Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
This article was downloaded by: [Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam] On: 05 May 2014, At: 07:51 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/urqe20 Gaze Behavior in Basketball Shooting a b b Rita F. de Oliveira , Raôul R. D. Oudejans & Peter J. Beek a Institute of Psychology , German Sport University b Research Institute MOVE , VU University Amsterdam Published online: 23 Jan 2013. To cite this article: Rita F. de Oliveira , Raôul R. D. Oudejans & Peter J. Beek (2008) Gaze Behavior in Basketball Shooting, Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 79:3, 399-404, DOI: 10.1080/02701367.2008.10599504 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02701367.2008.10599504 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content. This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and- conditions
de Oliveira, Research Note—Motor Oudejans, Control and Beek and Learning Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport ©2008 by the American Alliance for Health, Physical Education, Recreation and Dance Vol. 79, No. 3, pp. 399–404 Gaze Behavior in Basketball Shooting: Further Evidence for Online Visual Control Rita F. de Oliveira, Raôul R. D. Oudejans, and Peter J. Beek Downloaded by [Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam] at 07:51 05 May 2014 Key words: expert performance, far aiming, free throw, Subsequent research has challenged the notion jump shot that successful basketball shooting always involves, or should involve, extensive movement preprogramming. T he role of gaze behavior in sports has been examined extensively to identify visual search strategies and dif- ferences therein between skilled and less skilled athletes It became apparent that if the target was visible until ball release, seeing it for only the last 397 ms was suf- ficient for successful jump shooting (Oudejans, van de (e.g., Williams, Davids, & Williams, 1999). An important Langenberg, & Hutter, 2002). Using an occlusion para- finding is that experts look longer at relevant areas than digm, these authors found that shooters performed well nonexperts (e.g., Janelle et al., 2000; Vickers, 1996; Wil- when only late viewing was allowed but performed poorly liams, Singer, & Frehlich, 2002). For example, Vickers when ample early viewing was allowed. In this context, it used an eye-tracking system to record the gaze behavior is important to realize that arm kinematics in basketball of expert and near expert shooters during preparation shooting determines whether or not the basket is visible of the free throw. Target fixation durations showed that during the last elbow extension. If the propulsion hand expert shooters looked at the target area more than remains below the line of sight, the hands and ball oc- twice as long as near experts (972 vs. 357 ms). Vickers clude the target as soon as the elbow starts to extend. interpreted this finding to imply that long durations of The participants in the Vickers (1996) study used this low visual fixation are necessary to allow detailed param- (hand) shooting style. If the propulsion hand and ball eterization for the required shooting movements. This rise above the line of sight before elbow extension, the is in accordance with other authors who deemed this target is visible during the entire elbow extension until period of fixation to be essential for programming the ball release. Participants in the Oudejans et al. (2002) movement direction, force, and velocity, as well as limb study used the high (hand) shooting style. Thus, whether coordination and timing (Williams, Singer, & Frehlich, or not players can see the target after the ball and hands 2002). In addition, Vickers found that expert shooters enter the line of sight (mLoS = moment of line of sight) suppressed their vision of the target, either by blinking will determine their shooting style. So defined, shooting or looking away, as they initiated the final shooting move- style is an observable characteristic that is preserved in ment of the free throw. In line with the notion that far the presence of opponents and in shooting distances aiming movements are controlled in open-loop fashion, within (at least) the 3-point line (Elliott, 1992; Miller & she interpreted this finding as a strategy to reduce inter- Bartlett, 1993; Rojas, Cepero, Oña, & Gutierrez, 2000). ference between the visual and motor systems. Furthermore, using an intermittent viewing para- digm, de Oliveira, Oudejans, and Beek (2006) found that a late pick up of visual information in both low- and Submitted: January 1, 2007 high-style shooters characterizes expert performance of Accepted: September 5, 2007 the jump shot. In their study, long fixations were denied by virtue of intermittent occlusions, but gaze behavior Rita F. de Oliveira is with the Institute of Psychology at the German Sport University. Cologne Raôul R. D. Oudejans was not recorded. In a subsequent study, de Oliveira, and Peter J. Beek are with the Research Institute MOVE Huys, Oudejans, van de Langenberg, and Beek (2007) at VU University Amsterdam. found that basketball jump shot performance deterio- RQES: September 2008 399 de Oliveira.indd 399 8/19/2008 5:01:06 PM
