Original Article Insomnia disorder increases the risk of subjective memory decline in middle-aged and older adults: a longitudinal analysis of the ...
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
SLEEP, 2022, 1–13 https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/zsac176 Advance Access Publication Date: 25 July 2022 Original Article Original Article Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/sleep/article/45/11/zsac176/6649765 by guest on 19 January 2023 Insomnia disorder increases the risk of subjective memory decline in middle-aged and older adults: a longitudinal analysis of the Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging Jean-Louis Zhao1,2,†, Nathan Cross1,3,4,5,*,†, , Chun W. Yao5,6,7, , Julie Carrier1,5,8,9, Ronald B. Postuma5,8,10, Nadia Gosselin5,8,9, Lisa Kakinami3,11 and Thien Thanh Dang-Vu1,2,3,4,5,* 1 Institut Universitaire de Gériatrie de Montréal and CRIUGM, CIUSSS du Centre-Sud-de-l’Ile-de-Montréal, Montreal, Canada, 2Department of Neuroscience, Université de Montreal, Montreal, Canada, 3PERFORM Centre, Concordia University, Montreal, Canada, 4Department of Health, Kinesiology and Applied Physiology, Center for Studies in Behavioral Neurobiology, Concordia University, Montreal, Canada, 5Canadian Sleep and Circadian Network, Montreal, Canada, 6Integrated Program in Neuroscience, McGill University, Montreal, Canada, 7Research Institute of McGill University Health Center, Montreal, Canada, 8Center for Advanced Research in Sleep Medicine, Hôpital du Sacré-Coeur de Montreal, CIUSSS du Nord-de-l’Ile-de-Montréal, Montreal, Canada, 9Department of Psychology, Université de Montréal, Montreal, Canada, 10Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery, McGill University—Montreal General Hospital, Montreal, Canada and 11Department of Mathematics and Statistics, Concordia University, Montreal, Canada *Corresponding author. Nathan Cross and Thien Thanh Dang-Vu, Center for Studies in Behavioral Neurobiology and PERFORM Center, Concordia University, 7141 Sherbrooke St. West, SP 165.30, Montreal, QC, Canada. Email: nathan.cross@concordia.ca; tt.dangvu@concordia.ca. † These authors contributed equally. Abstract Study Objectives: To examine the longitudinal association between probable insomnia status and both subjective and objective memory decline in middle-aged and older adults. Methods: 26 363 participants, ≥45 years, completed baseline and follow-up (3 years after baseline) self-reported evaluations of sleep and memory, and neuropsychological testing in the following cognitive domains: memory, executive functions, and psychomotor speed. Participants were categorized as having probable insomnia disorder (PID), insomnia symptoms only (ISO), or no insomnia symptoms (NIS), based on sleep questionnaires. Participants were further grouped based on their sleep change over time. Prospective odds of self-reported memory worsening were assessed using logistic regression, and associations between insomnia and cognitive performance were assessed via linear mixed-effects modeling, adjusted for demographic, lifestyle, and medical factors. Results: An increased odds (OR 1.70; 95% CI 1.29–2.26) of self-reported memory worsening was observed for NIS participants at baseline who developed PID at follow-up compared to those who developed ISO or remained NIS. Additionally, participants whose sleep worsened from baseline to follow-up (i.e. transitioned from NIS to ISO, ISO to PID, or NIS to PID) displayed increased odds (OR 1.22; 95% CI 1.10–1.34) of subjective memory worsening at follow-up compared to those who remained insomnia- free or improved their sleep. There were no significant associations between the development of PID or worsening sleep and performance on neuropsychological tests. Conclusions: These findings of an increased odds for subjective memory decline in middle-aged and older adults with insomnia disorder suggest insomnia may be an important target for early interventions addressing age-related cognitive decline. Statement of Significance These findings demonstrate an increased odds of experiencing subjective memory decline in middle-aged and older adults who devel- oped insomnia disorder, compared to adults who developed insomnia symptoms alone or no insomnia symptoms. Subjective memory complaints often precede the onset of mild cognitive impairment and dementia; therefore, insomnia disorder may contribute to the early stages of cognitive decline. Clinical management of insomnia in middle-aged and older adults thus may have implications for reducing the risk of cognitive decline. Key words: insomnia; cognition; memory; aging; cohort; CLSA Submitted: 24 March, 2022; Revised: 13 July, 2022 © The Author(s) 2022. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of Sleep Research Society. All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: journals.permissions@oup.com 1
2 | SLEEP, 2022, Vol. 45, No. 11 Introduction controlling for age-related demographic, lifestyle, and medical factors. We hypothesized that we would observe higher propor- Insomnia disorder is characterized by difficulties initiating or tions of subjective and objective memory decline in adults who maintaining sleep, or early morning awakenings, accompanied developed PID at follow-up, compared to those who remained by daytime impairment and poor self-reported quality of sleep without insomnia, and that higher proportions of memory [1]. It is one of the most prevalent sleep disorders, accounting for decline would also be observed in adults who experienced about 10% of the adult population [2, 3]. Moreover, nearly one- worsening of their sleep quality (i.e. developing ISO or PID), com- third of adults experience insomnia symptoms without impair- pared to those whose sleep improved over the same interval. ments of daytime functioning and do not fulfill all criteria for an insomnia disorder diagnosis [2, 3]. Insomnia has been shown to be associated with cognitive impairment across multiple domains [4–10]. Concurrently, cog- Methods nitive