Responsibility and perfectionism in OCD: an experimental study

Page created by Rodney Stone
 
CONTINUE READING
BEHAVIOUR
                                                                                           RESEARCH AND
                                                                                                THERAPY
PERGAMON                        Behaviour Research and Therapy 37 (1999) 239±248

 Responsibility and perfectionism in OCD: an experimental
                            study
               Catherine Bouchard, JoseÂe RheÂaume, Robert Ladouceur *
                     Universite Laval, Ecole de Psychologie, Ste-Foy Quebec G1K 7P4, Canada
                                               Received 17 July 1998

Abstract

   Cognitive models of obsessive±compulsive disorder (OCD) suggest a number of di€erent variables
that may play a role in the development and maintenance of obsessive compulsive symptoms [Freeston,
M. H., RheÂaume, J., & Ladouceur, R. (1996) Correcting faulty appraisals of obsessional thoughts.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34, 433±446]. This study's aim was to verify the e€ect of perfectionism
and excessive responsibility on checking behaviors and related variables. Twenty-four moderately
perfectionistic subjects (MP) and 27 highly perfectionistic subjects (HP) were submitted to a
manipulation of responsibility (low and high). After each manipulation, they had to perform a
classi®cation task during which checking behaviors were observed. Results indicate that more checking
behaviors (hesitations, checking) occurred in the high responsibility condition than in the low
responsibility condition for subjects of both groups. After executing the task in the high responsibility
condition, HP subjects reported more in¯uence over and responsibility for negative consequences than
MP subjects. These results suggest that high perfectionistic tendencies could predispose individuals to
overestimate their perceived responsibility for negative events. Furthermore, perfectionism could be
conceived as playing a catalytic role in the perception of responsibility. Results are discussed according
to cognitive models of OCD. # 1999 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Perfectionism; Responsibility; Obsessive±compulsive disorder; Cognitive models

1. Introduction

  Behavior therapy is faced with some limits for the treatment of obsessive±compulsive
disorder (OCD). Almost 25% of patients refuse this type of treatment and approximately
25% do not bene®t from it (Foa, Steketee, Grayson, & Doppelt, 1983). Cognitive therapy

 * Corresponding author. Tel.: +1-418-656-396; Fax: +1-418-656-3646; E-mail: robert.ladouceur@psy.ulaval.ca

0005-7967/99/$ - see front matter # 1999 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.
PII: S 0 0 0 5 - 7 9 6 7 ( 9 8 ) 0 0 1 4 1 - 7
240               C. Bouchard et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 37 (1999) 239±248

is considered as an alternative or as a complement to traditional behavioral treatments (e.g.
van Oppen & Arntz, 1994; van Oppen et al., 1995; Freeston et al., 1996). In these
circumstances, it is important to know and understand the cognitive variables that are
involved in OCD in order to ®nd e€ective ways of correcting cognitive distortions.
Dysfunctional perfectionism and excessive responsibility have been identi®ed as part of the
®ve main cognitive variables associated with OCD (McFall & Wollersheim, 1979; Freeston
et al., 1996); the other principal variables being overestimation of the importance of
thoughts, overestimation of danger and the belief that anxiety caused by thoughts is
unacceptable.
   Recent de®nitions of responsibility and perfectionism contribute to our understanding of the
role these variables play in OCD. Excessive or in¯ated responsibility has been de®ned as the
belief which is pivotal to bring about or prevent subjectively crucial negative outcomes. They
may be actual, that is, having consequences in the real world and/or at a moral level
(Salkovskis et al., 1996). This de®nition has been empirically supported using a semi-
idiographic questionnaire (e.g. RheÂaume, Ladouceur, Freeston, & Letarte, 1995) as well as with
experimental manipulations of responsibility (e.g. Ladouceur et al., 1995; Ladouceur,
RheÂaume, & Aublet, 1997).
   The unidimensional de®nition of perfectionism used in this study is: `the belief that a
perfect state exists that one should try to attain' (Pacht, 1984). According to this
perspective, which is particularly pertinent in the study of OCD (RheÂaume, Freeston,
Dugas, Letarte, & Ladouceur, 1995), perfection does not exist and the attempt to attain
this perfect state would be associated with psychopathology. Hamachek (1978) points out
that perfectionism can be a positive personality trait and distinguishes between sane and
pathological perfectionism. The Perfectionism Questionnaire was devised to measure this
construct with respect to the distinction between functional and dysfunctional perfectionism.
The criterion and convergent validity of this instrument were established with questionnaire
(RheÂaume, Freeston, & Ladouceur, 1995) and behavior manipulation studies (RheÂaume et
al., 1995b).
   In the past few years, the concept of excessive responsibility has received a lot of
attention (Cottraux, 1990; Rachman, 1993; RheÂaume, Ladouceur, Freeston, & Letarte, 1994;
Tallis, 1994; van Oppen & Arntz, 1994; RheÂaume et al., 1995c). Salkovskis (1985, 1989)
made a great contribution to this theory by proposing a theoretical model whereby an
excessive sense of responsibility is at the core of OCD. According to this model,
obsessional patients would appraise intrusive thoughts as a function of possible harm to
themselves or others. This excessive sense of responsibility would produce automatic
negative thoughts, and discomfort would arise. The individual would then attempt to
reduce the anxiety through cognitive neutralization or compulsive behavior (e.g. checking
repetitively). Many studies support this model. In a number of clinical studies, the presence
of an excessive sense of responsibility was observed in OC patients (Salkovskis, 1989; van
Oppen et al., 1995; Ladouceur, LeÂger, RheÂaume, & DubeÂ, 1996). Furthermore,
questionnaire studies comparing OC patients to control subjects support the existence of a
link between responsibility and OC-type behaviors (Freeston, Ladouceur, Gagnon, &
Thibodeau, 1992, 1993; RheÂaume et al., 1995a). Finally, two recent experimental studies
manipulated the level of perceived responsibility. Lopatka and Rachman (1995) succeeded
C. Bouchard et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 37 (1999) 239±248        241