de Oliveira, Oudejans, and Beek rated when visual information was unavailable during previous findings, we hypothesized that low-style shoot- movement execution. When vision was occluded just ers would look at the target relatively long before their before initiating the shooting movement, there were hands and ball occlude the target in the free throw but marked decrements in performance as well as clear de- for a shorter duration in the jump shot. In addition, we creases in interjoint coordination strength and stability. hypothesized that high-style shooters would look at the Collectively, these findings underscored the importance target during the final elbow extension both in the free of the online use of visual information in basketball throw and the jump shot. shooting. However, there are three important caveats of those studies vis-à-vis the Vickers (1996) study, which, as it stands, preclude regarding the aforementioned conclusion as final or general. Method The first concerns the methods used. While Vickers (1996) used an eye tracking system to study the duration Participants and location of gaze behavior, subsequent studies (de Oliveira et al., 2006, 2007; Oudejans et al., 2002) used Six experienced basketball players participated in visual occlusion methods to study the timing of optical the experiment (4 men and 2 women, M age = 27.7 information pickup. Whereas gaze behavior informs years, SD = 7.9). All played in the two highest basketball Downloaded by [Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam] at 07:51 05 May 2014 about locations of interest in the environment, it does leagues in The Netherlands for 11 years on average (SD not inform about the relevance of visual information = 6.4). Three participants exhibited a high shooting at different movement moments. Conversely, whereas style (all men) and 3 a low shooting style (1 man and occlusion methods inform about the sufficient and nec- 2 women), which was confirmed by video footage after essary timing of visual information pick-up, they do not the experiment (cf., de Oliveira et al., 2006). The ethics inform about what locations are actually fixated. Because committee of the Faculty of Human Movement Sciences previous conclusions pertained to different methodolo- approved the experiment. Each participant provided gies, it is difficult to evaluate their relative merits and written informed consent before the experiment. validity, especially with regard to high-style shooters. To date, their gaze behavior has not been examined. Apparatus The second caveat concerns the shot type (i.e., free throw vs. jump shot). Vickers’ (1996) results pertained We placed a standard basketball backboard and to the free throw, in which the relative positions of target rim in a laboratory and marked a line on the floor 4.6 and performer are unchanged during the movement. m from the backboard, which is the official distance for Subsequent results pertained to the jump shot, a dy- taking free throws. Gaze behavior was registered using namic task involving a whole body movement in which an eye tracking system (Applied Science Laboratories the relative positions between player and target change 501, Bedford, MA) that consisted of a head-mounted continuously. Although it is unlikely that different shot scene camera (50 Hz) and a monocular corneal reflec- types would require different patterns of visual control, tion system. In brief, the system recorded the field of this has never been verified before. view with a superimposed marker that corresponded The third caveat is the possible influence of the ki- to gaze direction. A digital video camera (50 Hz) was nematic pattern of high- and low-style shooters on visual placed orthogonal to the shooting plane to determine information in basketball shooting. The kinematics of the moment of ball release (cf., de Oliveira et al., 2006). high-style shooters allow the target to be visible until ball Two light emitting diodes (LEDs), one at the left and one release, permitting visual information to be picked up in front of the participant, signaled each trial initiation. and used online, that is, during the shot. In contrast, Official FIBA regulation-size basketballs were used. low-style shooters occlude the target with their hands during the final elbow extension, lending support to Procedure the interpretation of open-loop control, in which visual information is picked up only before movement initia- After a brief explanation of the task, participants tion (Vickers, 1996; cf. Oudejans et al., 2002). took several warm-up shots, before and after the eye The goal of the present study was to resolve the tracking system was mounted, adjusted, and calibrated. current ambiguity in and inconclusiveness of the under- They were instructed to look at the LED below the standing of the visual control in basketball shooting by backboard and start the trial when the LED switched on. accounting for all three factors. Specifically, we examined This allowed the experimenter to verify the eye tracking the gaze behavior of 6 expert basketball players, 3 with a system calibration on each trial. Participants performed high and 3 with a low shooting style, while they prepared 10 free throws and 10 jump shots in blocked fashion. The and performed free throws and jump shots. Based on order of block execution was counterbalanced across 400 RQES: September 2008 de Oliveira.indd 400 8/19/2008 5:01:06 PM
