The effects of unintentional drowsiness on the velocity of eyelid movements during spontaneous blinks
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
Physiological Measurement PAPER • OPEN ACCESS The effects of unintentional drowsiness on the velocity of eyelid movements during spontaneous blinks To cite this article: Murray Johns and Christopher Hocking 2021 Physiol. Meas. 42 014003 View the article online for updates and enhancements. This content was downloaded from IP address 46.4.80.155 on 13/09/2021 at 17:00
Physiol. Meas. 42 (2021) 014003 https://doi.org/10.1088/1361-6579/abd5c3 PAPER The effects of unintentional drowsiness on the velocity of eyelid OPEN ACCESS movements during spontaneous blinks RECEIVED 24 August 2020 Murray Johns1,2 and Christopher Hocking1 REVISED 1 16 December 2020 Optalert Australia Pty Ltd, 112 Balmain Street, Richmond, Melbourne, Victoria, 3121, Australia 2 School of Health Sciences, Swinburne University of Technology, Hawthorn, Melbourne, Victoria, 3122, Australia ACCEPTED FOR PUBLICATION 22 December 2020 E-mail: mjohns@optalert.com PUBLISHED Keywords: blinks, unintentional drowsiness, blepharometry, amplitude–velocity ratios 4 February 2021 Original content from this work may be used under Abstract the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 Objective. Unintentional drowsiness, when we should be alert, as for example when driving a vehicle, licence. can be very dangerous. In this investigation we examined the effects of unintentional drowsiness on Any further distribution of this work must maintain the relative velocities of eyelid closing and reopening movements during spontaneous blinks. attribution to the Approach. Twenty-four young adults volunteered to take part in this experiment, and 18 were finally author(s) and the title of the work, journal citation accepted. They performed a 15 min visual reaction-time test at the same time of day and under the and DOI. same environmental conditions with and without overnight sleep deprivation, one week apart. Their eyelid movements during blinks were monitored by a system of infrared reflectance blepharometry during each test. Main results. Very close relationships between the amplitude and maximum velocity of eyelid closing and reopening movements were confirmed. Frequency histograms of amplitude– velocity ratios (AVRs) for eyelid closing and reopening movements showed significant differences between alert and drowsy conditions. With drowsiness, eyelid movements became slower and AVRs increased for many but not all blinks. We also described a time-on-task effect on the relative velocities of eyelid movements which was more apparent in the drowsy condition. Eyelid movements became progressively slower during the first half of the test. This was presumably due to a short-lived alerting effect of starting the test. Significance. The relative velocity of eyelid closing and reopening movements during spontaneous blinks decreases with unintentional drowsiness but is sensitive to the brief alerting stimulus of starting a reaction-time test. Introduction When we choose to fall asleep intentionally it is usually after we have selected a warm, quiet, and comfortable place in which to lie down—a bed in a bedroom. When we are ready, we switch the lights off and settle down in our preferred sleeping position. We close our eyelids voluntarily and consequently stop blinking. We usually enter the state of drowsiness, the transitional state between wakefulness and sleep, within a few minutes. This is not a dangerous state to be in under those circumstances. However, under other circumstances, drowsiness can arise unintentionally at times when we should remain awake, as for example when driving a vehicle, a task that requires almost continuous visual attention. Unintentional drowsiness under those circumstances is dangerous and is a major cause of road crashes (Sagaspe et al 2010, Ftouni et al 2013). Thus, the difference between intentional and unintentional drowsiness mainly relates to voluntary eyelid closure and the intention of falling asleep in the former instance, while in the latter instance, the intention is to remain awake, typically to perform some task. Blinks continue to occur during unintentional drowsiness although many of them change their characteristics, especially the velocity of their eyelid closing and reopening movements, the subject of this investigation. Drowsiness is known to fluctuate and to show what has been called ‘state instability’, with variations over periods of seconds as demonstrated by analysis of the EEG (Doran et al 2001). There are periods when the EEG shows characteristics that are typical of wakefulness alternating with brief periods that are typical of the © 2021 Institute of Physics and Engineering in Medicine
