Pure progressive amnesia: An atypical amnestic syndrome?
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 0000, 00 (0), 1 – 18 Pure progressive amnesia: An atypical amnestic syndrome? Emmanuel J. Barbeau, Mira Didic, Olivier Felician, and Eve Tramoni Laboratoire de Neurophysiologie et Neuropsychologie, INSERM EMI-U 9926, Faculté de Médecine, Université de la Méditerranée and Service de Neurologie et Neuropsychologie, AP-HM Timone, Marseille, France Eric Guedj Service Central de Biophysique et de Médecine Nucléaire, AP-HM Timone, Marseille, France Mathieu Ceccaldi and Michel Poncet Laboratoire de Neurophysiologie et Neuropsychologie, INSERM EMI-U 9926, Faculté de Médecine, Université de la Méditerranée and Service de Neurologie et Neuropsychologie, AP-HM Timone, Marseille, France We report on M.S., an 83-year-old patient with isolated pure progressive amnesia. This rare, recently identified, form of amnesia has been described in elderly patients. Neuropathological studies suggest that this syndrome is an atypical clinical presentation of Alzheimer’s disease. The aim of our study was to characterize the neuropsychological pattern of pure progressive amnesia in comparison with other amnestic syndromes and memory dissociations reported in the literature. Our results indicate that pure progressive amnesia is characterized by a highly unusual dissociation in the realm of memory, with severe deficits on tests based on recognition and recall of verbal and visual single items, contrast- ing with relatively preserved anterograde autobiographical and spatial memory and normal recall of complex material such as stories. These findings suggest that memory for single items could depend on an independent system. One hypothesis is that M.S.’s unusual memory profile results from relative dysfunction of the ventral medial temporal lobe pathway. An alternative explanation implicates cognitive reserve. Further studies are required in order to progress on this matter. In any case, pure progressive amnesia is a clinical syndrome that may provide further insight into the organization of declarative memory. INTRODUCTION syndromes has considerably improved the under- standing of how memory is organized in the A wide variety of amnestic syndromes in relation human brain. Several dissociations following to various pathological conditions and anatomical medial temporal lobe damage have been reported. substrates have been described (see Kopelman, For example, a dissociation between impaired epi- 2002, for a review). The characterization of these sodic and preserved semantic memory has been Correspondence should be addressed to Emmanuel Barbeau, Service de Neurologie et de Neuropsychologie, CHU Timone, 264, rue Saint-Pierre, 13385 Marseille Cedex 05, France (E-mail: emmanuel.barbeau@medecine.univ-mrs.fr). The authors wish to thank M.S. for his kindness and long-standing cooperation, as well as anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments. EB is supported by a grant from the Conseil Général des Bouches du Rhône. This study was partially financed by AP-HM PHRC 2001/54. # 2006 Psychology Press, an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an informa business 1 http://www.psypress.com/cogneuropsychology DOI:10.1080/02643290600893594
BARBEAU ET AL. described in three adolescents who suffered from Czarna, Mortensen, & Waldemar, 2005). This hippocampal damage (Vargha-Khadem et al., syndrome, which we refer to as pure progressive 1997). The adolescents showed impaired spatial, amnesia, is thought to result from medial temporal temporal, and autobiographical memory that sig- lobe dysfunction (Butters, Lopez, & Becker, 1996; nificantly restricted their independence. Despite Lucchelli et al., 1994). It is characterized by a their severe amnesia, they were able to attend severe amnestic syndrome of insidious onset, regular school and acquire a substantial amount with memory performance that can remain of factual knowledge. It was further demonstrated stable or decline slowly over several years. Across that one of the three adolescents, Jon, was able to this period of time, other cognitive domains acquire a significant amount of knowledge about remain preserved, as well as independence previously unknown events through repetition regarding most aspects of daily life. However, (Baddeley, Vargha-Khadem, & Mishkin, 2001). all patients for which follow-up data were These studies indicated that acquisition of seman- available have slowly progressed towards demen- tic memory could sometimes be preserved despite tia. In two cases, neuropathological hallmarks of severely impaired episodic memory, at least in Alzheimer’s disease (AD) have been found the context of developmental amnesia. (Caselli et al., 1998; Didic et al., 1998). Pure pro- Another dissociation, between impaired recol- gressive amnesia may therefore represent a rare lection and preserved familiarity, has been clinical presentation of AD, characterized by a reported with convincing evidence. The three ado- focal isolated memory deficit over a prolonged lescents mentioned above obtained normal scores period of time. In-depth investigation of this on tests assessing verbal and visual recognition amnestic syndrome has never been conducted, pre- memory. Additional investigations with Jon sumably because this syndrome is rare and can showed that he could recognize previously easily be confounded with the classical form of learned items despite poor recollection of the AD. In the present paper, we describe the neuro- context in which the items were learned psychological profile of a patient with pure pro- (Baddeley et al., 2001). It was suggested that Jon gressive amnesia in detail and report an unusual performed well on recognition tasks because he dissociation within the realm of memory. could rely on familiarity. A growing number of We report on M.S., a patient with a pure pro- single case and group studies of adults who gressive amnestic syndrome of insidious onset became amnesic during adulthood have reported and gradual worsening. This case-study revealed a similar dissociation, suggesting that familiarity an unusual pattern of amnesia, unlike those and recollection could be independent processes reported in the literature, with a dissociation (Aggleton et al., 2005; Barbeau et al., 2005a; between impaired single-item and preserved Mayes, Holdstock, Isaac, Hunkin, & Roberts, complex material memory acquisition. M.S. was 2002; Yonelinas et al., 2002). severely impaired on all tasks requiring recall and Patients with medial temporal lobe dysfunction recognition of verbal and visual single items, caused by degenerative lesions can equally give while anterograde semantic, autobiographical, insight into the organization of memory. Several and spatial memory were relatively intact. elderly patients with severe isolated amnestic syndromes of insidious onset and gradual pro- gression, contrasting with preserved autonomy, CASE DESCRIPTION have been reported (Caffara & Venneri, 1996; Caselli, Couce, Osborne, Deen, & Parisi, 1998; M.S. was first examined in our memory clinic in Didic, Ali Cherif, Gambarelli, Poncet, & March 2002, at the age of 83. He had received Boudouresques, 1998; Kritchevsky & Squire, 17 years of formal education and had graduated 1993; Lucchelli, De Renzi, Perani, & Fazio, in law. A successful politician during most of his 1994; Miceli et al., 1996; Stokholm, Jakobsen, career, he had, for example, been elected to the 2 COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 0000, 00 (0)