changes occurring with normal aging have also been well Study design and participants documented [11, 12]. Sleep quality also declines with age, and the Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/sleep/article/45/11/zsac176/6649765 by guest on 19 January 2023 The sample was derived from the 30 097 participants who com- link between sleep and age-related cognitive decline has been pleted the baseline assessments of the comprehensive cohort previously established [13]. In our previous cross-sectional study, from the Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging (CLSA). The using a large dataset of a comprehensive cohort study [14], we final sample consisted of 26 363 participants who completed examined the cognitive performance of middle-aged and older both the baseline assessments and the first wave follow-up adults with probable insomnia disorder (PID) based on ques- assessments (mean duration of 3.0 ± 0.3 years after baseline). tions aligned with DSM-5 criteria. In that study, we found that The CLSA is a national, 20-year, prospective cohort study that PID was associated specifically with poorer memory perform- started data collection in 2011. The CLSA study design and re- ance compared to those who experience insomnia symptoms cruitment process have been comprehensively described and only (ISO) or no insomnia symptoms (NIS), after controlling for published elsewhere [14]. This large-scale cohort study collects age-related demographic, lifestyle, and medical outcomes [15]. information on the biological, medical, cognitive, psychological, Yet, the predictive role of insomnia disorder in relation to subse- social, lifestyle, and economic aspects of middle-aged and older quent memory decline in later life remains unclear. Several lon- adults. Briefly, individuals were recruited through either health gitudinal studies have shown links between sleep complaints registration databases or random digit dialing. Eligible partici- (e.g. sleep duration, self-reported sleep quality, snoring) and the pants were aged 45–85 years at the recruitment, lived within 25 increasing risk of cognitive decline or dementia in middle-aged or 50 km of 1 of 11 data collection sites (depending on the site), and older adults [16–22]. However, the longitudinal associations were able to communicate in English or French, and exhibited between memory and insomnia disorder in older adults remain an absence of cognitive impairment at baseline. Moreover, per- to be addressed since earlier studies only evaluated broad sleep sons living in the three Canadian territories and remote areas, measures (e.g. sleep duration, overall self-reported sleep quality) on federal First Nations reserves and other settlements in the without a probable clinical diagnosis. provinces, full-time members of the Canadian Armed Forces, In older individuals, the presence of self-reported “sub- and institutionalized individuals were excluded from the CLSA. jective” memory complaints (SMC) (e.g. a complaint of de- For our study, further exclusion criteria included: any prior diag- creased memory performance) is associated with an elevated nosis of dementia, stroke, or major head injury resulting in a risk of eventual progression to mild cognitive impairment (MCI) loss of consciousness >20 min. To assess the relationship be- or dementia (~40.7% over 4-years) [23, 24]. Such conversion to tween insomnia and self-reported memory decline or objective MCI and dementia in individuals with SMC may occur pro- memory performance, medical and sleep comorbidities were gressively over 10–15 years, depending on factors such as age not part of the exclusion criteria. and the presence of other determined dementia risk factors, Study participants who are part of the CLSA compre- including sleep quality [25]. Additionally, memory performance hensive cohort were asked to provide information through decline assessed through neuropsychological testing, followed computer-assisted personal interviews, physical examinations, by a decline in other cognitive domains has been shown to neuropsychological battery, and biological samples—admin- often precede the clinical manifestations of dementias such istered per standardized protocols. Computer-assisted per- as Alzheimer’s disease [26, 27]. Hence, evaluating the associ- sonal interviews were administered at home, at baseline, and ation between insomnia disorder and the presence of SMC or follow-up. Neuropsychological data were collected during visits objective memory impairments in older adults is important to to data collection centers or during in-home interviews (when capture risk factors that may be involved in the earliest stages participants were unable to travel to the data collection center), of cognitive decline. at baseline and follow-up, for most participants (Supplementary Therefore, the primary aim of this study was to investigate Table 1). the longitudinal relationship between (1) the development of PID and the emergence of subjective and objective memory de- cline in middle-aged and older adults, and (2) the trajectory of Standard protocol approvals, registrations, and patient insomnia symptoms (i.e. worsening or improving) and the emer- consents gence of subjective and objective memory decline. Additionally, an exploratory aim of this study was to establish whether a The Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging was approved by 13 longitudinal association exists between the progression of no research ethics boards across Canada, as well as the Canadian insomnia symptoms to PID and objective deficits across other Institutes of Health Research (CIHR) Advisory Committee on cognitive domains. We assessed these relationships while Ethical, Legal and Social Issues (ELSI) for the CLSA. Written