Physiol. Meas. 42 (2021) 014003 M Johns and C Hocking Figure 1. Deep (left) and superficial (right) layers of eyelid muscles in the right eye. beginning of sleep, or microsleeps, during which there is loss of awareness of the here-and-now. That loss becomes continuous during sleep. The fluctuating nature of unintentional drowsiness can also be demonstrated by pupillometry (Yoss et al 1970), or by monitoring psychomotor performance, either intermittently during a reaction-time test such as the psychomotor vigilance test (PVT) (Dinges and Powell 1985, Dorrian et al 2005), or during a continuous tracking task, with lapses in performance associated with ‘behavioural microsleeps’ (Peiris et al 2006). In recent years, considerable emphasis has been placed on the investigation of unintentional drowsiness by measuring the characteristics of blinks (Cori et al 2019). Johns has coined the term blepharometry, which refers specifically to study of the eyelids, their various functions, and their movements. The terms oculography and oculometrics are less specific and refer mainly to the study of eye movements. For example, electrooculography has long been used to monitor eye movements as part of polysomnography in sleep laboratories (Chaudhary 2007). However, during investigations of unintentional drowsiness, it is more common for blinks to be monitored by other methods, particularly by infrared reflectance blepharometry (Caffier et al 2003, Johns et al 2007). This has been used to monitor blinks for prolonged periods during laboratory experiments (Anderson et al 2013, Wilkinson et al 2013) and while driving (Ftouni et al 2013, Soleimanloo et al 2019). Blinks can also be monitored by video camera images of the eyes and eyelids (Espinosa et al 2018). Another method, using a magnetic induction search coil attached to an upper eyelid while the head is fixed within a magnetic field, is very accurate but is suitable only for brief recordings in laboratory experiments (Evinger et al 1991). Blinks involve a coordinated sequence of eyelid closing and reopening movements, mainly due to the actions of two muscles in each eye, levator palpebrae superioris (LPS) and orbiculoris occuli (OO) (Evinger 1995, Bour et al 2000) (figure 1). The nerve supply to OO is via a branch of the facial nerve, whereas a branch of the oculomotor nerve supplies LPS. The ligaments which attach each end of LPS to the boney orbit are positioned such that, when LPS and OO muscles are relaxed, the eyelids remain closed, as happens during sleep. During wakefulness, the eyelids are open most of the time because of the tonic activation of LPS. Each blink begins with the phasic inhibition of the LPS (Aramideh et al 1994, Evinger 1995). A few milliseconds later there is phasic activation of OO to close the eyelids. This mainly involves activation of the ‘fast-twitch’ palpebral fibres of OO rather than its orbital fibres (Bour et al 2000). Each OO muscle acts in conjunction with forces due to the elastic properties of the ligamentous attachments of LPS which assist in closing the eyelids. Thus, there are two forces acting together to close the eyelids—the contraction of OO and the pull of elastic ligaments on LPS. In alert wakefulness the upper and lower eyelids are in contact for only a few milliseconds before OO relaxes and LPS contracts phasically to reopen the eyelids. Sometimes the reopening movement begins before the upper eyelid has made contact with the lower lid. These partial blinks sometimes make up a considerable proportion of all blinks (Ousler et al 2014). The contractile force of LPS in reopening the eyelids is partially counteracted by the downward pull on that muscle due its ligamentous attachments (Evinger 1995). At the end of each blink, the upper tarsal muscle (Müller’s muscle) helps LPS, in its tonic activation mode, to maintain the elevated position of the upper eyelid. Sensory 2