in changing the perceived responsibility for negative consequences in thirty compulsive
checkers. As expected, the lowering of responsibility was associated with a signi®cant drop
in discomfort and need to check. Lastly, two studies experimentally manipulated
responsibility in nonclinical subjects (Ladouceur et al., 1995, 1997). Results showed that
checking behaviors were more frequent in the group receiving high responsibility
instructions compared to controls.
   On the other hand, although it has been suggested for many years that perfectionism also
plays a key role in OCD (Hamachek, 1978; Burns, 1980; Pacht, 1984; Rasmussen & Eisen,
1989), this variable has not yet been fully studied. At the clinical level, Ladouceur et al. (1996)
observed that perfectionism was a common characteristic in a group of OC patients without
manifest compulsions, while responsibility was clearly less apparent in those same subjects.
Correlational studies have shown a signi®cant link between perfectionism and OC symptoms
(Hewitt & Flett, 1991; Hewitt, Flett, & Turnbull, 1992). Furthermore, two correlational studies
using the Maudsley OC symptom checklist with analogue subjects have shown that participants
with OC tendencies were more perfectionistic than noncompulsive individuals (Frost, Steketee,
Cohn, & Griess, 1994) and were also more perfectionistic than nonanxious controls (Gershuny
& Sher, 1995). Finally, in a recent experimental study using a variety of tasks with nonclinical
subjects, dysfunctional perfectionists obtained higher scores on the Padua Inventory and
performed more poorly in precision and decision making tasks compared to functional
perfectionists. These results support the link between perfectionism and OC symptoms
(RheÂaume et al., 1995b).
   At this point in time, the links between perfectionism and responsibility remain obscure.
Studies conducted by RheÂaume et al. (e.g. RheÂaume et al., 1995a; RheÂaume, Ladouceur, &
Freeston, 1998) have shown that responsibility and perfectionism are good independent
predictors of OC symptoms. Nonetheless, little is known about the nature of the
relationship between these two important factors. Considering the recent developments in
cognitive therapy (e.g. van Oppen et al., 1995; Ladouceur et al., 1996; Freeston et al.,
1997), it appears necessary to investigate how perfectionism and responsibility are linked
together and to OC symptoms, in order to develop speci®c cognitive interventions adapted
to this population.
   The aim of the current study is to explore the links between perfectionism and excessive
responsibility. This relationship will be studied by increasing and lowering perceived
responsibility in subjects showing di€erent degrees of perfectionism. We will verify if
perfectionism, together with an excessive sense of responsibility, has an impact on the appraisal
of intrusions and whether they predispose individuals to show OC tendencies. It is expected
that highly perfectionistic subjects will show more OC-type behaviors and will attribute more
responsibility to themselves than moderately perfectionistic subjects. In the high responsibility
condition, we should observe, for both groups, more OC-type behaviors than in the low
responsibility condition. Furthermore, this increase in checking and other related behaviors
should be more marked in highly perfectionistic subjects than in moderately perfectionistic
subjects.
242                 C. Bouchard et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 37 (1999) 239±248

