MOVEMENTS AND SOCIAL ORGANIZATION OF WILD HOUSE MICE (MUS DOMESTICUS) IN THE WHEATLANDS OF NORTHWESTERN VICTORIA, AUSTRALIA
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
Journal of Mammalogy, 81(1):59–69, 2000 MOVEMENTS AND SOCIAL ORGANIZATION OF WILD HOUSE MICE (MUS DOMESTICUS) IN THE WHEATLANDS OF NORTHWESTERN VICTORIA, AUSTRALIA LISA K. CHAMBERS,* GRANT R. SINGLETON, AND CHARLES J. KREBS Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jmammal/article/81/1/59/2372948 by 59662000 user on 12 February 2021 Commonwealth Scientific and Industrial Research Organisation, Wildlife and Ecology, GPO Box 284, Canberra, Australian Capital Territory 2601, Australia (LKC, GRS) Department of Zoology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia V6T 1Z4, Canada (CJK) From September 1996 to May 1997, 187 wild house mice (Mus domesticus) were fitted with radiotransmitters at an agricultural site in the wheatlands of northwestern Victoria, Australia, to examine movements and social organization. Males had slightly larger home- range areas than females. Home-range size was highly variable (0.0002–8.024 ha) but could not be predicted from body size or body condition in males and females, or by whether females were breeding. Mice were site-attached during the breeding season, with extensive intersexual overlap of home ranges but variable intrasexual overlap. Home ranges were significantly larger during the nonbreeding season compared with the breeding season. Evidence existed for exclusive home-range use by females at all densities of mice, low to moderate home-range overlap for males when densities were low and increasing, and an apparent switch to a more gregarious phase in male mice when the breeding season ceased and densities were high. Nonbreeding mice seemed to be nomadic when densities were low, which is consistent with an earlier study of home ranges and social organization of mice on the Darling Downs, Queensland. Key words: agricultural landscape, house mouse, movements, Mus domesticus, radiotelemetry, so- cial organization The house mouse (Mus domesticus) is a of mice in an agricultural context. Although significant pest in the grain-growing regions studies have examined social organization of eastern and southern Australia because in the laboratory (e.g., Butler 1980; Crow- of the ability of this rodent to form out- croft and Rowe 1963; Singleton and Hay breaks reaching 1,000 mice/ha (commonly 1983) and in outdoor enclosures (e.g., Lid- referred to as mouse plagues). Current ap- icker 1976; Noyes et al. 1982; Wolff 1985), proaches for controlling this pest include few studies have examined movements, poisoning (Brown et al. 1997; Kay et al. habitat use, and social organization of feral 1994; Mutze 1993; Twigg et al. 1991) and house mouse populations (Fitzgerald et al. habitat manipulation (Singleton 1997). The 1981; Krebs et al. 1995b; Newsome 1969a, potential use of a virally vectored immu- 1969b; Selander 1970; Singleton 1983). nocontraceptive incorporated into an over- Krebs et al. (1995a) argued that social all integrated control strategy also is under factors might regulate mouse populations at investigation (Chambers et al. 1997). These low densities; disruption of social regula- approaches require an understanding of tion could be a necessary condition for the movement patterns and social organization generation of a plague. They proposed 2 al- * Correspondent: lisakchambers@netscape.net ternative models on how social behavior 59
60 JOURNAL OF MAMMALOGY Vol. 81, No. 1 could regulate mouse populations and fa- climate for that area were described by Singleton vored 1 of those for agricultural landscapes and Chambers (1996). A narrow ephemeral in Australia. That model postulated a no- channel (about 2 m wide), providing water to madic social system at low population den- nearby dams for 2 months of the year, formed the northern border of the site. Alongside this sities, strongly territorial behavior during channel was a grassy strip varying from 5 to 30 population increases, and finally a break- m in width. A 2nd grassy strip (5 m wide) was down of territorial behavior when densities associated with a fenceline bordering the west Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jmammal/article/81/1/59/2372948 by 59662000 user on 12 February 2021 were high. We predict that if the above of the site. Grasses provided dense cover in model is true, a radiotelemetry study of a these strips in September but began seeding and house-mouse population at varying densi- dying off in October and November, respective- ties should show a high number of lost sig- ly. From December to May, vegetative cover nals or long-range movements of mice at along fencelines was sparse and dry. Barley and low densities, little intrasexual overlap in wheat were grown on the eastern side of the home-range areas as population density in- fenceline. They were at