).
    female) retained the capacity to suckle and supplementally
    feed the dominant female’s dependent young, increasing                   One final concern when using captive populations
    the chances of their survival. However, dominant female                  for behavioural experiments is the tendency to prefer
    infanticide has only been observed in this single captive                individuals who are suited to (or tolerate) captivity
    population. It is more than likely that the conditions of                and human interaction. For example, they do not show
    the study site (limited enclosure size, increased aggression,            fear response to the experimenter, equipment or testing

     H
                                                           Australian
                                                                 Zoologist                                                                 2020
The contribution of captive dingo populations to scientific research

A)                                                             B)

                                                                                                                               Downloaded from http://meridian.allenpress.com/australian-zoologist/article-pdf/doi/10.7882/AZ.2020.023/2645716/10.7882_az.2020.023.pdf by guest on 12 December 2020
C)                                                             D)

 Figure 3. A sample of research studies conducted at the Dingo Discovery Centre. (a) Testing the response of dingoes
 to fladry- Smith et al. 2019. (b) Cortisol sample being taken from a 3-week-old dingo cub- Smith, Flavel, and Simpson
 2016. (c) A dingo completing a problem solving task, with Dr Angie Johnston from Yale University. (d) Testing whether
 dingoes can follow human social gestures- Smith and Litchfield 2010b (Photo credits: Bradley Smith).

 scenario. This is particularly pertinent with dingoes,         not appear to have much impact on the expression of
 where many in captivity remain timid and exhibit               naturalistic parental behaviour. Where possible however,
 sensitivity to novelty (neophobia) and are almost always       care must be taken to limit the influence of captivity to
 excluded from testing (Smith 2015). Excluding such             experiments, limitations acknowledged, and caution used
 individuals can produce cleaner data (e.g., less failures),    when extrapolating findings to free-ranging populations.
 but can also lead to false interpretations of cognitive        Ideally, all captive based studies should be complemented
 ability (Miklósi, Topál, and Csányi 2004). Sample sizes of     and verified with studies conducted in wild populations.
 captive studies also remain low and tend to use samples
 from only one sanctuary/colony (see methods column in
                                                                Conclusion
 Table 1). Sample size limitations, and the potential for
 sanctuary specific behaviours (reflective of the samples       By highlighting the contribution of one organisation,
 history and genetics, as well as the conditions of the         we have shown that a wealth of knowledge can, and
 sanctuary) should be acknowledged.                             will continue to be gained about the dingo from captive
                                                                based studies across a number of discipline areas. Any
 Despite these important considerations, we argue that          of the issues related to captive based research can
 the impact and influence of captivity are likely to be         be mitigated with sound experimental design, and/or
 inconsequential to several disciplines (Smith and Watson       acknowledgements of limitations where applicable. There
 2015). As shown in Table 1, there are many studies that        are many exciting research opportunities and possibilities
 can be undertaken with captive populations that have           with captive dingoes that can help ensure the long-
 few issues or concerns relating to external validity, or       term future of the species. For example, facilitating the
 appear skewed due to captivity. Given the difficulty in        testing of effective non-lethal approaches to mitigate
 studying free-ranging dingoes, captive populations remain      dingo-livestock conflict, which remains one of the
 a valuable resource for scientific studies. For example,       biggest challenges to dingo conservation. We conclude
 observing parental behaviour in dens is difficult in the       by encouraging the recognition, utilisation and support of
 wild (especially with replicates), but can be conducted        private wildlife sanctuaries and organisations in Australia.
 in captivity with ease, and in many ways, and does

     2020
                                                 Australian
                                                       Zoologist                                                           I
Smith et al.

Acknowledgements
The authors wish to thank all those who have been                    years. None of this would be possible without them. We
involved with the Australian Dingo Foundation, and                   also thank two anonymous reviewers and Dan Lunney for
Dingo Discovery Sanctuary and Research Centre over the               their valuable feedback and suggestions.

