Resource caravans and resource caravan passageways: a new paradigm for trauma responding

Page created by Jordan Woods
 
CONTINUE READING
Hobfoll

       Resource caravans and resource
       caravan passageways: a new
       paradigm for trauma responding
       Stevan Hobfoll

       We have long outgrown the capacity of the accepted        viewpoint, with particular emotional com-
       clinical models of trauma, and a paradigm shift in        ponents based almost in their entirety on
       our thinking is long overdue.The data on traumatic        individuals seeking treatment for posttrau-
       stress were posited from a certain cognitive-beha-        matic stress disorder (PTSD), and focusing
       vioural viewpoint, with particular emotional com-         on that time frame. Further, this viewpoint
       ponents based almost in their entirety on western,        was accomplished mostly within a western,
       mostly white individuals seeking treatment for post-      European and American social context,
       traumatic stress disorder, and focusing on that time      and mainly by studying middle-class, white
       frame.Assuch,mechanismssuchasfearandemotional             patients. It then had a layer added by key
       conditioningtheory and the waystraumas are encoded        work in neuroscience, again almost entirely
       in memory only partially explain trauma response.         with the same limitations, which was then
       Conservation of resources theory posits that severe       force-¢tted to the cognitive-behavioural
       trauma responses occur when personal, social or           model. One important area of challenge
       material resources, which are key to the self, survival   emerged from those who argued that PTSD
       and social attachments, are lost severely and rapidly.    could be more complex than was originally
       These resources tend to aggregate or fail to aggregate    presented, meaning that it is more multifa-
       inwhatconservationofresourcestheoryterms ‘resource        ceted (Cloitre et al., 2009; van der Kolk,
       caravans’; they do not exist in isolation. Because        et al., 2005). However, all these viewpoints
       resource caravans are created and sustained within        focus on clinical, western, mostly white
       the environmental and social context of resource cara-    populations, which represent a small frac-
       vanpassageways,environmentalcontextisfundamen-            tion of those with PTSD or other trauma-
       tal to trauma response. It is argued that resource loss   related disorders (de Jong, 2004; 2005).
       and the maintaining of resource caravans are the best     A key limitation to this model building has
       predictorsoftraumaresponse,bothintermsofposttrau-         beenthe lack of consideration of ¢ndings from
       matic stress disorder and in terms of the idioms of       large scale studies that included nonpatient
       trauma distress across cultures.                          populations over longer periods of time, stu-
                                                                 dies from nonwestern nations and more
       Keywords: posttraumatic stress disorder,                  anthropological evidence. In particular,
       resilience, resource loss, stress, trauma                 research has focused naturally on the time
                                                                 around the trauma event for clinical popu-
                                                                 lations, or the time period when individuals
       Introduction                                              seek treatment. This, in turn, meant that
       It is my thesis in this paper that we have long           researchers did not incorporate the fuller time
       outgrown the capacity of the accepted                     sequenceincludingeventsbeforeandlongafter
       models of trauma. A paradigm shift in our                 trauma, which greatly impact PTSD and
       thinking is overdue (Hobfoll & de Jong,                   cause a di¡erentconceptualisationandtheory
       2013). The data on traumatic stress were pos-             to understand trauma response and recovery.
       ited from a certain cognitive-behavioural                 Finally, the consideration of resilience, and

                                                                                                              21
Copyright © War Trauma Foundation. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Resource caravans and resource caravan passageways: a new paradigm for trauma responding
       Intervention 2014, Volume 12, Supplement 1, Page 21 - 32

       the fact that none of the accepted trauma             and maintain resources when at their best,
       models account for the extent to which people         but that often produces the groundwork for
       (even those with PTSD) are resilient, has led         resource loss (Hobfoll, 1988; 1998). COR
       toafurtherchallengethat hasnotbeen incorp-            theory greatly broadens the landscape of
       orated into the accepted models of trauma             how we see PTSD. It emphasises, in particu-
       and trauma response.                                  lar, that trauma responding is deeply
                                                             a¡ected by life history—long prior, during
                                                             and long after events^which this paper will
       Resource caravans and their tie                       examine in depth (King, et al, 1999; Vogt
       to world view, body and brain                         et al., 2011). Traumatic circumstances not
       Conservation of resources (COR) theory                only challenge the individual, but compro-
       posits that individuals strive to obtain,             mise the ability of this social structure to sup-
       retrain and protect their personal, social            port and protect people.
       and material resources. COR theory envi-
       sions this process as a basic evolutionary
       principle that has many consequences and              Current models of trauma: their
       correlates. Foremost, it sees the seeking and         strengths and limitations
       preservation of resources as a primary                At the outset, I will outline which key ¢nd-
       human motivation, and Hobfoll (1991)                  ings must be incorporated in any trauma
       theorised that trauma response will occur             model from current clinical models. First,
       when there is major loss of fundamental               there is clearly something about exposure
       resources and where this loss occurs rapidly.         to events that threaten life or bodily integrity
       What appears to characterise traumatic                that produces a powerful cognitive,
       resource loss is a rupture of the constellation       emotional and physiological response. This
       of ¢ve principal resource groups: safety,             is well captured in emotional processing
       calmness, attachment, hope, and e⁄cacy                theory (Foa & Kozak, 1986; Rauch & Foa,
       (i.e., the ability to a¡ect positive change)          2006). Speci¢cally, trauma events create a
       (Hobfoll et al., 2007). As resource conserva-         fear structure that is comprised of associated
       tion is of primary concern, the processes             stimuli, responses and meaning elements
       inherent in building and maintaining ‘resource        that become linked. Further, for some indi-
       caravans’are also the essential building blocks       viduals, these fear structures become exces-
       of culture and society. As individuals strive         sive and resistant to modi¢cation. It follows
       to obtain, retrain and protect personal,              that treatment that could extinguish the
       social and material resources for the self,           interweaving of this fear structure with
       they create social structures that necessarily        responding would aid recovery, and there
       support this primary motivation.                      is ample evidence that this is the case
       COR theory has held, from its origin, that            (McLean & Foa, 2011).
       personal, social and material resources are           Second, there are neurological rami¢cations
       not possessed piecemeal, but that rather they         of trauma that are accompanied by bio-
       are developed and associate in aggregate              logical perturbations, some of which can
       (Hobfoll, 1989, 1998). More recently, Hobfoll         become chronic (Brewin, et al., 2010). While
       (2010) further developed the long-standing            these sequences are interesting and import-
       underpinning of COR that asserts that                 ant to note, they do not add greatly to
       resource caravans, the association of linked          emotional processing theory nor to the ear-
       resources, are created and sustained within           lier dual representation theorising of Brewin
       a resource caravan passageway. That is,               (Brewin & Holmes, 2003). Tying neurologi-
       families, organisations and societies create          cal ¢ndings to theory helps support theory,
       and maintain circumstances that create                but these neurological ¢ndings have not led

