Locked down in distress: a causal estimation of the mental-health fallout from the COVID-19 pandemic in the UK - WP 21/10 - University of York

Page created by Bertha Wright
 
CONTINUE READING
WP 20/

WP 21/10

 Locked down in distress: a causal estimation of the
 mental-health fallout from the COVID-19 pandemic in
 the UK

 Lina Anaya; Peter Howley; Muhammad Waqas and Gaston Yalonetzky

 June 2021

 http://www.york.ac.uk/economics/postgrad/herc/hedg/wps/
Locked down in distress: a causal estimation of the mental-health
 fallout from the COVID-19 pandemic in the UK

 Lina Anaya¹ Peter Howley² Muhammad Waqas³ Gaston Yalonetzky⁴
 University of University of
 University of Leeds University of Leeds
 Bradford Bradford

 Abstract
There is an extensive literature documenting the economic impacts of the Covid-19 pandemic. A
nascent literature is also beginning to detail the mental health impact. This research has, for
instance, told us much regarding the initial impacts of lockdowns and stay-at-home orders for
mental well-being, but a limitation with much of this work is that any reported findings generally
cannot be taken as causal estimates. In this study, we use a large-scale longitudinal survey coupled
with differences-in-differences and a regression-discontinuity design to estimate the impact of the
Covid-19 pandemic on mental health. We find substantive estimated increases in psychological
distress for the population overall but these impacts are not uniformly distributed. Specifically, the
costs in terms of mental health appear to be much more pronounced for females, those with
children, members of the BAME community and migrants. A further particularly important
moderating variable appears to be people’s own subjective assessment as to the adequacy of their
income.

Keywords: well-being; Covid-19; lockdown; UK
JEL: I12, I31, J22

¹ Postdoctoral Research Associate, University of Bradford. Email: l.anaya@bradford.ac.uk

² Professor of Economics and Behavioural Science, Leeds University Business School. Email: p.howley@leeds.ac.uk

³ Lecturer in Economics, University of Bradford. Email: M.Waqas9@bradford.ac.uk

⁴ Lecturer in Economics, Leeds University Business School. Email: G.Yalonetzky@leeds.ac.uk
1. Introduction

 The Covid-19 pandemic has become one of the most significant public health crises of

our time. In response, governments in the UK and elsewhere have taken unprecedented and

perhaps previously unimaginable steps to protect public health. One of the major weapons used

by governments to save as many lives as possible in the short and medium term has been to

impose lockdowns or other less restrictive measures such as social distancing guidelines in order

to minimise the spread of Covid-19. There has been much research quantifying the economic

cost associated with such initiatives coupled with the economic benefit of lives saved (Benzeval

et al., 2020; Goolsbee & Syverson, 2021; Gupta et al., 2020; Miles et al., 2021; Rojas et al.,

2020). In addition to these economic impacts, there are a number of other likely impacts in terms

of mental health and well-being.

 The implementation of physical distancing measures has, for instance, profoundly

impacted the way people live their lives and such changes will inevitably have consequences for

people’s mental well-being. Recognising this issue, there is a nascent literature concerned with

quantifying the mental health burden associated with the pandemic. Such efforts are an important

endeavour, as it is only by ascertaining the full welfare consequences of the pandemic and

associated mitigation strategies such as lockdowns, that we can begin to make more informed

decisions regarding the scope and nature for government intervention when it comes to

responding to this or indeed future pandemics. Additionally, having a better understanding of the

extent to which restrictions placed on people may affect mental health, and perhaps most

importantly for whom, will be essential in informing policies that could help mitigate these

detrimental impacts. In this paper, we employ longitudinal data from the UK Household

Longitudinal Study (UKHLS or Understanding Society) and the UKHLS COVID-19 panel

 2
coupled with a Difference in Difference and Regression Discontinuity research design to

evaluate the impact of the pandemic on mental health.

 A consistent finding in the emerging literature on this topic is that the Covid-19 pandemic

is associated with a substantive rise in psychological distress (see Banks et al., 2021 for a recent

review). This information pertaining to the mental health impacts has come from a wide array of

sources. A number of bespoke surveys have, for instance, been set up to track people’s mental

well-being during the pandemic. This includes the UCL Covid-19 Social Study which has been

collecting mental health and loneliness data from a large sample of UK adults since the start of

the first lockdown (Fancourt et al., 2020) and the USC Understanding America Study (Kapteyn

et al., 2020). The key advantage of these surveys is that they provide high frequency data and are

able to rapidly tailor their design to capture a variety of issues of direct relevance to the

pandemic. The disadvantage is that they do not contain estimates relating to how people felt

before the pandemic which makes it challenging to capture the causal impact of the pandemic.

