Mouse Models of Autism Spectrum Disorders: The Challenge for Behavioral Genetics
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
American Journal of Medical Genetics Part C (Semin. Med. Genet.) 142C:40 –51 (2006) A R T I C L E Mouse Models of Autism Spectrum Disorders: The Challenge for Behavioral Genetics SHERYL S. MOY,* JESSICA J. NADLER, TERRY R. MAGNUSON, AND JACQUELINE N. CRAWLEY Autism is a severe neurodevelopmental disorder, which typically emerges early in childhood. The core symptoms of autism include deficits in social interaction, impaired communication, and aberrant repetitive behavior, including self-injury. Despite the strong genetic component for the disease, most cases of autism have not been linked to mutations in a specific gene, and the etiology of the disorder has yet to be established. At the present time, there is no generally accepted therapeutic strategy to treat the core symptoms of autism, and there remains a critical need for appropriate animal models and relevant behavioral assays to promote the understanding and treatment of the clinical syndrome. Challenges for the development of valid mouse models include complex genetic interactions underlying the high heritability of the disease in humans, diagnosis based on deficits in social interaction and communication, and the lack of confirmatory neuropathological markers to provide validation for genetic models of the disorder. Research focusing on genes that mediate social behavior in mice may help identify neural circuitry essential for normal social interaction, and lead to novel genetic animal models of the autism behavioral phenotype. ß 2006 Wiley-Liss, Inc. KEY WORDS: autism; fragile X; mice; repetitive behavior; Rett syndrome; social interaction INTRODUCTION challenging. For example, the diagnostic AUTISM CLINICAL criteria for autism are based on social DISORDER Advances in the fields of genetics and and communication deficits, rather than a molecular biology have led to the general neurological pathology or neu- Diagnostic and Neuropathological creation of mouse models with genetic rochemical marker that could be model- Features aberrations characteristic of human clin- ed in an animal. In addition, the ASDs are Diagnosis of autism is based on beha- ical disorders. These models can be characterized by marked genetic and vioral symptomatology. The defining tremendously beneficial for determining phenotypic heterogeneity, suggesting characteristics include attenuated or disease etiology, effects on organic and that single-gene mouse mutants may not abnormal social interaction and com- cellular function, and therapeutic efficacy provide a model for the intrinsic com- munication, as well as aberrant repetitive of novel treatment strategies. However, plexity of these diseases. This review behavior, with symptoms emerging the development of mouse models for describes current genetic mouse models early in childhood [American Psychia- the autism spectrum disorders (ASDs), for the ASDs, and discusses methodolog- tric Association, 1994]. In addition to including autism, Asperger disorder, and ical and theoretical issues in their beha- significant language deficits, autistic Rett syndrome, has proven especially vioral phenotyping and use. children demonstrate rigid adherence to routines and restricted interests, often forming obsessional preoccupations with Sheryl S. Moy, Ph.D., is the Associate Director of the Mouse Behavioral Phenotyping Laboratory specific objects or topics. The autism of the Neurodevelopmental Disorders Research Center, and an Associate Professor in the phenotype also includes unusual motor- Department of Psychiatry at UNC. Dr. Moy’s work focuses on the development of mouse models relevant to human clinical disorders, including autism and schizophrenia. ic responses, such as tic-like stereotypies Jessica J. Nadler, Ph.D., is a Postdoctoral Fellow in the Department of Genetics at UNC. and self-injury [Bodfish et al., 2000], Dr. Nadler works on identifying the underlying gene expression changes associated with deficits impaired motor function, and abnormal in social and cognitive behaviors. Terry R. Magnuson, Ph.D., is the Chair of the UNC Department of Genetics and Director of the responses to sensory stimuli [Baranek, Carolina Center for Genome Sciences. 2002]. Mental retardation is observed in Jacqueline N. Crawley, Ph.D., is Director of the Laboratory of Behavioral Neuroscience at NIMH the majority of children diagnosed with and the Mouse Behavioral Phenotyping Laboratory of the Neurodevelopmental Disorders Research Center at UNC. Modeling neuropsychiatric disorders using mouse behavioral genetics autism, and seizures are present in an represents a theme of Dr. Crawley’s research program. estimated 30% of the autistic population Grant sponsor: STAART; Grant number: U54 MH66418; Grant sponsor: MRDDRC; Grant [Gillberg and Billstedt, 2000]. Other number: P30 HD03110; Grant sponsor: NIMH Intramural Research Program. *Correspondence to: Sheryl S. Moy, Neurodevelopmental Disorders Research Center, CB no. symptoms reported in autism include 7146, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC 27599-7146. E-mail: ssmoy@med.unc.edu anxiety [Muris et al., 1998], and sleep DOI 10.1002/ajmg.c.30081 disorders [Gillberg and Billstedt, 2000]. ß 2006 Wiley-Liss, Inc.
ARTICLE AMERICAN JOURNAL OF MEDICAL GENETICS PART C (SEMIN. MED. GENET.): DOI 10.1002/ajmg.c 41 Investigators have reported that polymorphisms for the disease conferr- protein. Mouse models based on tar- autistic subjects may have various altera- ing susceptibility in a non-deterministic geted disruption of genes that may be tions in brain size and neuroanatomy. way [Folstein and Rosen-Sheidley, involved in autism susceptibility include These differences include increased head 2001; Keller and Persico, 2003]. the Fmr1-null mouse, a model for fragile size and brain volume [Piven et al., 1996; X syndrome [Bakker et al., 1994], Bailey et al., 1998; Courchesne et al., Gabrb3- and Ube3a-null mice, models MOUSE MODELS FOR 2001], decreased volume of hippocam- for Angelman syndrome [DeLorey et al., AUTISM SPECTRUM pus, amygdala, and corpus callosum 1998; Jiang et al., 1998], the DHCR7- DISORDERS [Piven et al., 1997; Aylward et al., null mouse, a model for Smith–Lemli– 1999; Saitoh et al., 2001], and decreased Optiz syndrome [Fitzky et al., 2001], Candidate Gene Approach numbers of Purkinje cells in cerebellum and the Dvl1-null mouse [Lijam et al., [Bailey et al., 1998; Palmen et al., 2004]. With the advent of embryonic stem cell 1997] (Table I). Neuroimaging assessment of regional technology, mouse geneticists are able to While a targeted disruption can pro- brain function has demonstrated defic- generate mouse models carrying the vide clues on the role of the protein in ient or abnormal activity in frontal and targeted disruption or mutation of normal brain development and func- temporal cortex [Happé et al., 1996; specific candidate genes. However, in tion, a serious drawback of this approach Schultz et al., 2000]. Unfortunately, the case of the ASDs, determination of is that the null allele may not be many of the findings on neuropatholo- which genes to target has proven consistent with the allele present in the gical changes in autism have not proven problematic. Research in human popu- ASD population. A second approach for to be consistent across studies, or have lations suggests that the number of loci creating relevant animal models is to unknown functional ramifications, such associated with autism exceeds 15 engineer an allele known to be disease- as the behavioral consequences of in- [Risch et al., 1999]. Most mouse models associated in humans. This modification creased brain size. for the ASDs have been based on may be more likely to mimic the manipulations of loci that mediate molecular nature of the disease. How- single-gene human disorders character- ever, there are often several disease- Genetic Component of Autism ized by autistic symptoms, or that have associated alleles for a given disorder, and While the etiology of autism is not yet been identified through association or