PURE PROGRESSIVE AMNESIA French senate and held appointments to several subtests of the WAIS-III (Wechsler Adult international organizations. His memory problems Intelligence Scale; Wechsler, 2000), and there started gradually at the age of 79 (1998). He first was no difference between his verbal and perform- consulted a neurologist in April 2001. On exami- ance IQ. Working memory, executive functions, nation, there was an isolated memory impairment, language, praxis, and visuo-perceptive skills were as well as symptoms of mild depression, which all normal (results summarized in Table 1). quickly resolved with a serotonin reuptake inhibi- These preserved abilities contrasted with a severe tor. He was then referred to our memory clinic for deficit in the realm of memory. His general further evaluation. All experimental data reported delayed memory score on the WMS-III thereafter were collected 18 –24 months after this (Wechsler Memory Scale; Wechsler, 2001) was initial depression. 75 (5th percentile). When first examined in our department, he was It is noteworthy that M.S.’s performance on the complaining of forgetfulness of insidious onset auditory recognition index of the WMS-III was at that had slowly worsened over time. He reported the 0.5-percentile level. Concerning subtests of losing his personal belongings and missing the WMS-III, it should be noted that the use of appointments. He also had increasing difficulties scaled scores can hide valuable information for playing bridge because he forgot previous elderly people, presumably due to large standard announcements. M.S. was fully independent, was deviations in the control group (Collie & involved in various social committees, and exer- Maruff, 2000). For example, although M.S. did cised on a daily basis. As an example, he regularly not recall a single item of the delayed verbal went mountain climbing and skiing until July paired-associates subtest, he obtained a scaled 2002. A cheerful, witty man, very eager to under- score of 5. He also obtained a scaled score of 7 stand his memory problems, he enjoyed recalling on the delayed face subtest, although his perform- various trips around the world during his political ance was at the level of chance (raw score ¼ 26/48, career, which were recalled in great detail. chance ¼ 24/48). When examined on several occasions in March His memory impairment appeared equally on 2002 and September 2002, as well as in April 2003, the Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test he always came to his appointments by himself. (FCSR; Grober, Buschke, Crystal, Bang, & Elementary neurological examination was unre- Dresner, 1988; French adaptation: Ergis, Van markable. Sight and hearing were found to be der Linden, & Deweer, 1994). In the FCSR, normal. He did not drink or smoke. His personal recall is first assessed through free recall and then medical history included an angioplasty for coron- using cues for the words that have not been ary artery disease. No family history of neurologi- retrieved. This procedure, repeated three times in cal disease was identified, but both his parents died order to give the subject the opportunity to before the age of 65. A complete haematological improve his performance (Petersen, Smith, Ivnik, and biochemical screening was within normal Kokmen, & Tangalos, 1994), is followed by a limits (full blood cell count, thyroid, liver, and recognition subtest as well as delayed recall. renal function tests, serum glucose, electrolytes, Despite reinforced encoding, M.S. was found to serum protein electrophoresis, sedimentation be severely impaired on the FCSR. His perform- rate, B9 and B12 vitamin levels, serology for syphi- ance, both on delayed recall and on the recognition lis). An electroencephalogram (EEG), recorded on subtest, was 8 standard deviations below the mean two occasions, was also normal. for control subjects (Table 2). M.S. also underwent the DMS48, a visual recognition memory test (Barbeau et al., 2004). General neuropsychological assessment This test includes 48 targets, each target being M.S.’s IQ was above average (global IQ ¼ 114; shown simultaneously with a distractor during 82nd percentile). He obtained high scores on all the recognition procedure. Targets are equally COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 0000, 00 (0) 3
BARBEAU ET AL. Table 1. M.S.’s results on the standard neuropsychological Table 2. M.S.’s performance on the Free and Cued Selective assessment Reminding Test Test Result Score a Intelligence scale (WAIS-III) April 2001 April 2003 Vocabulary 16 Similarities 12 Max. Score Z-score Score Z-score Arithmetic 10 Digit span 11 Total recall 48 35 24.1 21 29.2 Information 13 (free þ cued) Comprehension 13 immediate recall Picture completion 12 Free delayed recall 16 4 22.5 0 23.9 Digit symbol-coding 12 Total 16 10 26.3 5 212.2 Block design 12 (free þ cued) Matrix reasoning 12 delayed recall Picture arrangement 9 Recognition 48 43 26.1 41 28.8 Verbal IQ 116 (86th percentile) Performance IQ 109 (73rd percentile) Note: Results show progressive worsening of M.S.’s memory Full scale IQ 114 (82nd percentile) impairment. Memory scale (WMS-III)a Logical memory I 10 Logical memory II 10 presented on either the left or the right side of the Face recognition I 8 Face recognition II 7 page. The participant is asked to identify the target Verbal paired associates I 6 that has been previously presented, if necessary Verbal paired associates II 5 using forced-choice recognition. This test is easy Family pictures I 8 for control subjects who make on average no more Family pictures II 7 than three errors. Yet M.S. performed 25.8 and General delayed memory 75 (5th percentile) 29.3 standard deviations (23 errors) below controls’ Executive functioning mean on immediate and delayed recognition, Trail Making Test A 51 s (60 + 26) respectively, performing only slightly above chance. Trail Making Test B 226 s (152 + 83) M.S.’s performance on the FCSR and the Copy of the 36/36 Type 1, 4 mn 08 DMS48 could not be explained by poor verbal Rey Osterreich figure (50th percentile) Word fluency “animals” 35 (29.7 + 11.9) abilities or visual dysfunction. He obtained nearly in 2 mn perfect scores on a picture-naming task, perform- Word fluency “p” in 2 mn 30 (20.8 + 7.3) ing at normal speed (Deloche & Hannequin, Stroop test Normal 1997). Propositional language and comprehension Frontal assessment battery 17/18 (17.3 + 0.8) were flawless. He performed above average on a Language visuo-perceptual task that requires matching Picture naming 79/80 (cut-off ¼ 69) unknown faces (Benton, Sirvan, De Hamsher, Pyramid–Palm Trees 48/52 (cut-off ¼ 49) Varney, & Spreen, 1983b), as well as on a visuo- Test (visual) spatial task (Benton, De Hamsher, Varney, & Spreen, 1983a). His performance was also Visual abilities Benton face perception 49 (41–54) normal on the Visual Object and Space Benton line orientation 28 (25.7) Perception battery (VOSP; Warrington & James, 2000), with the exception of the silhouette Praxis 29/29 subtest (22.1 standard deviations below mean). Note: Norms are presented in parentheses. WAIS ¼ Wechsler In comparison with the first neuropsychological Adult Intelligence Scale. WMS ¼ Wechsler Memory Scale. assessment in May 2001, M.S. obtained identical a Scaled scores: m ¼ 10 + 3. or better scores on all executive function tests in 4 COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 0000, 00 (0)