       22
Copyright © War Trauma Foundation. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Hobfoll

       to changes in what is predicted by fear pro-       and possibly much later in life. Indeed,
       cessing theory or dual representation theory.      resource loss is one of the best predictors of
       Of greater importance for my thesis here,          whether someone will develop PTSD or
       the neurological research has continued the        other trauma responses, a fact that is not
       focus on memories and images embedded              incorporated in the emotional processing
       at the time of the trauma event, and the           or dual representation theory. These
       rather unfounded supposition that persons          resource losses need not occur at the time
       with PTSD have‘repeated visual intrusions corre-   of trauma. They may occur in earlier child-
       sponding to a small number of real or imaginary    hood or earlier life or well after the event
       events’ (Brewin et al., 2010, p. 210). This        (Kaniasty, 2012; King et al., 1999; Vogt
       assumption is likely because so much               et al., 2011). Hence, studies ¢nd that exposure
       research has been on clinical samples where        to childhood trauma and possessing fewer
       there was a speci¢c target event. I do not         social resources combine to predict an adult
       believe that this key assumption, on which         trajectory of posttraumatic disorder (Lowe
       so much of current trauma theory rests, has        et al., 2014). Referring to a later lifetime
       ever been empirically tested.                      sequence, individuals who escaped the
       This point is already cogently covered in the      Katrina disaster appeared to develop PTSD
       discussion of complex PTSD (van der Kolk           after they returned home and witnessed
       et al., 2005). Those who have experienced          their destroyed homes (Adeola, 2009; De
       trauma at times have a principal or worst          Salvo et al., 2007). This moment was not
       set of memories, but many report that they         life-threatening, but it had elements that
       have multiple images and memories that             may both be critical and o¡er us insight as
       are often disorganised and unsequenced,            to a broader theory of PTSD. Speci¢cally,
       and are an amalgam. As proponents of com-          when people lose a sense of safety or have a
       plex PTSD have argued, traditional PTSD            chronic sense of lack of safety, and this is
       diagnosis o¡ers a rather limited, if partially     paired with a sequence of events in which
       correct, set of responses to trauma. Such          they can imagine or actually experience
       limitations are critical, as they con¢ne           events that are life threatening or threaten-
       understanding, research and intervention,          ing to their sense of bodily integrity, they
       which in turn have to be narrowly con-             can develop PTSD and other posttrauma
       strained in order to ¢t into more limited          sequelae.
       models. Giving just one concrete example:          This point is especially germane to those
       if shame or honour are not included in PTSD        who live in chronically unsafe circum-
       measures, then the many factor analyses of         stances, such as zones of con£ict or many
       what is PTSD appear to indicate that shame         urban inner-city environments that are
       and honour are not central. However, for           characterised by pervasive violence. Such
       many collectivist cultures, honour is central      environments result in an ongoing loss of
       to the trauma experience, not ancillary.           safety, fear of loss of loved ones and those
                                                          relationships, and a sense of future hopeless-
                                                          ness and the inability to a¡ect positive
       Resource caravans and                              change. This hopelessness may be better
       passageways                                        understood when we appreciate that it is rea-
       Trauma responding, including PTSD, is not          lity based and the darkened sense of future
       only a product of the occurrences around           is an accurate representation of likely future
       the time of the event, but is strongly a¡ected     events. Once this is understood, each
       by the cascade of personal, social and             moment of life a¡ords ample opportunity
       material losses that may occur weeks or            to tie visual, environmental and physiologi-
       months or years before or after the event,         cal sequelae in ways that may have more of

                                                                                                      23
Copyright © War Trauma Foundation. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Resource caravans and resource caravan passageways: a new paradigm for trauma responding
       Intervention 2014, Volume 12, Supplement 1, Page 21 - 32