Physiol. Meas. 42 (2021) 014003 M Johns and C Hocking feedback about the position and velocity of each upper eyelid is derived from mechanoreceptors in LPS, and especially in Müller’s muscle, which is embedded underneath LPS (Yuzuriha et al 2005). There is no such sensory feedback from OO muscles. The maximum velocity of each eyelid closing or reopening movement during a blink is controlled in relation to the amplitude of that movement (Evinger et al 1991, Evinger 1995, Cruz et al 2011). The further the eyelids move during a blink the higher their velocity, a relationship known as the ‘main sequence’. However, it is difficult to measure the maximum velocity of eyelid movements calibrated in absolute terms of degrees or millimeters per second, especially when repeated measurements are required over long periods. A system of infrared reflectance blepharometry, and the amplitude/velocity ratios (AVRs) that it measures, were introduced in 2003 (Johns 2003). The AVR for each eyelid movement was calculated as follows: (amplitude of eyelid movement in mv) ´ (conversion factor) A/ V = . (maximum change in eyelid position in mv per unit time) ´ (conversion factor) A conversion factor is necessary to convert each measurement from mv to mm or degrees per unit time. Because the conversion factor is measured at the same time and under the same circumstances for the ordinate and the abscissa, it cancels itself out of the ratio. The AVR for each eyelid movement can then be measured without the need for calibration in absolute terms (Johns et al 2007). These AVRs have the dimension of time, not of distance or velocity, so they measure the relative velocity of eyelid movements in relation to their amplitude, not their absolute velocity (Johns and Tucker 2005, Johns et al 2007). In 2003 the velocity of eyelid movements was calculated as the change in position in mv per 10 ms. That proved to be too sensitive to noise, and in 2005 it was changed to mv per 50 ms (Johns and Tucker 2005). Such AVRs have been measured frequently under different circumstances since then (Anderson et al 2013, Ftouni et al 2013, Wilkinson et al 2013, Soleimanloo et al 2019). The current investigation was part of ongoing studies to investigate the nature of unintentional drowsiness. Our focus was on the relationship between the amplitude and maximum velocity of eyelid closing and reopening movements during blinks, and the frequency distribution of AVRs, measured by infrared reflectance blepharometry in healthy young adults when they were alert after a ‘normal’ night’s sleep, and when drowsy because of overnight sleep deprivation. Each subject’s eyelid movements were monitored while they performed a 15 min visual reaction-time test under alert and drowsy conditions. Our particular objectives were as follows: 1. To confirm the relationships between the amplitude and maximum velocity (the ‘main sequence’) for eyelid closing and reopening movements during blinks as measured by infrared reflectance blepharometry in a large series of blinks. 2. To describe the frequency distributions of AVRs for eyelid closing and reopening movements, and how each is affected by unintentional drowsiness. 3. To investigate short-term changes in AVRs across five consecutive 3 min segments of recordings. Methods The system of infrared reflectance blepharometry used to measure the characteristics of blinks (manufactured by Optalert Australia Pty Ltd, Melbourne) and the visual reaction-time test have been described elsewhere (Johns et al 2007). Twenty-four people volunteered to take part in this investigation. They came from among the undergraduate and post-graduate students of Swinburne University of Technology, Melbourne. Initial inclusion criteria were that they were not being treated for any physical or mental disorder at the time and were ostensibly healthy, and had normal visual acuity without correction. They gave their informed written consent to the protocol which was approved by an ethics committee of Swinburne University. During the reaction-time test about 85 brief visual stimuli, each involving a change of shapes for 400 milliseconds on a computer screen, were presented at random intervals between 5 and 15 s. Subjects were asked to respond to each stimulus as soon as possible by pushing a button held in their dominant hand. They were familiarized with the protocol for several minutes during a ‘trial run’ before the recordings were made. Subjects answered the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS) about their state of alertness/drowsiness just before each recording session (Åkerstedt and Gillberg 1990). During recording sessions each subject sat alone at a desk in front of a computer screen, without interruption, in an office with ceiling lights. One recording session for each subject was during the morning after their usual night’s sleep, described in a sleep questionnaire. The other 3