it can be unclear which allele to known, support for a strong genetic linkage studies in human genetics. Other generate. The mouse models of ASDs component is evident from the 70% to mouse models involve mutations in path- discussed below represent both targeted- 80% concordance between monozygo- ways thought to be altered in autism. disruption and disease-associated types tic twins [Folstein and Rosen-Sheidley, of approaches and it is important to 2001]. Autism occurs about four times understand which type of allele is being more frequently in males than in females, investigated when evaluating the validity although this rate varies within subpop- Most mouse models for the of the model. ulations [Fombonne, 2002; Keller and ASDs have been based on Persico, 2003]. Association studies and manipulations of loci that Mouse Model for familial linkage analyses have been mediate single-gene human Fragile X Syndrome used to identify various candidate genes, including WNT2 [Wassink et al., 2001], disorders characterized by Fragile X syndrome is characterized by GABRB3 [Buxbaum et al., 2002], mental retardation, physical abnormal- UBE3A [Nurmi et al., 2001], and autistic symptoms, or that ities, and, in most cases, autistic-like RELN [Persico et al., 2001]. The autism have been identified through behavior [Hagerman et al., 1986]. The phenotype is also associated with muta- disease was mapped to a single locus on tions in FMR1 (the underlying abnorm- association or linkage studies in the X chromosome, Fragile X Mental ality in fragile X syndrome; [Hagerman human genetics. Other mouse Retardation 1 (FMR1). The most com- et al., 1986]) and MECP2 (the genetic models involve mutations in mon allele found in fragile X patients is basis for Rett disorder; [Shibayama et al., an expansion of a trinucleotide repeat 2004]). However, the percentage of pathways thought to be altered [Kremer et al., 1991]. In a normal FMR1 autism cases that can be linked to a in autism. allele, there are about 40 repeats of a known genetic aberration is only around CCG nucleotide motif. In patients with 10% (e.g., Barton and Volkmar [1998]). fragile X, the number of these repeats is Overall, studies suggest the autism The most common strategy for genetic dramatically increased, leading to the phenotype cannot be attributed to a modification is to generate a null allele, severe reduction of FMR1 protein single gene. Rather, the clinical syn- or targeted disruption, which deletes a (FMRP) expression. It is not known drome results from multiple genes inter- portion of the locus, and generally how the loss of FMRP in brain leads to acting together, with some genetic prevents the production of functional the deficits observed in the disease, but
42 AMERICAN JOURNAL OF MEDICAL GENETICS PART C (SEMIN. MED. GENET.): DOI 10.1002/ajmg.c ARTICLE TABLE I. Genes Linked to Autism and the Relevant Mouse Models Candidate gene Genetic mouse model Genes for neurodevelopmental disorders associated with autism FMR1 (Fragile X syndrome) Fmr1-null mouse MECP2 (Rett syndrome) Mecp2-null mouse, Mecp2308/Y mouse GABRB3, UBE3A (Angelman’s syndrome) Gabrb3-null mouse Ube3a-null mouse NF1 (Neurofibromatosis type 1) Nf1-null, heterozygous, and NF123a/ mouse DHCR7 (Smith–Lemli–Opitz syndrome) Dhcr7-null and heterozygous mouse TSC1, TSC2 (Tuberous sclerosis) Tsc1-null mouse Other genes that may contribute to autism susceptibility WNT2 Wnt2-null mouse Dvl1 (Dishevelled-1)-null mouse EN2 En1 and En2 (Engrailed) null mouse HOXA1, HOXA2 Hoxa1-null mouse SERT (SLC6A4) Sert-null and heterozygous mouse MAOA Maoa-null mouse RELN Reln-null and heterozygous mouse recent work suggests that normal FMRP normal performance is observed when including altered dendritic plays a role in RNA binding and the same mutation occurs on a C57BL/ regulating its translation [Oostra and spine morphology and 6J background [Dobkin et al., 2000; see Chiurazzi, 2001; Darnell et al., 2005]. macroorchidism, similar to also Paradee et al., 1999]. These differ- The Fmr1-null mouse is one of the ences suggest that there are multiple best characterized animal models for changes in fragile X patients. genetic interactions, which can modu- human disorders associated with autism. late complex behavioral phenotypes, even in a defined, single-locus disease. Comery et al., 1997]. The behavioral The Fmr1-null mouse is one phenotype of the fragile X-model mice Mouse Models for Rett Syndrome of the best characterized animal also reflects symptoms associated with the human disorder. The null mice Like fragile X syndrome, Rett syndrome models for human disorders show cognitive deficits, evidenced by maps to the X chromosome. The disease associated with autism. impaired performance on reversal learn- is observed primarily in females, with ing in the Morris water maze [Bakker symptoms including mental retardation, et al., 1994; Kooy et al., 1996], and slowed growth rate, hypoactivity, and While the targeted disruption in this altered sensitivity to acoustic stimuli autistic-like behavior. Mutations in a model is not the same as the common [Chen and Toth, 2001; Nielson et al., single gene, methyl-CpG-binding pro- human allele at the DNA level, the con- 2002; Frankland et al., 2004]. The mice tein-2 (MECP2), account for the major- sequence is likely similar: severe reduction also demonstrate changes in social beha- ity of Rett syndrome cases [Amir et al., or absence of the FMR1 gene product vior, suggestive of enhanced social 1999]. As its name implies, MECP2 (see Yan et al. [2004]). FMRP-deficient anxiety [Spencer et al., 2005]. However, protein binds to methylated CpG islands mice demonstrate cytoarchitectonic and some studies have found normal beha- in genomic DNA. MECP2 also associ- physiological aberrations, including al- vioral responses after disruption of the ates with a protein complex containing tered dendritic spine morphology and Fmr1 gene [Fisch et al., 1999; Peier histone deacetylase 1. Since DNA macroorchidism, similar to changes in et al., 2000; Nielson et al., 2002; Yan methylation and histone deacetylation fragile X patients [Bakker et al., 1994; et al., 2004]. These differences may be are two global methods of transcriptional attributed, in part, to the effect of the repression, the MECP2 protein con- different genetic backgrounds of the nects two major methods of down- FMRP-deficient mice mouse strains used for the Fmr1-null regulating gene expression throughout mice. In particular, the Fmr1-null allele the mammalian genome [Nan et al., demonstrate cytoarchitectonic on a mixed FVB/129 background leads 1998]. Alleles found in the patient and physiological aberrations, to deficits in spatial learning, while population include missense mutations,
ARTICLE AMERICAN JOURNAL OF MEDICAL GENETICS PART C (SEMIN. MED. GENET.): DOI 10.1002/ajmg.c 43 which cause amino acid changes in the mice show increased anxiety-like beha- gous for the Gabrb3 deletion have MECP2 protein [Amir et al., 1999; Wan vior, stereotypic forelimb movements significantly reduced levels of the beta et al., 1999], and nonsense mutations [Shahbazian et al., 2002], and altered 3 subunit in brain, dependent upon both that result in production of a truncated home-cage and social behavior [Moretti the parent of origin and gender, but do protein [Zappella et al., 2001]. Early et al., 2005]. In particular, Mecp2308/Y not show seizures or hyperactivity [Lil- truncation often leads to a classical Rett mice spend less time investigating an jelund et al., 2005]. Ube3a-null mice syndrome phenotype, while later trun- unfamiliar mouse, in comparison to demonstrate poor rotarod performance, cations are associated with the preserved normal mice, suggesting that mutation deficits in spatial learning in a Morris speech variant of the disease [Zappella of the Mecp2 locus can lead to deficits in water maze task and in conditioned fear, et al., 2001]. social interaction [Moretti et al., 2005]. and enhanced susceptibility for seizures, Mouse models of Rett syndrome Interestingly, a recent study has demon- dependent upon maternal transmission based on a null allele of the Mecp2 strated a link between