Maximizing the benefits of Pilates-inspired exercise for learning functional motor skills
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
REVIEW Maximizing the bene®ts of Pilates-inspired exercise for learning functional motor skills . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Claudia Lange, Viswanath Unnithan, Elizabeth Larkam, Paula M. Latta Abstract Joseph Pilates (1880±1967) created a system of ®tness exercises that are still practiced in a more or less modi®ed form. Within the last two decades, there has been a signi®cant increase in the popularity of such Pilates-inspired (PI) exercises. This paper describes current claims for the eectiveness of PI exercises and comments on their validity. Motor learning principles and ®ndings are applied to make recommendations for using PI exercises to enhance the execution of functional movement tasks. The learning-performance distinction, augmented information Claudia Lange PhD feedback, contextual interference, skill transfer and augmented verbal cues are Viswanath B. Unnithan PhD discussed. Finally, suggestions are made for aspiring PI practitioners seeking training Department of Exercise and Sport Science, and certi®cation. # 2000 Harcourt Publishers Ltd University of San Francisco Elizabeth Larkam MA Center for Sports Medicine, Saint Francis Memorial Joseph Pilates and and soon opened an exercise studio Hospital, San Francisco Paula M. Latta BS Contrology with his wife Clara. Pilates wrote a Peninsula RSI, Redwood City, CA slender volume (1934) outlining his Joseph Pilates was born in Germany system of Contrology, which he in 1880. A sickly child, he used described as a set of healthful Correspondence to: C. Lange Department of Exercise and Sport physical exercise to turn himself into lifestyle changes and corrective Science, University of San Francisco, a ®t adult. In 1912, Pilates moved to exercises. A later booklet (Pilates 2130 Fulton St, San Francisco, the UK and held a variety of & Miller 1945) features a number of CA 94117-1080, USA. occupations, including boxer and the exercises. His methods and Tel.: +1 415 422 6874; Fax: +1 415 422 6040; self-defense trainer. During World specialized ®tness equipment E-mail: langec@usfca.edu War I, while interned in a prison achieved popularity with dance This research was conducted in the Department of camp for German nationals, he professionals such as George Exercise and Sport Science, University of San taught his ®tness exercises to Balanchine and Martha Graham, Francisco, 2130 Fulton St, San Francisco, inmates. Upon returning to but were largely unknown to the CA 94117-1080, USA Germany after the war, he gave general public. Pilates died in 1967. exercise classes to police and A handful of students have carried Received October 1999 military personnel and interacted on his teachings and in¯uenced Revised November 1999 with members of the avant-garde numerous other individuals to learn Accepted December 1999 ........................................... dance community, such as Rudolf and teach his exercises. As a result, Journal of Bodywork and Movement Therapies (2000) von Laban and Hanya Holm. In the name Pilates has become 4(2), 99^108 # 2000 Harcourt Publishers Ltd 1926, he moved to New York City associated with a form of movement 99 J O U R NAL O F B O DY W O R K AN D MOV E M E N T TH E R APIE S APRIL 20 0 0
Lange et al. that is increasingly popular in Considering the widespread Fundamentals), and Chinese clinical and ®tness settings. application and adaptation of these medicine. Today, these (more or less Pilates (1934; Pilates & Miller exercises, there is a growing need to modi®ed) exercises are known 1945) made bold claims for the objectively assess their outcomes collectively as Pilates, Pilates-based bene®ts of Contrology. For and bene®ts. or Pilates-evolved. It is not the goal example, he stated that it would The purpose of this paper is to of this paper to distinguish among prevent coronary heart disease, evaluate current claims on the dierent exercise styles that are increase muscular power, and reduce learning bene®ts of exercises derived based on Pilates' teachings. Rather, the risk of respiratory ailments. He from the system developed by all exercises emanating from those also stated that it would result in Pilates. Motor learning principles established by Pilates will be de®ned complete voluntary control of the and ®ndings will be used as a model as Pilates-inspired (PI). There is an body. He described Contrology as a to make recommendations for ongoing discourse between Pilates' `balance' or `complete coordination administering Pilates-type exercises central ideas and movement of body, mind, and spirit' achieved to enhance functional movement routines, and other systems of by relinquishing strenuous exercises skills. Finally, suggestions will be movement and rehabilitation. The and deliberately performing made for aspiring practitioners of de®ning characteristics of PI functional activities in a posturally these