Zhao et al. | 3 informed consent was obtained from all individual participants number of words recalled in one immediate recall trial (RAVLT I) included in the study [14]. and one short delay recall trial (RAVLT II, with a delay of 5 min) were reported [35]. Furthermore, as part of an exploratory ana- lysis, participants additionally completed tests that assessed Sleep and insomnia measures performance across other cognitive domains such as executive functions and psychomotor speed using well-validated meas- Measures of sleep habits were collected at baseline and follow-up ures. Briefly, measures of executive functions were obtained through structured self-report questions that were designed for using the Mental Alternation Test (MAT) and the Stroop Test this study as part of a larger set of measures to assess phys- (Victoria version), whereas psychomotor speed was assessed ical functioning. The questions were drawn from validated sleep via the Choice Reaction Time (CRT) Test. For detailed informa- questionnaires [28–30] and captured important aspects of sleep tion, see the Supplementary Data information. All tests were and their relation to health. The questions covered six domains: scored in a standardized way in collaboration with a CLSA (1) participants’ satisfaction with the type of sleep they were co-investigator who is a clinical neuropsychologist. getting; (2) hours of nighttime sleep; (3) trouble falling asleep or Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/sleep/article/45/11/zsac176/6649765 by guest on 19 January 2023 staying asleep; (4) daytime sleepiness; (5) dream enactment be- havior; (6) if they experienced, recurrent, uncomfortable feelings or sensations in the legs, or urges to move their legs while sitting Demographic and lifestyle measures or lying down (Supplementary Table 2). Sociodemographic measures, including age, sex, language, eth- Symptoms related to insomnia were drawn from domains 1 nicity, household income, and years of education, were obtained and 3. These questions were utilized to make a probable diag- through questionnaire-based interviews at baseline and nosis of insomnia disorder based on standard diagnostic criteria follow-up. In addition, lifestyle variables, including tobacco con- from the DSM-5 [1]. For detailed information on the questions sumption (current and former smoking habits), weekly alcohol used, see the Supplementary Data information. Specifically, consumption, and activity level, were also collected at baseline only participants who experienced difficulties with sleep onset and follow-up. For detailed information on the questions used, or maintenance three times or more per week, for longer than see the Supplementary Data information. 3 months, and stated that it significantly interfered (≥“Much”) with their daily functioning, and additionally were dissatisfied with their sleep pattern (
4 | SLEEP, 2022, Vol. 45, No. 11 Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/sleep/article/45/11/zsac176/6649765 by guest on 19 January 2023 Figure 1. (A) Criteria used for categorization into PID and ISO. (B) Study flowchart and group classification for Analysis 1 and Analysis 2. NIS, no insomnia symptoms; ISO, insomnia symptoms only; PID, probable insomnia disorder. decline with PID. Secondary outcomes included self-reported progression from no insomnia symptoms (NIS) at baseline to ei- physician-diagnosed memory problem and the associations ther NIS, insomnia symptoms only (ISO), or probable insomnia between PID and other cognitive domains (executive functions disorder (PID) at follow-up (NIS to NIS; NIS to ISO; or NIS to PID), and psychomotor speed) with PID. All analyses were conducted whereas the second analysis (Analysis 2) was based on whether with R (R version 4.0.0) [36]. Differences in demographic, life- participants had improving (ISO to NIS, PID to ISO, PID to NIS) or style, and medical characteristics between groups at baseline worsening (NIS to ISO, NIS to PID, ISO to PID) insomnia status. were assessed using ANOVA with Tukey’s honest significance The association between the different insomnia groups and self- test, or Pearson’s chi-squared tests. In total, two main analyses reported memory decline or self-reported physician-diagnosed were performed (Figure 1, B). Briefly, the first analysis (Analysis memory problem at follow-up was analyzed with logistic regres- 1) examined differences between groups based on participants’ sion models (using the default algorithm in R). The association
Zhao et al. | 5 between the different insomnia groups and objective measures Participation retention of cognitive performance over the following domains: memory, From the individuals who met the criteria for this study at the executive functions, and psychomotor speed was assessed with baseline assessment, 2122 (7.5%) withdrew from data collec- linear mixed-effects models, probing the interaction between tion at follow-up. Participants who had withdrawn from the change in cognitive performance and the change in insomnia study appeared to be older and a higher proportion reported status, from baseline to follow-up. These analyses were per- worse insomnia symptoms at baseline (Supplementary Table formed using the “lmer” function of the “lme4” package in R 3). In total, 12.1% of individuals who were identified as PID at [36]. All models included participants as a random factor, while baseline withdrew, compared to 8.1% of participants who were group, time, as well as their interaction were set as fixed factors. identified as ISO, and 7.0% of those who were identified as NIS. Pairwise comparisons with false discovery rate (FDR) correction Causes for withdrawal included: participants developing health- method were performed post-hoc, separately for the group-level related issues; participants changing locations; participants un- comparison of demographic and clinical characteristics, and for able to continue due to cognitive decline; participants entering each linear mixed model for all cognitive tests. While this ap- long-term care; reasons related to study fatigue; and death [39]. Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/sleep/article/45/11/zsac176/6649765 by guest on 19 January 2023 proach is less conservative than other methods, type II errors are less likely, and the approach provides better control against errors originating from multiplicity [37]. Results of the associ- Insomnia status conversions over time ations between the insomnia groups and objective measures of cognitive performance for executive functions and psychomotor Across the entire sample, 12 145 (66.45%) participants who met speed are shown in the Supplementary Material. the criteria for NIS during baseline remained NIS in the follow-up For each cognitive outcome, two models were created with assessment, 5652 (30.92%) transitioned to ISO and 481 (2.63%) to increasing complexity. A minimally adjusted model (model 1) ad- PID. Of the participants who experienced ISO at baseline, 2166 justed for age, sex, a binary variable of total household income (30.31%) remained ISO at follow-up, while 4,804 (67.22%) con- (>50 000 $ or ≤50 000 $), years of education, ethnicity, language, verted to NIS and 176 (2.46%) to PID. Finally, of the participants time to follow-up, and percentage loss to follow-up, based on our who experienced PID at baseline, only 27 (2.88%) had persisting previous findings from the baseline study [14]. Percentage loss PID, while 311 (33.12%) reverted to ISO and 601 (64.00%) to NIS. to follow-up was included as a covariate in order to reduce any bias of a difference between groups in this variable. The second model (model 2) additionally adjusted for a range of a priori fac- Analysis 1—NIS at baseline to NIS, ISO, or PID at follow-up tors that were considered relevant in the context of cognitive aging. These factors included: BMI, alcohol consumption (≥4 Sample characteristics. times per week), diagnosis of cancer, anxiety disorder, clinical To probe the association between the development of insomnia depression, or hypertension, current level of smoking, presence disorder in middle and later life with changes in cognition, the of chronic pain, activity level, a report of daytime sleepiness, a first analysis included 18 278 adults who initially exhibited NIS witness report of breathing interruption during sleep (i.e. sleep during baseline, disaggregated by insomnia status progression. apnea), a self-report of RBD (pRBD), a self-report of RLS, a re- In total, 12 145 (66.4%) participants were classified in group 1 (NIS port of using sleep-related medications including hypnotics, to NIS), 5652 (30.9%) participants in group 2 (NIS to ISO) and 481 antidepressants, antipsychotics, antiepileptics, antihistamines (2.6%) participants in group 3 (NIS to PID). Baseline demographic, and analgesics, as well as insufficient sleep (
6 | SLEEP, 2022, Vol. 45, No. 11 Table 1. Baseline demographic, lifestyle, and medical characteristics of participants in Analysis 1 NIS-NIS NIS-ISO NIS-PID (Group 1) (Group 2) (Group 3) sig. sig. sig. n = 12 145 n = 5652 n = 481 2 vs. 1 3 vs. 1 3 vs. 2 Demographics Age, years ± SD 65.47 (9.99) 65.75 (10.21) 62.24 (8.87) 0.256
Zhao et al. | 7 Table 2. Associations between insomnia and subjective memory decline at follow-up—Analysis 1 Subjective memory Group 2 vs. Group 1† Group 3 vs. Group 1† Group 3 vs. Group 2† Self-reported memory worsening Model 1 OR [95% CI] 1.24*** [1.15–1.34] 2.00*** [1.57–2.55] 1.64*** [1.28–2.10] Model 2 OR [95% CI] 1.19*** [1.10–1.30] 1.70*** [1.29–2.26] 1.45* [1.09–1.94] Self-reported diagnosed memory problem Model 1 OR [95% CI] 0.94 [0.60–1.44] 3.92*** [1.86–7.73] 3.75*** [1.79–7.86] Model 2 OR [95% CI] 0.70 [0.41–1.21] 2.80* [1.16–6.77] 5.55*** [2.02–15.20] OR, odds ratio; 95% CI, 95% confidence intervals. Group 1 = NIS to NIS; Group 2 = NIS to ISO; Group 3 = NIS to PID. Model 1 = adjusted for age, sex, a binary variable of total household income (>50 000 $ or ≤50 000 $), years of education, ethnicity, language, time to follow-up, and Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/sleep/article/45/11/zsac176/6649765 by guest on 19 January 2023 percentage loss to follow-up; Model 2 = adjusted for age, sex, a binary variable of total household income (>50 000 $ or ≤50 000 $), years of education, ethnicity, lan- guage, time to follow-up, and percentage loss to follow-up, BMI, alcohol consumption (≥4 times per week), diagnosis of cancer, anxiety disorder, clinical depression, or hypertension, current level of smoking, presence of chronic pain, activity level, a report of daytime sleepiness, a witness report of breathing interruption during sleep, a self-report of RBD, a self-report of RLS, a report of using sleep-related medications including hypnotics, antidepressants, antipsychotics, antiepileptics, anti- histamines, and analgesics, as well as insufficient sleep (
8 | SLEEP, 2022, Vol. 45, No. 11 Table 4. Baseline demographic, lifestyle, and medical characteristics of participants in Analysis 2 Improving Worsening symptoms Symptom- symptoms sig. sig. sig. group free group group Free vs. Worsening vs. Worsening vs. n = 5716 n = 12 145 n = 6309 improving improving free Demographics Age, years ± SD 65.39 (10.04) 65.47 (9.99) 65.40 (10.15) 0.969 0.998 0.969 Sex, %F 47.81 47.8 59.6 0.975
Zhao et al. | 9 such as anxiety and depression compared to participants who Associations between insomnia and objective memory remained without symptoms or those with improving symp- decline. toms. Similarly, participants in the “worsening symptoms” Results of the association between insomnia and declarative group showed a larger prevalence of sleep-related medication memory (RAVLT) assessed via linear mixed-effects modeling for use, specifically for hypnotics and analgesics compared to the model 2 are reported in Table 6, while results for model 1 are other groups. shown in Supplementary Table 10. On the immediate recall trial (RAVLT I), after adjusting for model 1, there was a significant per- formance improvement over time (β = 0.70 ± 0.01; 95% CI 0.68– Associations between insomnia and subjective memory 0.73), as well as a significant group effect (i.e. insomnia groups). decline. Participants who had worsening insomnia status (worsening Results of the logistic regression analysis