PURE PROGRESSIVE AMNESIA April 2003 (for example, he was able to provide the Finally, his recall of complex material seemed to name of 20 animals and 27 words beginning with be more accurate than his ability to recall single letter “P” in 2001 compared to 30 animals and 35 items. For example, he obtained a normal scaled words in 2003; his digit span was 5 forward and 4 score on the logical memory subtest of the backward in 2001, and 6 forward and 4 backward WMS-III, which is based on the free recall of in 2003). These results may be explained by his two complex stories (scaled score of 10 on imme- positive response to the treatment of his slight diate recall and of 10 on delayed recall; M ¼ 10, initial depression. At the same time, memory per- SD ¼ 3). A similar result was observed with the formance worsened, as shown by the scores at the Rey–Osterrieth complex figure. M.S. obtained FCSR in 2001 and 2003 (Table 2). normal scores at both immediate recall (score ¼ 9; In summary, preliminary neuropsychological M ¼ 14.5, SD ¼ 6.3) and after a 30-min delay data revealed an isolated anterograde amnesia of (score ¼ 13; M ¼ 13.8, SD ¼ 6.1). These results insidious onset and progressive worsening. It is contrast with all scores obtained on single generally considered that a score is impaired if it stimuli-based tasks such as the FCSR, the is 2 standard deviations below controls’ mean. It DMS48, the verbal delayed paired associates, or has to be noted that M.S.’s delayed MQ (75) did the face recognition tests. With these questions not reach that criterion (21.67 standard devia- in mind and M.S.’s informed consent, further tions). However, his global IQ was of 1 standard evaluation was undertaken with the aim to deviation above mean (114), and the difference inquire for possible dissociations. between global IQ and delayed QM was found to be statistically significant (p , .01, WMS-III Assessment of item recognition memory statistical manual). M.S.’s memory impairment could be considered as severe, since he performed M.S. underwent a battery of 11 recognition well below 2 standard deviations on both the memory tests using single stimuli developed FCSR and the DMS48, suggesting marked diffi- in our laboratory. Despite a large variety of culties to process the kind of information used in stimuli (words, faces, fruits, vegetables, abstract these tests. Both tests are thought to be relatively shapes) and procedures (forced-choice or yes/no independent from executive functioning and sensi- responses), M.S.’s performance was found to be tive to medial temporal lobe dysfunction (Barbeau consistently impaired (Figure 1). On average, his et al., 2004; Petersen et al., 1994; Pillon et al., performance was at 26.9 standard deviations 1994). It is therefore plausible that M.S.’s (SD ¼ 4.0) below the mean of controls (mean amnesia resulted from medial temporal lobe age ¼ 79.3, SD ¼ 2.2). Level of chance for 8 of (MTL) dysfunction. the 11 tests was 50% (one distractor for one However, despite poor performance on stan- target; the level of chance for the three other dard memory assessment, several intriguing tests was 44, 33, and 25%). Performance of features were observed during his stay as an in- control subjects always remained well above the patient at the hospital. First, unlike other level of chance (the minimum difference between amnesic patients, he seemed to have no difficulty level of chance and performance on any of the recalling what he had done during the previous tests for all control subjects being 20%), suggesting days, such as the different examinations he had that these tasks were on the whole relatively easy as undergone, or the various members of the staff there was no floor effect. he had met. Thus, some aspects of anterograde Furthermore, M.S. performed below 10 stan- autobiographical memory appeared to be pre- dard deviations on three tests. In the forced- served. Secondly, he had no difficulty finding his choice (FC) verbal test, participants have to learn way in the hospital (in contrast to many healthy a series of 48 words and recognize them after a visitors and out-patients) suggesting a preserved delay of 3 and 60 minutes. M.S.’s Z-score after ability to remember new routes and locations. the 3-minute delay was 23.3 but dropped COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 0000, 00 (0) 5
BARBEAU ET AL. Figure 1. M.S.’s Z-scores on 11 laboratory recognition memory tasks. M.S. performed below cut-off score ( 2 2 standard deviations below mean) on all tests. FC ¼ forced-choice procedure, otherwise yes/no format. to 211.8 after 1 hour, answering exactly at random (longer delay before recognition, simultaneous at that moment. This suggests not only that M.S. recognition/encoding operations, recognition had difficulties encoding and storing verbal infor- among different distractors) affected M.S.’s per- mation in memory for a short delay (3 minutes), formance to a disproportionate extent relative to but that even the little information that he had controls. encoded was forgotten in the following hour. He also failed on a continuous recognition task (218.4 standard deviations) in which participants Assessment of autobiographical memory watch a series of pictures during 10 minutes and M.S.’s autobiographical memory was assessed with have to say for each picture whether it had a standardized test, designed to measure auto- already been presented in the series or not. This noetic recall (Piolino et al., 2003). Participants are task requires deciding simultaneously whether asked to provide a detailed description of specific each picture has been seen or not and, if not, events that have occurred during their life. They encoding it for the eventuality that it becomes a have to recall four events covering five major target later in the series. M.S. also failed on a periods of their lifespan. M.S. obtained normal task (214.0 standard deviations) in which each scores for all of the five periods (Figure 2). He target has to be found among three distractors notably obtained a normal score concerning the (FC 3 distractors) after a 2-minute delay. Thus previous year, suggesting preservation of auto- on the whole, manipulation of any variable of the biographical memory for events that had occurred recognition tasks that made the task more difficult after the onset of his memory disorder. 6 COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 0000, 00 (0)
PURE PROGRESSIVE AMNESIA was able to provide detailed information on 9/10 people (he failed concerning a young popstar; score ¼ 9, M ¼ 8.8, SD ¼ 1.2). He was also shown a series of photographs of famous people and was asked to provide the name, or alternatively as many biographical details about the person as possible. M.S. could only name 20 out of 40 faces (M ¼ 34.6, SD ¼ 5.4) and provided verbal details on only 8 out of the remaining 20 faces (total ¼ Figure 2. M.S.’s performance on a standard test of autobiographical 28, M ¼ 38.0, SD ¼ 2.4), performing lower than memory (TEMPau). He obtained normal performance concerning 23 control subjects matched for age. He was then all periods, including the last period (after presumable disease shown a series of 10 photographs of famous onset). Vertical lines represent standard deviations. events. His performance did not differ from that of control subjects (83%, M ¼ 87.8%, SD ¼ His ability to recall autobiographical events was 5.9%). Notably, he obtained a perfect score on all further evaluated in an ecological setting. M.S. was three events that happened after the onset of his accompanied on a 30-minute walk through the memory