 A further source of data comes from a number of pre-existing cross sectional or

longitudinal surveys, many of which have adapted their design (and/or data collection strategies)

in order to specifically collect data of relevance to the Covid-19 pandemic. The main advantage

of this approach relative to the bespoke surveys described earlier is that such pre-existing surveys

contain information from people before the pandemic. This means that one can develop a before-

and-after comparison when it comes to the mental health response to the pandemic. As an

example of this approach, in the UK the ONS reported a 29% point rise in those reporting

elevated anxiety between the last quarter of 2019 and March 2020 which gives an indication of

the initial mental health burden of the pandemic (ONS 2020a; 2020b). Similarly, several studies

in the US showed on average significantly higher levels of psychological distress and loneliness

 3
in surveys carried out in 2020 than similar surveys in 2018 (McGinty et al., 2020; Swaziek &

Wozniak, 2020).

 An important feature of this existing research is that it suggests that there may be

significant inequalities in the degree to which different groups will experience poor mental

health in response to the pandemic. In particular, studies have reported that women, ethnic

minorities, young adults, working parents, and people who face financial insecurity have been

disproportionately impacted in their mental well-being (Banks & Xu, 2020; Cheng et al., 2021;

Daly et al., 2020; Etheridge & Spantig, 2020; Giovanis & Ozdamar, 2020; Niedzwiedz et al.,

2021; Swaziek & Wozniak, 2020; Zamarro & Prados, 2021). As an illustration, using data from

the UKHLS, Etheridge & Spantig (2020) showed that women’s average mental wellbeing

declined by 0.25 standard deviations from 2018 to April 2020, which is twice as large as the

well-being loss observed by men during the same period. This result is also consistent with

further studies from the UK (Cheng et al., 2021; Daly et al., 2020; Giovanis & Ozdamar, 2020;

Zamarro & Prados, 2021), as well as evidence from the Worldwide COVID-19 Attitudes and

Beliefs survey data (Buyukkececi, 2021; Cheng et al., 2021).

 Another demographic group that seems to face a disproportionately high burden on their

mental health is young adults. Using data from Understanding Society, Daly et al. (2020)

reported that 18–34-year-olds showed the largest increase in mental health issues during the

pandemic, relative to other socio-demographic groups. There is also some evidence to suggest

that the immigrant population and Black, Asian, and Minority Ethnic (henceforth BAME)

communities, experienced larger declines in mental health in comparison to host country

residents and people from a White ethnic background respectively (Proto & Quintana-Domeque,

2021; Shen & Bartram, 2021).

 4
In summary, the existing evidence suggests that the initial mental health burden from the

pandemic can be substantive, but these estimated impacts are far from uniformly distributed.

While this research has been important, a limitation is that even when data is available before the

pandemic, any estimated effects cannot be taken as causal. The main reason is because these

studies are not able to precisely identify an appropriate counterfactual, namely what would have

happened in the absence of the pandemic. This identification is important as some mental health

measures have been trending downwards in the UK prior to the pandemic and therefore before-

and-after comparisons may overstate the mental health burden (Banks et al., 2021). Additionally,

there are seasonal patterns to reported mental health measures meaning that any reported

difference in mental health pre-and post-pandemic may be partly confounded with seasonal

trends.

 Some recent studies have tackled these causality issues with innovative research designs.

Banks & Xu, 2020, for example, developed estimates related to counterfactual levels of mental

health that would be observable in the absence of the pandemic using UKHLS data spanning

many years pre-pandemic. Using the General Health Questionnaire, as their measure of mental

health, they estimate that average GHQ scores rose by 0.9 points indicating a worsening of

mental health of approximately 0.17 of a standard deviation of the pre-pandemic distribution.

The advantage of this approach is that by modelling counterfactuals it can directly take into

account seasonal, age and gender specific trends. The disadvantage is that results are sensitive to

different model specifications designed to estimate the counterfactual as well as the time period

used to fit the model (Banks & Xu, 2020).