a vegetative stage at the creases (i.e., during the breeding season), beginning of the study, flowered in October, and and extensive overlapping of home-range matured by November. The crop was harvested in December, leaving stubble about 0.4 m in areas at high densities. height. This stubble was grazed by sheep for the Only 1 study has attempted to define the rest of the study. To the west of the fenceline social organization of house mice in agri- was pasture. A 0.3-ha patch of sparse Eucalyp- cultural landscapes in Australia using radio- tus (mainly E. largiflorens, E. camaldulensis, E. telemetry (Krebs et al. 1995b). That study aggregata, and E. viminalis) woodland with tall found that during the breeding season most wheatgrass (Agropyron elongatum) understory individuals demonstrated site fidelity with was present on a seepage area within the crop. extensive overlap of home ranges. Breeding Eucalyptus displayed the distinctive multi- males had larger home ranges and were stemmed growth form typical of vegetation in more active than breeding females. After this region (Parkes and Cheal 1990). breeding ceased, home ranges increased Collection of demographic data.—Mice were more than 10-fold, and most mice became live-trapped for 2–4 nights before each tracking nomadic. session using Longworth single-capture traps. Traps were set 10 m apart in grass strips along- We examined home-range sizes and the side the channel and fencelines and around the extent and degree of home-range overlap of woodland. A 5 by 5 grid was set in the wood- house mice in a cereal-growing region in land and a 7 by 7 grid was set in the barley crop. northwestern Victoria, Australia, during in- Trapping effort ranged from 287 trap nights to creasing mouse abundance. We tested null 725 trap nights each session depending on hypotheses that for breeding and nonbreed- mouse abundance. Each mouse was individually ing seasons of mice no difference existed marked using a Hauptner brass ear tag (Sieper in the extent or degree of home-range over- & Co., Sydney, Australia), weighed (60.1 g), lap, no difference existed in home-range and measured (head–body length, 61.0 mm), size, and no difference existed in the pro- and reproductive condition was assessed for fe- portion of lost signals. We also examined males (Singleton 1983). Individual body condi- body size and body condition in males and tion was calculated from the regression equation of Krebs and Singleton (1993), discounting females and female breeding condition as pregnant females. Breeding season was con- predictors of home-range size. firmed from autopsies of females trapped on nearby sites each month. MATERIALS AND METHODS Barrier fence.—In September–December, a Study site.—Our study was carried out on a barrier fence was constructed around the wood- farm 8 km S of Walpeup in northwestern Vic- land and grass areas where mice were trapped toria (618079S, 1428019E) from September 1996 before tracking. This fence was built to maxi- to May 1997. The crop regime, soil type, and mize captures and attempt a total enumeration
February 2000 CHAMBERS ET AL.—SOCIAL ORGANIZATION OF MUS 61 of the population in those habitats, a requirement tralia) receivers. More than 80% of animals for analysis of range overlap between individu- could be tracked to #1 m of their actual loca- als. The fence was constructed of 200 mm by tion. Signals that could not be detected were 0.9 m by 50 m lengths of plastic dug in at the searched for on foot within #1-km radius of the base to a depth of 0.10 m. The plastic was sup- last known location. ported by medium tensile, Flexibely fencing Analyses.—Home-range analyses were per- wire (Hall Rural Centre, Canberra, Australia) formed using RANGES V (Kenward and Hod- held at a height of about 0.7 m by fencing posts der 1996). Krebs et al. (1995b) identified poly- Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jmammal/article/81/1/59/2372948 by 59662000 user on 12 February 2021 spaced at 5-m intervals. The top 0.05–0.10 m of gon models and nonparametric utilization–dis- plastic was folded over the top of the wire and tribution models as the most appropriate for ex- secured with packing tape. When trapping was amining movements of wild mice. We calculated completed and tracking of mice was in progress, mononuclear convex polygons covering 100% the plastic was folded away from the wire fenc- and 95% of the observed points and the Kernel ing and covered with soil. When mouse numbers estimator for 100% and 95% of the observed were high in January–May 1997, the barrier data (Worton 1989). Home-range sizes using fence was not used because enumeration of the those methods were compared using Spearman’s population was not possible because of a high rank correlation analysis and were highly cor- rate of trap occupancy by mice. related (all rs 5 0.81–1.00, n 5 139, P , 0.001 Radiotelemetry.