problems. Finally, he was asked to recall hospital. After a 24-hour delay, he was asked to public events that had occurred after the onset of verbally recall this episode and all the events that his memory disorder (year 2001). He was spon- had occurred during the walk. He had no difficulty taneously able to provide an accurate account of providing a detailed report. This is a literal trans- 14 events covering the major highlights in politics lation of his spontaneous account: “We went to and sporting events of this period. have a drink on the ground floor, but we had to wait a long time for the elevator. She [the examiner] went to buy Le Point [a magazine], but I did not Assessment of spatial memory buy anything myself. We then drank a hot choco- Route learning in a real environment late in the small café, which is near the entrance. Methods. In order to evaluate to which extent We afterwards went to the children’s hospital, spatial memory was preserved in M.S., we which we visited for some time and from where designed a route-learning test in which M.S. and we saw the helicopter platform. Then we came normal controls had to learn two circuits in the back.” All episodes were correctly recalled with hospital (mean length: 267 m, SD ¼ 32). The the exception of the episode concerning the first route had 15 decision points (locations purchase of the magazine. He, not the examiner, where participants have to make a decision bought the magazine on the examiner’s request. whether to turn right, turn left, or continue straight on), and the second had 13. Each itinerary was completed in about 5 minutes. Participants Assessment of semantic memory were shown the route once before the evaluation M.S.’s scaled-score on the information subtest of began. During the evaluation, each error was cor- the WAIS-III (which assesses general, cultural rected. Trials were repeated until two consecutive knowledge about the world) was 1 standard devia- trials were successfully performed. After a 1-hour tion above mean. He also underwent a question- delayed trial, participants were asked (a) to point naire that assesses knowledge usually acquired in the direction of three decision points (point of through school, designed for patients of his departure, the nurse’s office, the library), (b) to age group. His score was perfect (20/20) for complete the same itinerary in the reverse way, both the historical (M ¼ 16.6, SD ¼ 4.0) and geo- (c) to draw the two routes on a floor map, and graphical (M ¼ 18.8, SD ¼ 2.2) parts of the test. (d) to verbally provide a detailed description of When shown the names of famous people, he each route. COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 0000, 00 (0) 7
BARBEAU ET AL. Results. M.S. perfectly succeeded on all of these tasks (Figure 3). He could point to all directions without hesitation. He made no error when com- pleting the routes in the opposite direction and suc- cessfully drew paths on the map (Figure 4). His verbal recall suggested that he was able to imagine himself walking on the path. M.S. was asked to complete the first path 8 months after he had learned it. He remembered it without hesitation and made only one error out of 15 decision points. Route learning in a virtual environment M.S. may have been familiar with the environ- ment of our previous experiment as an in-patient. In order to control for this effect, a virtual spatial task was designed. Methods. The itinerary was video-recorded from the front of a car driving through the suburb of a Figure 4. A: Path Number 2. The star indicates the beginning and town from the perspective of the driver. The the return point of the path. B: M.S.’s drawing of the path on a blank map, indicating that he was able to recall the path he town was unknown to all participants. The video learned and to draw its abstract representation on a map. was shown on two occasions, since older partici- pants could be unfamiliar with this type of pro- cedure. The length of the itinerary was about 4 Results. M.S.’s performance improved at the same kilometres; it lasted for 6 minutes and contained rate as that of controls and remained intact after a 15 decision points. At each intersection, the 24-hour delay (Figure 5). participant had to decide whether the car had to turn right, turn left, or continue straight ahead. Stylus maze Performance was assessed in three consecutive M.S. underwent the stylus maze test (Milner, trials. In addition, M.S. performed a trial after a 1972). Like previous spatial memory tasks, this 24-hour delay. Figure 5. M.S.’s performance on a virtual route learning test with 15 decision points. He obtained normal performance on all trials, Figure 3. M.S.’s performance on two routes learning tests in a real with no loss after a 24-hour delay. Control subjects did not setting. His performance was normal on all trials. Vertical lines undergo the test at the 24-hour delay. Vertical lines represent represent standard deviations. standard deviations. 8 COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 0000, 00 (0)
PURE PROGRESSIVE AMNESIA task evaluates sequence learning, but in intra- The data were recorded in a 128 128 matrix. personal space. This task was chosen in reference SPECT images were then reconstructed from pro- to the case study of H.M., whose performance jection data using the filtered back-projection was consistently impaired (Milner, 1972). algorithm with a 0.30 cut-off frequency Butterworth filter and a software zoom of 2 Methods. Participants have to learn a path on a (matrix, 128 128 128; voxel size, 1.7 board covered by a matrix of 10 10 bolts. We 1.7 1.7 mm). No attenuation correction was used the same path that H.M. had to learn, performed. Reconstructed brain slices were reor- which was made of 28 decision points. In order iented according to the bicommissural line. to reach criterion, the path has to be repeated A proportional Talairach’s grid was semiauto- with no error on three consecutive trials. matically drawn by Neurogam@ (SEGAMI@ Software), and 30 cortical regions of interest Results. M.S. learned the procedure after 23 trials, (ROIs) were defined on each hemisphere, using at a similar rate to that of control subjects (Table 3, stereotactic coordinates of the Talairach and controls from Milner, 1972). This is in sharp con- Tournoux atlas (Talairach & Tournoux, 1988), trast with H.M., who never learned the task after segmentation. For each ROI, cerebral blood despite an impressive amount of trials (more flow (CBF) was evaluated using a perfusion than 215 trials). The same procedure was repeated index calculated as the mean cortex-to-cerebellum at day þ 1 and day þ 2. M.S. consistently ratio and also expressed in number of standard improved his performance (number of errors deviations compared to a reference population of before reaching criterion at Day 0 ¼ 158; at Day 24 participants matched for age contained in the 1 ¼ 70; at Day 3 ¼ 13). software. Perfusion was considered abnormal if it was more than 2 standard deviations below that of control subjects’ mean. Brain imaging Cerebral blood flow study Results. Using a cut-off value of 22.0 standard Methods. Acquisition of single photon emission deviations below mean, there was marked hypo- computed tomography (SPECT) images was per- perfusion of the medial temporal lobes bilater- formed with a double-head gamma camera (DST; ally, involving all hippocampal as well as Sopha Medical Vision International) with fan- subhippocampal structures predominating in the beam collimators. The images