References
Allen, L.R. 2015. Demographic and functional responses of            Déaux, E.C., Allen, A.P., Clarke, J.A., and Charrier, I. 2016b.
wild dogs to poison baiting. Ecological Management & Restoration     Concatenation of ‘alert’ and ‘identity’ segments in dingoes’ alarm
16: 58-66. doi: doi.org/10.1111/emr.12138                            calls. Scientific Reports 6: 30556. doi: https://doi.org/10.1038/

                                                                                                                                           Downloaded from http://meridian.allenpress.com/australian-zoologist/article-pdf/doi/10.7882/AZ.2020.023/2645716/10.7882_az.2020.023.pdf by guest on 12 December 2020
                                                                     srep30556
Byosiere, S.E., Espinosa, J., and Smith, B. 2018. The function
of play bows in Canis lupus and its variants: A comparison           Edgar, J.P., Appleby, R.G., and Jones, D.N. 2007. Efficacy of
of dingo (Canis lupus dingo), dog (Canis lupus familiaris) and       an ultrasonic device as a deterrent to dingoes (Canis lupus dingo):
wolf puppies (Canis lupus). Behaviour 155: 369-388. doi: doi.        A preliminary investigation. Journal of Ethology 25: 209-213. doi:
org/10.1163/1568539X-00003495                                        doi.org/10.1007/s10164-006-0004-1

Cairns, K. M., Shannon, L. M., Koler-Matznick, J., Ballard,          Flavel, M. 2012. The effect of age, gender, and antibiotics on
J. W. O., and Boyko, A. R. 2018. Elucidating biogeographical         the canine gut microbiome. Unpublished Honours Thesis, La
patterns in Australian native canids using genome wide SNPs.         Trobe University, Melbourne, VIC.
PloS ONE 13(6): e0198754. doi: doi.org/10.1371/journal.
pone.0198754                                                         Gese, E. M., Waddell, W. T., Terletzky, P. A., Lucash, C. F.,
                                                                     McLellan, S. R., and Behrns, S. K. 2018. Cross-fostering as a
Cairns, K. M., Nesbitt, B. J., Laffan, S. W., Letnic, M., and        conservation tool to augment endangered carnivore populations.
Crowther, M. S. 2019. Geographic hot spots of dingo genetic          Journal of Mammalogy 99: 1033-1041. doi: doi.org/10.1093/
ancestry in southeastern Australia despite hybridisation with        jmammal/gyy087
domestic dogs. Conservation Genetics, 1-14. doi: doi.org/10.1007/
s10592-019-01230-z                                                   Green, B. 1978. Estimation of food consumption of the dingo,
                                                                     Canis familiaris dingo, by means of ^(22)Na turnover. Ecology
Catling, P.C. 1979. Seasonal variation in plasma testosterone        59: 207-210. doi: doi.org/10.1111/emr.12138
and testis in captive male dingoes, Canis familiaris dingo.
Australian Journal of Zoology 27: 939-944. doi: doi.org/10.1071/     Hartstone-Rose, A., Selvey, H., Villari, J. R., Atwell, M., and
ZO9790939                                                            Schmidt, T. 2014. The three-dimensional morphological effects
                                                                     of captivity. PloS ONE 9: e113437. doi: doi.org/10.1371/journal.
Catling, P.C., Corbett, L.K., and Newsome, A.E. 1992.                pone.0113437
Reproduction in captive and wild dingoes (Canis familiaris
dingo) in temperate and arid environments of Australia. Wildlife     Hill, S. P., and Broom, D. M. 2009. Measuring zoo animal
Research 19: 195-209. doi: doi.org/10.1071/WR9920195                 welfare: Theory and practice. Zoo Biology 28: 531-544. doi:
                                                                     10.1002/zoo.20276
Catling, P.C., Corbett, L.K., and Westcott, M. 1991. Age
determination in the dingo and crossbreeds. Wildlife Research 18:    Hudson, R., Rödel, H., Elizalde, M., Arteaga, L., Kennedy,
75-83. doi: doi.org/10.1071/WR9910075                                G., and Smith, B. 2016. Pattern of nipple use by puppies:
                                                                     A comparison of the Australian dingo and the domestic dog.
Corbett, L. 1988. Social dynamics of a captive dingo pack:           Journal of Comparative Psychology 130: 269-277. doi: doi.
Population regulation by dominant female infanticide. Ecology        org/10.1037/com0000023
78: 177-198. doi: doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1988.tb00229.x
                                                                     Hudson R., Elizalde M., Kennedy G., Rödel, H., and Smith
Corbett, L. K., and Newsome, A. E. 1975. Dingo society and           B. 2019. Diurnal pattern of pre-weaning den visits and nursing
its maintenance: A preliminary analysis. Pp. 369-79 in The Wild      in breeding pairs of captive dingoes (Canis dingo). Mammalian
Canids. Their Systematics, Behavioural Ecology and Evolution,        Biology 94: 86-91. doi: doi.org/10.1016/j.mambio.2018.07.002
edited by M.W. Fox. Van Nostrand Reinhold Co., New York, NY.
                                                                     Johnston, A.M., Holden, P.C., and Santos, L.R. 2016.
Corbett, L.K. 2001. The dingo in Australia and Asia. JB Books,       Exploring the evolutionary origins of overimitation: A
Marleston, SA.                                                       comparison across domesticated canids. Developmental Science
                                                                     20: e12460. doi: doi.org/10.1111/desc.12460
Déaux, E.C., Charrier, I. and Clarke, J.A. 2016a. The
bark, the howl and the bark-howl: Identity cues in dingoes’
multicomponent calls. Behavioural Processes 129: 94-100. doi:
doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2016.06.012