—We used single-stage trans- for all coefficients). Therefore, we used the sim- mitters (Sirtrack, Havelock North, New Zealand) pler 95% convex polygon home ranges for sub- attached to a cable tie that functioned as a ra- sequent analyses. diocollar around the neck of the mouse. Only Home-range overlap was calculated during adult mice .75 mm or .13 g were tracked. The the breeding and nonbreeding seasons for males weight of a transmitter (1.2–1.4 g) was about 8– and females and was presented as the percentage 9% of the average body weight of mice with of overlapping ranges from the number of po- collars (X 5 15.6 g). Battery life of transmitters tential overlaps. Potential overlaps were the was 12–15 days, restricting each tracking ses- number of possible combinations of overlapping sion to 5–10 days. individuals (e.g., if 5 individuals were tracked, Between 13 and 29 animals were radiocol- there were 20 potential overlaps). Degree of lared (average 24) in each of 8 sessions between overlap (as a percentage of the total home-range September 1996 and May 1997. We attempted size) was calculated for overlapping males dur- to fit transmitters to an equal number of males ing the breeding season. Regression analysis and females during each session, but this was was used to determine if home-range size could not achieved in September, January, April, and be predicted by body size or body condition of May because few large adult females were mice. caught. When animals of suitable size were in Population abundance was estimated as the excess of the number of collars available, those number of mice caught per 100 trap nights, ad- caught along the grass areas and within the justed using Caughley’s (1977) density–frequen- woodland were given priority over those caught cy transformation. The proportion of females in the cropland. This was done to minimize breeding was based on the number of females tracking of transient mice and measure home lactating or pregnant (determined by palpation) ranges of residents that had burrow sites in the divided by the number of adult females (.72 grass and woodland habitats. Eight animals were mm). Two-way analysis of variance was used to tracked on more than 1 occasion: 6 of them test for effects of sex (male versus female) and twice and 1 each for 3 and 4 times, respectively. breeding season (breeding versus nonbreeding) Potentially 4 radiolocations were obtained on home-range size (95% polygons). each day, 3 each night between 1800 and 0100 h and once during the day. All fixes were sep- RESULTS arated by at least 1 h. All animals were tracked on foot using a handheld 3-element Yagi antenna Based on autopsy data, mice began and TR4 (Sirtrack, Havelock North, New Zea- breeding in August and weaned the last lit- land), TR2 (Biotrack, Dorset, United Kingdom), ter for the season around mid-April. In ear- or Regal 2000 (Titley Electronics, Ballina, Aus- ly September, 14 of 25 adult females were
62 JOURNAL OF MAMMALOGY Vol. 81, No. 1 grass and woodland, 63–100% were fitted with transmitters at any 1 session. Of 187 mice fitted with transmitters, 159 (85%) were tracked throughout their track- ing session; however, 139 of those were used in analyses of home range. Twenty mice died before a sufficient number of fix- Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jmammal/article/81/1/59/2372948 by 59662000 user on 12 February 2021 es could be obtained based on an incremen- tal area plot of number of fixes versus the cumulative home-range area. That plot in- dicated that about 60% of the home range was defined after 12 fixes, and that had in- creased to about 90% by 22 fixes. We in- cluded only mice with .12 locations (n 5 FIG. 1.—Percent adjusted trap success (line) 139); most of those (78% of the 139 used and percentage of adult female house mice preg- in the analyses) had $20 locations defining nant or lactating (bars) in the wheatlands of their home range. Movements and trap sites northwestern Victoria, Australia. Numbers of the other 48 mice, and their proximity to above each bar are sample sizes. mice with home ranges used in the formal analyses, were noted. For 40 mice whose location was un- pregnant (5 were 3rd trimester), and 12 of known at the completion of the tracking 25 had recent placental scars and were lac- session, 22 (55%) mice dropped their trans- tating. In the last week of February, 8 of 14 mitter and 18 (45%) mice had undetectable adult females were pregnant (4 in 1st tri- radio signals. Twelve of the 40 mice had mester), and by early April, no mice were their home-range area estimated, because pregnant (n 5 20). .12 fixes were obtained before the animal Results of livetrapping indicated that was lost. However, home ranges for those breeding activity peaked in October 1996 mice may have been underestimated. Per- when 80% of females caught were either centage of signals lost per tracking session lactating or pregnant (Fig. 1). From Sep- was