were acquired 1 temporal lobes medially, extending to ventral hour after intravenous injection of 740 MBq 99m and anterior temporal lobe regions (Table 4). Tc-ethylcysteinate dimer (ECD) with Within medial temporal lobe structures, hypo- dimmed lights, in a quiet surrounding. The par- perfusion predominated in the hippocampus ticipant’s head was safely positioned in an adjusta- (mean right and left Z-score: 24.40), very ble head folder. A total of 64 angular views of 60 s closely followed by the perirhinal (24.10) and each were obtained through a 3608 circular orbit. entorhinal cortices (24.05). The subcallosal cortex showed hypoperfusion to a similar extent Table 3. M.S.’s performance on the stylus maze test (24.45). Hypoperfusion, to a lesser degree, was also observed in the posterior cingulate gyrus M.S. Controls H.M. (21.95) and in the occipito-temporo-parietal Number of trials to criterion 23 17.0 215 þ junction bilaterally (22.30). Hypoperfusions Number of errors to criterion 158 91.8 2,877 þ were bilateral in all cases. The remaining areas Note: Results show that M.S. performed at the level of controls within the temporal, frontal, parietal, and occipi- and much better than H.M. Controls (younger in age than tal cortex, as well as the thalami on both sides, M.S.) and H.M. reported by Milner (1972). showed normal perfusion (i.e., above cut-off). COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 0000, 00 (0) 9
BARBEAU ET AL. Table 4. SPECT results for M.S. isotropic voxels. Segmentation of the grey matter was manually performed on coronal sections per- Z-score pendicular to the grand axis of the hippocampus. Left Right In order to measure the subregions of the medial Region hemisphere hemisphere temporal lobe, regions of interest based on specific anatomical landmarks were defined (Duvernoy, Area 38 (occipito-temporo- 22.1 22.5 parietal junction) 1998; Insausti et al., 1998). They included the hip- Area 23 (posterior cingulate) 22.2 21.7 pocampus (hippocampus proper, dentate gyrus Area 20 (anterior basal 22.9 21.8 and subicular complex) as well as entorhinal, peri- temporal lobe) rhinal, and parahippocampal cortices. In order to Area 36 (perirhinal cortex) 24.8 23.4 control the effect of interindividual variability of Area 28 (entorhinal cortex) 24.3 23.8 Area 25 (subcallosal cortex) 24.3 24.6 participants’ head size on the volumes of the Hippocampus 24.7 24.1 studied structures, the volumes were normalized to the intracranial area (Eritaia et al., 2000). A Note: Results expressed in number of standard deviations below total of 5 participants (mean age: 72.0 years, SD mean (Z-score). Hypoperfusion shown when more than 2.0 standard deviations below that of control subjects. The ¼ 4.8) served as controls for this study. All under- remaining brain areas were found to show perfusion above went three tests of visual and two tests of verbal the cut-off of 2 2.0 standard deviations. recognition memory, on which they obtained normal results. Morphometric study with MRI Results. Detailed results are summarized in Table 5. Images of M.S.’s brain were acquired with a 1.5 T Within the medial temporal lobe, atrophy predomi- Magnetom MRI (Siemens, Erlangen), using a nated on the hippocampus when compared with standard head coil. It revealed diffuse and homo- results for control subjects (mean Z-score, left and genous atrophy. The anterior temporal horns right: 21.27), followed by perirhinal cortices were enlarged, and medial temporal lobe structures (20.93) and entorhinal cortices (20.56). Volumes were atrophied on both sides. T2-weighted images of the parahippocampal cortices did not differ showed punctuate foci of increased signal in the from that of control subjects (20.03). deep white matter of both hemispheres as well as in the putamen and the pallidum. No such lesions were observed in the thalami (with special DISCUSSION attention brought to the anterior and medio- dorsal nuclei), hippocampus, and parahippocampal Our case study of M.S., an 83-year-old patient gyrus. with an amnestic syndrome and preserved overall intellectual efficiency, revealed a highly unusual dissociation in the realm of memory. M.S. failed Medial temporal lobes volumetry on memory tasks for single items, such as recall Methods. A magnetization prepared rapid gradient of lists of words or recognition of verbal and recalled echo sequence allowing 3D reconstruction visual items. In contrast, recall of complex material acquired in the sagittal plane, aligned on the axis of such as autobiographical and spatial memory was the hippocampus, and a tilted coronal gradient relatively preserved. Performance on formal echo sequence was acquired on a 1.5 T memory tests based on complex relational Magnetom (TR ¼ 1,320 ms, TE ¼ 3.93 ms, TI ¼ material, such as words embedded in stories 800 ms, flip angle ¼ 158, FOV ¼ 256 256, (WMS-III logical memory) and the recall of matrix ¼ 256 256, slice thickness ¼ 1.5). The Rey’s complex figure, was also preserved. images were reconstructed in the axial and The comparison of two sets of tasks, one eval- coronal planes and reformatted to obtain 1-mm3 uating memory of single items, the other 10 COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 0000, 00 (0)
PURE PROGRESSIVE AMNESIA Table 5. M.S.’s medial temporal lobe volumetry Controls M.S. Region Mean volumea SD Volumea Percentage Z–score Left Hippocampus 2,936 593 2,230 76 21.19 Entorhinal 1,210 317 1151 95 20.19 Perirhinal 1,981 400 1,581 80 21.00 Parahippocampal 926 143 846 91 20.56 Right Hippocampus 3,175 612 2,357 74 21.34 Entorhinal 1,210 204 1,021 84 20.93 Perirhinal 2,007 620 1,478 74 20.85 Parahippocampal 932 238 1,051 113 þ0.50 a In mm3. evaluating memory for complex material, revealed storing single items, which then become unavail- a significant difference ( p , .02, Wilcoxon test, able through either recall or recognition, regardless details in Table 6). Memory for single items was of the sensory modality used. evaluated through recognition (11 tests from the This pattern of memory impairment is highly recognition memory battery developed in our atypical and differs from previously reported laboratory, the DMS48, the face subtest of the amnestic syndromes. M.S.’s memory impairment WMS-III), recall (the FCSR word list), and differs from that observed in the three adolescents paired association (the paired-associate subtest of reported by Vargha-Khadem et al. (1997). M.S. the WMS-III). Memory for complex material showed relatively preserved autobiographical and was assessed through autobiographical recall spatial memory as well as independence, whereas (TEMPau), spatial tasks (route learning; virtual these aspects were impaired in these young partici- route learning; maze task) as well as story and pants. On the other hand, M.S. consistently failed figure recall (WMS-III logical memory subtest; on all recognition memory tests (as well as on tests Rey’s complex figure; WMS-III family pictures). of recall of single items), while recognition was This result was unexpected, since most tests for preserved in the adolescents. Also, the adolescents single items consist of recognition tests, which succeeded on paired-associate tasks, providing that are easier than tests of recall. There was no the stimuli remained in the same modality (word – ceiling effect for the tasks containing complex word or picture – picture associations), while M.S. material, which could have explained this failed on this type of task (e.g., performance on the pattern. Also, there were no deficits in other