       the elements that have been tied to complex           (Brewin, 2001) fail to explain more than a
       PTSD (van der Kolk et al., 2005) and fewer            small portion of how PTSD comes about or
       of the elements that have been tied classically       how it is sustained. These theories are
       to PTSD with a particular well-framed                 important, but they are partial and
       memory of a speci¢c event. This also avails           represent a small element of the whole in
       intervention to a broad array of alternative          terms of predictive capacity. Rather, as
       foci for intervention and types of interven-          COR theory predicts, PTSD is predicted
       tion. Traditional theories focus on the mind          by the rupture of personal, social and
       and perception; however, the alternative              material resource losses that combatants
       presented here further encourages social              experience, and the web of safety and con-
       and environmental intervention, so as to              nections at the home front have as much to
       shape environments to enable healing.                 do with PTSD as fear conditioning or the
       Several studies of veterans make these points         laying down of traumatic memories. Further,
       cogently. In the ¢rst study, King et al.              these results challenge neurological ¢ndings,
       (1999) found that PTSD was the outgrowth              which are important, but clearly are being
       of multiple cumulative e¡ects of stressors,           interpreted without consideration of the
       beginning in family and personal life events          full context.
       decades earlier. Most important, this study
       found that the cascade of resource loss was
       the key element in the endpoint of PTSD.              Cultural adaptation models
       Indeed, events around the time of deploy-             Crosscultural ¢ndings also illustrate the
       ment were only partially related to PTSD,             need for a paradigm shift and indicate the
       and the fuller context of postdeployment              partiality, and even inaccuracy, of current
       experience was as fundamental. More                   trauma models. PTSD is one manifestation
       recently, Vogt et al. (2011) found that PTSD          of trauma responding. It appears to be uni-
       is best explained by multiple chains of risk,         versal, but is not necessarily the principal
       with many of these originating in predeploy-          aspect of trauma response in non western
       ment experiences. These pathways led to               cultures (Akello, Richters, & Reis, 2009; de
       major psychosocial and material resource lo-          Jong & Reis, 2010; Hagengimana & Hinton,
       ss and an inability to access critical resources      2009; Hinton & Lewis-Ferna¤ndez, 2011;
       when needed. Central to the discussion of             Hobfoll & de Jong, 2013; Igreja, 2008; van
       resource caravans and passageways, not only           Duijl et al. 2010; van Ommeren et al., 2001).
       was the availability of postdeployment social         This should appear obvious, but it is not a
       support a larger predictor of posttraumatic           well accepted supposition. The reason it
       response than was exposure to warfare, but            should be obvious is that it follows in
       that social support was largely in£uenced             a straightforward manner from both
       by childhood family resources, relationship           emotional processing theory (Foa & Kozak,
       disruptions, perceived threat of warfare              1986) and dual processing theory (Brewin
       exposure and postdeployment stressors.This            & Holmes, 2003), if they are not so strictly
       was examined more closely by Interian                 tied to western cognitive models and if we
       et al. (2014), who found that home-front stres-       do not prematurely tie neurological evidence
       sors predicted PTSD whether they occurred             as meaning that mind^brain connections
       before or after deployment. Moreover, these           are universal.
       factors were stronger predictors of PTSD              What needs to be understood is that, in many
       than combat exposure or unit cohesion.                cultures, the borders between real and
       These studies further clearly illustrate that         unreal, this world and the dream world,
       emotional processing theory (Foa & Kozak,             and the very acceptance of the linear nature
       1986) and dual representation theory                  of events are looser, or even rejected. Further,

       24
Copyright © War Trauma Foundation. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Hobfoll

       there is good evidence that somatic response      regimen for Cambodians that does not relate
       to psychological distress and illness has quite   to the memory and cognition based treat-
       varied somatic correlations that are cultu-       ments e¡ective in western populations. Con-
       rally evidenced. Recent research on neural        sider also that, whereas in American and
       plasticity (Chiao, 2009; Dom|¤ nguez, Turner,     western European patients, panic disorder
       Lewis, & Egan, 2010) would even mean that         typically leads to further panic disorder,
       the brain itself would develop to accommo-        Puerto Ricans who experience ataque de ner-
       date these views of the world. This would         vios, which close resembles panic disorder,
       help to explain why the principal reactions       widely report relief after the attack (Lewis-
       to traumatic events in many cultures have         Ferna¤ndez et al., 2002). Such a paradoxical
       been reported to be so culturally speci¢c         reaction means either that emotional proces-
       (Hinton & Kirmayer, 2013) and produce             sing theory (Foa & Kozak, 1986) and the
       di¡erent idioms of distress and di¡erent          dual processing^neural systems model (Bre-
       pathways to wellness.                             win et al., 2010) are specious (which I do
       Hinton & Otto (2006), in their careful            not think is the case) or that they are partial
       research on Cambodian refugees of a certain       and have been over-generalised, especially
       era, are illuminating on this point. Consider     regarding to the argument that neurological
       their description of many of the Cambodian        evidence supports these models.
       refugees that they have studied and treated.      Miller & Rasmussen (2010) similarly chal-
       Profound and rapid loss of personal, social       lenge cognitive-emotional theories of PTSD.
       and often material resources are common           Based on their work with adults in Afghani-
       elements of their reactions, as would ¢t what     stan, they found that PTSD was not the
       is found in western European and American         major pathway of expression of traumatic
       trauma (Hobfoll, 1991). However, the symp-        experience. They found, like Hinton, that
       tom expressions on cognitive, social,             indigenous idioms of distress were more
       emotional and physical levels are quite           common and primary than PTSD, even if
       di¡erent in remarkable ways. The refugees         PTSD certainly did occur. For example, jigar
       experience frequent palpitations, startled        khun was a long-term kind of melancholy,
       responses and poor appetite and sleep, feel       which adults reported as more salient than
       physically weak (khsaoy) and report a weak        intrusive images. Asabi was described as a
       heart (khsaoy beh doung). They often report       synthesis of nervousness and anger that often
       khyal attacks, which in some ways resemble        led to verbal and physical violence and self-
       panic attacks, but which are also accom-          beating. What is also key here was that
       panied by catastrophic cognitions about           Miller & Rasmussen found these culturally
       imminent bodily dysfunction and loss of           speci¢c symptoms to be more predictive of
       use of their arms and legs. Sleep paralysis,      functional impairment than was PTSD.
       which is rather uncommonly reported in            Bracken, Giller & Summer¢eld (1995) made
       the PTSD literature, was found to be evi-         a similar argument based on their studies
       denced by 67% of Cambodians with PTSD             in Uganda. For example, among Ugandans,
       (Hinton et al., 2005).                            dissociation in the form of spirit possession
       Hinton et al. (2012) found that both natural      is a common pathway of expression for those
       healing and e¡ective treatment pathways           exposed to trauma (van Duijl et al., 2010).
       between cultures likewise di¡er markedly.         A sense of spirit possession is consistent with
       This is a key point, as if trauma produced        cultural norms and social learning. Hence,
       speci¢c emotional and memory sequences,           it is not surprising that, among northern
       then recovery and treatment pathways              Ugandan former child soldiers, being
       would ¢t well across cultures. Hinton prac-       haunted by spirits called Cen is common post
       tices a somatic-psychological treatment           trauma (Akello et al., 2009). Add to this that