J
                                                     Australian
                                                           Zoologist                                                             2020
The contribution of captive dingo populations to scientific research

Johnston, A., Turrin, C., Watson, L., Arre, A., and Santos,          Newsome, T.M., Ballard, G.A., Crowther, M.S., Dellinger, J.A.,
L.R. 2017. Uncovering the origins of dog–human eye contact:          Fleming, P.J., Glen, A.S., ... and Nimmo, D.G. 2015. Resolving
Dingoes establish eye contact more than wolves, but less than        the value of the dingo in ecological restoration. Restoration
dogs. Animal Behaviour 133: 123-129. doi: doi.org/10.1016/j.         Ecology 23: 201-208. doi: doi.org/10.1111/rec.12186
anbehav.2017.09.002
                                                                     O’Regan, H.J., and Kitchener, A.C. 2005. The effects of
Kershenbaum, A., Root-Gutteridge, H., Habib, B., Koler-              captivity on the morphology of captive, domesticated and feral
Matznick, J., Mitchell, B., Palacios, V., and Waller, S.             mammals. Mammal Review 35: 215-230. doi: doi.org/10.1111/
2016. Disentangling canid howls across multiple species and          j.1365-2907.2005.00070.x
subspecies: Structure in a complex communication channel.

                                                                                                                                         Downloaded from http://meridian.allenpress.com/australian-zoologist/article-pdf/doi/10.7882/AZ.2020.023/2645716/10.7882_az.2020.023.pdf by guest on 12 December 2020
Behavioural Processes 124: 149-157. doi: doi.org/10.1016/j.          Parsons, M.H., and Blumstein, D.T. 2010. Familiarity
beproc.2016.01.006                                                   breeds contempt: Kangaroos persistently avoid areas with
                                                                     experimentally deployed dingo scents. PLOS ONE 5. doi: doi.
Klinghammer, E., and Goodmann, P.A. 1987. Socialisation              org/10.1371/journal.pone.0010403
and management of wolves in captivity. Pp. 31-59 in Man and
Wolf, edited by H Frank. Dr W. Junk Publishers, Dordrecht,           Reynolds, J.E., Dortch, J., and Balme, J. 2016. Dingo scat-
Netherlands.                                                         bone ‘signature patterns’: An actualistic study and comparison
                                                                     of wild and captive scat-bone assemblages and interpretation of
Koungoulos, L., Faulkner, P., and Asmussen, B. 2018.                 bone fragments from Witchcliffe Rock Shelter, south western
Analysis of pit and score tooth-mark sizes from bones modified       Australia. Australian Archaeology 82: 218-231. doi: doi.org/10.10
by Holocene Australian terrestrial fauna in relation to body         80/03122417.2016.1240136
size. Journal of Archaeological Science: Reports 20: 271-283. doi:
doi.org/10.1016/j.jasrep.2018.05.006                                 Ritchie, E. G., Elmhagen, B., Glen, A. S., Letnic, M., Ludwig,
                                                                     G., and McDonald, R. A. 2012. Ecosystem restoration with
Lord, K., Feinstein, M., Smith, B., and Coppinger, R. 2013.          teeth: what role for predators? Trends in Ecology & Evolution 27:
Variation in reproductive traits of members of the genus Canis       265-271. doi: doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2012.01.001