deficient MECP2 of the mutation [Jiang et al., 1998; Miura locus demonstrate a phenotype con- expression in human brain or in mouse et al., 2002]. sistent with the human disease. The models, and expression deficits in Mortality early in development is an mice exhibit normal development until UBE3A and GABRB3, two genes issue for other mouse models relevant about 5 weeks of age, followed by the linked to Angelman syndrome [Samaco to the ASDs. For example, gestational emergence of motor impairment, tre- et al., 2005]. death is found with the Nf1-null mouse mors, hypoactivity, aberrant hind- model for neurofibromatosis type 1 limb clasping, and respiratory symptoms (NF1), an autosomal dominant disorder characterized by cognitive and language Mouse Models for Other deficits, poor motor skills, and tumors of Disorders Associated the peripheral nerves [Silva et al., 1997; Mouse models of Rett syndrome With Autism North, 2000]. However, behavioral based on a null allele of the Angelman syndrome is a severe neuro- testing of heterozygotes has revealed that developmental disorder with clinical Nf1þ/ mice demonstrate deficits in Mecp2 locus demonstrate a features of mental retardation, deficits spatial learning in the Morris water phenotype consistent with the in language, movement disorder, and maze, as well as an enhanced predisposi- human disease. The mice seizures [Williams, 2005]. The disease tion for tumors [Silva et al., 1997; Costa has a complex genetic etiology, invol- et al., 2002]. Further, mice carrying a exhibit normal development ving deletions in chromosome 15q11, an specific allele of Nf1 show deficient until about 5 weeks of age, area containing the GABAA receptor social transmission of food preference, beta 3 subunit (GABRB3) gene and impaired fear conditioning and spatial followed by the emergence of the ubiquitin ligase gene, UBE3A. learning, and retarded acquisition of motor impairment, tremors, GABRB3 was the first gene mapped to motor performance on a rotarod, but hypoactivity, aberrant the Prader-Willi/Angelman syndrome without the increased tumor incidence (PW/AS) region of 15q [Wagstaff et al., [Costa et al., 2001]. hind-limb clasping, and 1991]. Due to imprinting (differential Unfortunately, early mortality has respiratory symptoms. expression of genes based on parent of precluded behavioral phenotyping of origin), the phenotype of these deletions some animal models, including the varies, depending upon which parent model for Smith–Lemli–Opitz/RSH [Chen et al., 2001; Guy et al., 2001]. transmitted the deletion. Transmission syndrome (SLOS), an autosomal reces- Female animals heterozygous for the disequilibrium has been demonstrated sive disorder arising from mutations in targeted disruption have delayed onset of with a marker of GABRB3, as well DHCR7 [Fitzky et al., 1998]. The gene the phenotype and evidence milder as surrounding markers, in autism encodes an enzyme crucial for choles- symptoms. A conditional mutation of [Buxbaum et al., 2002]. A targeted terol biosynthesis, suggesting a possible the locus, where the gene is disrupted disruption of the mouse Gabrb3 locus link between developmental cholesterol only in the central nervous system, reveals a severe phenotype, with high deprivation and autistic spectrum beha- shows a similar phenotype to the mortality in neonatal homozygotes viors. The targeted disruption of Dhcr7 Mecp2-null profile [Chen et al., 2001; [Homanics et al., 1997]. The behavioral in mice leads to severe respiratory failure Guy et al., 2001], indicating that the phenotype of surviving Gabrb3-null and death in newborn homozygotes protein’s role in the disease process can mice includes seizures, hyperactivity, [Fitzky et al., 2001; Wassif et al., 2001; be attributed solely to its function in learning and memory deficits, poor see also Yu et al., 2005]. Similarly, neural tissue, rather than in the periph- motor performance on a rotarod task, the Tsc1-null mouse, a model for ery. Another allele, Mecp2308, is similar and stereotyped behavior, such as repe- tuberous sclerosis 1, is characterized by to the truncation allele found in the titive circling [Homanics et al., 1997; an embryonic lethal phenotype [Wilson human patient population. Mecp2308/Y DeLorey et al., 1998]. Mice heterozy- et al., 2005].
44 AMERICAN JOURNAL OF MEDICAL GENETICS PART C (SEMIN. MED. GENET.): DOI 10.1002/ajmg.c ARTICLE Genes Implicated by Human The HOXA1 locus is located in the including longer immobility Genetic Studies of Autism region of human chromosome 7p14.2- 15. A polymorphism changing a histi- in a forced swim task. Multiple studies in human populations dine residue to an arginine is enriched have provided evidence that chromo- in patients with ASD over both their some 7q contains genes conferring unaffected relatives and the rest of [Holmes et al., 2002, 2003; Lira et al., susceptibility for autism [e.g., Collabo- the population [Ingram et al., 2000]. 2003]. Enhanced anxiety-like behavior rative Linkage Study of Autism, 1999]. Hoxa1-null mice show aberrant prenatal is not observed, however, when the null This region includes several genes development, with delayed hindbrain allele for Sert is placed on a 129S6 strain important for early brain development, neural tube closure, absent cranial background [Holmes et al., 2003]. including WNT2, Engrailed 2, and nerves, and malformed inner ear struc- Levels of serotonin are partially HOXA1. WNT2 encodes a secreted tures [Lufkin et al., 1991]. Neither the mediated by monoamine oxidase A growth factor, which triggers a canoni- En2- nor the Hoxa1-null mice have been (MAOA), an enzyme of the mitochon- cal signal transduction cascade, utilizing screened for autistic-like behaviors [see drial outer membrane involved in the the Disheveled 1 (Dvl1) protein. In Murcia et al., 2004]. metabolism of catecholamine and indo- Drosophila, this signal is required for lamine neurotransmitters. In one study, expression of the Engrailed locus [Vin- the alleles regulating levels of MAOA cent and Lawrence, 1994]. The WNT2 were found to correlate with autism Serotonergic Neurotransmission symptom severity, with the lowest- locus was suggested to have a role in autism due to its location within the The strongest evidence for aberrant expressing alleles associated with lower 7q31-3 chromosomal region of interest, neurotransmission in autism involves IQs and more severe autistic behavior a premise supported by the identification alterations in the serotonergic system, [Cohen et al., 2003]. Mice homozygous of an autistic patient with a chromoso- with elevated levels of serotonin in blood for the null allele for Maoa have increased mal breakpoint in the WNT2 gene. reported for up to 45% of autistic serotonin and norepinephrine concen- Additionally, a single nucleotide poly- patients [Anderson et al., 1990; Cook trations in the brain [Cases et al., 1995]. morphism associated with autism was and Leventhal, 1996]. Clinical trials have Pups exhibit trembling, difficulty right- found in the locus [IMGSAC, 2001; provided evidence that treatment with ing themselves, and fearfulness, while Wassink et al., 2001]. A null allele of the fluoxetine, a selective inhibitor of the adults show increased aggression [Cases mouse Wnt2 locus results in a runting serotonin transporter, can have benefi- et al., 1995]. The MAOA-deficient mice phenotype, with half of the mutant pups cial effects in the treatment of autism also demonstrate increased freezing in a dying perinatally due to a placental [Fatemi et al., 1998; Hollander et al., conditioned fear task [Kim et al., 1997], defect potentially unrelated to autism 2005]. Human genetic studies have hyper-responsivity to acoustic stimuli, [Monkley et al., 1996]. A mouse model found a link between a short variant of and reduced exploration in a holeboard with targeted disruption of another the serotonin transporter allele (SERT) test [Popova et al., 2000]. Overall, Sert- member of the signaling pathway, the and autism [Kim et al., 2002; Conroy and Maoa-null mice demonstrate the Dvl1-null mouse, shows behavioral and et al., 2004]. significant behavioral effects of