WMS-III paired-associate subtest). However, areas of cognition that could explain M.S.’s poor semantic memory was relatively preserved in performance on tests of single items. His level of both M.S. and the adolescents. This fact therefore verbal competence was high (in the high precludes a complete double dissociation between average), and he showed intact visuo-perceptive these types of amnesia. Furthermore, a dis- abilities. This was shown, for example, by his per- sociation between impaired recollection and pre- formance on the facial recognition test and on the served familiarity has been demonstrated in VOSP. Attention and working memory was several studies. Yet such a dissociation is not normal, as reflected by normal scores on various likely to account for M.S.’s performance either. tests assessing these functions (e.g., digit span, Although these aspects have not been evaluated Trail Making Test A and B). We therefore in detail, it appears that neither familiarity nor suggest that M.S.’s memory impairment is recollection enabled him to recognize items. In related to a major difficulty in encoding and/or summary, the pattern of dissociation observed in COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 0000, 00 (0) 11
BARBEAU ET AL. Table 6. Comparison of results on single-item and complex- although it is frequently used to refer to a single material tests stimulus that has to be memorized (Cohen, Test Results Poldrack, & Eichenbaum, 1997; Henke et al., 1999; Mayes, Isaac, Holdstock, Hunkin, & Single-item Montaldi, 2001; Turriziani, Fadda, Caltagirone, FC verbal 1 hra 211.84 FC verbal 3 mina 23.33 & Carlesimo, 2004). In fact, a wide variety of Continuousa 218.38 memory tests in the animal and human literature 110 distractorsa 23.29 are based on such material (e.g., visual recognition Shapea 25.04 memory tasks, word lists). These tasks can be distin- Modifieda 22.90 guished from those that require establishing con- FC 3 distractorsa 214.05 junctions or relations among elements (such as Facesa 24.70 30 wordsa 22.51 stimulus association tests, configurational tests, Fruits & vegetablesa 28.06 object–place tests, place/time discontiguous events Abstract 1200a 25.85 tests) or from those assessing context-rich memory DMS48 29.30 such as spatial or autobiographical memory. WMS–III face subtestb 21.00 FCSR word list 28.80 Eichenbaum and collaborators (Eichenbaum, WMS–III paired-associate subtest 21.67 2000; Eichenbaum, Otto, & Cohen, 1994; Mean 26.7 Eichenbaum, Schoenbaum, Young, & Bunsey, SD 5.0 1996) have introduced a model of memory in Complex-material which information is first memorized in an inflexi- TEMPau 0.61 Route learning 0.71 ble manner at the single-item level. This stage is Virtual route learning 20.32 also critical for paired-associate tasks within the Maze task 0.00 same modality when the relation between the WMS–III logical memory subtest 0.00 items is inflexible and can be stored as a single- Rey’s figure 20.13 item equivalent. In this model, it is thought that Family pictures 21.00 flexible relations between items are established in Mean 0.0 SD 0.6 a subsequent stage, in particular when they are spatially or temporally discontiguous, or when Note: Performance on delayed tests when available. FC ¼ transitivity across items is required (Wallenstein, forced choice. FCSR ¼ Free and Cued Selective Reminding Eichenbaum, & Hasselmo, 1998). Beyond their Test. WMS ¼ Wechsler Memory Scale. a Tests from the recognition memory battery. bPerformance 1 respective differences, other groups have elabo- standard deviation below mean but performed at the level of rated similar hierarchical models of memory. chance by M.S. (see General Neuropsychological Assessment Mishkin and collaborators have suggested that section). “context-free” memory such as recognition based on familiarity or semantic memory could be disso- ciated from “context-rich” memory such as M.S. cannot easily be related to previously memory for episodic events (Mishkin, Suzuki, reported amnestic syndromes. Gadian, & Vargha-Khadem, 1997; Mishkin, Vargha-Khadem, & Gadian, 1998). Aggleton and Brown (1999) also provide evidence for a dis- Single-item memory and models of tinction between two memory systems, one being declarative memory crucial for familiarity judgements, while the By “single item”, we refer to a concrete or abstract other is thought to be critical for episodic and concept that is not embedded in a story (such as a spatial memory. Although these models signi- word), or that is processed as an isolated or single ficantly differ in many aspects, all converge to part, such as a picture. The term single item is not the notion that memory for single items could be very well defined in the literature on memory, dissociated from memory for complex material 12 COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 0000, 00 (0)
PURE PROGRESSIVE AMNESIA and context-rich episodes. In addition, these famous face test, with a lower performance than models converge concerning the involvement of that of 23 control subjects. Thompson, Graham, critical brain structures for these stages: Item and Patterson, Sahakian, and Hodges (2002) showed context-free memory, as well as familiarity, are in a group of patients with questionable dementia thought to depend on anterior subhippocampal of the Alzheimer’s type (QDAT) that patients areas (amongst which the perirhinal cortex with an initial impairment on a famous-people plays a major role), while relational, context-rich test had a high risk to convert to DAT after memory is thought to depend on the hippocampal a 1 –2 years period. M.S.’s performance on a formation. similar test may thus indicate evolution to a This perspective concerning declarative more widespread semantic impairment. memory raises an intriguing issue. The hierarchi- Furthermore, M.S., as an intellectual and a pre- cal structure of these models suggests that impair- vious high-level politician, probably had a particu- ment at the single-item level should interfere with larly high level of premorbid semantic memory. It the upstream acquisition of relational, context-rich could thus be that semantic memory appears pre- information. However, the model of Mishkin and served in a comparison with patients of his age collaborators (Mishkin et al., 1997, 1998) indi- group, whereas it may show a significant decline cates that there are two dissociable streams of compared to his personal premorbid level (thus information, one involved in single-item proces- demonstrating retrograde semantic loss). sing (the ventral pathway) and another involved Although semantic memory is often referred to in configurational and spatial processing (the as context-free memory, this is only partially the dorsal pathway). According to this view, a dis- case since any piece of new information on a sociation between the impairment of one given event is integrated in the previous know- pathway and the preservation of the other would ledge that the participant already has about this thus be possible. At the anatomical level, it has event. For example, information on Lady Diana’s recently been argued that parallel cortical routes death read in a newspaper will automatically be could relay sensory information to the hippo- integrated with what we already know about her. campus in the absence of the perirhinal cortex Within this frame, M.S.’s semantic memory may (Aggleton, Kyd, & Bilkey, 2004). Within this appear preserved because most new pieces of infor- theoretical framework, M.S.’s performance