2. Method

2.1. Participants

  Fifty-one adults from a nonclinical population participated in the study (41 women, 10 men,
mean age 23.3 years). Subjects were recruited through questionnaire studies conducted in
university classes. Subjects participating in the study were eligible for a draw prize of CAN$50.
In order to increase the credibility of the task, subjects were not to be psychology students and
not to have already participated in a laboratory study. Subjects were selected on the basis of
their scores on the PQ. Two groups were formed: the highly perfectionistic group (HP),
composed of subjects whose scores ranked over the 90th percentile on the perfectionistic
tendencies scale of the PQ and the moderately perfectionistic group (MP), composed of
subjects whose scores ranked under the 40th percentile on this scale. Three subjects were
excluded due to the fact that they did not believe the manipulation context.

2.2. Procedure

  Subjects of both groups participated in an individual 35 min session. The experimenter
brie¯y explained the procedure which consisted of performing a classi®cation task two times
and completing a questionnaire. He explained that the session would be recorded on video for
research purposes. The task consisted of classifying 50 drug capsules (10 kinds, ®ve of each)
previously emptied of their active substances and ®lled with sugar into 12 semitransparent
bottles. Subjects had to pick one capsule at a time and put each type of capsule in a di€erent
bottle aligned in front of them. If they believed that they made a mistake, Ss could check and
move the capsules during the task. Subjects were instructed to proceed as fast as possible while
completing the task as best as they could.

2.3. Manipulation of responsibility

2.3.1. Low responsibility
  The subject was asked to do the task a ®rst time as a simple practice. Moreover the
experimenter explained to the subject that his personal results had no importance because it
was only a practice trial. After receiving these low responsibility instructions the subject
completed the task a ®rst time while the experimenter was out of the room.

2.3.2. High responsibility
  The experimenter then explained to the subject that our research group was specialized in
the perception of colors and had been mandated by a pharmaceutical company for a project
concerning the exportation of a medication for a virus which was presently very widespread in
a Southeast Asian country. Moreover, the subject was told that, as this region was very poor
and its population poorly educated, there was a need for developing a system of colors that
would make the distribution of medication safer for the inhabitants. The subject was also told
that he had great responsibility in the project, because his results in the classi®cation of
capsules could directly in¯uence the manufacturing of the medication. Moreover, it was
C. Bouchard et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 37 (1999) 239±248      243

essential that he completed the task as seriously as possible in order to prevent serious harm
and completed the task with the high responsibility instructions in mind.
  After the task was completed, the experimenter counted the mistakes while the subject
completed the retrospective questionnaire on the classi®cation task. Finally, the experimenter
debriefed the subject, explained the real goals of the study and asked him to sign a ®nal
consent form if he still accepted that his results be used for analyses.

2.4. Instruments

   The Perfectionism Questionnaire (PQ; RheÂaume et al., 1995c) contains 64 items divided into
three subscales: (1) perfectionistic tendencies (10 items, a = 0.82), (2) domains a€ected by
perfectionism (30 items, a = 0.88) and (3) negative consequences of perfectionism (24 items,
a = 0.96). The PQ has good construct and convergent validities (RheÂaume et al., 1998). This
instrument is used to form the groups.
   The retrospective questionnaire on the classi®cation task is a slightly modi®ed version of the
retrospective questionnaire used in the Ladouceur et al. (1995) study. It contains 10 questions
evaluating responsibility and subjective variables.

2.5. Manipulation checks

  The mean score of the HP and MP groups on the perfectionistic tendencies subscale of the
PQ were compared to determine wether the groups were signi®cantly di€erent on this
dimension.

2.6. Responsibility variables

  Four questions of the retrospective questionnaire evaluated perception of the probability and
the severity of negative consequences, the in¯uence of the subject over these consequences, and
perceived responsibility. Since the intrasubject design of the current study did not allow for a
manipulation check measure to be introduced after the LR condition, we compared the mean
scores for the responsibility variables for the HR condition to those obtained in the study of
Ladouceur et al. (1995) in order to support the ecacy of the responsibility manipulation.