                                                                                                      25
Copyright © War Trauma Foundation. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Resource caravans and resource caravan passageways: a new paradigm for trauma responding
       Intervention 2014, Volume 12, Supplement 1, Page 21 - 32

       mass dissociative trance behaviour has been           were key predictors of PTSD and depression
       reported among Bhutanese refugees in                  among Iraqi asylum seekers. These factors
       Nepal (van Ommeren et al., 2001), in post-            outweighed the impact of direct war-related
       war Guinea Bissau (de Jong & Reis, 2010),             exposure, which is inconsistent with domi-
       in Mozambique (Igreja, 2008) and in                   nant models of PTSD (Laban et al., 2008),
       Rwanda (Hagengimana & Hinton, 2009).                  but highly consistent with COR theory.
       Trauma response follows cultural patterns             This was also noted in the responding of
       that have some common bases, but there is             those a¡ected by the World Trade Center
       much elasticity because the environmental             (WTC) attack on 11 September 2001. On
       conditions that create, sustain or impede             one hand, peritraumatic reaction was a
       resource acquisition and maintenance are              major predictor of PTSD outcomes (Galea
       culturally embedded. Resource caravans                et al., 2002). However, loss of job and loss of
       will ¢t cultural and environmental impera-            possessions were as predictive of PTSD as
       tives through social development and life-            was any aspect of the experience that
       long social processing, and intervention              occurred closer in time to the trauma. Again,
       must occur along the paths of these caravans          these factors are not part of the trauma mem-
       or the structures that support caravan path-          ory and are not even present when fear pro-
       ways. Hence, we will see responding that              cessing associated with the event occurs.
       resembles PTSD worldwide, but as we                   By focusing on the trauma memory and
       become more distanced from western cul-               initial fear responding, we remove ourselves
       ture, the major expressions of trauma will            from context, which appears to be a para-
       look increasingly less like PTSD, or PTSD             mount factor found for those who more care-
       will become a more secondary or                       fully include context, resource caravans
       tertiary response.                                    and resource passageways in their designs.
                                                             So, studying survivors of Hurricane Katrina,
                                                             Adeola (2009) found that the most signi¢-
       How current models remove us                          cant predictors of distress were: residency
       from factors of social context                        in the poorest parishes of New Orleans, hav-
       and social intervention                               ing dependent children, unemployment,
       If we do not study social context, and the car-       degree of property damage, and ¢nancial
       avan of resources and resource passageways            impacts sustained due to the disaster. Like-
       that are contextually evidenced, we miss              wise, others noted that, among Katrina
       both a deeper understanding of PTSD as                evacuees, not being insured, the degree of
       well as many avenues for potential social             home destruction and human loss were the
       intervention. Following the 1992 Hurricane            strongest predictors of posttrauma exposure
       Andrew in Florida, Ironson et al. (1997)              distress (De Salvo et al., 2007; Lee, Shen, &
       found that cognitive-emotional or infor-              Tran, 2009). However, even this human loss
       mation-processing variables were hardly               was seldom witnessed. Add to this, De Salvo
       related to trauma responding. Rather, they            et al. (2007) found that lack of property
       found that the extent of material resource            insurance, longer evacuation and commut-
       loss and length of time before receipt of             ing distance to work during the rebuilding
       insurance settlements were the major predic-          period and obstacles to obtaining quality
       tors. Further, the extent of material loss and        new residences were important predictors
       waiting for insurance settlements that would          of PTSD symptoms. Again, for most people,
       allow rebuilding were only modestly related           they were evacuated before home destruc-
       to the initial trauma experience. Likewise,           tion, and this was witnessed only when
       the prolonged time period waiting for asy-            they were allowed to return weeks or
       lum and posttraumatic living conditions               months later.

       26
Copyright © War Trauma Foundation. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Hobfoll

       Incorporating resilience in                          and Gaza during a period of intense con£ict.
       trauma models                                        This is one of the few multiwave studies in a
       Nowhere are the limitations ofcurrentclinical        zone of con£ict, and the only to examine both
       models of trauma more evident than in their          PTSD symptoms and positive adaptation as
       irrelevance to people’s resilience in the face of    measuredbydegreeofengagementinlifetasks.
       trauma. Accepted trauma models, being                Engagement is a concept adopted from organ-
       pathology-based and concentrating on the             isationalpsychology. It is comprisedof dedica-
       time of the trauma event, fail to account for        tion, absorption and vigour (Schaufeli et al.,
       aspects of resilience that are present, even         2002). In many ways,theseelementsofengage-
       when individuals develop PTSD.The paucity            mentcanbeseenasthepolaroppositeofPTSD,
       of attention, and therefore the future need for      whichresults inavoidance,inabilitytoconcen-
       emphasis on resilience inthe face of traumatic       trate and be positive and drained energy and
       stress, is cogently presented in a recent key        depressive e¡ect.
       paper by Southwick & Charney (2012) on               The results of path analysis indicated clearly
       resilience and depression. I am referring not        that trauma exposure is only weakly related
       only to resilience in terms of not experiencing      to engagement (Hobfoll et al., 2012). As pre-
       a powerful initial response, quick or early          dicted by COR theory, and consistent with
       recovery from severe response, or only experi-       the model of resource caravans, the impact of
       encing a moderate response. Rather, the              resource loss on both trauma symptoms and
       accepted models fail to make or even to              engagement far outweighs the impact of
       attempt to make any predictions about who            trauma exposure. Likewise, positive aspects
       continues to engage and even enjoy life tasks.       of social support were related to greater
       Even those with severe PTSD have a wide              engagement, but again, as COR theory pre-
       range of levels of engagement in life tasks and      dicts, resource gains in the form of sustained
       in resilience processes.                             social support are overshadowedby the larger
       In contrast to leading clinical theories of          in£uence of resource loss.
       PTSD, COR theory makes speci¢c predic-               In another of the rare prospective studies of
       tionsaboutbothlevelsofpsychologicaldistress          resilience in the face of the kind that is often
       as well as resilience.These predictions are well     linked with PTSD, Pietrzak et al. (2014)
       supported, although the literature regarding         examined more than 10,000 (World Trade
       adult resilience is still nascent and requires       Center) WTC responders 3, 6, and 8 years
       much more exploration. In contrast to a more         after the WTC attacks. They also examined
       developed child literature on resilience (see        the di¡erential responding of police versus
       Masten & Narayan, 2012), the adult literature        nontraditional responders who were gener-
       has focused only more recently on resilience         ally less well trained, such as construction
       and bolstering and protecting personal, social       workers security guards and transportation
       and material resources (Bonanno, Westphal,           workers. Here, resilience was de¢ned as the
       & Mancini, 2011). Basically, COR theory pre-         degree to which PTSD symptoms were not
       dicts that, to the extent that the caravan of        appreciably experienced at any time point.
       resources that people possess remains intact,        The majority of police and nontraditional
       the more likely they will be resilient, resistant    responders were resilient, but the police were
       or quickly recover. That is, their lifetime          signi¢cantly more likely to be resilient than
       resourcetrajectoryand resource reservoir will        nontraditional responders. Correspond-
       be more central to their response than will be       ingly, nontraditional responders were more
       the type or extent of trauma exposure.               likely to have chronic PTSD.
       InoneofthefewstudiesofPTSDandresilience              In this situation, the predictors of trauma
       in a zone of con£ict, Hobfoll et al. (2012) inter-   sensitivity (i.e., lower likelihood of being
       viewed1196PalestinianadultsoftheWestBank             resilient) were Hispanic ethnicity, prior