are shown in symptoms group) performed significantly worse than partici- Table 5. On the first dichotomous dependent variable—sub- pants who had improving insomnia status (improving symp- jective memory worsening (Yes/No), 57.2% of participants toms group) across both assessments (β = −0.08 ± 0.03; 95% CI Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/sleep/article/45/11/zsac176/6649765 by guest on 19 January 2023 across all groups reported experiencing memory worsening −0.14–[−0.01]). However, there were no significant differences at follow-up. For the minimally adjusted model (model 1), the between all other insomnia groups. There was no significant odds of having memory worsening at follow-up were 27.3% interaction between time and insomnia groups. After adjusting greater (OR 1.27; 95% CI 1.17–1.39) for participants who ex- for model 2, the significant positive effect of time remained hibited deteriorating insomnia status (worsening symptoms (β = 0.72 ± 0.02; 95% CI 0.69–0.75), but the insomnia group effect group) compared to those who exhibited improving insomnia was no longer significant. Furthermore, there was no significant status (improving symptoms group) and 28.7% higher (OR 1.29; interaction between time and insomnia groups. On the delayed 95% CI 1.19–1.39) for participants in the “worsening symp- recall trial (RAVLT II), after adjusting for model 1, there was a sig- toms” group compared to participants who remained without nificant positive effect of time (β = 0.59 ± 0.02; 95% CI 0.56–0.62), symptoms (symptom-free group). Similarly, after adjusting for as well as a significant insomnia group effect. Similarly, parti- model 2, the odds of having memory worsening at follow-up cipants in the “worsening symptoms” group performed signifi- were 21.5% higher (OR 1.22; 95% CI 1.10–1.34) for participants cantly worse than participants in the “improving symptoms” in the “worsening symptoms” group compared to those in the group across both assessments (β = −0.11 ± 0.04; 95% CI −0.18– “improving symptoms” group and 21.8% higher (OR 1.22; 95% [−0.04]), however, no significant difference between other in- CI 1.12–1.33) for the “worsening symptoms” group compared somnia groups was found. There was no significant interaction to the “symptom-free” group. Furthermore, there were no sig- between time and insomnia groups. After adjusting for model 2, nificant differences in the odds of having subjective memory the significant positive effect of time (β = 0.59 ± 0.02; 95% CI 0.57– worsening at follow-up for participants in the “symptom-free” 0.62) remained, but the insomnia group effect was no longer sig- group compared to the “improving symptoms” group in both nificant. There was no significant interaction between time and model 1 and model 2. insomnia groups. Results of the associations between insomnia Lastly, on the second dichotomous dependent variable— and the other cognitive tests are shown in Supplementary Table self-reported diagnosed memory problem (Yes/No), 0.8% of par- 11. Briefly, there was no significant interaction between time ticipants across all groups self-reported being diagnosed with and insomnia groups in executive functions in both models. a memory problem at follow-up. Results were not statistically Moreover, there was no significant interaction between time and significant after adjusting for model 1 or model 2. insomnia groups in psychomotor speed in all models. Table 5. Associations between insomnia and subjective memory decline at follow-up—Analysis 2 Subjective memory Symptom-free vs. improving Worsening symptoms vs. improving Worsening symptoms vs. symptoms† symptoms† symptom-free† Self-reported memory worsening Model 1 OR [95% CI] 0.99 [0.92–1.06] 1.27*** [1.17–1.39] 1.29*** [1.19–1.39] Model 2 OR [95% CI] 1.00 [0.92–1.08] 1.21*** [1.10–1.34] 1.22*** [1.12–1.33] Self-reported diagnosed memory problem Model 1 OR [95% CI] 1.13 [0.74–1.74] 1.35 [0.84–2.17] 1.16 [0.78–1.72] Model 2 OR [95% CI] 1.09 [0.67–1.78] 0.97 [0.55–1.72] 0.85 [0.52–1.39] OR, odds ratio; 95% CI, 95% confidence intervals. Model 1 = adjusted for age, sex, a binary variable of total household income (>50 000 $ or ≤50 000 $), years of education, ethnicity, language, time to follow-up, and percentage loss to follow-up; Model 2 = adjusted for age, sex, a binary variable of total household income (>50 000 $ or ≤50 000 $), years of education, ethnicity, lan- guage, time to follow-up, and percentage loss to follow-up, BMI, alcohol consumption (≥4 times per week), diagnosis of cancer, anxiety disorder, clinical depression, or hypertension, current level of smoking, presence of chronic pain, activity level, a report of daytime sleepiness, a witness report of breathing interruption during sleep, a self-report of RBD, a self-report of RLS, a report of using sleep-related medications including hypnotics, antidepressants, antipsychotics, antiepileptics, anti- histamines, and analgesics, as well as insufficient sleep (
10 | SLEEP, 2022, Vol. 45, No. 11 Table 6. Estimated fixed effects for declarative memory—Analysis 2 Estimates of fixed effects Memory Parameter Estimate (SE) df t p 95% CI RAVLT I Model 2 Time (baseline vs. follow-up) 0.722 (0.02) 15 250 45.440