highest in October (23%), April (14%), tember to February, 40–60% of females and May (17%), but otherwise was gener- were breeding. In April, only 7% of females ally low (4–11%). When the signal was caught were breeding (all lactating), and lost, it was not known whether the animal therefore all males had ceased mating. had left the area or the radio had failed. Of Thus, our study had 7 tracking sessions dur- 25 mice (13%) that died during tracking, 7 ing the breeding season (September–April) deaths were known to occur from predation and 1 during the nonbreeding season by snakes or raptors. Five of the 25 mice (May). were still included in the analyses because Mouse abundance increased from 8% .12 fixes were obtained. trap success to 69% trap success from Sep- Frequency of home-range sizes was high- tember 1996 to May 1997 (Fig. 1). That ly skewed (Fig. 2), and varied from 0.0002 was equivalent to about 15–420 mice/ha to 8.024 ha, with a mean of 0.271 ha and (G. Singleton, in litt.). When the plastic bar- a median of 0.04 ha. Using log-transformed rier fences were in place, 66% (range, 50– estimates, home-range size did not differ 87%) of captures were recaptures from the between males and females (F 5 3.465, d.f. previous night(s). Without the fence, num- 5 1, 135, P 5 0.065). However, the P value ber of recaptures dropped to 16% (range, was close to the 0.05 level of significance, 10–29%). Of the large mice caught in the suggesting that male home ranges were
February 2000 CHAMBERS ET AL.—SOCIAL ORGANIZATION OF MUS 63 overlaps), but overlap generally was small (0–10% of the home range; Fig. 2). On av- erage, when home ranges of males over- lapped, 23% of the area was shared. For each tracking session, percentage of home ranges overlapping ranged from 0% to 25% (September, December, January, and May; Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jmammal/article/81/1/59/2372948 by 59662000 user on 12 February 2021 Fig. 3). During the nonbreeding season when mouse densities were at their peak for this study, the percentage of male home ranges overlapping increased to 57% (based on 132 potential overlaps; Fig. 3), but the degree of overlap remained similar to that observed during the breeding season. For females, 13% had overlapping home ranges during the breeding season (based on 262 potential overlaps), and no overlap was observed for females during the non- breeding season (based on 20 potential overlaps). The degree of overlap between females was generally less than that ob- served for males (65% overlapped by ,10%), and only 1 instance of 90–100% overlap was found between individual FIG. 2.—Frequency plots of a) home-range home ranges. For each tracking session, sizes (95% convex polygons) for male and fe- male house mice (n 5 139) and b) degree of percentage of home ranges overlapping overlap (as a percentage of home-range area) be- ranged from 0% to 27% (September, De- tween adult male house mice with overlapping cember, January, and May; Fig. 3). In all home ranges during the breeding season (n 5 months, home ranges of males and females 159 overlaps). overlapped (Fig. 3). Eight animals were tracked more than once (2– 4 sessions). Each animal was tracked in a similar location at each track- slightly larger than those of females. Home ing session, with 21–57% of their home ranges were significantly larger for mice ranges from different trapping sessions tracked during the nonbreeding season overlapping. No consistent trend of increas- compared with the breeding season (F 5 ing or decreasing home-range size was 4.018, d.f. 5 1, 135, P 5 0.047). No dif- found between consecutive sessions. ferences occurred among home ranges of Animals fitted with radiotransmitters, but lactating, pregnant, or nonbreeding females not used in the analyses of home-range area (F 5 0.099, d.f. 5 2, 45, P 5 0.91). Further, and overlap, supported trends observed in home-range size did not increase with in- degree of home-range overlaps. Numbers of creasing body size (r2 5 0.004, n 5 137, P animals in that category for each month 5 0.483) or body condition (r2 5 0.002, n from September to May were 5, 2, 4, 5, 1, 5 130, P 5 0.620) for males and females 6, 13, and 12, respectively (total 5 48). The combined. few movements or initial trap locations for During the breeding season, 18% of those individuals showed exclusive intra- males had home ranges that overlapped sexual spatial use. Six individuals from the with other males (based on 906 potential April and May tracking sessions showed