with special attention to the domestic dog (Canis familiaris).
Behavioural Processes 92: 131-142. doi: doi.org/10.1016/j.           Robley, A., Lindeman, M., Cook, I., Woodford, L., and
beproc.2012.10.009                                                   Moloney, P. 2015. Dingo semiochemicals: Towards a non-lethal
                                                                     control tool for the management of dingoes and wild dogs in
McAuliffe, K.J. 2013. The Evolution and Development of               Australia. Arthur Rylah Institute for Environmental Research,
Inequity Aversion. Unpublished Doctoral Thesis. Harvard              Technical Report Series No. 263, Victoria.
University, MA.
                                                                     Schenkel, R. 1947. Expression studies of the wolf. Behaviour 1:
Mech, L.D. 1970. The wolf: Ecology and behaviour of an               81-129. doi: doi.org/10.1163/156853948X00065
endangered species. Natural History Press, New York, NY.
                                                                     Smith B., and Litchfield, C. 2010a. How well do dingoes
Miklósi, A., Kubinyi, E., Topál, J., Gácsi, M., Virányi, Z., and     (Canis dingo) perform on the detour task. Animal Behaviour 80:
Csányi, V. 2003. A simple reason for a big difference: Wolves do     155-162. doi: doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.04.017
not look back at humans, but dogs do. Current Biology 13: 763-
766. doi: doi.org/10.1016/S0960-9822(03)00263-X                      Smith, B., and Litchfield, C. 2010b. Dingoes (Canis dingo) can
                                                                     use human social cues to locate hidden food. Animal Cognition
Miklósi, A., Topál, J., and Csányi, V. 2004. Comparative social      13: 367-376. doi: doi.org/10.1007/s10071-009-0287-z
cognition: What can dogs teach us? Animal Behaviour 67: 995-
1004. doi: doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.10.008                     Smith, B., Appleby, R., and Litchfield, C. 2012. Spontaneous
                                                                     tool-use: An observation of a dingo (Canis dingo) using a table
Morrant, D. 2015. Chasing the yellow dog’s tale: The science         to access an out-of-reach food reward. Behavioural Processes 89:
of studying dingoes. Pp. 159-190 in The dingo debate: origins,       219-224. doi: doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2011.11.004
behaviour and conservation, edited by B. Smith. CSIRO
Publishing, Melbourne, VIC.                                          Smith, B., and Litchfield, C. 2013. Looking back at
                                                                     ‘looking back’: Operationalizing referential gaze for dingoes
Newsome, A.E., Corbett, L.K., Best, L.W., and Green, B.              in an unsolvable task. Animal Cognition 16: 961-971. doi: doi.
1972. The dingo. Australian Meat Research Committee Review           org/10.1007/s10071-013-0629-8
14: 1-11.
                                                                     Smith, B. 2015. Dingo intelligence: A dingo’s brain is sharper
Newsome, A.E., and Corbett, L. 1982. The identity of the             than it’s teeth. Pp. 215–249 in The dingo debate: Origins,
dingo. II. Hybridisation with domestic dogs in captivity and         behaviour and conservation, edited by B. Smith. CSIRO
in the wild. Australian Journal of Zoology 28: 615-625. doi: doi.    Publishing, Melbourne, VIC.
org/10.1071/ZO9820365