Zhao et al. | 11 to cognitive decline and dementia [42, 43]. Nevertheless, fol- worsening was only asked during the follow-up assessment and lowing adjustment of these risk factors, our results demon- was not available at baseline. As such, it is not possible to dis- strated that symptom-free adults who developed PID remained tinguish whether participants already had subjective memory at increased odds of subjective memory decline compared with complaints at baseline. However, based on the phrasing of the adults who converted to ISO or remained NIS. This suggests that question (“Do you feel like your memory is becoming worse?”), there also still exists an independent association between prob- it is implied that their memory deteriorated since the base- able insomnia disorder and subjective memory decline. line assessment. Furthermore, the classification of PID was In contrast, objective neuropsychological testing did not seemingly unstable from baseline to follow-up as only a few reveal any significant associations between insomnia and participants (2.88%) who had PID at baseline remained PID at memory performance, or other cognitive domains. Given the follow-up. Therefore, the long-term consequences of persistent short time interval between both assessments in this study insomnia disorder could not yet be investigated within this co- (~3 years) and considering the age distribution of the CLSA co- hort. This is surprising as studies have shown that insomnia hort, it is plausible that the current follow-up period is not a as a disorder is often a persistent condition, with a remission Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/sleep/article/45/11/zsac176/6649765 by guest on 19 January 2023 long enough duration for the association between insomnia rate of only 14%–41% over 1–5 years [53]. The low rate of per- and objective cognitive decline to be determined yet within this sistent insomnia disorder within this cohort could partly be due study cohort. Participants who developed PID and exhibited to the evaluation methods. A classification of PID was meas- subjective memory decline at this stage could potentially show ured using questions reflecting the DSM-5 criteria, but these further cognitive decline in the upcoming years. The examin- were not part of a standardized clinical interview conducted by ation of this cohort in future will be important to understand a qualified clinician. Thus, this cannot be regarded as a definite if probable insomnia disorder may increase the risk of devel- in-person diagnosis of insomnia disorder, although this would opment of neurodegenerative diseases and dementia in older be difficult to achieve in such a large sample size. Additionally, adults over time. despite including percentage lost to follow up as a confound, However, our results revealed that compared to female par- participants who withdrew from the study before the follow-up ticipants, male adults with NIS at baseline who developed PID period were likely to be older and a higher proportion reported exhibited significantly poorer memory performance (RAVLT II) worse insomnia symptoms at baseline, which may have in- over time compared with those who converted to ISO or re- fluenced these findings. Lastly, another limitation could in- mained symptom-free, despite females with PID showing a volve the CLSA study design and stringent selection criteria of greater prevalence of SMC. One possible explanation for this incorporating only cognitively healthy adults with an absence disparity could be that men are often more reluctant to seek of cognitive impairment, dementia, or history of brain trauma help and express their symptoms related to health issues [44, at baseline. Participants also seemed to be healthier, more edu- 45], however, the prevalence of SMC among male and female cated, and have higher household income than the general older adults is mixed in the literature [46–49]. Another explan- Canadian population [39]. Given the profile of the CLSA cohort, ation could be that women have previously been shown to out- these associations may differ in a sample more representative perform men on verbal memory tasks, specifically in adults with of the general population. Nonetheless, the fact that significant SMC [50] which could potentially indicate a sex-specific form of differences in subjective memory performance were still able to cognitive reserve delaying memory impairment prior to further be detected demonstrates the sensitive relationship between cognitive decline [51, 52]. However, the interaction between sex insomnia and memory in aging. Stronger associations may be- and insomnia status on objective memory change in this study come more prevalent as cognitive function declines over time, was no longer significant after adjusting for age-related demo- and more diagnoses of dementia occur within the cohort. graphic, lifestyle, and medical factors. Therefore, there appears In conclusion, these findings suggest that insomnia disorder to be a complex relationship between subjective and objective in middle-aged and older adults is associated with higher pro- memory impairments in older adults. portions of subjective memory decline, even after controlling for Taken together, these findings suggest that insomnia as a lifestyle factors and comorbidities. Objective memory deficits disorder, rather than symptoms of insomnia alone, is important were found only in men who developed probable insomnia dis- in the context of early changes in cognitive function in later order and could indicate a potential sex effect specific to the life. Subjective reports of daytime difficulties should be taken onset of insomnia in older adults rather than the worsening of into consideration when monitoring cognitive decline in older sleep, however this relationship appears complex and any sex adults with sleep complaints. Insomnia disorder thus may be differences require further targeted investigation. Insomnia dis- considered part of a combination of adverse factors that occur order may be a predictor of further cognitive decline and early in middle age and older adults, which are linked to higher risk signs of dementia, however more longitudinal evidence is re- of cognitive decline and may lead to MCI and dementia in later quired to confirm this. life. Importantly, adults who had improving sleep symptoms did not develop subjective memory decline at follow-up, suggesting that the adverse cognitive effects of insomnia disorder may be Supplementary Material halted. This highlights the importance of properly diagnosing Supplementary material is available at SLEEP online. and managing insomnia as early as possible in older adults. Adequately treating insomnia disorder might become an im- portant preventive measure for cognitive decline and mitigate the incidence of dementias in later life. Acknowledgments There are some limitations that must be considered in this This research was made possible using the data/biospecimens study. First, causation cannot be inferred from an observational collected by the Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging (CLSA). study. Further, the question surrounding subjective memory Funding for the Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging (CLSA) is