altera- neuropathological defects, including a Mice with targeted disruption of tions in neurotransmission, and are lack of homecage nesting and social the Sert locus evidence marked decrea- promising candidates for investigation interaction deficits [Lijam et al., 1997; ses in brain serotonin levels [Bengel et al., of behaviors relevant to the core symp- Long et al., 2004]. Dvl1-null mice also 1998], as well as decreased aggre- toms of autism. evidence reduced dendritic arborization ssion, increased anxiety, and increased [Rosso et al., 2005]. depression-related responses, including Reeler Mouse The Engrailed 2 locus on human longer immobility in a forced swim task chromosome 7q36 has been implicated The RELN locus encodes a large protein in ASD by an association study [Gharani that acts as a serine protease of the et al., 2004]. A targeted disruption of the Mice with targeted extracellular matrix [Fatemi, 2005]. The mouse homolog was generated prior to locus was originally identified as a the link with ASD. Homozygotes are disruption of the Sert spontaneous mouse mutation, Reeler. viable and fertile, but have abnormal locus evidence marked Reeler animals showed impaired motor foliation of the cerebellum [Joyner et al., decreases in brain coordination, tremors and ataxia 1991]. Further studies utilizing the En2 [Falconer, 1951]. The gene was subse- mutant mouse have suggested a role for serotonin levels, as well as quently identified and the Reeler allele the gene in regulating development of decreased aggression, increased shown to be a deletion of about 150 kb, serotonergic, noradrenergic, and dopa- generating a null allele. Examination of minergic neurons [Alberi et al., 2004; anxiety, and increased Reeler brains revealed that neurons failed Simon et al., 2005]. depression-related responses, to migrate to the correct location,
ARTICLE AMERICAN JOURNAL OF MEDICAL GENETICS PART C (SEMIN. MED. GENET.): DOI 10.1002/ajmg.c 45 resulting in disorganization of lamina- criteria. Therefore, validity of mouse tained in one side chamber of the test ted regions, such as the cerebellar and models for this disorder is dependent box, or remaining alone in the center or cerebral cortices. Studies have reported upon reflection of core behavioral opposite side chambers. The unfamiliar that specific human polymorphisms in symptoms, including impairments in stranger mouse is enclosed in a small the RELN locus can be associated with social interaction and deficiencies in wire cage, which allows visual, auditory, autism susceptibility [Persico et al., other functional domains. While the olfactory, and some tactile contact, but 2001; Skaar et al., 2004], although this use of a behavioral testing screen with precludes aggressive or sexual interac- association is not observed in all autism measures relevant to the clinical syn- tions. An identical wire cage is placed in patient populations [Bonora et al., drome is crucial for the development and the opposite side chamber to control for 2003]. evaluation of appropriate animal models the effects of object novelty. Measures Due to overt neuroanatomical [Rodier, 1996; Insel, 2001; Murcia et al., are taken of the amount of time spent in pathology, the Reeler mouse has been 2004], few mouse tests have been and entries into each side of the test extensively investigated as an animal targeted toward the constellation of box, as well as time spent sniffing the model for brain dysfunction. In addition symptoms characteristic of the ASDs. wire cages. Initial work with this test to the association with autism, changes Our research group at the University of for social approach has shown that in RELN have also been linked with North Carolina, in consultation with most, but not all, of selected inbred many neuropsychiatric disorders, inclu- clinicians, therapists, and other profes- mouse strains choose to spend more time ding schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and sionals working in the field of autism, has near the unfamiliar stranger. This pre- depression, as well as with lissencephaly designed a set of mouse behavioral tasks ference for social proximity can be [Fatemi, 2001], suggesting that the Reeler to model specific aspects of autistic-like observed in juvenile and adult mice, mouse does not provide a model for behavior, such as tendencies for social and in males and females [Brodkin et al., brain alterations specific to autism. Apart avoidance [Moy et al., 2004; Nadler 2004; Moy et al., 2004; Nadler et al., from the overt motoric deficits, Reln et al., 2004], repetitive responses, and 2004]. mutant mice show increased anxiety and resistance to change. The testing screen The second phase of the test assesses stereotypy in an open field, increased is composed of assays for sociability, preference for social novelty. In this case, social dominance in a tube test, and preference for social novelty, and learn- the mice are given a choice between learning deficits in the Morris water ing acquisition and reversal, as well as the unfamiliar mouse from the first phase maze task [Salinger et al., 2003; Lalonde control measures for motor and sensory (stranger 1), and a new unfamiliar mouse et al., 2004]. Some studies have reported function, including olfaction. Table II (stranger 2) placed in a wire cage in behavioral abnormalities in heterozy- presents a summary of mouse behavioral the opposite side of the test box. We have gous Reeler mice, which have levels of tasks that may be relevant to elements of observed that most, but not all, of the RELN protein in brain reduced by the autism phenotype. inbred mouse strains we have tested approximately 50% [Tuetling et al., demonstrate a shift in preference, so that 1999]. Aberrant responses observed more time is spent with the novel Measures of Social Behavior in Relnþ/ mice included increased stranger 2, rather than the more-familiar anxiety, deficits in prepulse inhibition Given that impairments in social inter- stranger 1. This phase of the social of acoustic startle, and learning delays action and communication are the preference test provides information on in an olfactory discrimination task primary diagnostic indicators in ASDs the ability of the mice to distinguish [Tuetling et al., 1999; Larson et al., [American Psychiatric Association, between two unfamiliar conspecifics. 2003]. Heterozygous Reeler mice were 1994], we considered the assessment of The social preference tests allow the also found to have decreased dendritic social behavior as an essential compo- assessment of both social approach and spine density [Liu et al., 2001] and nent for our behavioral testing battery. avoidance, using a relatively rapid, auto- reduced levels of oxytocin receptors in The first measure involves simple obser- mated task [Nadler et al., 2004]. Further brain [Liu et al., 2005]. However, other vations of nest-building and huddling in characterization of social behavior can studies have failed to find differences the home cage, which can provide early utilize observations of social interaction between Relnþ/ mice and normal indications of aberrant social behavior in an open field or neutral cage, the controls, suggesting the lack of a clear [Lijam et al., 1997; Mohn et al., 1999; resident-intruder paradigm, or tube tests gene-dosage relationship [Salinger et al., Moretti et al., 2005]. For a more quan- for social dominance [Mineur et al., 2003]. titative evaluation of social approach and 2002; Duncan et al., 2004; Long et al., avoidance, we conduct a social investi- 2004; Spencer et al., 2005]. Measure- gation task in a three-chambered appa- ment of ultrasonic vocalizations in MOUSE BEHAVIORAL ratus [Moy et al., 2004; Nadler et al., mouse pups removed from the nesting MEASURES RELEVANT TO 2004; see also Brodkin et al., 2004]. In area can provide information on respon- THE AUTISM PHENOTYPE the first phase of this test, mice are given ses to social isolation early in develop- The diagnosis of autism is based on a choice between spending time in the ment [Holtzman et al., 1996; Moles behavioral, rather than physiological, proximity of an unfamiliar mouse, con- et al., 2004].