may mation can be integrated in a previously acquired be explained by the preservation of a parallel context that helps memorizing, whereas attempts route-processing memory for complex material. to learn new single-item stimuli would meet Another intriguing issue is the status of seman- with failure since they could not be related to tic memory in M.S. Performance on most tests any previous knowledge. that evaluate semantic memory were within normal limits, when compared with controls of Pure progressive amnesia his age group, as demonstrated, for example, by his performance on the WAIS-III Information The clinical features of M.S. are reminiscent of subtest (13; M ¼ 10, SD ¼ 3) and on other patients presenting with pure progressive tests, including tests of anterograde semantic amnesia. M.S. showed isolated amnesia, of memory. Yet this is surprising as there is evidence gradual installation and worsening over the to suggest that semantic memory depends upon course of several years. No identifiable cause, similar systems that support single-item memory such as tumour or significant vascular disease, (Barbeau et al., 2005b; Davies, Graham, Xuereb, was found to account for his memory loss. Williams, & Hodges, 2004). Therefore, should The clinical pattern of M.S. resembles that of not both be impaired to a similar extent? other patients with pure amnestic amnesia like, Although M.S. obtained normal results on most for example, T.T., who remained well oriented tests assessing semantic memory, he failed on a in time and space during a follow-up period of COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 0000, 00 (0) 13
BARBEAU ET AL. 8 years (Caffara & Venneri, 1996). meeting the criteria for Alzheimer’s disease Autobiographical memory, evaluated five years (Squire & Kritchevsky, 1996). In two other after the study onset, was still globally preserved. patients who came to autopsy, neuropathological Like M.S., T.T. was severely impaired on the data showed widespread Alzheimer-type changes Buschke Selective Reminding Test (a word list (Caselli et al., 1998; Didic et al., 1998). In memory test), as well as on paired-associate learn- addition, group studies have identified pure pro- ing. A similar pattern of memory impairment was gressive amnesia as a distinct and rare clinical reported in the patient of Lucchelli et al. (1994). variant of Alzheimer’s disease (Butters et al., She was also well oriented in time and space and 1996). Such a severe and isolated memory loss obtained a normal score on a maze-learning task. has been estimated to occur in about 3% of patients This contrasted with impaired performance on with a newly recognized cognitive impairment paired-associate learning and on face recognition (Bowen, Teri, Kukull, McCormick, McCurry, & tasks. A recent case-study of pure progressive Larson, 1997). Taken together, these data suggest amnesia with follow-up over a 8-year period had that pure progressive amnesia may represent an very similar features (Stokholm et al., 2005). atypical presentation of Alzheimer’s disease. Memory problems remained isolated until the last year when a more widespread impairment The neural substrate of M.S.’s memory was observed. At the time of the first examination, impairment: Hypotheses the patient was already found to be severely impaired at word list learning and on recognition Based on M.S.’s clinical features, Alzheimer’s memory tasks (22.8 standard deviations on the disease with a highly unusual clinical course is word part of the Recognition Memory Test). the most likely pathological substrate of M.S.’s Memory performance on these tests slowly wor- condition. Other conditions such as encephalitis, sened over the years. Despite these severe deficits seizures, Korsakoff’s syndrome, paraneoplastic dis- in a formal evaluation, he was reported to be inde- orders, transient global amnesia, and tumour are pendent in daily life. For example, during most of unlikely to account for his memory impairment. the follow-up period, he remained able to travel by Periventricular signal hyperintensities on himself, to shop for groceries, to continue garden- T2-weighted magnetic resonance imaging ing, and to take on small repair jobs in the house. (MRI), such as those found in our patient, From these ecological data, it is tempting to deserve some consideration. Chronic ischaemic deduct that his spatial memory was globally pre- injury could have damaged brain areas that are served and that his memory difficulties did not sig- crucial for memory, such as the hippocampus or nificantly interfere with his daily life. Overall, all the anterior and dorso-medial thalamic nuclei patients with pure progressive amnesia were (Aggleton & Brown, 1999). However, a neuroi- described as independent, with no impairment in maging study on M.S. revealed no evidence of vas- everyday activities (Bozoki, Giordani, cular damage in these structures. Heidebrink, Berent, & Foster, 2001). In Alzheimer’s disease, neurofibrillary tangles In previous reports on pure progressive (NFT) emerge in the ento- and perirhinal cortex amnesia, most of the discussion has focused on stages (I and II) before spreading to the hippo- the pathological changes underlying the impair- campus (Braak & Braak, 1991). The subsequent ments. Although the possibility of ischaemic stages (III and IV) are characterized by severe damage has been raised (Kritchevsky & Squire, destruction of the rhinal cortices and spreading 1993), a neurodegenerative condition seems to the hippocampal formation. It is tempting to more likely to be the origin of this disorder. Two suggest that M.S.’s memory disorder may rep- patients who had suffered from a long-standing resent an atypical clinical correlate of Stage isolated memory impairment ultimately pro- II/III of Braak and Braak’s classification. In a pre- gressed to a pattern of cognitive deterioration vious section, we referred to models of declarative 14 COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 0000, 00 (0)
PURE PROGRESSIVE AMNESIA memory, which postulate that item and context- Seltzer & Van Hoesen, 1979). These functional free memory, as well as familiarity, depend on networks may support spatial and autobiographical anterior subhippocampal structures, while rela- memory in M.S. Further anatomical and func- tional context-rich memory depends on the hippo- tional imaging studies are required to solve this campal formation. In this line of thought, issue. relatively circumscribed medial temporal lobe An alternative hypothesis is based on the lesions to rhinal cortices could explain M.S.’s concept of cognitive reserve. M.S. had a relatively unusual pattern of amnestic syndrome and in par- high IQ (114), as have most patients with pure ticular its relative specificity to single-item progressive amnesia (M ¼ 115.0, SD ¼ 11.7 for memory. In line with the hypothesis that rhinal the 5 patients in whom IQ was provided; Caffara cortices could be impaired in isolation, the clinical & Venneri, 1996; Didic et al., 1998; Kritchevsky data clearly suggest residual function of the hippo- & Squire, 1993; Lucchelli et al., 1994; campus in M.S. Unlike amnesic patients with hip- Stokholm, et al., 2005). The concept of cognitive pocampal damage (e.g., Vargha-Khadem et al., reserve suggests that nonacquired variables (e.g., 1997), anterograde autobiographical and spatial larger head size, greater neuronal density) and life memory was spared in M.S., suggesting that experience (e.g., high educational and occupational M.S.’s hippocampi