12 | SLEEP, 2022, Vol. 45, No. 11 provided by the Government of Canada through the Canadian 4. Patel D, et al. Insomnia in the elderly: a review. J Clin Sleep Institutes of Health Research (CIHR) under grant reference: LSA Med. 2018;14(6):1017–1024. doi:10.5664/jcsm.7172 94473 and the Canada Foundation for Innovation, as well as the 5. Altena E, et al. Sleep loss affects vigilance: effects of chronic following provinces, Newfoundland, Nova Scotia, Quebec, Ontario, insomnia and sleep therapy. J Sleep Res. 2008;17(3):335–343. Manitoba, Alberta, and British Columbia. This research has been doi:10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00671.x conducted using the CLSA Baseline Comprehensive Dataset ver- 6. Crenshaw MC, et al. Slow-wave sleep and waking cogni- sion 4.0 and Follow-up 1 Comprehensive Dataset version 1.0, tive performance among older adults with and without under Application ID 1906008. The CLSA is led by Drs. Parminder insomnia complaints. Physiol Behav. 1999;66(3):485–492. doi:10.1016/s0031-9384(98)00316-3 Raina, Christina Wolfson, and Susan Kirkland. The opinions ex- 7. Haimov I, et al. Chronic insomnia and cognitive functioning pressed in this manuscript are the author’s own and do not re- among older adults. Behav Sleep Med. 2008;6(1):32–54. flect the views of the Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging. doi:10.1080/15402000701796080 8. Orff HJ, et al. Discrepancy between subjective symptom- atology and objective neuropsychological performance Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/sleep/article/45/11/zsac176/6649765 by guest on 19 January 2023 Funding in insomnia. Sleep. 2007;30(9):1205–1211. doi:10.1093/ Financial disclosure: RP reports grants and personal fees from sleep/30.9.1205 Fonds de Recherche du Quebec-Sante, the Canadian Institute 9. Varkevisser M, et al. Chronic insomnia and daytime func- of Health Research, Parkinson Canada, the Weston-Garfield tioning: an ambulatory assessment. Behav Sleep Med. 2007;5(4):279–296. doi:10.1080/15402000701557425 Foundation, the Michael J. Fox Foundation, the Webster 10. Fortier-Brochu E, et al. Insomnia and daytime cognitive per- Foundation, and personal fees from Takeda, Roche/Prothena, formance: a meta-analysis. Sleep Med Rev. 2012;16(1):83–94. Teva Neurosciences, Novartis Canada, Biogen, Boehringer doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2011.03.008 Ingelheim, Theranexus, GE HealthCare, Jazz Pharmaceuticals, 11. Murman DL. The impact of age on cognition. Semin Hear. Abbvie, Jannsen, Phytopharmics, Inception Sciences, and 2015;36(3):111–121. doi:10.1055/s-0035-1555115 Otsuko, outside the submitted work. NG is funded by a CIHR 12. Glisky EL. Changes in cognitive function in human aging. In: Foundation grant, #FDN154291 and FRQS Chercheur boursier, Riddle DR, ed. Brain Aging: Models, Methods, and Mechanisms. #34776. TDV reports personal fees from Eisai, Paladin Labs and Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press/Taylor & Francis; 2007. Chapter 1. Jazz Pharmaceuticals outside the submitted work, and grants 13. Cross N, et al. A human neuroimaging perspective on sleep from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council in normative and pathological ageing. Curr Sleep Med Rep. of Canada (RGPIN 6990-2019), the Canadian Institutes of Health 2019;5:1–12. Research (MOP 142191, PJT 153115, PJT 156125 and PJT 166167) 14. Raina PS, et al. The Canadian Longitudinal Study on and the Fonds de Recherche du Quebec-Sante. JC reports con- Aging (CLSA). Can J Aging. 2009;28(3):221–229. doi:10.1017/ sultant fees from Eisai and Haleo and grants from NSERC S0714980809990055 (RGPIN-2016-05149) and CIHR PJT-153259. 15. Cross NE, et al. Association between insomnia disorder and cognitive function in middle-aged and older adults: a cross-sectional analysis of the Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging. Sleep. 2019;42(8). doi:10.1093/sleep/ Conflict of Interest zsz114 Nonfinancial disclosure: The authors have no conflicts of 16. Ferrie JE, et al. Change in sleep duration and cognitive interest. function: findings from the Whitehall II Study. Sleep. 2011;34(5):565–573. doi:10.1093/sleep/34.5.565 17. Potvin O, et al. Sleep quality and 1-year incident cognitive impairment in community-dwelling older adults. Sleep. Data Availability 2012;35(4):491–499. doi:10.5665/sleep.1732 Data are available from the Canadian Longitudinal Study on 18. Tworoger SS, et al. The association of self-reported sleep Aging (www.clsa-elcv.ca) for researchers who meet the criteria duration, difficulty sleeping, and snoring with cogni- for access to de-identified CLSA data. tive function in older women. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2006;20(1):41–48. doi:10.1097/01.wad.0000201850.52707.80 19. Ma Y, et al. Association between sleep duration and cog- nitive decline. JAMA Netw Open. 2020;3(9):e2013573. References doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.13573 1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical 20. Osorio RS, et al. Greater risk of Alzheimer’s disease in older Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5. Arlington, VA: American adults with insomnia. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2011;59(3):559–562. Psychiatric Association; 2013. doi:10.1111/j.1532-5415.2010.03288.x 2. Morin CM, et al. Epidemiology of insomnia: prevalence, 21. Foley D, et al. Daytime sleepiness is associated with 3-year self-help treatments, consultations, and determinants incident dementia and cognitive decline in older Japanese- of help-seeking behaviors. Sleep Med. 2006;7(2):123–130. American men. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2001;49(12):1628–1632. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2005.08.008 doi:10.1046/j.1532-5415.2001.t01-1-49271.x 3. Ohayon MM, et al. Epidemiological and clinical rele- 22. Sabia S, et al. Association of sleep duration in middle and old vance of insomnia diagnosis algorithms according to the age with incidence of dementia. Nat Commun. 2021;12:2289. DSM-IV and the international classification of sleep dis- doi:10.1038/s41467-021-22354-2 orders (ICSD). Sleep Med. 2009;10(9):952–960. doi:10.1016/j. 23. Jessen F, et al. Prediction of dementia by subjective sleep.2009.07.008 memory impairment: effects of severity and temporal