Physiol. Meas. 42 (2021) 014003 M Johns and C Hocking session was during the morning after they stayed awake and missed that night’s sleep. The order of those sessions was randomized. Subjectively, this was quite a boring task, especially after the first few minutes. After the recordings were analyzed, only those subjects who fulfilled additional criteria were subsequently included in the investigation. In the alert condition, they had to have responded to every stimulus in the reaction-time test within two seconds, and to have scored less than six on the KSS just before the test. In the drowsy condition, they had to have made several errors of omission in the reaction-time test (with no response to the visual stimulus within two seconds) and to have scored at least six on the KSS. On that basis, six of the 24 subjects were excluded from further investigation. The remaining 18 participants (12 men) had a mean age of 21.9 years (range 19–30). All their blinks during 270 min of recording in each condition, and the results of the reaction-time tests, were analyzed using proprietary software. The resolution of those measurements was two milliseconds. The relationships between particular blinks and performance of the reaction-time test are not reported here. Statistical analysis The ‘main sequence’ relationships between the amplitude and maximum velocity of eyelid closing and reopening movements in alert and drowsy conditions were assessed by linear regression and Spearman’s R. The frequency distributions of AVRs were tested for normality by chi2 tests, in which the degrees of freedom were adjusted when cells with few data points were collapsed together. Differences between the AVRs in five consecutive 3 min segments of the recordings were analyzed by Kruskal–Wallis ANOVA and Mann–Whitney U-tests. Statistical significance was accepted at p
Physiol. Meas. 42 (2021) 014003 M Johns and C Hocking Figure 2. Relationships between the amplitude (A) and maximum velocity of eyelid movements during blinks (V ): (a) eyelid closing movements when alert, (b) eyelid closing movements when drowsy, (c) eyelid reopening movements when alert, (d) eyelid reopening movements when drowsy. (A=amplitude in mv; V=max change in eyelid position (mv) per 50 ms.) drowsiness in 15 of the 18 participants. The other three subjects already had a high blink rate (>25 blinks per minute) in the alert condition. So far, we have been comparing the AVRs of eyelid closing and reopening movements during spontaneous blinks recorded during 15 min test sessions in alert and drowsy conditions, separated by several days. Because drowsiness is known to be a rapidly fluctuating state, we also wanted to know if there were changes in AVRs between consecutive 3 min segments of the recordings. Figure 4(a) shows the mean of AVRs (with 95 per cent confidence intervals) for eyelid closing movements during each segment, plotted separately for alert and drowsy conditions. There was a statistically significant difference between segments for eyelid closing AVRs which was more obvious in the drowsy condition (Kruskal–Wallis H=63.7 (4, n=7380), P
Physiol. Meas. 42 (2021) 014003 M Johns and C Hocking Figure 3. The frequency histograms for AVRs during blinks: (a) AVRs for eyelid closing movements when alert and drowsy, (b) AVRs for eyelid reopening movements when alert and drowsy. those from an earlier study with far fewer blinks, using the same methods (Johns and Tucker 2005). They are also consistent with the results of investigations using a magnetic induction search coil (Evinger et al 1991, Aramideh et al 1994) or a video camera (Cruz et al 2011, Espinosa et al 2018). Since the correlation coefficients we have reported involved thousands of blinks, they indicate an extraordinary degree of control over eyelid movements during blinks, especially during eyelid closing movements. That is possible because each neuromuscular unit of OO comprises one motor neuron with axonal branches to about 10 muscle fibres. This provides greater control of OO than is possible for most skeletal muscles, which have up to 100 muscle fibres per motor neuron (Bour et al 2000). Each AVR represents the slope of the relevant relationship between the amplitude and maximum velocity of eyelid movements during blinks. We found that the frequency distributions of AVRs were significantly different from normal, regardless of how we transformed them. This requires further investigation. We used nonparametric methods to show that the distribution of closing AVRs was different from that of reopening AVRs, and that both were increased by unintentional drowsiness. That is, relative velocities of eyelid movements during blinks were reduced by drowsiness. We speculate that this was because fewer neuromuscular units of OO and LPS muscles were recruited at the time. However, drowsiness increased the AVRs of some blinks but not 6