46 AMERICAN JOURNAL OF MEDICAL GENETICS PART C (SEMIN. MED. GENET.): DOI 10.1002/ajmg.c ARTICLE TABLE II. Mouse Behavioral Tasks for Modeling the Autism Phenotype Symptom in autism Mouse behavioral task References Social interaction deficits Home cage observation Lijam et al. [1997]; Moretti et al. [2005] Social approach tests Moy et al. [2004]; Nadler et al. [2004] Resident-intruder interaction Mohn et al. [1999]; Moretti et al. [2005] First symptoms early in development, Ultrasonic vocalization in pups Holtzman et al. [1996]; Moles et al. [2004] communication deficits Restricted interests, repetitive behavior; Home cage observation Wu and Melton [1993]; Presti et al. [2003] stereotypies, self-injury Behavior in an open field DeLorey et al. [1998]; Turner et al. [2001] Nose-poke, hole board test Popova et al. [2000]; Bell et al. [2003] Novel object test Salinger et al. [2003]; Yan et al. [2004] Resistance to change Reversal learning Bakker et al. [1994]; Kooy et al. [1996] Anxiety Elevated plus maze Griebel et al. [2000]; Ren-Patterson et al. [2005] Light-dark transition test Bouwknecht and Paylor [2002]; Peier et al. [2000] Changes in sensory reactivity Prepulse inhibition of startle responses Tuetling et al. [1999]; Nielson et al. [2002] Hot plate analgesia test Martinez-Cue et al. [1999]; Long et al. [2004] Motor deficits, clumsiness Balance beam, climbing pole tests Lalonde et al. [2004]; Moretti et al. [2005] Rotarod performance Gerlai et al. [1996]; Miura et al. [2002] Mental retardation Morris water maze, hidden platform task Sago et al. [1998]; Lalonde et al. [2004] Radial arm maze Mineur et al. [2002]; Yan et al. [2004] Operant conditioning Driscoll et al. [2004]; Frankland et al. [2004] Passive avoidance DeLorey et al. [1998]; Lira et al. [2003] Conditioned fear Kim et al. [1997]; Costa et al. [2001] Measures for Repetitive Behavior the task is measured by removal of the forcement is switched, so that mice must and Restricted Interests escape platform before a final probe trial. now choose the opposite arm to earn Normal animals will swim to and circle rewards. One disadvantage of this task is In mice, repetitive behavior can include over the area where the platform had that mice must be food-deprived before perseverative sniffing, circling, digging, been located. In the next phase of acquisition, which can lead to issues or continuous cage-running and jump- testing, the escape platform is placed in of motivational levels across strains. ing [Cases et al., 1995; Homanics et al., a different region of the pool, and mice Another procedure using the T-maze is 1997; Presti et al., 2003]. Repetitive are re-assessed on learning the new the spontaneous alternation test, which grooming and scratching can lead to self- location. Delayed or deficient acquisi- relies on the nature tendency of mice to injury, such as torn ears and skin lesions tion during reversal learning could serve alternate arm choices in a maze. Mice [Wu and Melton, 1993]. Aberrant as a model for autistic-like cognitive with the Relnrl-orl mutation demonstrate stereotypies can be noted during rigidity. In line with this premise, perseverative responses in this test home-cage observations or during Fmr1-null mice show normal acquisi- [Lalonde et al., 2004]. open-field testing. tion of spatial learning in the Morris Other ways to assess repetitive beha- The predisposition for repetitive water maze task, but impaired learning vior include measures of exploration on a behavior in autism can also be seen during the reversal phase [Bakker et al., hole-board, with counts taken of nose- in an excessive adherence to routine. 1994; Kooy et al., 1996]. pokes into a restricted set of holes, versus Reversal learning in the Morris water Mice with visual or motor impair- the entire board [Popova et al., 2000; maze task can be used as a measure of ments may not be able to perform in Bell et al., 2003], and the investigation of resistance to change a learned pattern of the water maze test. In these cases, a novel objects in an open field [Salinger behavior. In this task, mice are trained to simple T-maze task can be used to et al., 2003; Yan et al., 2004]. locate a hidden escape platform, which is evaluate learning acquisition and rever- slightly submerged in a circular pool of sal. Mice are trained to enter one Interpretation of Results From opaque water. Over several days, mice particular arm (left or right) of the maze Behavioral Tests learn to swim directly to the escape to obtain a food reward. When a high platform from any release point in the rate of correct choices has been While deficits in social interaction and periphery of the pool. Spatial learning in observed, the arm designated for rein- aberrant repetitive behavior may be key
ARTICLE AMERICAN JOURNAL OF MEDICAL GENETICS PART C (SEMIN. MED. GENET.): DOI 10.1002/ajmg.c 47 features of the autism behavioral phe- mutation, a higher rate of inactivation ches which can address the genetics of notype, the human disorder is character- for the mutation-bearing chromosome these diverse pathways to achieve a more ized by the variable inclusion of many can result in a less severe phenotype. global view of the etiology of autism. other symptoms [e.g., Muris et al., However, the effect of X inactivation Discovering genes important for normal 1998]. Table II lists several functional can be controlled in Mecp2-mutant social behavior in mice could lead to domains of behavior, including anxiety, mice by only using males as subjects the identification of brain mechanisms sensory reactivity, motor skills, and [Shahbazian et al., 2002]. In Angelman that play a role in the impaired inter- cognitive abilities, that may or may not syndrome, symptoms are dependent on action and communication characteriz- be changed in mouse models for ASDs. whether the chromosome bearing the ing ASDs. Gene expression profiles for In addition, the behavioral battery mutation was inherited from the father mouse models characterized by aberrant utilized by our laboratory includes con- or the mother [for review, see Williams, social behavior, such as oxytocin or trol measures, such as tests for activity 2005]. Parent of origin imprinting vasopressin deficient mice [Insel et al., level and olfactory ability, that can be causes expression of a subset of genes 1999; Lim et al., 2005], 5-HT1A- and crucial for the interpretation of observed from only one of two copies, maternal 1B- knockouts [Zhuang et al., 1999; deficits in social preference [Moy et al., and paternal, present in the genome. see also Gingrich and Hen, 2001], 2004], as well as other tasks relevant to The effect of imprinting can be con- dopamine transporter knockout mice autism. trolled in mouse models by setting up [Rodriguiz et al., 2004], or mice with crosses that transmit the mutation from reduced levels of NMDA receptors in the appropriate parent [e.g., Jiang et al., brain [Mohn et al., 1999; Duncan et al., DISCUSSION 1998; Miura et al., 2002; Liljelund et al., 2004] could suggest new genetic targets The complex interaction of genes that 2005]. for studies in human populations. Beha- appears to underlie susceptibility for the Possible interactions between the vioral tasks relevant to the autism autism phenotype is problematic for disease mutation and the genetic back- phenotype could also be revealing in development of animal models of the ground of the mouse model should also the case of mouse models which reflect ASDs. Many of the mouse models be considered. Susceptibility for the neuropathology relevant to autism, described in this review involve targeted effects of the targeted locus may differ including changes in cerebellum [e.g., disruption of a candidate gene. One across background strains, as observed in the Engrailed mouse, Joyner et al., 1991; disadvantage of this approach is that the Fmr1-null mouse [Dobkin et al., 2000], see also Murcia et al., 2004] or brain complete removal of a protein in brain Sert-null mice [Holmes et al., 2003], overgrowth [e.g., IGF-overexpressing can lead to severe impairments or early and Tsc1 heterozygotes [Wilson et al., mice, Hodge et al., 2005]. Thorough mortality, as observed with mouse 2005]. Mouse geneticists should also behavioral phenotyping provides an models for Angelman and Smith– consider strain characteristics when gen- advantageous first approach to deter- Lemli–Optiz syndromes [Homanics erating mouse models. For example, the mine which mouse models for ASDs et al., 1997; Fitzky et al., 2001]. In 129SvEv inbred mouse strain, a line would offer the most valuable candidates addition, the null allele might not widely used in laboratory research, is for detailed genetic characterization. properly recapitulate the disease pheno- characterized by reduction or loss of the type in the animal model. In the case of corpus callosum and, in some cases, poor ASDs, many of the alleles identified in performance in learning tasks [Balogh candidate genes are not null alleles. et al., 1999; Wahlsten et al., 2001]. Thorough behavioral Rather than a complete loss of function, In addition, several inbred mouse phenotyping provides an autism-susceptibility alleles may cause strains, including FVB/NJ, C3H/HeJ, alterations in the structure of the result- and SJL/J, carry the gene for retinal advantageous first approach to ing proteins, affect the amount of protein degeneration, with blindness occurring determine which mouse models produced, or protein stability within the by weaning age [The Jackson Labora- cell. tory, 2002]. Intrinsic alterations in brain for ASDs would offer the most Other biological processes contri- morphology, sensory function, and valuable candidates for detailed bute to the variability seen in disease learning ability are confounding factors phenotype. Some genetic complexity, in the use of these strains to model genetic characterization. such as X-inactivation or parent-of- autistic-like behaviors. origin imprinting, can be controlled The genes involved in the regula- for in mouse models. For example, the tion of complex biological processes, gene for Rett syndrome, MECP2, is such as development, metabolism, REFERENCES carried on the X chromosome. In imprinting, and X-inactivation, are all Alberi L, Sgado P, Simon HH. 2004. Engrailed affected females, in every cell, one X likely to impinge upon the observable genes are cell-autonomously required to prevent apoptosis in mesencephalic dopa- chromosome is inactivated, or silenced. autism phenotype in a human or a minergic neurons. Development 131:3229– In cases where only one X carries a mouse. Thus arises the need for approa- 3236.