                                                                                                         27
Copyright © War Trauma Foundation. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Resource caravans and resource caravan passageways: a new paradigm for trauma responding
       Intervention 2014, Volume 12, Supplement 1, Page 21 - 32

       psychiatric history, WTC exposure severity,           chronic nature of the trauma and the low h-
       number of life stressors in the year prior to         ope for future positive political relief (Hob-
       the attack, number of WTC-related medical             foll, Mancini, Hall, Canetti, & Bonanno,
       conditions that developed after the attack            2011). The context of ongoing trauma, which
       and having less family support. Consistent            is characterised as a resource caravan passa-
       with COR’s resource caravan model, prior life         geway with intense trauma exposure and
       stressors, having a medical condition follow-         daily threat, and little hope for future change
       ing the events and the level of social support        resulted in a virtual reverse of the proportion
       combined to have a much greater in£uence              of individuals who were resilient versus
       thanWTC exposure severity.                            experienced chronic symptoms of trauma.
       That two aspects of status and role, being a          Indeed, the level of chronic distress versus
       police o⁄cer and Hispanic ethnicity, were so          resilience was so great as to result in a recon-
       critical is a re£ection of the di¡erent resource      sideration of resilience theory by Bonanno,
       caravans and resource caravan passageways             one of the co-authors. As previously
       that these groups’status re£ects. Hispanic eth-       Bonanno had theorised that resilience was
       nicity is likely to be explained as a factor by       evidenced by most of those facing trauma,
       language and cultural gaps that are obstacles         this key aspect of his pioneering theorising
       to translation of resources to action, as in di⁄-     on resilience had to be modi¢ed for those liv-
       culty of getting accurate news from main-             ing within chronic traumatic circumstances
       stream media, di⁄culty in accessing                   (Bonanno et al., 2011).
       treatment and possibly having more precar-
       ious employment stability. Importantly,
       police selection and training made a major            Conclusions
       di¡erencein resilience outcomes, eventhough           Responding is ¢rst and foremost an expres-
       the police had much greater trauma exposure           sion of the extent and chronicity of trauma
       in witnessing dead bodies, threats to their           in the environment and people’s ability to
       own lives and witnessing the death of peers.          seek safety, retain close attachments and
       Clearly, their selection for traits of strength       realistically hope for an end to the risk of
       and resilience and training on dealing with           trauma. When circumstances limit people’s
       trauma were critical determinants of PTSD             ability to retain or recreate resources and
       versus resilience.                                    where personal, social and material resource
       Examination of the trauma literature, incor-          loss is prominent, then high levels of PTSD
       porating clinical, epidemiological and more           and depression occurs, and recovery path-
       anthropological study, illustrates the import-        ways become blocked. The resource caravan
       ance of resource caravan passageways, when            and resource caravan pathways concept are
       we compare them to a similar study of resili-         key predictors of both pathological and
       ence trajectories examined in a resilience            resilience outcomes. This is especially true
       trajectory study with the abovementioned              for vulnerable populations and low resource
       cohort of Palestinians. In this regard, the           settings, as in such cases there are often
       critical di¡erence in ¢ndings compared to             multiple traumas occurring over a lifetime,
       the WTC study was that the majority of                with a continued spiralling of resource loss.
       respondents were not resilient. Indeed, more          Only a partial element to predictive models
       than four times the proportion of partici-            is added by clinically based theories of
       pants were in the severe, chronic distress            emotions or recorded memories. Nor would
       group in this study than in the WTC study,            neurological ¢ndings add much at this stage
       and even those in the best trajectories were          of research.
       nevertheless, experiencing considerable dis-          We must incorporate social context as cen-
       tress. This di¡erence was attributed to the           tral to responding. By focusing on the