Physiol. Meas. 42 (2021) 014003 M Johns and C Hocking Figure 4. The mean and 95% confidence intervals of AVRs during each 3 min segment of the 15 min recordings: (a) eyelid closing movements when alert and drowsy, (b) eyelid reopening movements when alert and drowsy. others. As a result, drowsiness involved some AVRs that were typical of alert wakefulness. This is consistent with drowsiness being a fluctuating state, showing ‘state instability’. We have described a time-on-task effect on AVRs, which increased progressively during the first half of the recordings and then remained relatively constant. This was especially so in the drowsy condition. We postulate that this was caused by starting the test in anticipation of a challenge which acted as an alerting stimulus, a stimulus that was dissipated over several minutes. This presumably had more effect in the drowsy state because general levels of alertness were lower then, after sleep deprivation, when the ability to maintain alertness was impaired. This effect may not have been present if starting the test did not act as an alerting stimulus. As a practical example of the relevance of this finding, we suggest that it poses a potential problem for attempts to determine who is (or was) too drowsy to drive a vehicle after having been stopped by road traffic authorities. The possibility of facing legal consequences would presumably provide an alerting stimulus for most drivers, especially when confronted with a testing procedure. A test of their alertness-drowsiness after the event may need to last several minutes to allow sufficient time for the alerting effect of starting the test to decline, and for drowsiness to become manifest. That makes the measurement of alertness-drowsiness at a particular time different from the measurement of blood alcohol. Alertness-drowsiness can be influenced by the very fact of being measured, whereas the blood alcohol concentration is not. 7
Physiol. Meas. 42 (2021) 014003 M Johns and C Hocking A limitation of this investigation was that it involved only healthy young adults. The effect of age on AVRs remains to be investigated. We did not investigate the differences in AVRs between subjects which could be expected because there was no requirement that they should all be equally drowsy after missing a night’s sleep. We have incidentally confirmed that the blink rate increases after sleep deprivation (Caffier et al 2003), an observation that is currently unexplained, and which requires further investigation. Conclusions On the basis of this investigation we conclude the following: • The ‘main sequence’ relationships between the amplitude and maximum velocity of eyelid closing and reopening movements during blinks, measured by infrared reflectance blepharometry, are consistent with those measured by other methods. • The distribution of AVRs for eyelid closing movements during blinks is different from that of eyelid reopening movements. Both are affected by unintentional drowsiness. • Unintentional drowsiness increases the AVRs for some, but not all blinks. This is consistent with the concept of ‘state instability’. • There are changes in AVRs during a 15 min psychomotor performance task that reflect a short-term time-on- task effect, especially in the drowsy condition. This may have implications for the assessment of drowsiness in drivers. Acknowledgments We acknowledge the assistance of Dr Andrew Tucker, Prof John Patterson, Dr Kate Crowley, Dr Natalie Michael and Robert Chapman with collection of the data that formed the basis of this investigation. Author contributions MJ designed this experiment, analyzed the data, and was the main author of this manuscript. CH helped collect the data, prepared the figures, and helped write the manuscript. Conflict of interest Dr Johns is a member of the Board of Directors and a shareholder of Optalert Australia Pty Ltd, the maker of Optalert technology which was used here to monitor and analyze blinks. That technology is the subject of various patents. Mr Hocking is an employee of the same company. Funding of this research No funds were received for this research or the preparation of this report. ORCID iDs Murray Johns https://orcid.org/0000-0001-7195-3173 References Åkerstedt A and Gillberg M 1990 Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual Int. J. Neurosci. 