de Oliveira, Oudejans, and Beek participants. The free throw consisted of shooting from ing the final elbow extension, whereas in all high-style the line within the official 5 s. The jump shot consisted players the target remained visible after the ball passed of taking a step and a dribble, then stopping and jump their line of sight until ball release. Two researchers shooting from the line in a continuous self-paced move- independently repeated the procedure with 100% agree- ment. Finally, the eye-tracking system was removed and ment. This analysis confirmed that 3 participants had a participants took another 10 free throws and 10 jump low shooting style and 3 had a high shooting style. shots to establish their percentage of hits for undisturbed shooting. Each trial was registered as a hit or miss. Indi- Looking Behavior vidual testing lasted about 45 min. The total duration of looking behavior was indepen- dent of shot type and style group. A significant effect of Data Reduction period, F(1, 4) = 9.08, p = .039, H2 = .69, revealed that par- Looking behavior was coded for each frame starting ticipants looked longer at the target before mLoS than with LED illumination and ending with ball release. The after. There was also a significant Period × Style Group scores ranged from 0 to 1 such that looking at the rim was interaction, F(1, 4) = 10.66, p = .031, H2 = .73. This effect 1, the basket’s net or the small square on the backboard occurred because participants in the low-style group was .8, the remaining backboard was .6, other locations looked at the target significantly longer before mLoS Downloaded by [Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam] at 07:51 05 May 2014 were .4, and no gaze behavior was 0. We registered the than after, T(N = 6) = 2.21, p = .03, whereas there was no moment when the ball entered the participants’ field such difference in the high-style group, T(N = 6) = 0.27, of view (further denoted mLoS, the moment of passing p = .79. Complementary pairwise comparisons showed the line of sight) and the duration of target occlusion. that looking before mLoS was significantly longer in the For each condition, we calculated the average duration low-style than in the high-style group, T(N = 12) = 2.09, of looking behavior directed at the target (i.e., basket p = .04, whereas, conversely, looking after mLoS was or backboard, scores q .6) and submitted those average significantly longer in the high-style than the low-style durations to a repeated measures 2 x 2 x 2 analysis of group, T(N = 12) = 2.95, p < .01 (see Figure 1). variance (ANOVA) with shot type (2 levels: free throw, jump shot) and period (2 levels: before, after mLoS) as within-participant factors and style group (2 levels: low, high style) as between-participants factor. Five partici- pants started looking at the target less than 1.2 s before mLoS; therefore, we depicted this period in Figure 2. In addition, we calculated the duration of target occlusion (i.e., the duration of mLoS) and the final period (i.e., from start of mLoS to ball release) and submitted those durations to repeated measures 2 x 2 ANOVAs with shot type as within-participants factor and style group as between-participants factor. The percentage of hits was submitted to (nonparametric) Wilcoxon Signed Ranks Tests. Significance for each analysis was set at p < .05. Results Shooting Style To verify participants’ shooting styles, we analyzed images from the cameras. Footage from the side view camera showed whether or not participants brought the Figure 1. Average duration of looking behavior for each style group and shot type. Triangles pointing left indicate average ball above the head before the final elbow extension, durations before the ball and hands passed the players’ line displaying a high or low shooting style. Previously, this of sight (mLoS); triangles pointing right indicate durations method was used to assess shooting style and corrobo- after mLoS. Bars represent 2 standard errors (SE) of the rated more elaborate methods (cf., de Oliveira et al., mean. The third bar replicates the result found by Vickers (M 2006). In addition, footage of the head-mounted camera = 1,213 ms, SE = 351), while the sixth replicates the result showed that all low-style players occluded the target dur- found by Oudejans et al. (M = 360 ms, SE = 25). RQES: September 2008 401 de Oliveira.indd 401 8/19/2008 5:01:07 PM