 In an innovative approach, Brodeur et al. (2021) used google trends data to test whether

Covid-19 and the associated lockdowns led to changes in well-being-related search terms. Their

 5
main results came from a Difference-in-Difference (henceforth DiD) estimation that compared

search terms pre-and post-lockdown to the same dates in 2019. They also supplemented this

approach by simultaneously combining a regression discontinuity design (henceforth RDD) and

a DiD model to estimate the immediate impact of lockdowns. They report a significant increase

in search intensity for boredom in Europe and the US during lockdown periods and also

significant increases in searches for loneliness, worry and sadness, while searches for stress,

suicide and divorce fell.

 Our approach is close in spirit to the approach by Brodeur et al. (2021). We also

implement a DiD model and RDD in order to estimate the impact of lockdowns (in this case the

one implemented in the UK on March 23rd 2020) for mental health but with some important

additions. First, by using the UKHLS (as opposed to search terms from google trends) we are

able to take advantage of a more direct measure of mental health, namely the General Health

Questionnaire (commonly referred to as the GHQ-12). Second, we are able to explore

heterogeneity in impact across the population. Finally, we are also able to estimate the immediate

impact of lockdowns on mental health by employing different bandwidths around the lockdown

announcement date. The idea behind this approach is that, by focusing on measurements of

people’s mental health close to the lockdown announcement date, we can compare a more

homogenous sample of individuals who are exposed to relatively similar circumstances right

before and after the lockdown announcement to obtain a cleaner measure of the immediate effect

on mental health.

 Our main findings suggest that people’s mental health may have been severely impacted

by the pandemic. As an illustration, the average estimated well-being loss observed between

March 23rd and May 31st 2020 in the UK (the dates of the first nationwide lockdown) as

 6
compared to the same period in 2019 equates to approximately half the estimated impact of

unemployment for mental health and exceeds that of other commonly observed negative

correlates with well-being such as divorce and widowhood. Given that these are net population

estimates (as opposed to a specific sub-group such as the unemployed) it suggests the initial

utility loss (as proxied by the GHQ-12) associated with the pandemic was substantive. Our

findings are also indicative of significant inequalities in impact with females, those with

children, members of the BAME community, migrants and in particular those who feel under

stress financially much more negatively impacted in terms of their mental health than other

cohorts.

 The rest of the paper proceeds as follows. Section 2 describes our dataset based on the

UK Household Longitudinal Study and its special covid-19 pandemic supplement. Section 3

presents our two estimation strategies: a set of DID models, followed by an augmented set of

models combining RDD with DID features. Section 4 presents our results and discusses its

implications. The paper concludes with some remarks in section 5.

 2. Data

 We employ data from the UK Household Longitudinal Study (UKHLS) also known as

Understanding Society. The UKHLS is a household panel that captures, among other things,

information from adults about their economic and social circumstances, lifestyle, employment,

family relationships, and mental health. Our key outcome variable of mental health contained in

this survey dataset is the 12-item version of the General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-12). This

12-item scale is designed to assess somatic symptoms, anxiety and insomnia, social dysfunction,

and general happiness (Goldberg & Hillier, 1979). It is possibly the most commonly used

 7
measure of subjective (self-reported) well-being (Jackson, 2007). The GHQ offers an advantage

over single question measures of subjective well-being, such as happiness and life satisfaction as

it is based on responses to 12 separate questions.1 Each of the 12 items is scored on a four-point

scale. The overall GHQ score can take values from 0 to 36, with 36 representing the lowest level

of psychological well-being. The higher the score, the more likely it is that respondents are

suffering from some form of psychological distress.

 The UKHLS contains information from approximately 50,000 individuals for the

‘mainstage waves’ 1-8 which were collected from 2009 to 2018. Each wave spans three

overlapping years, albeit the vast majority of interviews take place in the first two years, so that

wave 1 runs from 2009 to 2011, wave 2 from 2010 to 2012 and so on. All adults aged 16 or older

in each household are re-interviewed approximately one year apart, which means we can track

changes in mental health as well as other characteristics of the same people over time. The

sample is weighted to be nationally representative. Beginning in April 2020 (and thereafter

continued monthly), participants of the UKHLS were asked to complete a short online survey on

the impact of the COVID-19 pandemic. This included the General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-

12) as well as socio-demographic characteristics. By including the GHQ-12 in this special

Covid-19 survey2, we are able to track to what extent the mental health of people changed over

the course of the pandemic.