2.7. Dependent behavioral variables

  Five behavioral variables were measured: (1) hesitations, de®ned as close examination of a
capsule for more than 2 s or by a movement of the Ss hand between two di€erent pill bottles
for at least 2 s; (2) checking, de®ned as picking up the bottle to look inside or emptying the
content of the bottle into the palm of the hand; (3) modi®cations, referred to as any change,
addition or withdrawal of one or more capsules from a given pill bottle; (4) number of errors
made; and (5) time to complete the task. An inter-judge agreement was established for each of
these behavioral measures (see Ladouceur et al., 1995, 1996).
244                          C. Bouchard et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 37 (1999) 239±248

2.8. Dependent subjective variables

  The subjective variables were evaluated with the retrospective questionnaire: (1) doubt, (2)
preoccupation with errors, (3) need to check, (4) desired additional time to check and (5)
anxiety.

3. Results

3.1. Manipulation checks

  A t-test on the integrity variable revealed a signi®cant di€erence between the HP group and
the MP group on their score on the perfectionistic tendencies subscale of the PQ (t = 19.88,
p < 0.0001). The HP group reported signi®cantly more perfectionistic tendencies than the MP
group. The two groups also di€ered on their score on the negative consequences subscale of
the PQ (t = 5.19, p < 0.0001), the HP group reporting signi®cantly more negative
consequences related to their perfectionistic tendencies than the MP group.

3.2. Responsibility variables

  Table 1 presents the mean scores of both groups for responsibility variables: probability and
severity of possible negative consequences and perceived in¯uence and responsibility for these
consequences. A Manova conducted on these variables revealed a signi®cant group e€ect
(F(4,46) = 5.15; p < 0.002). After completing the task in the high responsibility condition,
subjects from the HP group reported more in¯uence and responsibility for possible negative
consequences than Ss from the MP group.

3.3. Dependent behavioral variables

  Table 2 presents the mean scores of both groups for behavioral variables. A Manova on the
®ve behavioral variables revealed a signi®cant intragroup e€ect (responsibility) (F(5,44) = 5.25;
p < 0.0008), but no group (perfectionism) nor interaction e€ects. Subsequent Anovas indicated

Table 1
Responsibility variables (HR condition)

Variables                          MP group                              HP group

                                   M                  S.D.               M                  S.D.     F

Severity                           5.58               1.56               6.48               2.26       2.66
Probability                        4.67               1.43               4.70               1.88
C. Bouchard et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 37 (1999) 239±248               245

Table 2
Dependant behavioral variables

Variables         Low responsibility condition               High responsibility condition            HR/LR

                  MP group              HP group             MP group              HP group

                  M          S.D.       M          S.D.      M           S.D.      M          S.D.    F

Hesitations        17.67      8.85       16.88     11.28      21.87      10.09      19.11     11.92     7.73*
Checking            4.04      5.08        3.50      4.37       5.75       5.37       6.15      6.55    10.34*
Modi®cations        2.58      2.60        1.31      1.19       1.88       1.39       1.46      1.58
246                C. Bouchard et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 37 (1999) 239±248

   Analyses con®rm that the identi®cation of two levels of perfectionism was successful since
the HP group reported signi®cantly more perfectionistic tendencies and negative consequences
associated to these tendencies than the MP group. It also appears that the experimental
manipulation of responsibility was successful. Indeed, when we compare the mean scores in the
HR condition (HP and MP groups combined) to those obtained in the study of Ladouceur et
al. (1995), respectively, results are comparable for all four responsibility variables.
   Analyses conducted on the responsibility variables reveal that the HP group perceived more
in¯uence and responsibility for negative consequences than the MP group. On a subjective
level, HP subjects were therefore more a€ected by an increase in responsibility than less
perfectionistic subjects. This result suggests that perfectionism may predispose individuals to
feel responsible. Highly perfectionistic individuals would react more strongly in a situation of
increased responsibility, perceiving more personal in¯uence on the situation than they actually
have. On the other hand, we notice that the HP group does not perceive greater probability
nor severity of negative consequences than the MP group. In previous manipulation studies,
signi®cant di€erences were obtained on these variables (Ladouceur et al., 1995, 1997). This
result is therefore puzzling. It is possible that this result is nonsigni®cant simply due to a lack
of power; a tendency was observed for the gravity variable. It is also possible that the
relationship between perfectionism and responsibility only a€ects the perception of having a
pivotal role in the situation and not the appraisal of the consequences themselves; the
evaluation of consequences being independent of perfectionism.
   Results concerning the number of errors and the time to complete the task, partially con®rm
the intragroup di€erences hypothesis. Both groups made less mistakes and took more time to
complete the task in the high responsibility condition compared to the LR condition. This
con®rms the e€ect of responsibility at a behavioral level. Concerning the subjective variables,
no di€erence was observed for doubt, need to check, anxiety or desired additional time to
check. These results discon®rm the hypothesis of subjective di€erences between groups. This
could be due to the simplicity of the task and to the fact that subjects could check as much as
they wished, which left little room for a persistence of doubt or need to check.
   The absence of signi®cant behavioral di€erences between groups is surprising considering
that signi®cant intergroup di€erences were found on other levels. A ®rst hypothesis that could
explain this absence of di€erence concerns the assignment of subjects. The score of the MP
group on the negative consequences subscale appears higher than would normally be expected.
Surprisingly, these subjects who report being only slightly perfectionistic on the perfectionistic
tendencies subscale also report su€ering a great deal from the negative consequences associated
to perfectionism (see RheÂaume et al., 1995b). If these MP subjects present a high degree of
dysfunction, they would clearly distinguish themselves less from HP subjects, which would
explain the similar checking behavior of the two groups in the classi®cation task. Another
hypothesis is the fact that the manipulation of responsibility was too powerful. Indeed, it was
expected that the HP group would check more than the MP group, especially in the high
responsibility condition. It is possible that this was not observed due to a ceiling e€ect: the
increase in responsibility was so marked that both groups increased their checking rate to a
maximum, leaving no room for a distinction between groups. It is therefore possible that the
optimal way to use such a manipulation is with random groups (Stevens, 1980); indeed, the
C. Bouchard et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 37 (1999) 239±248                                    247