were still partially functional. achievement) may provide a buffer against brain Furthermore, M.S.’s preserved ability to draw dysfunction when confronted with acquired central maps of routes that he had just learned is incompa- nervous system dysfunction (Legendre, Stern, tible with complete hippocampal dysfunction. Solomon, Furman, & Smith, 2003). It is possible However, this hypothesis remains speculative. that M.S. benefited from cognitive reserve in such Brain volumetry performed in M.S. did not a way that most cognitive domains were clinically support this interpretation, showing atrophy in preserved, although the disease process was more both anterior subhippocampal and hippocampal widespread. Relational context-rich memories may structures. A similar pattern of hypoperfusion rely on wider cortical networks than those for was found on SPECT. single-item memory, which may depend on a A tempting explanation to conciliate clinical specific module (Cohen et al., 1997). Within this and anatomical data would be to hypothesize context, the patient may also have undergone exten- that M.S.’s memory deficit for single items sive reorganization of function, in favour of com- results from damage of the ventral MLT pensatory mechanisms at an advanced age. pathway involving the perirhinal cortex and the In summary, M.S.’s memory impairment is hippocampus. Conversely, M.S.’s preserved abili- probably related to an atypical course of ties to solve spatial memory tasks could suggest Alzheimer’s disease, leading to an unusual sparing of the dorsal MTL pathway (Epstein, pattern of memory impairment. In this context, Harris, Stanley, & Kanwisher, 1999; Habib & we propose two hypotheses, not mutually exclu- Sirigu, 1987; Parkinson, Murray, & Mishkin, sive, that can account for this pattern of memory 1988). This is supported by the relative preser- impairment. One focuses on the initial site of vation of the posterior parahippocampal gyrus by NFT lesions beginning in subhippocampal struc- volumetry (Z-score ¼ 2 0.03) and SPECT (the tures leading to dysfunction of the ventral MTL hippocampus and anterior subhippocampal struc- route. The other focuses on cognitive reserve. tures showed hypoperfusion around 2 4.0 stan- However, further studies are required to make dard deviations whereas the posterior progress on this matter. parahippocampal gyrus was above the cut-off score of 2 2.0 standard deviations). Anatomical Manuscript received 14 July 2004 studies have also shown direct connections from Revised manuscript received 4 July 2006 parietal and frontal lobes to the hippocampus Revised manuscript accepted 5 July 2006 First published online day month year (Goldman-Rakic, Selemon, & Schwartz, 1984; COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 0000, 00 (0) 15
BARBEAU ET AL. REFERENCES Bozoki, A., Giordani, B., Heidebrink, J. L., Berent, S., & Foster, N. L. (2001). Mild cognitive impairments Aggleton, J. P., & Brown, M. W. (1999). Episodic predict dementia in nondemented elderly patients memory, amnesia, and the hippocampal-anterior with memory loss. Archives of Neurology, 58, 411– 416. thalamic axis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, Braak, H., & Braak, E. (1991). Neuropathological 425– 444; discussion, 444– 489. stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Aggleton, J. P., Kyd, R. J., & Bilkey, D. K. (2004). Neuropathologica (Berlin), 82, 239– 259. When is the perirhinal cortex necessary for the per- Butters, M. A., Lopez, O. L., & Becker, J. T. (1996). formance of spatial memory tasks? Neuroscience and Focal temporal lobe dysfunction in probable Biobehavioral Reviews, 28, 611– 624. Alzheimer’s disease predicts a slow rate of cognitive Aggleton, J. P., Vann, S. D., Denby, C., Dix, S., decline. Neurology, 46, 687– 692. Mayes, A. R., Roberts, N., et al. (2005) Sparing of Caffara, P., & Venneri, A. (1996). Isolated degenerative the familiarity component of recognition memory amnesia without dementia: An 8-year longitudinal in a patient with hippocampal pathology. study. Neurocase, 2, 99 – 106. Neuropsychologia, 43, 1810 –1823. Caselli, R. J., Couce, M. E., Osborne, D., Deen, H. G., & Baddeley, A., Vargha-Khadem, F., & Mishkin, M. Parisi, J. P. (1998). From slowly progressive amnesic (2001). Preserved recognition in a case of develop- syndrome to rapidly progressive Alzheimer disease. mental amnesia: Implications for the acquisition of Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 12, semantic memory? Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 251 – 253. 13, 357– 369. Cohen, N. J., Poldrack, R. A., & Eichenbaum, H. Barbeau, E., Didic, M., Tramoni, E., Felician, O., (1997). Memory for items and memory for relations Joubert, S., Sontheimer, A., et al. (2004). in the procedural/declarative memory framework. Evaluation of visual recognition memory in MCI Memory, 5, 131– 178. patients. Neurology, 62, 1317–1322. Collie, A., & Maruff, P. (2000). The neuropsychology Barbeau, E. J., Felician, O., Joubert, S., Sontheimer, A., of preclinical Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive Ceccaldi, M., & Poncet, M. (2005a). Preserved impairment. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, visual recognition memory in a patient with severe 24, 365– 374. hippocampal damage. Hippocampus, 15, 587– 596. Davies, R. R., Graham, K. S., Xuereb, J. H., Barbeau, E. J, Wendling, F., Régis, J., Duncan, R., Williams, G. B., & Hodges, J. R. (2004). The Poncet, M., Chauvel, P., et al. (2005b). human perirhinal cortex and semantic memory. Recollection of vivid memories after perirhinal European Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 2441– 2446. region stimulations: Synchronization in the theta Deloche, G., & Hannequin, D. (1997). Test de dénomi- range of spatially distributed brain areas. nation orale d’images DO80. Paris: Les Editions du Neuropsychologia, 43, 1329 –1337. Centre de Psychologie Appliquée. Benton, A., Hamsher, K de S., Varney, N., & Didic, M., Ali Cherif, A., Gambarelli, D., Poncet, M., Spreen, O. (1983a). Judgment of line orientation, & Boudouresques, J. (1998). A permanent pure form H. In Benton, A. L., Sirvan, A. B., amnestic syndrome of insidious onset related to Hamsher, K de S., Varney, N. R., & Spreen, O. Alzheimer’s disease. Annals of Neurology, 43, 526– (Eds.), Contributions to neuropsychological assessment. 530. New York: Oxford University Press Inc. Duvernoy, H. M. (1998). The human hippocampus (2nd Benton, A., Sirvan, A., Hamsher, K de S., Varney, N., ed.). New York: Springler-Verlag Press. & Spreen, O. (1983b). Facial recognition: Stimulus Eichenbaum, H. (2000). A cortical-hippocampal system and multiple choice pictures. In Benton, A. L., for declarative memory. Nature Review Neuroscience, Sirvan, A. B., Hamsher, L de S., Varney, N. R., & 1, 41 – 50. Spreen, O. (Eds.), Contributions to neuropsycho- Eichenbaum, H., Otto, T., & Cohen, N. (1994). Two logical assessment. New York: Oxford University functional components of the hippocampal memory Press Inc. system. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 449– 472. Bowen, J., Teri, L., Kukull, W., McCormick, W., Eichenbaum, H., Schoenbaum, G., Young, B., & McCurry, S. M., & Larson, E. B. (1997). Bunsey, M. (1996). Functional organization of the Progression to dementia in patients with isolated hippocampal memory system. Proceedings of the memory loss. Lancet, 349, 763– 765. National Academy of Science USA, 93, 13500– 13507. 16 COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 0000, 00 (0)
You can also read