       28
Copyright © War Trauma Foundation. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Hobfoll

       caravan of resources and resource passage-           cultural understanding of resource caravan
       ways that are contextually evidenced, we             passageways, helps to predict and explain
       create a deeper, more ecologically valid             trauma responding and expands insights
       understanding of PTSD and the array of               for intervention across cultures and settings.
       idioms of distress that are represented in           Future research needs to examine trauma
       di¡erent cultures. This, in turn, opens              in the social and cultural context. It will be
       multiple new avenues for potential clinical          important to examine whether and to which
       and social intervention. By way of example,          extent more traditional western concepts of
       the centrality of honour to many collectivist        PTSD ¢t within these other contexts, but at
       cultures would require intervention to focus         the same time to be creative in conceptualis-
       on steps to ‘repair tears in the fabric’ of honour   ing distinctions that are more culturally
       and shame through community reinte-                  speci¢c. By expanding our view of time, by
       gration, acceptance and recognition. This            comparing groups in context and by com-
       means that intervention would not be only,           paring groups in di¡erent contexts, we will
       or perhaps not at all, on the individual level,      engender a richer view of trauma responding
       but instead would be a community process,            and how treatment can likewise evolve.
       incorporating such concepts as collective
       e⁄cacy (Benight, 2004).
       As Norris, Sherrieb & Pfe¡erbaum (2011)              References
       prescribe, intervention must work to build           Adeola, F. O. (2009). Mental health and psycho-
       communities’ economic resources, increase            social distress sequelae of Katrina: An empirical
       access to services and mitigate risks associ-        study of survivors. Research in Human Ecology, 16(2),
       ated with social injustice in order to build         195-210.
       resilience systems. Social support should be         Akello, G., Richters, A. & Reis, R. (2009). Coming
       translated to building stronger social net-          totermswithaccountability:Why the reintegration
       works and enhance natural social supports,           of former child soldiers in Northern Uganda fails.
       ensuring robust linkages that can resist the         In: P. Gobodo-Madikizela & C. van der Merwe
       destructive impact of a disaster or trauma.          (Eds.), Memory, Narrative, and Forgiveness: Perspectives
       An important point here is that trauma is            on the un¢nished journeys of the past (188^212). Cam-
       compounded by social injustice and the               bridge: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.
       unfair access to resources that in some con-
       texts occurs by class, gender, sexual orien-         Benight, C. C. (2004). Collective e⁄cacy following
       tation, ethnicity or race.                           a series of natural diasters. Anxiety,Stress,and Coping,
       By focusing on the trauma memory and                 17(4), 401-420.
       initial fear responding, we remove ourselves
       from context because we focus on internal            Bonanno, G. A., Westphal, M. & Mancini, A. D.
       cognitive and emotional processes. This, of          (2011). Resilience to loss and potential trauma.
       course, arti¢cially leaves these aspects out         Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 7, 511-535 doi:
       of our clinical models. We lose predictive           10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032210-104526.
       value, become circular in explaining new             Bracken, P. J., Giller, J. E. & Summer¢eld, D.
       ¢ndings that are shoe horned into existing           (1995). Psychological responses to war and atro-
       models and leave most of those with PTSD             city: The limitations of current concepts. Social
       and other trauma responses untreated.                Science and Medicine, 40(8),1073-1082.
       Finally, as we begin to think more about
       resilience, we are poorly informed by clinical       Brewin, C. R. (2001). A cognitive neuroscience
       models. COR theory, with its emphasis on             account of posttraumatic stress disorder and its
       loss and gain of resources, the concept of           treatment. Behavior Research and Therapy, 39(4),
       resource caravans and the social and                 373-393 doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(00)00087-5.

                                                                                                                 29
Copyright © War Trauma Foundation. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Resource caravans and resource caravan passageways: a new paradigm for trauma responding
       Intervention 2014, Volume 12, Supplement 1, Page 21 - 32