1
 Factor analysis shows that most of the variance within these 12 item measures can be explained by one overall
general factor. In essence the GHQ-12 is unidimensional (Gnambs & Staufenbiel, 2018).
2
 Full details of sample design, response rates and response patterns are given in Institute for Social and Economic
Research (2020).

 8
3. Methodology

 a. Difference-in-difference (DiD) model

 Quantifying the impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on mental health requires an estimate

of the counterfactual, namely how mental health would have changed in the absence of the

pandemic. While one could conduct a simple before-and-after comparison to ascertain how much

people’s mental health changed after the pandemic, such an approach may confound the impact

of the pandemic with seasonal patterns or other trends in mental health measures over time. To

overcome this potential issue, we adopt a differences-in-differences research design. In

implementing this approach, we compare the mental health changes observed for people

interviewed pre and post March 23rd 2020, which is the start date for the first UK lockdown,

with that of those interviewed pre and post the same date in 2019. We select May 31st 2020 as

our end point as that is when the first statewide lockdown in force in the UK ended.

 Our main assumption with this approach is that the pandemic was an unanticipated shock

and that, in the absence of the pandemic, the mental health during the lockdown period between

March 23rd and May 31st of 2020 would have changed identically to that of the period of March

23rd and May 31st of 2019 simply because the interview dates are randomised across individuals

in each survey year. By adopting this approach, any trends such as seasonal patterns should not

impact our results. Upon adopting these time periods we are left with a baseline of 50,812

observations (which shrinks in some alternative specifications depending on the choice of

covariates).

 Table 1 presents the summary statistics for the DiD analytical sample for the periods

between January 1st 2019 and May 31st of 2020. The subjective wellbeing in 2019 is on average

about 25 out of 36 points, while in 2020 it is about 24 points. More than half of the individuals in

 9
our sample are female, between 8 and 10 percent are BAME, and roughly between 7 and 10

percent were not born in the UK. On average, on each period, more than 70 percent of the

participants on each period report their financial situation as living comfortably or doing alright.

 Formally we can write our DiD models as follows:

 (1)
 = 0 + 1 ∗ + 2 + 3 + ′ + 

 Where our dependent variable corresponds to the mental health (GHQ-12) of

individual reported at the beginning of the data collection interview on date . The variable 

is a dummy that takes the value of one after the lockdown date was announced. The variable

 represents a dummy for the 2020 year.

 The vector includes demographic characteristics such as a dummy of female, BAME

(i.e., Black, Asian, and Minority Ethnic), whether the respondent was not born in the UK, and an

indicator of subjective financial situation. Finally, corresponds to the error term,

Our estimate of interest is 1, representing the change in mental health levels as a result of the

implementation of the lockdown policy in 2020. The key assumption in our estimation is that the

mental health levels we observe in 2019 would have been those of 2020 in the absence of

lockdown and that other determinants of the outcome do not behave discontinuously at the cut-

off.

 b. RDD-DID models

 To test for the immediate impact caused by the lockdown we also adopt a regression

discontinuity design (RDD) which identifies the initial structural break in two parametric series

estimated pre-and post-lockdown. Similarly to our DiD estimates we compare these structural

 10
breaks to those that occur over the same period in 2019. Thus, we end up with an RDD-DiD

model that can be written as follows:

 = 0 + 1 ∗ + 2 + 3 + 4 + 5 ∗ + 6 ∗ (2)
 + 7 ∗ ∗ + 

 is a running variable that captures the beginning date of the data collection interview

 for individual . This variable is measured in days elapsed since the lockdown announcement.

It takes negative values if the interview began days before the lockdown and positive values if

the interview began days after. The variable equals zero if the interview began on the date of the

lockdown announcement. Therefore, here our estimate of interest is 1 + 7 ∗ , representing

the change in mental health levels as a result of the implementation of the lockdown policy in

2020. That is, now we are allowing for heterogeneity in the causal estimate of the lockdown

effect due to timing differences between the respective lockdown and initial interview dates.