exercises seeking training and exercises are listed in Box 1. appropriate manner. Despite Pilates' certi®cation. Examples of equipment and assurances (1934, Pilates & Miller exercises based on Pilates' designs 1945) that he had `scienti®cally' are shown in Figures 1±5. proven the eectiveness of his Claims on the learning approach, he did not subject his e¡ectiveness of Current claims for claims to experimental testing and Pilates-inspired Pilates-inspired exercises maintained that such testing was exercises unnecessary. A sampling of relevant sources The legacy of Pilates Within the last two decades, there reveals three categories of claims on has been a signi®cant increase in the Pilates was in¯uenced by hatha the bene®ts of PI exercises: popularity of exercises based on yoga, gymnastics, modern dance enhanced physiological functioning, Pilates' teachings. In the USA alone, and other movement systems. He enhanced psychological functioning more than 700 studio and designed about 40 mat exercises and and learning or re-learning of rehabilitation sites use his methods hundreds of exercises for his functionally eective postural sets (Larkam & Brownstein 1998). A specialized equipment (Larkam & and motor patterns (e.g. Robinson directory of UK practitioners (Body Nichols 1999). His exercises have & Thomson 1997, Balanced Body1 Control Pilates 1999) lists roughly been further in¯uenced by ®elds as 1999, Gallagher & Kryzanowska 100 studios. Practitioners can also diverse as physical therapy, somatics 1999, Physicalmind Institute 1999, be found in Africa, Asia, Australia (e.g. Feldenkrais MethodTM, Body/ Stott Conditioning 1999, The and New Zealand, Europe and Mind Centering1, Bartenie Balanced Body Studio 1999, Pilates South America. In order to understand the signi®cance of Box 1 De¢ning characteristics of Pilates-inspired exercises Pilates for functional skill improvement, it is necessary to be . Progressively enhance breath, core (body center), shoulder girdle, and limb control. aware of claims made by current . Movements are slow and deliberate, with few repetitions (usually 5±10). practitioners regarding the learning . Programs are highly individualized. One-on-one, dyad, and small group sessions are bene®ts of their exercise sessions. conducted. This can serve as a basis for . Exercises are done on ¯oor mats and equipment. . Exercises are done from lying, sitting, kneeling, standing and other postures. practical recommendations about . Some equipment uses springs to provide variable resistance. improving the quality of sessions. . Main piece of equipment is the Universal Reformer, a frame with a padded carriage Such recommendations can that slides back and forth within the frame. Adjustable springs attach from the carriage generically be derived from the to the frame. The carriage may be moved by pushing it, or by pulling on straps placed ®ndings of existing experiments on around arms or legs. . Other large pieces of equipment include the TrapezeTable (Cadillac), Combo (Wunda) motor skill learning. However, more Chair, Ladder and Step Barrels, and Ped-a-Pul. Pilates-speci®c recommendations . Small pieces of equipment include exercise balls, foam rollers, rotating disks, balance can only emerge with scienti®c study boards, resistance rings, small arcs, and boxes. of Pilates-type exercises themselves. 100 J O U R NAL O F B O DY W O R K AN D MOV E M E N T TH E R APIE S APRIL 20 0 0
Pilates-inspired exercise Box 2 Claims made on the e¡ectiveness of Pilates-inspired exercises Enhanced physiological functioning . ¯exibility and range of motion . muscular strength . muscular endurance . muscular power . cardiorespiratory ®tness Enhanced psychological functioning . mood . motivational state . attentional focus . enjoyment of life . energy and zest Enhanced motor learning . core control . static and dynamic posture . intralimb and interlimb coordination . aesthetically pleasing movement form . body awareness . static and dynamic balance 1994, Krasnow et al. 1997, McMillan et al. 1998). Roughly an equal number of studies also report the failure of PI exercises to elicit Fig. 1 A golfer has been diagnosed with spinal facette lock. During rehabilitation, the therapist is improvements (Fitt et al. 1994, giving verbal and tactile information to guide her to the appropriate position of spine extension Krasnow et al. 1997, McLain et al. and rotation. The patient is lying on the Trapeze Table and is using the spring-loaded pedals of 1997). Despite the lack of the Combo (Wunda) Chair to assist her action. Desired functional activity might be a golf swing. supportive, research-based data on Reproduced by kind permission of Balanced Body1, Inc. (1999) and Polestar1 EducationLLC PI exercises, anecdotal reports by (1999). practitioners and clients indicate that signi®cant bene®ts do indeed exist. It is therefore worthwhile to Institute 1999). To give an idea of disrupt normal coordination, such make preliminary recommendations the extent of these claims, examples as