present experimental protocol (2  2) did not reach the necessary power to study a
perfectionism  responsibility interaction on the behavioral level.
  The overall results suggest a link between checking behavior and the perception of in¯ated
responsibility and show a relation between perfectionistic tendencies and an increased
perception of responsibility and personal in¯uence. These results have important implications
for the cognitive models of OCD. They suggest that perfectionism, when it reaches a
dysfunctional level, could predispose the individual to overestimate his or her own
responsibility for negative events, which in turn could potentially contribute to an increase in
checking behavior. Increased responsibility has the e€ect of increasing checking behavior, and
furthermore, perfectionism could be conceived as playing a catalytic role in the perception of
responsibility.

Acknowledgements

  This study was conducted while the ®rst author was supported by the Fonds pour la
Recherche en Sante du QueÂbec and while the third author received grants from the Medical
Research Council of Canada and the Fonds de Recherche en Sante du QueÂbec.

References

Burns, D. (1980). The perfectionist's script for self-defeat. Psychology Today, November, 34±51.
Cottraux, J. (1990). Therapie cognitive des obsessions-compulsions. Encephale, 16, 347±353.
Foa, E. B., Steketee, G. S., Grayson, J. B., & Doppelt, H. G. (1983). Treatment of obsessive±compulsives: when do we fail? In E.B. Foa
  & P.M.G. Emmelkamp (Eds.), Failures in behavior therapy (pp. 10±33). New York: Wiley and Sons.
Freeston, M. H., Ladouceur, R., Gagnon, F., & Thibodeau, N. (1992). Intrusive thoughts, worry and obsessions: empirical and theor-
  etical distinctions. Paper presented at the World Congress of Cognitive Therapy, Toronto.
Freeston, M. H., Ladouceur, R., Gagnon, F., & Thibodeau, N. (1993). Beliefs about obsessional thoughts. Journal of Psychopathology
  and Behavioral Assessment, 15, 1±21.
Freeston, M. H., Ladouceur, R., Gagnon, F., Thibodeau, N., RheÂaume, J., Letarte, H., & Bujold, A. (1997). Cognitive behavioral
  treatment of obsessive thoughts: a controlled study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 405±423.
Freeston, M. H., RheÂaume, J., & Ladouceur, R. (1996). Correcting faulty appraisals of obsessional thoughts. Behaviour Research and
  Therapy, 34, 433±446.
Frost, R., Steketee, G., Cohn, L., & Griess, K. E. (1994). Personality traits in sub clinical and nonobsessive compulsive volunteers and
  their parents. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32, 47±56.
Gershuny, B. S., & Sher, K. J. (1995). Compulsive checking and anxiety in a nonclinical sample: di€erences in cognition, behavior,
  personality and a€ect. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 17, 19±38.
Hamachek, D. E. (1978). Psychodynamics of normal and neurotic perfectionism. Psychology, 15, 34±52.
Hewitt, P. L., & Flett, G. L. (1991). Perfectionism in the self and social contexts: conceptualization, assessment and association with
  psychopathology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 456±470.
Hewitt, P. L., Flett, G. L., & Turnbull, W. (1992). Perfectionism and multiphasic personality inventory (MMPI) indices of personality
  disorder. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 14, 323±335.
Ladouceur, R., LeÂger, EÂ., RheÂaume, J., & DubeÂ, D. (1996). Correction of in¯ated responsibility in the treatment of obsessive±com-
  pulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34, 767±774.
Ladouceur, R., RheÂaume, J., Freeston, M. H., Aublet, F., Jean, K., Lachance, S., Langlois, F., & De Pokomandy-Morin, K. (1995).
  Experimental manipulation of responsibility: an analog test for models of obsessive±compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and
  Therapy, 33, 937±946.
248                       C. Bouchard et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 37 (1999) 239±248