de Oliveira, Oudejans, and Beek Figure 2 depicts the individual pattern of looking of style but significantly longer for the free throw than behavior during each trial for each shot type. As sug- for the jump shot, F(1, 4) = 9.90, p < .05, H2 = .71. Finally, gested by the figure, the final period duration was longer the percentage of hits (overall 68%, SD = 15) was inde- in the high-style than the low-style group, F(1, 4) = 6.97, p pendent of shooting style group, shot type, and the use = .058, H2 = .64, independent of shot type. Furthermore, of the eye-tracking system, all T(2 or 4) < 2.24, p > .08. the duration of target occlusion immediately following The percentages of hits for each participant and each mLoS (depicted as a gap in each plot) was independent shot type are also reported in Figure 2. Downloaded by [Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam] at 07:51 05 May 2014 Figure 2. Each participant’s looking behavior pattern and shot type (jump shot and free throw) for each trial. Accumulated scores greater than .6 indicate the participant was looking at the target area. A gap indicates the average duration of the line of sight (mLoS) for each participant and condition. For representational purposes, mLoS = 0 s. Filled circles indicate the moment of ball release for each trial. Corresponding shooting percentages are indicated in the upper right corner of each panel. 402 RQES: September 2008 de Oliveira.indd 402 8/19/2008 5:01:07 PM
de Oliveira, Oudejans, and Beek Discussion associated with about 400 ms of vision after mLoS and practically no vision before mLoS (see Figure 2). The aim of the present study was to help resolve In combination with previous results (de Oliveira conflicting findings and interpretations regarding the et al., 2006, 2007; Oudejans et al., 2002), the present visual control of basketball shooting by examining the findings corroborate the view that basketball shooting looking behavior of 6 expert basketball players (3 with is largely controlled online by vision, in the sense that a low shooting style and 3 with a high shooting style) visual information is picked up and used during move- executing both free throws and jump shots. As hypoth- ment execution. The specifics of the timing of optical esized, the expert low-style shooters looked compara- information pick-up depend on both the prevailing tively long at the target area when taking free throws, shot type and shooting style. These findings derive their as was the case in previous research (Vickers, 1996). relevance from the failure in previous studies to account However, this does not imply that low-style shooters for the confounding influence of shooting style, which used this long fixation duration to preprogram their resulted in ill-grounded conclusions. movements. Although several authors have assumed that Despite the improved understanding of visual con- long visual fixations are necessary for preprogramming trol in basketball shooting achieved here, it is unknown various movement parameters, like direction, force, ve- what information expert players use as they organize and locity, timing, and limb coordination (cf., Williams et al., deliver a basketball shot. To date, research has focused Downloaded by [Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam] at 07:51 05 May 2014 2002), they have said little about the nature and details predominantly on retinal sources of information by of preprogramming. Nevertheless, it has been argued investigating either gaze behavior or consequences of that long target fixations may enhance performance, visual occlusion. A notable exception is the Ripoll, Bard, because performers would use this time for psychologi- and Paillard (1986) study of head and eye stabilization cal and physiological regulation (Williams et al., 2002). in basketball shooting, which showed that head stabiliza- Moreover, contrary to what Vickers reported, none of tion is a reference for subsequent movement. In other the participants blinked or looked away from the target tasks, kinesthetic information about the orientation at movement initiation. Instead, the participants contin- and movements of head and eyes has also been found ued fixating the target either until the hands and ball to play a prominent role (e.g., Ooi, Wu, & He, 2001; occluded the target or until ball release, depending on Oudejans, Michaels, Bakker, & Davids, 1999). Besides their shooting style (see Figure 2). these variables, it has been suggested that relatively in- With the more dynamic shot task (i.e., the jump variant factors, such as eye level or the official height of shot), we found that low-style shooters looked at the the basket, are used in basketball shooting (de Oliveira, target only half as long (1 vs. 0.5 s) as in the free shot with- Oudejans, & Beek, in