orthopedic surgery, stroke, and for maximizing the bene®ts of PI are listed in Box 2. head trauma. They also see PI exercises to enhance coordination, Of speci®c interest to this paper exercises as a possible tool for sensory awareness, and performance are claims related to motor learning. management or improvement of on functional tasks. These A main aspect of PI sessions physical ailments including multiple recommendations are derived from marketed to the ®tness consumer is sclerosis, ®bromyalgia and chronic the ®eld of motor learning, which the aesthetic bene®t of a longer, pain. deals with the experimental study of leaner appearance that comes from The claims made by current PI skill acquisition and performance. improved posture. Dancers are told practitioners regarding the motor they can develop more graceful learning bene®ts of their exercises movement patterns. Athletes are are overwhelmingly Recommendations assured they will maximize their unsubstantiated. Only a small to practitioners biomechanical eciency through number of published experimental of Pilates-inspired more appropriate muscle synergies. studies document measured movement A growing number of rehabilitation improvements in posture or Practitioner goals professionals are attracted to PI functional tasks that are exercises due to the promise of unequivocally attributable to PI PI practitioners have three regained function after events that exercises (Parrott 1993, Fitt et al. important learning goals, which are 101 J O U R NAL O F B O DY W O R K AN D MOV E M E N T TH E R APIE S APRIL 20 0 0
Lange et al. environment. Such tasks include walking, reaching, lifting and other activities of daily living and work. They also include complex movement sequences executed by athletes and performing artists. Third, practitioners want to structure practice to facilitate retention and transfer. Though practice by itself is often considered the most important variable for successful retention and transfer-hence the adage that practice makes perfect-it is certainly not the only one. The conditions under which practice takes place have a great in¯uence on the amount of skill learning. Some practice conditions appear to have a positive eect on learning, whereas others seem to result in less successful learning. It is beyond the scope of this paper to provide a Fig. 2 In a health club, clients are kneeling on the spring-loaded carriages of portable Reformers thorough discussion of all practice and using their pectoral, biceps brachii, and anterior deltoid muscles to pull against the arm conditions. For more in-depth straps. This action results in a backward movement of the spring-loaded carriage. Equal attention information, the reader is referred to is paid to concentric and eccentric contractions. The group exercise instructor is providing verbal several appropriate sources (Feltz & cues and imagery. Desired functional activities include any actions requiring dynamic trunk and Landers 1983, Landin 1994, Magill scapular stabilization. Reproduced by kind permission of Balanced Body1, Inc. (1999) and Polestar1 EducationLLC (1999). 1998, McCullagh 1993, Rose 1997, Schmidt & Lee 1999, Shea et al. 1993). The structure of practice outlined in Box 3. First, they want Second, practitioners want clients to becomes all the more relevant for PI clients to retain the exercises they are be able to transfer the coordination routines, because the latter rely on teaching. In other words, they want patterns and sensory awareness low numbers of repetitions. Pilates clients to develop independent learned during sessions to functional (1934, Pilates & Miller 1945) control of new movement patterns. tasks outside of the practice stressed the importance of few repetitions (usually less than 10 per exercise). Instead, he emphasized Box 3 Learning goals of Pilates-inspired practitioners deliberate practice during which conscious attention is focused on . Clients retain PI exercises taught by practitioner and can perform PI exercises without relevant task-related stimuli and practitioner's corrections. processes. Current PI practitioners . Breathing, core control, body awareness, and coordination patterns learned from PI follow his lead and recommend 5±10 exercises transfer to functional tasks. . Practice is structured to facilitate retention and transfer. repetitions for most exercises (e.g. Robinson & Thomson 1997; Evans & Stott Merrithew 1998a, 1998b; Gallagher & Kryzanowska 1999). Box 4 How to increase the e¡ectiveness of Pilates-inspired exercises Hence, the amount of practice may be less critical for learning than the . Observe the learning-performance distinction. conditions of practice, and PI . Use augmented verbal cues, self-talk and imagery. . Use augmented information feedback judiciously. practitioners play an essential role in . Vary the context of practice trials. structuring these conditions. Five . Use the right practice tasks to maximize transfer to functional tasks. important recommendations for how practice conditions may be 102 J O U R NAL O F B O DY W O R K AN D MOV E M E N T TH E R APIE S APRIL 20 0 0