Ladouceur, R., RheÂaume, J., & Aublet, F. (1997). Excessive responsibility in obsessional concerns: a ®ne-grained experimental analysis.
  Behaviour, Research and Therapy, 35, 423±427.
Lopatka, C., & Rachman, S. J. (1995). Perceived responsibility and compulsive checking: an experimental analysis. Behaviour Research
  and Therapy, 33, 673±684.
McFall, M. E., & Wollersheim, J. P. (1979). Obsessive±compulsive neurosis: a cognitive behavioral formulation and approach to treat-
  ment. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 3, 333±348.
Pacht, A. R. (1984). Re¯ections on perfection. American Psychologist, 39, 386±390.
Rachman, S. J. (1993). Obsessions, responsibility and guilt. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31, 149±154.
Rasmussen, S. A., & Eisen, J. L. (1989). Clinical features and phenomenology of obsessive±compulsive disorder. Psychiatric Annals,
  19, 67±72.
RheÂaume, J., Freeston, M. H., Dugas, M., Letarte, H., & Ladouceur, R. (1995a). Perfectionism, responsibility and obsessive±com-
  pulsive symptoms. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 785±794.
RheÂaume, J., Freeston, M. H., Ladouceur, R., Bouchard, C., Gallant, L., Talbot, F., & VallieÁres, A. (1995b). Perfectionism and obses-
  sive±compulsive type behaviors. Paper presented at the 29th annual Congress of the Association for the Advancement of Behavior
  Therapy, Washington, DC.
RheÂaume, J., Freeston, M. H., & Ladouceur, R. (1995, July). Functional and dysfunctional perfectionism: construct validity of a new
  instrument. Paper presented at the 1st annual World Congress of Behavioral Cognitive Therapy, Copenhagen.
RheÂaume, J., Ladouceur, R., Freeston, M. H., & Letarte, H. (1994). In¯ated responsibility and its role in OCD. II. Psychometric stu-
  dies of a semi-idiographic measure. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavior Assessment, 16, 265±276.
RheÂaume, J., Ladouceur, R., Freeston, M. H., & Letarte, H. (1995c). In¯ated responsibility in obsessive±compulsive disorder. I.
  Validation of an operational de®nition. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 159±169.
RheÂaume, J., Ladouceur, R. & Freeston, M. H. (1998). The prediction of obsessive±compulsive symptoms: new evidence for multiple
  cognitive vulnerability factors, submitted for publication.
Salkovskis, P. M. (1985). Obsessional±compulsive problems: a cognitive behavioural analysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 23,
  571±583.
Salkovskis, P. M. (1989). Cognitive behavioural factors and the persistence of intrusive thoughts in obsessional problems. Behaviour
  Research and Therapy, 27, 677±682.
Salkovskis, P. M., Rachman, S. L., Ladouceur, R., Freeston, M. H., Taylor, S., Kyrios, M., & Sica, C. (1996). De®ning responsibility
  in obsessional problems. In Proceedings of the Smith College Women's Room after the Toronto Cafeteria.
Stevens, J. P. (1980). Power of the multivariate analysis of variance tests. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 728±737.
Tallis, F. (1994). Obsessions, responsibility and guilt: two case reports suggesting a common and speci®c etiology. Behaviour Research
  and Therapy, 32, 143±145.
van Oppen, P., & Arntz, A. (1994). Cognitive therapy for obsessive±compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32, 79±87.
van Oppen, P., de Haan, E., van Balkom, A. J. L. M., Spinhoven, P., Hoogduin, K., & van Dyck, R. (1995). Cognitive therapy and
  exposure in vivo in the treatment of obsessive compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 379±390.
You can also read