64 JOURNAL OF MAMMALOGY Vol. 81, No. 1 Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jmammal/article/81/1/59/2372948 by 59662000 user on 12 February 2021 FIG. 3.—Home-range plots (95% convex polygon areas) for all house mice radiotracked in a–c) 3 months during the breeding season, and d) 1 month during the nonbreeding season. During the a) September and b) December tracking sessions, a barrier fence was used to enumerate the population before tracking but was not used in c) January or d) May. Percent overlap is the number of ranges overlapping divided by the number of potential overlaps multiplied by 100. Low Density, Increasing, and High Density refer to abundance of mice at the site at each tracking session. long-range movements of 300–1,000 m, DISCUSSION with an additional 12 individuals either dropping their transmitter or with locations Home-range areas for house mice unknown because of loss of signal. throughout this 9-month study were consis-
February 2000 CHAMBERS ET AL.—SOCIAL ORGANIZATION OF MUS 65 TABLE 1.—Summary of mouse home-range data in northwestern Victoria, Australia (present study) and on the Darling Downs, Queensland (Krebs et al. 1994, 1995b). Sites were about 1,500 km apart and had markedly different farming systems. Pe 5 Petersen estimate of number of mice per hectare; TS 5 trap success per 100 trap nights adjusted using Caughley’s density–frequency transformation (Caughley 1977). Darling Downs Northwestern Victoria Home range data Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jmammal/article/81/1/59/2372948 by 59662000 user on 12 February 2021 Males versus females Males . females Males $ females Frequency distribution Highly positively skewed Highly positively skewed Median home-range size Breeding 0.014 ha (n 5 94) 0.037 ha (n 5 122) Nonbreeding 0.199 ha (n 5 13) 0.119 ha (n 5 17) Range overlap Extensive intra- and intersexual overlap Extensive intersexual overlap; variable intrasexual overlap Breeding season versus 10 times increase in home-range size 3 times increase in home-range size nonbreeding season during nonbreeding season compared during nonbreeding compared with with breeding season breeding season Breeding condition No effect on home-range size No effect on home-range size Potential nomadism Breeding season 5 30% nomadic; non- Breeding season 5 8% nomadic; non- breeding season 5 66% nomadic breeding 5 17% nomadic Density dependent No; range of mouse densities 5 10– No; range of mouse densities 5 15– 500 (Pe), or 25–325 (TS) 260 (Pe), or 10–70 (TS) Farming system Winter and summer crops; heavy clay Winter crops only; light sandy soils; soils; predominantly summer rainfall; predominantly winter rainfall; fences no fences tent with those obtained by Krebs et al. ranges occurred consistently throughout the (1995b) in mouse populations on a cereal study. farm on the Darling Downs in southeastern To examine the extent and degree of Queensland (about 1,500 km from Wal- overlap between home-range areas, and in peup; Table 1). Home ranges were ,0.1 ha particular to detect if animals are territorial, for 68% of mice and ,0.01 ha for 21% of requires that a significant proportion of the mice. Similar home-range areas also have population is tracked during each tracking been measured during a radiotelemetry session. In our study, we attempted to cap- study of Mus spretus in grassland in Por- ture all individuals in the study area when tugal (Gray et al. 1998). population density was low by using a plas- For males, intrasexual home-range over- tic barrier fence during trapping to contain lap varied between breeding and nonbreed- the population. The proportion of recap- ing seasons, and, therefore, between in- tures to new captures on the last night of creasing and high-density populations (Fig. each trapping session indicated that we 1). Percentage of home ranges overlapping were close to achieving this. When barrier was low to moderate in the breeding sea- fences were in place (September–Decem- son. In the nonbreeding season, males ber), 66% of captures were recaptures and showed an apparent switch to a more gre- 63–100% of mice weighing .13 g were fit- garious phase, with 57% of ranges overlap- ted with transmitters. Therefore, the low ping. No such difference was found for fe- percentage (average 14% for males and fe- male mice: high exclusive use of home males) of intrasexual home-range overlap
66 JOURNAL OF MAMMALOGY Vol. 81, No. 1 from September to December, the period [1995b] had only 1 tracking session during when a large proportion of the adult popu- the nonbreeding season), or the influence lation was caught and tracked, suggests that that social behavior has on the dispersal of mice may have had a territorial social or- young. For example, will territorial behav- ganization at this time. ior result in presaturation dispersal of Without the fence, from January onward, young mice (Lidicker 1975) and therefore number of recaptures dropped to 16%, sug- play an important role in how mice use the Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jmammal/article/81/1/59/2372948 by 59662000 user on 12 February 2021 gesting that the proportion of the population dynamic mosaic of agricultural ecosystems? trapped was low and that our estimates of Determining the underlying social struc- home-range