Pilates-inspired exercise structured, in order to accomplish the goals of retention and transfer, will be described in the following paragraphs. Recommendations are summarized in Box 4. Recommendations for practitioners Recommendation1: observe the learning-performance distinction Motor learning has been de®ned as `a set of processes associated with practice or experience leading to relatively permanent changes in the capability for movement' (Schmidt & Lee 1999). One important aspect of this de®nition is the so-called learning-performance distinction. Learning is not a merely a change in behaviour, because changes can be Fig. 3 In a studio setting, a client is straddling the Ladder Barrel. She is lifting the pelvis o the transient. For instance, augmented barrel by activating her adductors, hamstrings, pelvic ¯oor muscles, and external rotators. This information feedback (AIF) can be complex posture can also be done with feet parallel. The instructor may provide cues or feedback. provided to learners about the Examples of applications include ballet and horseback riding. Reproduced by kind permission of success of their movements in the Elizabeth Larkam and Balanced Body1, Inc. (1999). environment, or about the quality of their movement patterns. An Fig. 4 In a studio or rehabilitation setting, a ballet dancer is using the Trapeze Table (Cadillac) to increase awareness of proper hamstring and hip extensor activation, in conjunction with core control and spine articulation. She is pressing with her left foot against a spring-loaded bar. A therapist may spot for safety by placing a hand over the dancer's left foot to prevent it from slipping o the bar. This exercise can be done in many variations to facilitate intratask transfer. Reproduced by kind permission of Elizabeth Larkam and Balanced Body1, Inc. (1999). 103 J O U R NAL O F B O DY W O R K AN D MOV E M E N T TH E R APIE S APRIL 20 0 0
Lange et al. instructors to direct the learner's attention to task-relevant stimuli and to prompt correct task execution (Landin 1994). Furthermore, cue words can be used by learners themselves prior to carrying out a task, in which case they are referred to as self-talk regimens (STR). Short-term memory is considered to be limited to ®ve to nine separate pieces of information (Miller 1956; Rose 1997). The number may be lower for some populations, such as stroke patients. Therefore, too many instructions may overload a learner trying to attend to task-relevant stimuli and organize a new response. AVC are somewhat like instructions, but because they consist of fewer words Fig. 5 During an advanced mat class, a ®tness client is executing a front leg pull. This action and only address a few important requires activation of deep abdominal and spinal musculature, as well as posterior shoulder girdle aspects of the task, they are thought stabilizers. It can also be done prone (facing down) as well as sidelying. Switching among dierent to place less load on short-term postures assists the client in maintaining axial elongation and core control during a variety of memory and increase chances that upright functional activities and is a precursor to sport-speci®c conditioning. Reproduced by kind permission of Elizabeth Larkam and Balanced Body1, Inc. (1999). the learner will retain the correct motor pattern. For example, in the Reformer group exercise depicted in Figure 2, the practitioner might use example of AIF might be a sessions. Rather, the temporary and the cue `funnel your ribs into your practitioner's verbal or tactile relatively permanent in¯uences of pelvic bowl.' Such a cue might be information about the degree of instructional methods must be taken more eective than giving a detailed spine extension and rotation into account. The learning- explanation on how to use the deep achieved during the prone Chair performance distinction is a crucial abdominal, spinal and pelvic ¯oor exercise depicted in Figure 1. Until aspect of evaluating the success of muscles for dynamic trunk recently, it was thought that any rehabilitation or conditioning stabilization. Similarly, in the providing a lot of AIF was better for system. Practitioners should strive Ladder Barrel exercise in learning. Indeed, laboratory to be aware of this distinction and Figure 3, the phrase `sit tall in the experiments show that when understand that clients may be saddle' may be more eective in learners receive AIF after each performing PI exercises or constraining the body's degrees of practice trial, their performance is functional tasks very dierently freedom than telling the client to usually better when compared to when not in the rehabilitation center activate a variety of separate that of learners who receive or studio. One way to assess learning muscle groups. feedback after fewer trials. is to ask the client at the beginning Although AVC and STR have not However, once AIF is removed