press; Oudejans, Koedijker, Blei- out any consequence for their shooting performance. jendaal, & Bakker, 2005). Therefore, investigating the This finding is consistent with previous evidence for informational basis of basketball shooting is an exciting the informative value of the last moments before mLoS and rich avenue for future research on the coupling of (de Oliveira et al., 2006) as well as with the finding that perception and action in far aiming tasks. viewing the target for 3 s prior to movement initiation was insufficient for accurate performance if no vision was allowed during the movement (de Oliveira et al., 2007). In this context, the argument that low-style shooters must References preprogram their movements due to the target occlu- de Oliveira, R. F., Huys, R., Oudejans, R. R. D., van de Langen- sion following mLoS is invalid. Because the duration of berg, R., & Beek, P. J. (2007). Basketball jump shooting mLoS is shorter than the visual motor delays reported in is controlled online by vision. Experimental Psychology, 54, the literature, low-style shooters may have used updated 180–186. optical information at the moment of ball release. de Oliveira, R. F., Oudejans, R. R. D., & Beek, P. J. (2006). Again as hypothesized, high-style shooters looked Late information pick-up is preferred in basketball jump at the target during the final shooting movements. This shooting. Journal of Sports Sciences, 24, 933–940. hypothesis was based on previous occlusion studies in de Oliveira, R. F., Oudejans, R. R. D., & Beek, P. J. (in press). which a visible target after mLoS was sufficient and nec- Experts appear to use angle of elevation information in basketball shooting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Hu- essary for accurate jump shooting using a high style (de man Perception and Performance. Oliveira et al., 2006; Oudejans et al., 2002). The present Elliott, B. (1992). A kinematic comparison of the male and results extend this previous finding by demonstrating female two-point and three-point jump shots in basket- that players actually gaze at the target while airborne, ball. Australian Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 24, and that the pattern of looking behavior is similar to that 111–118. in the free throw. Participant 4 was the best illustration Janelle, C. M., Hillman, C. H., Apparies, R. J., Murray, N. P., in this regard, because his excellent performance was Meili, L., Fallon, E. A., et al. (2000). Expertise differences RQES: September 2008 403 de Oliveira.indd 403 8/19/2008 5:01:07 PM
de Oliveira, Oudejans, and Beek in cortical activation and gaze behavior during rifle shoot- Rojas, F. J., Cepero, M., Oña, A., & Gutierrez, M. (2000). Ki- ing. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 22, 167–182. nematic adjustments in the basketball jump shot against Miller, S., & Bartlett, R. M. (1993). The effects of increased an opponent. Ergonomics, 43, 1651–1660. shooting distance in the basketball jump shot. Journal of Vickers, J. N. (1996). Visual control when aiming at a far target. Sports Sciences, 11, 285–293. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Ooi, T. L., Wu, B., & He, Z. J. (2001). Distance determined Performance, 22, 342–354. by the angular declination below the horizon. Nature, Williams, A. M., Davids, K., & Williams, J. G. (1999). Visual 414, 197–200. perception and action in sport. London: Routledge. Oudejans, R. R. D., Koedijker, J. M., Bleijendaal, I., & Bakker, Williams, A. M., Singer, R. N., & Frehlich, S. G. (2002). Quiet F. C. (2005). The education of attention in aiming at eye duration, expertise, and task complexity in near and a far target: Training visual control in basketball jump far aiming tasks. Journal of Motor Behavior, 34, 197–207. shooting. International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychol- ogy, 3, 197–221. Oudejans, R. R. D., Michaels, C. F., Bakker, F. C., & Davids, K. (1999). Shedding some light on catching in the dark: Authors’ Notes Perceptual mechanisms for catching fly balls. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 531–542. At the time of this study, the first author was with the Oudejans, R. R. D., van de Langenberg, R. W., & Hutter, R. Faculty of Human Sciences at VU University Amsterdam. Downloaded by [Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam] at 07:51 05 May 2014 I. (2002). Aiming at a far target under different viewing Please address all correspondence concerning this conditions: Visual control in basketball jump shooting. article to Rita Ferraz de Oliveira, German Sport Uni- Human Movement Science, 21, 457–480. versity Cologne, Institute of Psychology, Am Sportpark Ripoll, H., Bard, C., & Paillard, J. (1986). Stabilization of head Müngersdorf 6, Cologne, Germany 50933. and eyes on target as a factor in successful basketball shooting. Human Movement Science, 5, 47–58. E-mail: r.oliveira@dshs-koeln.de 404 RQES: September 2008 de Oliveira.indd 404 8/19/2008 5:01:07 PM
You can also read