overlap were conservative. The ture of mouse populations in agricultural location of individuals for which we had landscapes is crucial to developing and im- ,12 locations supported our conclusions of plementing an effective management strat- exclusive intrasexual home-range use dur- egy. For example, virally vectored immu- ing the breeding season and long-range nocontraception has been identified as a movements during the nonbreeding season. promising control technique for house mice These results support the suggestion that in Australia (Chambers et al. 1997; Shellam male mice have high behavioral plasticity, 1994; Singleton 1994). Social organization ranging from strongly territorial behavior to will influence the rate of spread of a virally gregarious social groups (Redhead 1982). vectored sterilizing agent through the direct However, our results contrast with studies inter- and intrasexual interactions between that indicate that mice maintain territorial males and females and indirect effects on behavior for most of the year (Anderson dispersion or retention of young mice at 1961; Fitzgerald et al. 1981; Selander their natal site (Lambin 1994; Lambin and 1970). The potential for mice to form gre- Krebs 1991; Lambin and Yoccoz 1998; garious units is consistent with the high Wolff 1992). Although the former may be densities observed during mouse plagues in examined using radiotelemetry, the latter is the grain-growing regions of Australia (Sin- difficult to quantify under field conditions. gleton and Redhead 1990). Use of radioisotopes to follow movements Our study supports predictions of the of neonates from their mother’s home range model proposed by Krebs et al. (1995a) for is a possible technique (Tamarin et al. mice in Australian agricultural landscapes. 1983), but may not be permitted in crop- Evidence was found of nomadism at low land. density (23% of signals were lost in Octo- One problem when working with an out- ber) and after the breeding season ceased in breaking species such as house mice is that May (17% of signals lost). Home-range temporal replication in a study such as this overlap for males also was low to moderate is difficult. We probably would have to wait during the breeding season (18%) when the a further 5–7 years for similar population population was increasing, but overlap rose dynamics, given that this is the average to 57% after the population ceased breeding time between outbreaks in southern Austra- and densities were high. This contrasts with lia (Singleton and Redhead 1989). During the Darling Downs study (Krebs et al. most of 1997 and 1998 after our study was 1994) where mice were relatively nomadic, completed, capture rates of ,5 mice/1,000 regardless of density and breeding season trap nights were the norm, with traps dis- (Table 1). Such results emphasize the need tributed over a much larger area than used to examine social structure of mice in field in our study (G. R. Singleton, in litt.). This populations at a finer scale (Gray et al. is typical of mouse densities in the interim 1998). In particular, little is known of social between outbreaks (Pech et al. 1999). organization during the nonbreeding season Therefore, to replicate a study such as this (both our study and that by Krebs et al. temporally is not a trivial exercise. We sug-
February 2000 CHAMBERS ET AL.—SOCIAL ORGANIZATION OF MUS 67 gest that a further radiotelemetry study at a ulations, a detailed radiotelemetry study is similar stage of house-mouse population required for a better understanding of the dynamics would be beneficial to test the social organization of mice in cereal-grow- generality of our results. ing regions of eastern Australia. A similar drawback is the difficulty of spatial replication. Given the considerable ACKNOWLEDGMENTS resources required to radiotrack a signifi- This study would not have been possible Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jmammal/article/81/1/59/2372948 by 59662000 user on 12 February 2021 cant proportion of the population at 1 site, without the generous and dedicated assistance of as we have attempted here, it was not pos- technical staff and volunteers of the Common- sible for us to examine movements of mice wealth Scientific and Industrial Research Orga- at more than 1 site. However, the similarity nisation, who spent many hours trapping and ra- in results obtained by this study and the ear- diotracking mice in the field. In particular, we lier study by Krebs et al. (1995b), about would like to thank A. Lewis, M. Davies, D. Jones, J. Stanger, R. Perry, K. Lewis, S. Ogborn, 1,500 km away, suggests that results may K. Cheney, and J. Winsbury. We also thank J. be generally applicable to cereal-growing and J. Symes for allowing us to track mice on regions of southern and eastern Australia. their farm and G. Stratford and his staff at the Further studies are encouraged to test this Mallee