Zhao et al. | 13 association with cognitive impairment. Arch Gen Psychiatry. neuropsychological performance: a general population 2010;67(4):414–422. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.30 study. Sleep. 2010;33(4):459–465. doi:10.1093/sleep/33.4.459 24. Mitchell AJ, et al. Risk of dementia and mild cognitive impair- 39. Raina P, et al. Cohort profile: the Canadian Longitudinal ment in older people with subjective memory complaints: Study on Aging (CLSA). Int J Epidemiol. 2019;48(6):1752–1753j. meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2014;130(6):439–451. doi:10.1093/ije/dyz173 doi:10.1111/acps.12336 40. Baumgart M, et al. Summary of the evidence on modi- 25. Norton S, et al. Potential for primary prevention of fiable risk factors for cognitive decline and dementia: Alzheimer’s disease: an analysis of population-based a population-based perspective. Alzheimer’s Dementia. data. Lancet Neurol. 2014;13(8):788–794. doi:10.1016/ 2015;11(6):718–726. S1474-4422(14)70136-X 41. Jee HJ, et al. Impact of sleep disorder as a risk factor for de- 26. Rabin LA, et al. Predicting Alzheimer’s disease: neuro- mentia in men and women. Biomol Ther. 2020;28(1):58–73. psychological tests, self reports, and informant re- doi:10.4062/biomolther.2019.192 ports of cognitive difficulties. J Am Geriatr Soc. 42. Livingston G, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, 2021;60(6):1128–1134. and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. Lancet. Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/sleep/article/45/11/zsac176/6649765 by guest on 19 January 2023 27. Bondi MW, et al. Neuropsychological contributions to the 2020;396(10248):413–446. early identification of Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychol Rev. 43. Shamim SA, et al. Insomnia: risk factor for neurodegenerative 2008;18(1):73–90. doi:10.1007/s11065-008-9054-1 diseases. Cureus. 2019;11(10):e6004. 28. Bastien CH, et al. Validation of the insomnia severity index 44. Banks I. No man’s land: men, illness, and the NHS. BMJ. as an outcome measure for insomnia research. Sleep Med. 2001;323(7320):1058–1060. doi:10.1136/bmj.323.7320.1058 2001;2(4):297–307. doi:10.1016/s1389-9457(00)00065-4 45. Seidler ZE, et al. The role of masculinity in men’s help- 29. Buysse DJ, et al. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new seeking for depression: a systematic review. Clin Psychol Rev. instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry 2016;49:106–118. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2016.09.002 Res. 1989;28(2):193–213. doi:10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4 46. Holmen J, et al. Gender differences in subjective memory 30. Ikehara S, et al. Association of sleep duration with mortality impairment in a general population: the HUNT study. from cardiovascular disease and other causes for Japanese Norway. BMC Psychol. 2013;1(1):1-9. men and women: the JACC study. Sleep. 2009;32(3):295–301. 47. Iliffe S, et al. Subjective memory problems. BMJ. doi:10.1093/sleep/32.3.295 2010;340:703-706. 31. Neto AS, et al. Subjective cognitive decline: the first clinical 48. Genziani M, et al. Subjective memory impairment, objective manifestation of Alzheimer’s disease? Dement Neuropsychol. cognitive functionning and social activity in French older 2016;10(3):170–177. people: findings from the Three Cities study. Geriatr Gerontol 32. Reid LM, et al. Subjective memory complaints and cogni- Int. 2013;13(1):139–145. doi:10.1111/j.1447-0594.2012.00873.x tive impairment in older people. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 49. Paradise MB, et al. Subjective memory complaints, vascular 2006;22:471–485. doi:10.1159/000096295 risk factors and psychological distress in the middle-aged: 33. Jahn H. Memory loss in Alzheimer’s disease. Dialogues Clin a cross-sectional study. BMC Psychiatry. 2011;11:108–110. Neurosci. 2013;15(4):445–454. doi:10.1186/1471-244X-11-108 34. Lezak M. Neuropsychological Assessment. New York: Oxford 50. Wang L, et al.; Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. University Press; 1982. Gender differences in elderly with subjective cogni- 35. Tuokko H, et al. Cognitive measures in the Canadian tive decline. Front Aging Neurosci. 2018;10:166. 10.3389/ Longitudinal Study on Aging. Clin Neuropsychol. fnagi.2018.00166 2016;31(1):233–250. doi:10.1080/13854046.2016.1254279 51. Levine DA, et al. Sex differences in cognitive decline among 36. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R US adults. JAMA Netw Open. 2021;4(2):e210169. doi:10.1001/ Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2013. Accessed June jamanetworkopen.2021.0169 26, 2020. http://www.r-project.org/ 52. Sundermann EE, et al. Female advantage in verbal 37. Benjamini Y, et al. Controlling the false discovery rate: a memory. Neurology. 2016;87(18):1916–1924. doi:10.1212/ practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat WNL.0000000000003288 Soc Ser B Methodol. 1995;57(1):289–300. 53. Morin CM, et al. Incidence, persistence, and remission rate 38. Fernandez-Mendoza J, et al. Insomnia with objective of insomnia over 5 years. JAMA Netw Open. 2020;3(11):e2018 short sleep duration is associated with deficits in 782e2018782. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.18782
You can also read