       Brewin, C. R., Gregory, J. D., Lipton, M. & Bur-         Social Cognitive and A¡ective Neuroscience Advance,
       gess, N. (2010). Intrusive images in psychological       5(2^3),138-147 doi:10.1093/scan/nsp024.
       disorders: Characteristics, neural mechanisms,
       and treatment implications. Psychological Review,        Foa, E. B. & Kozak, M. J. (1986). Emotional pro-
       117(1), 210-232 doi:10.1037/a0018113.                    cessing of fear: Exposure to corrective infor-
                                                                mation. Psychological Bulletin, 99(1), 20-35 doi:
       Brewin, C. R. & Holmes, E. A. (2003). Psychologi-        10.1037/0033-2909.99.1.20.
       cal theories of posttraumatic stress disorder.
       Clinical Psychology Review, 23(3), 339-376               Galea, S., Ahern, J., Resnick, H., Kilpatrick, D.,
       doi:10.1016/S0272-7358(03)00033-3.                       Bucuvalas, M., Gold, J., . . . Vlahov, D. (2002).
                                                                Psychological sequelae of the September 11 terrorist
       Chiao, J.Y. (2009). Cultural neuroscience: A once        attacks in New York City.The New EnglandJournal of
       and future discipline. Progress in Brain Research,       Medicine,346(13),982-987.doi:10.1056/NEJMsa013404.
       178, 287-304 doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(09)17821-4.
                                                                Hagengimana, A. & Hinton, D. E. (2009). Ihaha-
       Cloitre, M., Stolbach, B. C., Herman, J. L., van der     muka, a Rwandan syndrome of response to the
       Kolk, B., Pynoos, R., Wang, J., . . . Petkova, E.        genocide: Blocked £ow, spirit assault, and short-
       (2009). A developmental approach to complex              ness of breath. In: D. E. Hinton & B. J. Good
       PTSD: Childhood and adult cumulative trauma              (Eds.), Culture and Panic Disorder (205^229). Red-
       as predictors of symptom complexity. Journal of          wood City, CA: Stanford University Press.
       Traumatic Stress, 22(5), 399-408, doi: 10.1002/
       jts.20444.                                               Hinton, D. E. & Kirmayer, L. J. (2013). Local
                                                                responses to trauma: Symptom, a¡ect, and heal-
       De Jong, J.T.V.M. (2004). Public mental health and       ing. Transcultural Psychiatry, 50(5), 607-621 doi:
       culture: Disasters as a challenge to Western mental      10.1177/1363461513506529.
       health care models, the self, and PTSD. In: J. P.
       Wilson & B. Drozdek (Eds.), Broken Sirits: The treat-    Hinton, D. E. & Lewis-Ferna¤ndez, R. (2011). The
       ment of asylum seekers and refugees with PTSD. (159^     cross-cultural validity of posttraumatic stress dis-
       179). New York, NY: Brunner/Routledge Press.             order: Implications for DSM-5. Depress Anxiety,
                                                                28(9),783-801 doi:10.1002/da.20753.
       De Jong, J.T.V.M. (2005). Commentary: Decon-
       structing critiques on the internationalization of       Hinton, D. E. & Otto, M. W. (2006). Symptom
       PTSD. Culture, Medicine, and Psychiatry, 29(3), 361-     presentation and symptom meaning among trau-
       370discussion371-366.doi:10.1007/s11013-005-9172-7.      matized Cambodian refugees: Relevance to a
                                                                somatically focused cognitive-behavior therapy.
       DeJong, J.T.V. M.& Reis, R. (2010). Kiyang-yang,         Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 13(4), 249-260 doi:
       a West-African postwar idiom of distress. Culture,       10.1016/j.cbpra.2006.04.006.
       Medicine, and Psychiatry, 34(2), 301-321 doi: 10.1007/
       s11013-010-9178-7.                                       Hinton, D. E., Pich,V., Chhean, D. & Pollack, M.
                                                                H. (2005). ‘The ghost pushes you down’: Sleep
       De Salvo, K. B., Hyre, A. D., Ompad, D. C.,              paralysis-type panic attacks in a Khmer refugee
       Menke, A.,Tynes, L. L. & Munter, P. (2007). Symp-        population. Transcultural Psychiatry, 42(1), 46-77
       toms of posttraumatic stress disorder in a New           doi:10.1177/1363461505050710.
       Orleans workforce following Hurricane Katrina.
       JournalofUrbanHealth:BulletinoftheNewYorkAcademy         Hinton, D. E., Rivera, E. I., Hofmann, S. G., Bar-
       ofMedicine,84,142-152 doi:10.1007/s11524-006-9147-1.     low, D. H. & Otto, M. W. (2012). Adapting CBT
                                                                for traumatized refugees and ethnic minority
       Dom|¤ nguez, D. J. F., Turner, R., Lewis, E. D. &        patients: Examples from culturally adapted CBT
       Egan, G. (2010). Neuroanthropology: A humanis-           (CA-CBT). Transcultural Psychiatry, 49(2), 340-365
       tic science for the study of the culture-brain nexus.    doi:10.1177/1363461512441595.

       30
Copyright © War Trauma Foundation. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Hobfoll

       Hobfoll, S. E. (1988).The Ecology of Stress.Washing-       Mozambique. Journal of the Royal Anthropological
       ton, DC: Hemisphere.                                       Institute (N.S.), 14, 350-367.

       Hobfoll, S. E. (1989). Conservation of resources. A        Interian, A., Kline, A., Janal, M., Glynn, S. &
       new attempt at conceptualizing stress. American            Losonczy, M. (2014). Multiple deployments
       Psychologist, 44(3), 513-524 doi: 10.1037/0003-            and combat trauma: Do homefront stressors
       066X.44.3.513.                                             increase the risk for posttraumatic stress symp-
                                                                  toms? Journal of Traumatic Stress, 27(1), 90-97 doi:
       Hobfoll, S. E. (1991). Traumatic stress: A theory          10.1002/jts.21885.
       based on rapid loss of resources. Anxiety Research:
       An InternationalJournal, 4,187-197.                        Ironson, G.,Wynings, C., Schneiderman, N., Baum,
                                                                  A., Rodriguez, M., Greenwood, D. & Fletcher, M.
       Hobfoll, S. E. (1998). Stress, Culture, and Community:     A. (1997). Posttraumatic stress symptoms, intrusive
       The psychology and philosophy of stress. New York,         thoughts, loss, and immune functionafter Hurricane
       NY: Plenum Press.                                          Andrew. Psychosomatic Medicine, 59(2),128-141.
       Hobfoll, S. E. (2010). Conservation of resources           Kaniasty, K. (2012). Predicting social psychologi-
       theory: Its implication for stress. In: S. Folkma (Ed.),   cal well-being following trauma: The role of post-
       The Oxford Handbook of Stress, Health, and Coping (127^    disaster social support. Psychological Trauma:
       147). NewYork, NY: Oxford University Press.                Theory, Research, and Practice, 4(1), 22-33.
       Hobfoll, S. E. & deJong, J.T.V. M.(2013). Sociocul-        King, D.W., King, L. A., Foy, D.W., Keane,T. M.
       tural and ecological views of trauma: Replacing            & Fairbank, J. A. (1999). Posttraumatic stress dis-
       cognitive-emotional models. In: N. C. Feeny &              order in a national sample of female and maleViet-
       L. Zoellner (Eds.), Facilitating Resilienceand Recovery    nam veterans: Risk factors, war-zone stressors,
       FollowingTraumatic Events (69^90). New York, NY:           and resilience-recovery variables. Journal of Abnor-
       Guilford.                                                  mal Psychology, 108(1),164-170.
       Hobfoll, S. E., Johnson, R. J., Canetti, D., Pal-          Laban, C. J., Komproe, I. H., Gernaat, H. B. & de
       mieri, P. A., Hall, B. J., Lavi, I., . . . Galea, S.       Jong, J.T. (2008).The impact of a long asylum pro-
       (2012). Can people remain engaged and vigorous             cedure on quality of life, disability and physical
       inthe face of trauma? Palestinians intheWest Bank          health in Iraqi asylum seekers in the Netherlands.
       and Gaza. Psychiatry, 75(1), 60-75. doi: 10.1521/          Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 43(7),
       psyc.2012.75.1.60;10.1521/psyc.2012.75.1.60.               507-515 doi:10.1007/s00127-008-0333-1.
       Hobfoll, S. E., Mancini, A. D., Hall, B. J., Canetti,      Lee, E. K. O., Shen, C. & Tran,T.V. (2009). Coping
       D. & Bonanno, G. A. (2011).The limits of resilience:       with Hurricane Katrina: Psychological distress
       Distress following chronic politicalviolence among         and resilience among African-American evac-
       Palestinians. Social Science and Medicine, 72(8),1400-     uees. Journal of Black Psychology, 35(1), 5-23 doi:
       1408 doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.02.022.                  10.1177/0095798408323354.
       Hobfoll, S. E.,Watson, P., Bell, C. C., Bryant, R. A.,     Lewis-Ferna¤ndez, R., Garrido-Castillo, P.,
       Brymer, M. J., Friedman, M. J. & Ursano, R. J.             Bennasar, M. C., Parrilla, E. M., Laria, A. J.,
       (2007). Five essential elements of immediate and           Ma, G., . . . Petkova, E. (2002). Dissociation, child-
       mid-term mass trauma intervention: Empirical               hood trauma, and ataque de nervios among
       evidence. Psychiatry: Biological and Interpersonal         Puerto Rican psychiatric outpatients.TheAmerican
       Issues, 70(4), 283-315 discussion 316-369. doi:            Journal of Psychiatry,159(9),1603-1605.
       10.1521/psyc.2007.70.4.283.
                                                                  Lowe, S. R., Galea, S., Uddin, M. & Koenen, K. C.
       Igreja,V. (2008). Gamba spirits, gender relations,         (2014). Trajectories of postratraumatic stress
       and healing in post-civil war Gorongosa,                   among urban residents. American Journal of