Research Station, Walpeup, for logistical hypothesis. support. D. Saunders, A. Newsome, and 2 anon- With radiotelemetry studies, the risk al- ymous referees provided helpful comments on ways exists that the presence of the radio an earlier draft of this manuscript. This project will somehow hinder movements of the an- was supported by the National Science and En- imals and negatively bias home-range sizes. gineering Research Council, Canada, and the In the present study, we tracked 8 individ- Grains Research and Development Corporation uals on more than 1 occasion. In all cases, (project CSV7), Australia, and was conducted in accordance with the Australian code of practice mice were retrapped in the areas where they for the care and use of animals for scientific pur- had been originally radiotracked, suggest- poses. ing that movements observed while animals were carrying transmitters were represen- LITERATURE CITED tative of their true home range. This is con- ANDERSON, P. K. 1961. Density, social structure, and sistent with laboratory and field trials of ef- nonsocial environment in house-mouse populations fects of transmitters on wild mice, which and the implications for the regulation of numbers. have indicated that movements, survival, Transactions of the New York Academy of Sciences, Series II 23:447–451. and dominance status are unaffected by a BROWN, P. R., G. R. SINGLETON, B. KEARNS, AND J. transmitter (Pouliquen et al. 1990). GRIFFITHS. 1997. Evaluation and cost-effectiveness Our study in the wheatlands of north- of strychnine for control of populations of wild house mice (Mus domesticus) in Victoria. Wildlife western Victoria provided evidence that Research 24:159–172. mice tend toward exclusive intrasexual BUTLER, R. G. 1980. Population size, social behaviour, home ranges when mouse populations are and dispersal in house mice: a quantitative investi- gation. Animal Behaviour 28:78–85. breeding and at low density, particularly for CAUGHLEY, G. 1977. Analysis of vertebrate popula- females. Males become gregarious and tions. John Wiley & Sons, London, United King- home ranges increase in size as mouse den- dom. CHAMBERS, L. K., G. R. SINGLETON, AND G. M. HOOD. sities increase and the breeding season 1997. Immunocontraception as a potential control ends. The high proportion of lost radio sig- method of wild rodent populations. Belgian Journal nals in April and May also suggests that of Zoology 127:145–156. CROWCROFT, P., AND F. P. ROWE. 1963. Social organi- mice may have more nomadic movement sation and territorial behaviour in the wild house patterns during the start of the nonbreeding mouse (Mus musculus L.). Proceedings of the Zoo- season. Because of the paucity of informa- logical Society of London 140:517–531. FITZGERALD, B. M., B. J. KARL, AND H. MOLLER. 1981. tion obtained during the nonbreeding sea- Spatial organisation and ecology of a sparse popu- son on mouse social behavior in field pop- lation of house mice (Mus musculus) in a New Zea-
68 JOURNAL OF MAMMALOGY Vol. 81, No. 1 land forest. The Journal of Animal Ecology 50:489– tioning. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 10: 518. 157–163. GRAY, S. J, J. L. HURST, R. STIDWORTHY, J. SMITH, R. PARKES, D. M., AND D. C. CHEAL. 1990. Perceptions PRESTON, AND R. MACDOUGALL. 1998. Microhabitat of mallee vegetation. Pp. 3–7 in The mallee lands: and spatial dispersion of the grassland mouse (Mus a conservation perspective (J. C. Noble, P. J. Joss, spretus Lataste). Journal of Zoology (London) 246: and G. K. Jones, eds.). Commonwealth Scientific 299–308. and Industrial Research Organisation, Melbourne, KAY, B. J., L. E. TWIGG, AND H. I. NICHOL. 1994. The Australia. strategic use of rodenticides against house mice PECH, R. P., G. M. HOOD, G. R. SINGLETON, E. SALMON, (Mus domesticus) prior to crop invasion. Wildlife R. I. FORRESTER, AND P. R. BROWN. 1999. Models for Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jmammal/article/81/1/59/2372948 by 59662000 user on 12 February 2021 Research 21:11–19. predicting plagues of house mice (Mus domesticus) KENWARD, R. E., AND K. H. HODDER. 1996. RANGES in Australia. Pp. 81–112 in Ecologically-based ro- V: an analysis system for biological location data. dent management (G. R. Singleton, L. A. Hinds, H. Institute of Terrestrial Ecology, Dorset, United King- Leirs, and Z. Zhang, eds.). Australian Centre for In- dom. ternational Agricultural Research, Canberra, Austra- KREBS, C. J., D. CHITTY, G. SINGLETON, AND R. BOON- lia. STRA. 1995a. Can changes in social behaviour help POULIQUEN, O., M. LEISHMAN, AND T. D. REDHEAD. to explain house mouse plagues in Australia? Oikos 1990. Effects of radio collars on wild mice, Mus 73:429–434. domesticus. Canadian Journal of Zoology 68:1607– KREBS, C. J., A. J. KENNEY, AND G. R. SINGLETON. 1609. 