48 AMERICAN JOURNAL OF MEDICAL GENETICS PART C (SEMIN. MED. GENET.): DOI 10.1002/ajmg.c ARTICLE American Psychiatric Association. 1994. Diag- Fang Y, Song C-Y, Vitale R. 2002. Associa- specific learning impairment. Neurosci 100: nostic and statistical manual of mental tion between a GABRB3 polymorphism and 423–429. disorders (DSM-IV). Washington, DC. autism. Mol Psychiatry 7: 311–316. Driscoll LL, Carroll JC, Moon J, Crnic LS, Amir RE, Van den Veyver IB, Wan M, Tran CQ, Cases O, Seif I, Grimsby J, Gaspar P, Chen K, Levitsky DA, Strupp BJ. 2004. Impaired Francke U, Zoghbi HY. 1999. Rett Pournin S, Muller U, Aguet M, Babinet C, sustained attention and error-induced syndrome is caused by mutations in X- Shih JC, De Maeyer E. 1995. Aggressive stereotypy in the aged Ts65Dn mouse: A linked MECP2, encoding methyl-CpG- behavior and altered amounts of brain mouse model of Down syndrome and binding protein 2. Nat Genet 23:185–188. serotonin and norepinephrine in mice Alzheimer’s disease. Behav Neurosci 118: Anderson GM, Horne WC, Chatterjee D, Cohen lacking MAOA. Science 268:1763–1766. 1196–1205. DJ. 1990. The hyperserotoninemia of aut- Chen L, Toth M. 2001. Fragile X mice develop Duncan GE, Moy SS, Perez A, Eddy DM, Zinow ism. Ann NY Acad Sci 600:331–340. sensory hyperreactivity to auditory stimuli. W, Lieberman JA, Snouwaert JN, Koller Aylward EH, Minshew NJ, Goldstein G, Honey- Neuroscience 103:1043–1050. BH. 2004. Deficits in sensorimotor gating cutt NA, Augustine AM, Yates KO, Barta Chen RZ, Akbarian S, Tudor M, Jaenisch R. and tests of social behavior in a genetic PE, Pearlson GD. 1999. MRI volumes of 2001. Deficiency of methyl-CpG binding model of reduced NMDA receptor func- amygdala and hippocampus in non-mentally protein-2 in CNS neurons results in a Rett- tion. Behav Brain Res 153:507–518. retarded autistic adolescents and adults. like phenotype in mice. Nat Genet 27:327– Falconer DS. 1951. Two new mutants. J Genet 50: Neurology 53:2145–2150. 331. 192–201. Bailey A, Luthert P, Dean A, Harding B, Janota I, Cohen IL, Liu X, Schutz C, White BN, Jenkins Fatemi SH. 2001. Reelin mutations in mouse and Montgomery M, Rutter M, Lantos P. 1998. EC, Brown WT, Holden JJA. 2003. Asso- man: From reeler mouse to schizophrenia, A clinicopathological study of autism. Brain ciation of autism severity with a monoamine mood disorders, autism and lissencephaly. 121:889–905. oxidase A functional polymorphism. Clin Mol Psychiatry 6:129–133. Bakker CE, Verheij C, Willemsen R, van der Genet 64:190–197. Fatemi SH. 2005. Reelin glycoprotein: Structure, Helm R, Oerlemans F, Vermey M, Bygrave Collaborative Linkage Study of Autism. 1999. An biology and roles in health and disease. Mol A, Hoogeveen AT, Oostra BA, Reyniers E, autosomal genomic screen for autism. Am J Psychiatry 10:251–257. De Boulle K, D’Hooge R, Cras P, van Med Genet (Neuropsychiatr Genet) 88: Fatemi SH, Realmuto GM, Khan L, Thuras P. Velzen D, Nagels G, Martin J-J, De Deyn 609–615. 1998. Fluoxetine in treatment of adoles- PP, Darby JK, Willems PJ. 1994. Fmr1 Comery TA, Harris JB, Willems PJ, Oostra BA, cent patients with autism: A longitudinal knockout mice: A model to study fragile X Irwin SA, Weiler IJ, Greenough WT. 1997. open trial. J Aut Dev Disorders 28:303– mental retardation. Cell 78:23–33. Abnormal dendritic spines in fragile X 307. Balogh SA, McDowell CS, Stavnezer AJ, Denen- knockout mice: Maturation and pruning Fisch GS, Hao HK, Bakker C, Oostra BA. 1999. berg VH. 1999. A behavioral and neuroa- deficits. Proc Natl Acad Sci 94:5401–5404. Learning and memory in the FMR1 knock- natomical assessment of an inbred substrain Conroy J, Meally E, Kearney G, Fitzgerald M, Gill out mouse. Am J Med Genet 84:277– of 129 mice with behavioral comparisons to M, Gallagher L. 2004. Serotonin transporter 282. C57BL/6J mice. Brain Res 836:38–48. gene and autism: A haplotype analysis in an Fitzky BU, Witsch-Baumgartner M, Erdel M, Lee Baranek GT. 2002. Efficacy of sensory and motor Irish autistic population. Mol Psychiatry J-N, Paik Y-K, Glossmann H, Utermann G, interventions for children with autism. J 9:587–593. Moebius FF. 1998. Mutations in the 7-sterol Autism Dev Disord 32:397–422. Cook EH, Leventhal BL. 1996. The serotonin sys- reductase gene in patients with the Smith- Barton M, Volkmar F. 1998. How commonly are tem in autism. Curr Opin Pediatr 8:348– Lemli-Opitz syndrome. Proc Natl Acad Sci known medical conditions associated with 354. USA 95:8181–8186. autism? J Aut Dev Dis 28:273–278. Costa RM, Yang T, Huynh DP, Pulst SM, Fitzky BU, Moebius FF, Asaoka H, Waage-Baudet Bell K, Shokrian D, Potenzieri C, Whitaker- Viskochil DH, Silva AJ, Brannan CI. 2001. H, Xu L, Xu G, Maeda N, Kluckman K, Azmitia PM. 2003. Harm avoidance, anxi- Learning deficits, but normal development Hiller S, Yu H, Batta AK, Shefer S, Chen ety, and response to novelty in the adoles- and tumor predisposition, in mice lacking T, Salen G, Sulik K, Simoni RD, Ness GC, cent S-100B transgenic mouse: Role of exon 23a of Nf1. Nat Genet 27:399–405. Glossmann H, Patel SB, Tint GS. 2001. serotonin and relevance to Down syndrome. Costa RM, Federov NB, Kogan JH, Murphy GG, 7-dehydrocholesterol-dependent proteolysis Neuropsychopharmacol 28:1810–1816. Stern J, Ohno M, Kucherlapati R, Jacks T, of HMG-CoA reductase suppresses sterol Bengel D, Murphy DL, Andrews AM, Wichems Silva AJ. 2002. Mechanism for the learning biosynthesis in a mouse model of Smith– CH, Feltner D, Heils A, Mossner R, deficits in a mouse model of neurofibroma- Lemli–Opitz/RSH syndrome. J Clin Invest Westphal H, Lesch KP. 1998. Altered brain tosis type 1. Nature 415:526–530. 