                                                                                                                     31
Copyright © War Trauma Foundation. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Resource caravans and resource caravan passageways: a new paradigm for trauma responding
       Intervention 2014, Volume 12, Supplement 1, Page 21 - 32

       Community Psychology, 53(1^2), 159-172 doi: 10.1007/    con¢rmatory factor analytic approach. Journal
       s10464-014-9634-6.                                      of Happiness Studies, 3(1),71-92.

       Masten, A. S. & Narayan, A. J. (2012). Child devel-     Southwick, S. M. & Charney, D. S. (2012). The
       opment in the context of disaster, war, and terror-     science of resilience: Implications for the preven-
       ism: Pathways of risk and resilience. Annual            tion and treatment of depression. Science,
       Review of Psychology, 63, 227-257.                      338(6103),79-82.

       McLean, C. P. & Foa, E. B. (2011). Prolonged            Van der Kolk, B. A., Roth, S., Pelcovitz, D., Sun-
       exposure therapy for post-traumatic stress dis-         day, S. & Spinazzola, J. (2005). Disorders of
       order: A review of evidence and dissemination.          extreme stress:The empirical foundation of a com-
       Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics, 11(8), 1151-1163    plex adaptation to trauma. Journal of Traumatic
       doi:10.1586/ern.11.94.                                  Stress, 18(5), 389-399 doi:10.1002/jts.20047.

       Miller, K. E. & Rasmussen, A. (2010). War               Van Duijl, M., Nijenhuis, E., Komproe, I. H., Ger-
       exposure, daily stressors, and mental health in         naat, H. B. & de Jong, J. T. (2010). Dissociative
       con£ict and post-con£ict settings: Bridging the         symptoms and reported trauma among patients
       divide between trauma-focused and psychosocial          with spirit possession and matched healthy con-
       frameworks. Social Science and Medicine, 70(1), 7-16    trols in Uganda. Culture, Medicine, and Psychiatry,
       doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.09.029.                    34(2), 380-400 doi:10.1007/s11013-010-9171-1.

       Norris, F. H., Sherrieb, K. & Pfe¡erbaum, B.            Van Ommeren, M., de Jong, J. T., Sharma, B.,
       (2011). Community resilience: Concepts, assess-         Komproe, I., Thapa, S. B. & Carden‹a, E. (2001).
       ment, and implications for intervention. In: S.         Psychiatric disorders among tortured Bhutanese
       M. Southwick, B. T. Litz, D. Charney & M. J.            refugees in Nepal. Archives of General Psychiatry,
       Friedman (Eds.), Resilience and Mental Health: Chal-    58(5), 475-482 doi:10.1001/archpsyc.58.5.475.
       lengesacrossthe lifespan (162^175). Cambridge: Cam-
       bridge University Press.                                Vogt, D., Smith, B., Elwy, R., Martin, J., Schultz,
                                                               M., Drainoni, M. L., . . . Eisen, S. (2011). Prede-
       Pietrzak, R. H., Feder, A., Singh, R., Schechter, C.    ployment, deployment, and postdeployment risk
       B., Bromet, E. J., Katz, C. L. & Southwick, S. M.       factors for posttraumatic stress symptomatology
       (2014). Trajectories of PTSD risk and resilience in     in female and male OEF/OIF veterans. Journal of
       World Trade Center responders: An 8-year pro-           Abnormal Psychology, 120(4), 819-831. doi: 10.1037/
       spective cohort study. Psychological Medicine, 44(1),   a0024457.
       205-219 doi:10.1017/S0033291713000597.

       Rauch, S. & Foa, E. (2006). Emotional Processing
       Theory (EPT) and exposure therapy for PTSD.              Stevan E. Hobfoll, PhD, is the Judd and
       Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy, 36, 61-65.        Marjorie Weinberg Presidential Professor and
                                                                Chairofthe Department of Behavioural Sciences
       Schaufeli,W. B., Salanova, M., Gonzalez-Roma,            at Rush University Medical Center, Chicago,
       V. & Bakker, A. B. (2002). The measurement               Illinois, USA.
       of engagement and burnout: A two sample                  email: stevan_hobfoll@rush.edu

       32
Copyright © War Trauma Foundation. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
You can also read