52 29–37 Anderson C, Chang A-M, Sullivan J P, Ronda J M and Czeisler C A 2013 Assessment of drowsiness based on ocular parameters detected by infrared reflectance oculography J. Clin. Sleep Med. 9 907–20 Aramideh M, Ongerboer de Visser B W, Devriese P P, Bour L J and Speelman J D 1994 Electromyographic features of levator palpebrae superioris and orbicularis oculi muscles in blepharospam Brain 117 27–38 Bour L J, Aramideh M and Ongerboer De Visser B W 2000 Neurophysiological aspects of eye and eyelid movements during blinks in humans J. Neurophysiol. 83 166–76 Caffier P P, Erdman U and Ullsperger P 2003 Experimental evaluation of eye-blink parameters as a drowsiness measure Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 89 319–25 8
Physiol. Meas. 42 (2021) 014003 M Johns and C Hocking Chaudhary B A 2007 Introduction to polysomnography Primary Care Sleep Medicine. Current Clinical Practice ed J F Pagel and S R Pandi-Perumal (Totowa, NJ: Humana Press) pp 293–304 Cori J M, Anderson C, Soleimanloo S S, Jackson M L and Howard M E 2019 Narrative review: do spontaneous eye blink parameters provide a useful assessment of state drowsiness? Sleep Med. Rev. 45 95–104 Cruz A A, Garcia D M, Pinto C T and Cecchetti S P 2011 Spontaneous eyeblink activity Ocular. Surf. 9 29–41 Dinges D F and Powell J W 1985 Microcomputer analysis of performance on a portable, visual RT task during sustained operations Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 17 652–5 Doran S M et al 2001 Sustained attention performance during sleep deprivation: evidence of state instability Arch. Ital. Biol. 139 253–67 Dorrian J, Rogers N L and Dinges D F 2005 Psychomotor vigilance performance: neurocognitive assay sensitive to sleep loss Sleep Deprivation Clinical Issues, Pharmacology, and Sleep Loss Effects ed C A Kushida (New York: Marcel Dekker) pp 39–70 Espinosa J, Domenech B, Vázquez C, Pérez J and Mas D 2018 Blinking characterization from high speed video records. Application to biometric authentication PloS One 13 e0196125 Evinger C 1995 A brain stem reflex in the blink of an eye Physiology 10 147–53 Evinger C, Manning K A and Sibony P A 1991 Eyelid movements Investigative Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 32 387–400 Ftouni S, Rahman S A, Crowley K E, Anderson C, Rajaratnam S M W and Lockley S W 2013 Temporal dynamics of ocular indicators of sleepiness across sleep restriction J. Biol. Rhythm 28 412–24 Johns M W 2003 The amplitude velocity ratio of blinks: a new method for monitoring drowsiness Sleep 26 A51–2 (Abstract) Johns M W and Tucker A J 2005 The amplitude–velocity ratios of eyelid movements during blinks: changes with drowsiness Sleep 28 A122 (Abstract) Johns M W, Tucker A, Chapman R, Crowley K and Michael N 2007 Monitoring eye and eyelid movements by infrared reflectance oculography to measure drowsiness in drivers Somnologie 11 234–42 Ousler G W 3rd, Abelson M B, Johnston P R, Rodriquez J, Lane K and Smith L M 2014 Blink patterns and lid-contact times in dry-eye and normal subjects Clin. Ophthalmol. 8 869–74 Peiris M T R, Jones R D, Davidson P R, Carroll G J and Bones P J 2006 Frequent lapses of responsiveness during an extended visuomotor tracking task in non-sleep-deprived subjects J. Sleep Res. 15 291–300 Sagaspe P, Taillard J, Bayon V, Lagarde E, Moore N, Boussuge J, Chaumet G, Bioulec B and Philip P 2010 Sleepiness, near-misses and driving accidents among a representative population of French drivers J. Sleep Res. 9 578–84 Soleimanloo S S, Wilkinson V E, Cori J M, Westlake J, Stevens B and Shiferaw B A 2019 Eye-blink parameters detect on-road track-driving impairment following severe sleep deprivation J. Clin. Sleep Med. 15 1271–84 Wilkinson V E, Jackson M L, Westlake J, Stevens B, Barnes M, Swann P, Rajaratnam S M W and Howard M E 2013 The accuracy of eyelid movement parameters for drowsiness detection J. Clin. Sleep Med. 9 1315–24 Yoss R E, Moyer N J and Hollenhorst R W 1970 Pupil size and spontaneous pupillary waves associated with alertness, drowsiness and sleep Neurology 20 543–54 Yuzuriha S, Matsuo K, Ishigaki N, Kawagishi N and Monizumi T 2005 Efferent and afferent innervations of Mueller’s muscle related to involuntary contraction of the levator muscle: important for avoiding injury during surgery JPRAS 88 42–52 9
You can also read