1995b. Movements of feral house mice in agricul- REDHEAD, T. D. 1982. Reproduction, growth and pop- tural landscapes. Australian Journal of Zoology 43: ulation dynamics of house mice in irrigated and non- 293–302. irrigated cereal farms in New South Wales. Ph.D. KREBS, C. J., AND G. R. SINGLETON. 1993. Indices of dissertation, Australian National University, Canber- ra, Australia. condition for small mammals. Australian Journal of SELANDER, R. K. 1970. Behavior and genetic variation Zoology 41:317–323. in natural populations. American Zoologist 10:53– KREBS, C. J., G. R. SINGLETON, AND A. J. KENNEY. 66. 1994. Six reasons why feral house mouse popula- SHELLAM, G. R. 1994. The potential of murine cyto- tions might have low recapture rates. Wildlife Re- megalovirus as a viral vector for immunocontracep- search 21:559–567. tion. Reproduction, Fertility and Development 6: LAMBIN, X. 1994. Territory acquisition and social fa- 401–409. cilitation by litter-mate Townsend’s voles (Microtus SINGLETON, G. R. 1983. The social and genetic struc- townsendii). Ethology Ecology & Evolution 6:213– ture of a natural colony of house mice, Mus mus- 220. culus, at Healesville Wildlife Sanctuary. Australian LAMBIN, X., AND C. J. KREBS. 1991. Can changes in Journal of Zoology 31:155–166. female relatedness influence microtine population SINGLETON, G. R. 1994. The prospects and associated dynamics? Oikos 61:126–132. challenges for the biological control of rodents. Pp. LAMBIN, X., AND N. YOCCOZ. 1998. The impact of pop- 301–307 in Proceedings of the 16th Vertebrate Pest ulation kin-structure on nestling survival in Town- Conference (W. S. Halverson and A. C. Crabb, eds.). send’s vole, Microtus townsendii. The Journal of University of California, Davis. Animal Ecology 67:1–16. SINGLETON, G. R. 1997. Integrated management of ro- LIDICKER, W. Z., JR. 1975. The role of dispersal in the dents: a southeast Asian and Australian perspective. demography of small mammals. Pp. 103–128 in Belgian Journal of Zoology 127:157–169. Small mammals: their production and population dy- SINGLETON, G. R., AND L. K. CHAMBERS. 1996. A ma- namics (K. Petrusewicz, F. B. Golley, and L. Rysz- nipulative field experiment to examine the effect of kowski, eds.). Cambridge University Press, London, Capillaria hepatica (Nematoda) on wild mouse pop- United Kingdom. ulations in southern Australia. International Journal LIDICKER, W. Z., JR. 1976. Social behaviour and den- for Parasitology 26:383–398. sity regulation in house mice living in large enclo- SINGLETON, G. R., AND D. A. HAY. 1983. The effect of sures. The Journal of Animal Ecology 45:677–697. social organization on reproductive success and gene MUTZE, G. J. 1993. Cost-effectiveness of poison bait flow in colonies of wild house mice, Mus musculus. trials for control of house mice in mallee cereal Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 12:49–56. crops. Wildlife Research 20:445–456. SINGLETON, G. R., AND T. D. REDHEAD. 1989. House NEWSOME, A. E. 1969a. A population study of house- mouse plagues. Pp. 418–433 in Mediterranean land- mice temporarily inhabiting a South Australian scapes in Australia: mallee ecosystems and their wheatfield. The Journal of Animal Ecology 38:341– management (J. C. Noble and R. A. Bradstock, eds.). 359. Commonwealth Scientific and Industrial Research NEWSOME, A. E. 1969b. A population study of house- Organisation, Melbourne, Australia. mice permanently inhabiting a reed-bed in South SINGLETON, G. R., AND T. D. REDHEAD. 1990. Structure Australia. The Journal of Animal Ecology 38:361– and biology of house mouse populations that plague 377. irregularly: an evolutionary perspective. Biological NOYES, R. F., G. W. BARRETT, AND D. H. TAYLOR. Journal of the Linnean Society 41:285–300. 1982. Social structure of feral house mouse (Mus TAMARIN, R. H., M. SHERIDAN, AND C. K. LEVY. 1983. musculus L.) populations: effects of resource parti- Determining matrilineal kinship in natural popula-
February 2000 CHAMBERS ET AL.—SOCIAL ORGANIZATION OF MUS 69 tions of rodents using radionuclides. Canadian Jour- choice: their relation to social structure in wild nal of Zoology 61:271–274. caught house mice (Mus musculus) housed in a TWIGG, L. E., G. R. SINGLETON, AND B. J. KAY. 1991. semi-natural environment. Journal of Zoology (Lon- Evaluation of bromadiolone against house mouse don) 207:43–51. (Mus domesticus) populations in irrigated soybean WORTON, B. J. 1989. Kernel methods for estimating crops. I. Efficacy of control. Wildlife Research 18: the utilization distribution in home-range studies. 265–274. Ecology 70:164–168. WOLFF, J. O. 1992. Parents suppress reproduction and Submitted 15 September 1998. Accepted 22 April stimulate dispersal in opposite-sex juvenile white- 1999. Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jmammal/article/81/1/59/2372948 by 59662000 user on 12 February 2021 footed mice. Nature 358:409–410. WOLFF, R. J. 1985. Mating behaviour and female Associate Editor was Robert L. Lochmiller.
You can also read