108:905–915. serotonin homeostasis and locomotor insen- Courchesne E, Karns CM, Davis HR, Ziccardi R, Folstein SE, Rosen-Sheidley B. 2001. Genetics of sitivity to 3,4-methylenedioxymethampheta- Carper RA, Tigue ZD, Chisum HJ, Moses autism: Complex aetiology for a hetero- mine (‘‘Ecstasy’’) in serotonin transporter- P, Pierce K, Lord C, Lincoln AJ, Pizzo S, geneous disorder. Nat Rev Genet 2:943– deficient mice. Mol Pharmacol 53:649–655. Schreibman L, Haas RH, Akshoomoff NA, 955. Bodfish JW, Symons FJ, Parker DE, Lewis M. Courchesne RY. 2001. Unusual brain Fombonne E. 2002. Epidemiological trends in 2000. Varieties of repetitive behavior in growth patterns in early life in patients with rates of autism. Mol Psychiatry 7:S4–S6. autism: Comparisons to mental retardation. autistic disorder. An MRI study. Neurology Frankland PW, Wang Y, Rosner B, Shimizu T, J Aut Dev Disorders 30:237–243. 57:245–254. Balleine BW, Dykens EM, Ornitz EM, Silva Bonora E, Beyer KS, Lamb JA, Parr JR, Klauck Darnell JC, Fraser CE, Mostovetsky O, Stefani G, AJ. 2004. Sensorimotor gating abnormalities SM, Benner A, Paolucci M, Abbott A, Jones TA, Eddy SR, Darnell RB. 2005. in young males with fragile X syndrome and Ragoussis I, Poustka A, Bailey AJ, Monaco Kissing complex RNAs mediate interaction Fmr1-knockout mice. Mol Psychiatry 9: AP, International Molecular Genetic Study between the Fragile-X mental retardation 417–425. of Autism (IMGSAC). 2003. Analysis of protein KH2 domain and brain polyribo- Gerlai R, Millen KJ, Herrup K, Fabien K, Joyner reelin as a candidate gene for autism. Mol somes. Genes Dev 19:903–918. AL, Roder J. 1996. Impaired motor learning Psychiatry 8:885–892. DeLorey TM, Handforth A, Anagnostaras SG, performance in cerebellar En-2 mutant Bouwknecht JA, Paylor R. 2002. Behavioral and Homanics GE, Minassian BA, Asatourian A, mice. Behav Neurosci 110:126–133. physiological mouse assays for anxiety: A Fanselow MS, Delgado-Escueta A, Ellison Gharani N, Benayed R, Mancuso V, Brzustowicz survey in nine mouse strains. Behav Brain GD, Olsen RW. 1998. Mice lacking the b3 LM, Millonig JH. 2004. Association of the Res 136:489–501. subunit of the GABAA receptor have the homeobox transcription factor, ENGRAILED Brodkin ES, Hagemann A, Nemetski SM, Silver epilepsy phenotype and many of the beha- 2, 3, with autism spectrum disorder. Mol LM. 2004. Social approach-avoidance beha- vioral characteristics of Angelman syn- Psychiatry 9(5):474–484. vior of inbred mouse strains towards DBA/2 drome. J Neurosci 18:8505–8514. Gillberg C, Billstedt E. 2000. Autism and mice. Brain Res 1002:151–157. Dobkin C, Rabe A, Dumas R, El Idrissi A, Asperger syndrome: Coexistence with other Buxbaum JD, Silverman JM, Smith CJ, Greenberg Haubenstock H, Brown WT. 2000. Fmr1 clinical disorders. Acta Psychiatr Scand 102: DA, Kilifarski M, Reichert J, Cook EH Jr, knockout mouse has a distinctive strain- 321–330.
ARTICLE AMERICAN JOURNAL OF MEDICAL GENETICS PART C (SEMIN. MED. GENET.): DOI 10.1002/ajmg.c 49 Gingrich JA, Hen R. 2001. Dissecting the role of Insel TR, O’Brien DJ, Leckman JF. 1999. Liu WS, Pesold C, Rodriguez MA, Carboni G, the serotonin system in neuropsychiatric Oxytocin, vasopressin, and autism: Is there Auta J, Lacor P, Larson J, Condie BG, disorders using knockout mice. Psychophar- a connection? Biol Psychiatry 45:145–157. Guidotti A, Costa E. 2001. Down-regula- macology 155:1–10. Jiang Y, Armstrong D, Albrecht U, Atkins CM, tion of dendritic spine and glutamic acid Griebel G, Belzung C, Perrault G, Sanger DJ. Noebels JL, Eichele G, Sweatt JD, Beaudet decarboxylase 67 expressions in the reelin 2000. Differences in anxiety-related beha- AL. 1998. Mutation of the Angelman haploinsufficient heterozygous reeler mouse. viours and in sensitivity to diazepam in ubiquitin ligase in mice causes increased Proc Natl Acad Sci 98:3477–3482. inbred and outbred strains of mice. Psycho- cytoplasmic p53 and deficits of contextual Liu W, Pappas GD, Carter CS. 2005. Oxytocin pharmacology 148:164–170. learning and long-term potentiation. Neu- receptors in brain cortical regions are Guy J, Hendrich B, Holmes M, Martin JE, Bird ron 21:799–811. reduced in haploinsufficient (þ/) reeler A. 2001. A mouse Mecp2-null mutation Joyner AL, Herrup K, Auerbach BA, Davis CA, mice. Neurol Res 27(4):339–345. causes neurological symptoms that mimic Rossant J. 1991. Subtle cerebellar pheno- Long JM, LaPorte P, Paylor R, Wynshaw-Boris A. Rett syndrome. Nat Genet 27:322–326. type in mice homozygous for a targeted 2004. Expanded characterization of the Hagerman RJ, Jackson AWIII, Levitas A, Rim- deletion of the En-2 homeobox. Science social interaction abnormalities in mice land B, Braden M. 1986. An analysis of 251:1239–1243. lacking Dvl1. Genes Brain Behav 3:51–62. autism in fifty males with the fragile X Keller F, Persico AM. 2003. The neurobiological Lufkin T, Dierich A, LeMeur M, Mark M, syndrome. Am J Med Genet 23:359–374. context of autism. Mol Neurobiol 28:1–22. Chambon P. 1991. Disruption of the Hox- Happé F, Ehlers S, Fletcher P, Frith U, Johansson M, Kim JJ, Shih JC, Chen K, Chen L, Bao S, Maren 1.6 homeobox gene results in defects in a Gillberg C, Dolan R, Frackowiak R, Frith C. S, Anagnostaras SG, Fanselow MS, Maeyer region corresponding to its rostral domain of 1996.