and of the session to demonstrate an been as extensively studied as some learners must rely on their own exercise or functional activity that other instructional methods, recollection of the movement, those was practiced last session. This type research evidence (Landin 1994) who received less in practice are of approach can give an estimate of suggests they are quite useful. typically more successful (Salmoni et how much was learned. Landin makes several al. 1984, Schmidt 1991a, Schmidt & recommendations for using verbal Lee 1999). Recommendation 2: use augmented cues: First, he suggests using two It is evident from the example of verbal cues, self-talk, and imagery words or less for STR, and up to AIF that one cannot always predict Augmented verbal cues (AVC) are four words for AVC. Second, if cues learning from the quality of concise phrases, often consisting of address dierent parts of a task, it is performance during practice one or two words, which are used by important to consider how those 104 J O U R NAL O F B O DY W O R K AN D MOV E M E N T TH E R APIE S APRIL 20 0 0
Pilates-inspired exercise parts are interrelated. Cues should physical practice. It can also be used action (Winstein & Schmidt 1990). not disrupt the normal rhythm of by itself, in which case it is called A third guideline for AIF is that the the movement pattern by drawing mental practice. Although not learner must know how to interpret too much attention to one part of always eective, telling learners to it (Schmidt 1991b). A person with the task and creating arti®cial mentally imagine or mentally little knowledge of anatomy will not breaks between parts. For example, practice the correct movement be able to understand a statement breathing relies on a smooth pattern frequently appears to have such as `you aren't using your transition between in-breath and positive eects on retention intercostals.' Finally, giving AIF out-breath. Too much emphasis on Feltz & Landers 1983, Schmidt & during, or right after, a movement the in-breath can create breath Lee 1999). Therefore, cues may not always be a good idea. A holding and destroy the movement's such as `funnel your ribs' or learner who is trying to master an ¯ow. Landin's third `sit tall' may act like mnemonic exercise involving all four limbs recommendation is to consider the prompts that help evoke task- while maintaining core control, such variability of the environment and relevant muscular activity during as the Trapeze Table exercise in whether it is important for the exercises or related functional Figure 4, may be overly distracted if learner to focus on internal body activities. the practitioner interrupts with stimuli, or external environmental helpful advice. For extended PI stimuli, or both. Many tasks in sport routines that last 30 s or more, it and everyday life require switching Recommendation 3: use augmented may be best to give AIF attention between internal and information feedback judiciously periodically. For discrete PI external stimuli (Nideer 1993). PI As already stated, AIF is task- movements like doing a head and exercises typically focus the learner related information that helps the spine roll-up to a sitting position, it entirely on internal body sensations, learner correct errors in the may be best to wait a few seconds which may be necessary during movement's outcome and pattern. after a trial before giving AIF. This initial phases of recovery or Research suggests that it should be may give the learner an opportunity conditioning, or when an exercise is used judiciously. First and most to focus on the ¯eeting sensory ®rst introduced. However, if importantly, too much information impression of what the movement switching between an internal and about errors may overload the felt like, before receiving external focus is part of the learner's short-term memory, direct information about its correctness functional activities the client must attention away from important (Swinnen et al. 1990). perform, practitioners should create sensory stimuli, and prevent the situations that require switching. development of proper coordination Recommendation 4: vary the context Fourth, Landin states that STR can patterns. Therefore, PI practitioners of practice trials be used in some cases to supplant should resist the temptation to point Whereas practice conditions such as AIF by priming the correct action out all the errors a learner is making frequent AIF temporarily enhance before it takes place. This is and suggest corrections. It is more performance but degrade learning, particularly useful in group exercise useful to select one or two errors others have the opposite eect. One classes, outside of class when the appropriate to skill level. For example for this occurs when instructor is not present, or if there example, beginners might need contextual interference is is only one chance to `do it right', a corrections related to breathing and manipulated during practice. Low situation that exists in many