2020
                                                      Australian
                                                            Zoologist                                                              K
Smith et al.

Smith, B., and Appleby, R. 2015. Forging a new future                  Tribe, A. 2001. Captive wildlife tourism in Australia (Vol. 14).
for the Australian dingo. Pp. 301–315 in The dingo debate:             Gold Coast, Australia: CRC for Sustainable Tourism.
Origins, behaviour and conservation, edited by B. Smith.
CSIRO Publishing, Melbourne, VIC.                                      Tribe, A., and Booth, R. 2003. Assessing the role of zoos in
                                                                       wildlife conservation. Human Dimensions of Wildlife 8: 65-74.
Smith, B., and Watson, L. 2015. The role of private sanctuaries
in dingo conservation and the management of dingoes in                 van Bommel, L., and Johnson, C.N. 2015. How guardian
captivity. Pp. 277–299 in The dingo debate: origins, behaviour         dogs protect livestock from predators: Territorial enforcement by
and conservation, edited by B. Smith. CSIRO Publishing,                Maremma sheepdogs. Wildlife Research 41: 662-672. doi: https://
Melbourne, VIC.                                                        doi.org/10.1071/WR14190

                                                                                                                                           Downloaded from http://meridian.allenpress.com/australian-zoologist/article-pdf/doi/10.7882/AZ.2020.023/2645716/10.7882_az.2020.023.pdf by guest on 12 December 2020
Smith, B., Flavel, M., and Simpson, B. 2016. Quantification            Ward, S. J., and Melfi, V. 2015. Keeper-animal interactions:
of salivary cortisol from captive dingoes (Canis dingo) in relation    Differences between the behaviour of zoo animals affect
to age, gender, and breeding season. Australian Mammalogy 38:          stockmanship. PloS one, 10(10). doi: https://doi.org/10.1371/
21-28. doi: doi.org/10.1071/AM15017                                    journal.pone.0140237

Smith, B., and Vague, A-L. 2017. The denning behaviour of              Wilton, A.N., Steward, D.J., and Zafiris, K. 1999. Microsatellite
dingoes (Canis dingo) living in a human-modified environment.          variation in the Australian dingo. Journal of Heredity 90: 108-
Australian Mammalogy 39: 161-168. doi: doi.org/10.1071/                111. doi: doi.org/10.1093/jhered/90.1.108
AM16027
                                                                       Wilton, A.N. 2001. DNA methods of assessing dingo purity. Pp.
Smith, B., Moore, R., Appleby, R., and Blacker, A. 2019.               49–56 in A Symposium on the Dingo, edited by C.R. Dickman
Fladry as a non-lethal deterrent for dingoes. Poster presented         and D. Lunney. Royal Zoological Society of New South Wales,
at the Royal Zoological Society of New South Wales special             Mossman, NSW.
forum, ‘The dingo dilemma: cull, contain or conserve’. Sydney,
Australia. September 7, 2019.

Smith, B., Jaques, N., Appleby, R., Morris, S., and Jordan,
N. (2020). Automated shepherds: Captive dingo responses
to sound and an inflatable, moving effigy. Pacific Conservation
Biology, in press.

L
                                                       Australian
                                                             Zoologist                                                            2020
You can also read