‘‘Theoryofmind’’ inthebrain. Evidence ED, Seif I, Thompson RF. 1997. Selective expression. Cell 66:1105–1119. from a PET scan study of Asperger syndrome. enhancement of emotional, but not motor, Martinez-Cue C, Baamonde C, Lumbreras MA, Neuro Report 8(1): 197–201. learning in monoamine oxidase A-deficient Vallina IF, Dierssen M, Florez J. 1999. A Hodge RD, D’Ercole AJ, O’Kusky JR. 2005. mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci 94:5929–5933. murine model for Down syndrome shows Increased expression of insulin-like growth Kim S-J, Cox N, Courchesne R, Lord C, Corsello reduced responsiveness to pain. Neurore- factor-I (IGF-I) during embryonic develop- C, Akshoomoff N, Guter S, Leventhal BL, port 10:1119–1122. ment produces neocortical overgrowth with Courchesne E, Cook EHJr. 2002. Transmis- Mineur YS, Sluyter F, de Wit S, Oostra BA, Crusio differentially greater effects on specific sion disequilibrium mapping at the seroto- WE. 2002. Behavioral and neuroanatomical cytoarchitectonic areas and cortical layers. nin transporter gene (SLC6A4) region in characterization of the Fmr1 knockout Dev Brain Res 154:227–237. autistic disorder. Mol Psychiatry 7:278–288. mouse. Hippocampus 12:39–46. Hollander E, Phillips A, Chaplin W, Zagursky K, Kooy RF, D’Hooge R, Reyniers E, Bakker CE, Miura K, Kishino T, Li E, Webber H, Dikkes P, Novotny S, Wasserman S, Iyengar R. 2005. Nagels G, De Boulle K, Storm K, Clincke Holmes GL, Wagstaff J. 2002. Neurobeha- A placebo controlled crossover trial of liquid G, De Deyn PP, Oostra BA, Willems PJ. vioral and electroencephalographic abnor- fluoxetine on repetitive behaviors in child- 1996. Transgenic mouse model for the malities in Ube3a maternal-deficient mice. hood and adolescent autism. Neuropsycho- fragile X syndrome. American J Med Gen Neurobio Dis 9:149–159. pharmacology 30:582–589. 64:241–245. Mohn AR, Gainetdinov RR, Caron MG, Koller Holmes A, Yang RJ, Murphy DL, Crawley JN. Kremer EJ, Pritchard M, Lynch M, Yu S, Holman BH. 1999. Mice with reduced NMDA 2002. Evaluation of antidepressant-related K, Baker E, Warren ST, Schlessinger D, receptor expression display behaviors related behavioral responses in mice lacking the Sutherland GR, Richards RI. 1991. Map- to schizophrenia. Cell 98:427–436. serotonin transporter. Neuropsychophar- ping of DNA instability at the fragile X to a Moles A, Kieffer BL, D’Amato F. 2004. Deficit macology 27(6):914–923. trinucleotide repeat sequence p(CCG)n. in attachment behavior in mice lacking the Holmes A, Li Q, Murphy DL, Gold E, Crawley Science 252:1711–1714. m-opioid receptor gene. Science 304:1983– JN. 2003. Abnormal anxiety-related beha- Lalonde R, Hayzoun K, Derer M, Mariani J, 1986. vior in serotonin transporter null mutant Strazielle C. 2004. Neurobehavioral evalua- Monkley SJ, Delaney SJ, Pennisi DJ, Christiansen mice: The influence of genetic background. tion of Relnrl-orl mutant mice and correla- JH, Wainwright BJ. 1996. Targeted disrup- Genes Brain Behav 2(6):365–380. tions with cytochrome oxidase activity. tion of the Wnt2 gene results in placentation Holtzman DM, Santucci D, Kilbridge J, Chua- Neurosci Res 49:297–305. defects. Development 122:3343–3353. Couzens J, Fontana DJ, Daniels SE, Johnson Larson J, Hoffman JS, Guidotti A, Costa E. 2003. Moretti P, Bouwknecht JA, Teague R, Paylor R, RM, Chen K, Sun Y, Carlson E, Alleva E, Olfactory discrimination learning deficit in Zoghbi HY. 2005. Abnormalities of social Epstein CJ, Mobley WC. 1996. Develop- heterozygous reeler mice. Brain Res 971: interactions and home-cage behavior in a mental abnormalities and age-related neu- 40–46. mouse model of Rett syndrome. Hum Mol rodegeneration in a mouse model of Down Lijam N, Paylor R, McDonald MP, Crawley JN, Gen 14:205–220. syndrome. Proc Natl Acad Sci 93:13333– Deng C-X, Herrup K, Stevens KE, Macca- Moy SS, Nadler JJ, Perez A, Barbaro RP, Johns 13338. ferri G, McBain CJ, Sussman DJ, Wynshaw- JM, Magnuson TR, Piven J, Crawley JN. Homanics GE, DeLorey TM, Firestone LL, Boris A. 1997. Social interaction and 2004. Sociability and preference for social Quinlan JJ, Handforth A, Harrison NL, sensorimotor gating abnormalities in mice novelty in five inbred strains: An approach to Krasowski MD, Rick CEM, Korpi ER, lacking Dv/1. Cell 90:895–905. assess autistic-like behavior in mice. Genes Makela R, Brilliant MH, Hagiwara N, Liljelund P, Handforth A, Homanics GE, Olsen Brain Behav 3:287–302. Ferguson C, Synder K, Olsen RW. 1997. RW. 2005. GABAA receptor b3 subunit Murcia CL, Gulden F, Herrup K. 2004. A Mice devoid of a-aminobutyrate type A gene-deficient heterozygous mice show question of balance: A proposal for new receptor b3 subunit have epilepsy, cleft parent-of-origin and gender-related differ- mouse models of autism. Int J Devl palate, and hypersensitive behavior. Proc ences in b3 subunit levels, EEG, and Neurosci 23:265–275. Natl Acad Sci USA 94:4143–4148. behavior. Dev Brain Res 157:150–161. Muris P, Steerneman P, Merckelbach H, Holdri- IMGSAC. 2001. A genomewide screen for Lim MM, Bielsky IF, Young LJ. 2005. Neuropep- net I, Meesters C. 1998. Comorbid anxiety autism: Strong evidence for linkage to tides and the social brain: Potential rodent symptoms in children with pervasive devel- chromosomes 2q, 7q, and 16p. Am J Hum models of autism. Int J Devl Neurosci 23: opmental disorders. J Anxiety Dis 12:387– Genet 69:570–581. 235–243. 393. Ingram JL, Stodgell CJ, Hyman SL, Figlewicz DA, Lira A, Zhou M, Castanon N, Ansorge MS, Nadler JJ, Moy SS, Dold G, Trang D, Simmons N, Weitkamp LR, Rodier PM. 2000. Discov- Gordon JA, Francis JH, Bradley-Moore M, Perez A, Young NB, Barbaro RP, Piven J, ery of allelic variants of HOXA1 and Lira J, Underwood MD, Arango V, Kung Magnuson T, Crawley JN. 2004. Automated HOXB1: Genetic susceptibility to autism HF, Hofer MA, Hen R, Gingrich JA. 2003. apparatus for rapid quantitation of autism- spectrum disorders. Teratology 62:393–405. Altered depression-related behaviors and like social deficits in mice. Genes Brain Insel TR. 2001. Mouse models for autism: Report functional changes in the dorsal raphe Behav 3:303–314. from a meeting. Mamm Genome 12:755– nucleus of serotonin transporter-deficient Nan X, Ng H-H, Johnson CA, Laherty CD, 757. mice. Bio Psychiatry 54:960–971. Turner BM, Eisenman RN, Bird A. 1998.
You can also read