maintaining a neutral spine while contextual interference occurs when competitive sports. Finally, skill doing supine leg circles on the mat. all repetitions of Task A are level must be considered when More advanced clients might need followed by all repetitions of Task designing cues. Beginners need cues pointers on how to maintain B, all repetitions of Task C, and so that re¯ect their less re®ned dynamic trunk alignment and on. For example, a client might do 8 understanding of the task and allow scapulo-humeral rhythm when supine leg presses on the Reformer, them to get the general idea of the doing any exercises involving trunk followed by eight toe raises and correct action, whereas more lateral ¯exion and arm reaching. A eight jumps. By contrast, high advanced learners may need to focus second recommendation for using contextual interference is present on speci®c errors. Rose (1997) AIF is to taper o the amount of when one repetition of Task A is makes the additional feedback as the learner becomes followed by one repetition of Task recommendation that cues should more pro®cient and can increasingly B, one repetition of Task C, and so help evoke task-relevant imagery. rely on internal representations and on. In other words, a leg press is Mental imagery can be used during sensory awareness of the correct followed by a toe raise, a jump, 105 J O U R NAL O F B O DY W O R K AN D MOV E M E N T TH E R APIE S APRIL 20 0 0
Lange et al. another leg press, another toe raise, has been de®ned as a gain or loss in However, even when movement another jump, and so on. High the capability for performing a patterns appear similar, positive contextual interference is thought to criterion task as a result of prior transfer can be negligible. For represent a number of `real-life' experience with a practice task (Ellis example, supine jumps on the movement conditions with 1965, Schmidt & Young 1987). One Reformer are practiced to prepare constantly alternating tasks. goal of practice is to develop the dancers and athletes for standing Compared to low contextual capability to adapt a previously vertical jumps. Yet research interference, high contextual learned action to new settings, or to indicates that supine jumping interference tends to depress perform variations of the action. practice does not necessarily result performance during practice. This is called near transfer or in positive transfer to upright Beginners sometimes report feeling intratask transfer (Sage 1984, Magill jumping (McLain et al. 1997). The frustrated or confused by high 1998, Schmidt 1991b). For example, authors of the latter study remarked contextual interference. It is during the hip extensor and that a during a jump on the therefore tempting to conclude that hamstring exercise performed on the Reformer, the legs may be placed up it also causes less skill learning. Trapeze Table in Figure 1, positive to 30 degrees above the plane of the However, numerous experiments intratask transfer means successfully body, creating signi®cant dierences have shown that even for beginners, doing the movement with internal or in jump strategies between the high contextual interference is often external hip rotation, with the entire supine and standing conditions and more bene®cial to skill learning than spine resting on the Table, with both possibly reducing successful low contextual interference (Magill legs at once, and other transfer. In a biomechanical study, 1998, Schmidt & Lee 1999). modi®cations. On the whole, Self et al. (1994) found a dierent Therefore, practice under high- intratask transfer as described above relationship between force and knee interference conditions may actually tends to be positive, though not angle during a supine plie on the be preferable in the long run. always very large (Schmidt & Young Reformer and a standing plieÂ, Practitioners should be aware that 1987). suggesting that the two tasks are learners will be often be performing A second goal of practice is to use dierent. functional tasks under conditions of prior experience with one action to PI practitioners should bear in high contextual interference and successfully execute a dierent mind that two overtly similar tasks should create routines that action. This is called far transfer or may in fact require dierent muscle progressively approximate these intertask transfer (Sage 1984, Magill synergies and be accompanied by conditions. Using the example of the 1998, Schmidt 1991b). The overall dierent sensory input. Experience mat exercise in Figure 5, a client similarity of coordination patterns with one task may not give the might do 1±2 repetitions of a seems to be one of several factors learner the necessary capability to posture that requires supine trunk determining the degree of positive do the other task. Conversely, two and shoulder girdle stabilization, intertask transfer. Very dierent tasks that appear dissimilar may followed by 1±2 repetitions of the patterns, such as swimming and actually involve similar underlying same posture in a prone position, volleyball, do not transfer positively muscle synergies, sensory stimuli, or and 1±2 sidelying. The client is to one another (Nelson 1957, cognitive strategies. This principle is challenged to exert sensory Schmidt & Lee 1999). The same referred to as transfer-appropriate awareness, and proper control of principle probably applies to processing (Lee 1988, Rose 1997). core and shoulder girdle muscles, two PI exercises that use both Practitioners should ask themselves while switching tasks. dierent pieces of equipment and whether the exercises they assign dierent body parts, such provide learners with an as lateral trunk ¯exion on the opportunity to practice the internal Recommendation 5: use the right Reformer and seated leg processing they will need for practice tasks to maximize skill extension on the Chair. When functional activities in sport, dance, transfer movement patterns are reasonably or everyday life. Care should be Learning is typically evaluated similar, intertask transfer is taken to ensure a transition from through retention and transfer tests. frequently small-to-moderate and supine, non-weight bearing activities Retention involves executing a given positive (Schmidt & Lee 1999). For to bipedal weightbearing activities movement at a later time, whereas example, doing a pelvic bridge on (Loosli & Herold 1992). Practice transfer is the application of a the mat may transfer positively to with supine exercises alone may not previously acquired movement skill doing the same exercise on the translate into improved alignment to new situations. Formally, transfer Reformer. or stability when standing up. 106 J O U R NAL O F B O DY W O R K AN D MOV E M E N T TH E R APIE S APRIL 20 0 0
Pilates-inspired exercise Evans B, Stott Merrithew M 1998b Essential Box 5 Checklist for aspiring practitioners Reformer Manual. Toronto: Stott Feltz DL, Landers DM 1983 The eects of . Investigate a number of programs. mental practice on motor skill learning . Consider program availability, cost, and duration. and performance: a meta-analysis. . Trainers and certi®ers must have sucient experience with the desired client population. Journal of Sport Psychology 5: 25±57 . Instructional modules dealing with anatomy, physiology, and clinical populations should Fitt S, Sturman J, McClain-Smith S 1994 be overseen by individuals with proper training. Eects of Pilates-based conditioning on . Certi®cation examinations should include oral, written, and case study portions. strength, alignment, and range of motion . To avoid con¯ict of interest, trainers should not have dual roles as certi®ers. in university ballet and modern dancers. . Trainers should have up-to-date knowledge of motor learning principles and ®ndings. Kinesiology and Medicine for Dance 16: 36±51 Gallagher S, Kryzanowska R 1999 The Pilates Method of Body Conditioning. BanBridge Books, Philadelphia Summary and 1999, Polestar1 Education 1999, Krasnow DH, Chat®eld SJ, Barr S, Jensen recommendations for Stott Conditioning 1999). Some JL, Dufek JS 1997 Imagery and the future training centers also mention conditioning practices for dancers. Dance practice conditions and teaching Research Journal 29: 43±64 strategies in their informational Larkam E 1999 Personal communication In the preceding paragraphs, several Larkam E, Brownstein B 1998 Balanced recommendations for maximizing literature (e.g. Physicalmind body: application of Pilates-evolved the learning bene®ts of PI sessions Institute 1999, Polestar1 Education exercises in ®tness, training and were introduced. However, these 1999, Stott Conditioning 1999, The rehabilitation. Unpublished manuscript recommendations are only Pilates Center 1999), which suggests Larkam E, Nichols J 1999 The history and preliminary. To date, no published that the trainers have some formal evolution of techniques in¯uenced by those of Joseph H. Pilates, 1920±1999. studies have been conducted in knowledge of motor learning Presentation at the American College of which an attempt was made to principles. However, it is best for Sportsmedicine, New Orleans discover causal links between prospective students to inquire Landin D 1994 The role of verbal cues in skill practice conditions, such as the ones before beginning any program. learning. Quest 46: 299±313 This paper was intended to Lee TD 1988 Transfer-appropriate described in this paper, and processing: a framework for improvements on PI exercises or provide an overview of current conceptualizing practice eects in motor functional skills. This oversight claims on the eectiveness of PI learning. In: Meijer OG, Roth K. eds. should certainly be remedied. exercises. Readers were introduced Complex Movement Behaviour: `The' A second critical issue is the to several practice conditions and Motor-Action Controversy. Elsevier, training and certi®cation that PI principles of motor learning that Amsterdam Loosli AR, Herold D 1992 Knee practitioners receive. Assumptions may enhance the usefulness of PI rehabilitation for dancers using a Pilates- about learning, and the instructional exercises for improving functional based technique. Kinesiology and techniques that practitioners movement. Suggestions were made Medicine for Dance 14: 1±12 themselves are taught, may be the for aspiring practitioners seeking Magill RA 1998 Motor Learning: Concepts most important sources of in¯uence training and certi®cation. Finally, it and Applications (5th ed). Columbus, Ohio, McGraw-Hill on their own teaching methods and must again be stated that controlled McCullagh P 1993 Modeling: learning, behaviours (Larkam 1999). experimental studies are urgently developmental, and social psychological Therefore, the selection of a suitable needed to verify the eectiveness of considerations. In: Singer RN, Murphy training and certi®cation program is PI exercises for enhancing M, Tennant LK, eds. Handbook of functional skills in a diversity of Research on Sport Psychology. critical for the aspiring practitioner. Macmillan, New York A checklist of important populations. McLain S, Carter CL, Abel J 1997 The eect considerations is provided in Box 5. of a conditioning and alignment program Training courses typically introduce on the measurement of supine jump the practitioner to functional height and pelvic alignment when using anatomy, a wide range of mat and REFERENCES the Current Concepts Reformer. Journal of Dance Medicine and Science 1: equipment exercises, and supervised Balanced Body1 1999 149±154 hands-on teaching experiences. www.balancedbody.com McMillan A, Proteau L, LeÁbe R-M 1998 The Written and/or oral certi®cation Body Control Pilates 1999 eect of Pilates-based training on examinations are also provided www.bodycontrol.co.uk dancers' dynamic posture. Journal of Ellis HC 1965 The Transfer of Learning. New Dance Medicine and Science 2: 101±107 (e.g. Body Control Pilates 1999, York: Macmillan Miller GA 1956 The magical number seven, Physicalmind Institute 1999, Pilates Evans B, Stott Merrithew M 1998a Mat plus minus two: some limits on our Institute 1999, Pilates1 Method Manual. Toronto: Stott 107 J O U R NAL O F B O DY W O R K AN D MOV E M E N T TH E R APIE S APRIL 20 0 0
Lange et al. capacity for processing information. Rose DJ 1997 A Mulitlevel Approach to the Self BP, Bagley AM, Triplett TL, Paulos LE Psychological Review 63: 81±97 Study of Motor Control and Learning. 1996 Functional biomechanical analysis Nelson DO 1957 Eects of swimming on the Allyn & Bacon, Massachusetts: Needham of the Pilates-based reformer during learning of selected gross motor skills. Heights demi-plie movements. Journal of Applied Research Quarterly 28: 374±378 Sage GH 1984 Motor Learning and Control: Biomechanics 12: 326±337 Nideer RN 1993 Attention control training. A Neuropsychological Approach. Wm. Shea CH, Shebilske WL, Worchel S 1993 In: Singer RN, Murphy M, Tennant LK C. Brown, Dubuque, Iowa Motor Learning and Control. Prentice- (eds) Handbook of Research on Sport Salmoni AW, Schmidt RA, Walter CB 1984 Hall: Englewood Clis, New Jersey Psychology. Macmillan, New York Knowledge of results and motor learning: Stott Conditioning 1999 Parrott AA 1993 The eects of Pilates a review and critical reappraisal. www.stottconditioning.com technique and aerobic conditioning on Psychological Bulletin 95: 355±386 Swinnen SP, Schmidt RA, Nicholson DE, dancers' technique and aesthetic. Schmidt RA 1991a Frequent augmented Shapiro DC 1990 Information feedback Kinesiology and Medicine for Dance feedback can degrade learning: Evidence for skill acquisition: instantaneous 15: 45±64 and interpretations. In: Requin J, knowledge of results degrades learning. Physicalmind Institute 1999 Stelmach GE, eds. Tutorials in motor Journal of Experimental Psychology: www.themethod.com neuroscience. Kluwer, Dordrecht, Learning, Memory and Cognition Pilates JH 1934 Your Health. Presentation Netherlands 16: 706±716 Dynamics, Incline Village, Nevada Schmidt RA 1991b Motor learning and The Balanced Body Studio 1999 Pilates JH, Miller WJ 1945 Return to Life performance. Human Kinetics, www.pilates.uk.com through Contrology. Presentation Champaign, Illinois The Pilates Center 1999 Dynamics, Incline Village, Nevada Schmidt RA, Lee TD 1999 Motor control and www.thepilatescenter.com Pilates Institute 1999 www.pilates.net learning, 3rd ed. Champaign, Illinois: Winstein CJ, Schmidt RA 1990 Reduced Pilates1 Method 1999 Human Kinetics frequency of knowledge of results www.pilates-studio.com Schmidt RA, Young DE 1987 Transfer of enhances motor skill learning. Polestar1 Education 1999 movement control in motor learning. In: Journal of Experimental Psychology: www.polestareducation.com Cormier SM, Hagman JD, eds. Transfer Learning, Memory and Cognition 16: Robinson L, Thomson G 1997 Body Control: of Learning. Academic Press, Orlando, 677±691 Using Techniques Developed by Joseph H. Florida Pilates. BanBridge Books, Philadelphia 108 J O U R NAL O F B O DY W O R K AN D MOV E M E N T TH E R APIE S APRIL 20 0 0
You can also read