 One of the key methodological choices in RDD is the bandwidth magnitude (Lee &

Lemieux, 2010) determining the sample sizes of the treated and untreated groups (lockdown and

pre-lockdown experiences in our setting, respectively). The proposed optimal rules for

bandwidths strike a balance between the need for sufficiently large sample sizes (calling for

wider bandwidths) and the need to minimise heterogeneity between the two compared groups

(calling for narrower bandwidths), which in our setting requires prioritising the closest days

before and after the lockdown announcement date. To select the appropriate bandwidth, we

follow optimal bandwidth algorithms developed by Calonico et al. (2017) and Cattaneo &

Vazquez-Bare (2016). These methods feature, inter alia, data-driven bandwidth optimal selection

as well as different bandwidths on either side of the cut-off point. Our preferred model

specification employs the bandwidth of 17.5 days around the threshold since it produces more

 11
conservative estimates and a more balanced sample in observable characteristics (see Table 3

and appendix A1).

 4. Results

 a. DID estimates

Table 2 presents our initial DiD estimates of the impact of the pandemic during the lockdown

period of March 23rd and May 31st of 2020 on mental health. The first column presents the net

mental health impact of the pandemic for the population as a whole. The DiD estimate, namely

our interaction term Interviewed Post Lockdown*Year 2020, attracts a statistically significant

coefficient and suggests that the lockdown or more precisely the mental health impact of the

pandemic during the March 23rd to May 31st of 2020 period is associated with an increase of

0.76 units in psychological distress as measured by the GHQ.

 How large/small is this effect? We can compare it to the impacts of other major life

events on this measure of psychological distress found in the wider literature. It would, for

example, be approximately 40-60% of the estimated disutility associated with unemployment

and at least as large as the typical estimated effects of divorce and widowhood for mental

health.3 Clearly then, the consequences of the pandemic for mental health are substantive, all the

more so considering that these initial estimates relate to the overall population impact, as

opposed to specific sub-groups such as the unemployed.

 After calculating the initial population impact, we tested whether these estimated impacts

were moderated by gender, ethnicity, and migrant status. Additionally, we supplemented our

DiD design with a subjective measure relating to people’s own perception as to the adequacy of

3
 For example, Howley and Knight (2021), Clark and Oswald (1994) and Flint et al. (2013) estimate an impact of
1.58, 1.55 and 2.2 units respectively when it comes to unemployment.

 12
their income. In this subjective measure, respondents are simply asked how well they are

managing their finances these days, where 1 is “Living comfortably”, 2 is “Doing alright”, 3 is

“Just about getting by”, 4 is “Finding it quite difficult”, and 5 is “Finding it very difficult”. In the

analysis that follows, Living comfortably is the reference category. For simplicity, this variable

appears as subjective financial health (SFH).4

 In column 2 to 7 we present the results of three-way interaction models combining each

of our potential moderating variables with our Interview post lockdown*Year 2020 interaction

term. Column 2 shows that Female*Interview post Covid*Year 2020 attracts a statistically

significant coefficient. There is of course uncertainty around these estimates, but they do suggest

that Females endured a higher mental health toll from the pandemic vis-a-vis Males. Likewise,

we find that both migrants (as opposed to natives, see column 4), and members of the BAME

community (as opposed to White, see column 3), were more negatively affected. In column 5 we

can see that people with children experienced worse mental health relative to people without

children.

 Finally we find that people’s own subjective evaluation of how well they are managing

their finances ‘these days’ plays an important moderating role in shaping the psychological

distress associated with the pandemic. Looking at columns 6 and 7 we can see, for example, that

individuals who perceive themselves as doing less well financially are much more negatively

impacted by the pandemic in terms of their mental health. The relationship also appears broadly

monotonic, albeit there looks to be a more considerable jump in psychological distress when we

get to people who report finding it very difficult to manage their finances these days.

4
 We considered also including an objective measure of income. However, there are understandably many missing
observations in the special covid-19 monthly surveys which makes direct comparisons between our pre and
treatment period challenging.

 13
b. RDD-DID estimates

To test for the immediate mental health impact associated with the lockdown enforced on March

23rd we adopted a regression discontinuity design (RDD). To further improve precision we

compare our estimated structural break on March 23rd to that estimated over the same period in

2019 (RDD-DiD estimation). These estimated breaks are visually illustrated in Figures 1 and 2.

We present the associated RDD-DiD estimates in Table 3. Looking first at Figure 1 which

illustrates the predicted mental health for the periods between January and May of 2020 and

2019 we can see that a discontinuity in mental health occurs, which is indicative of higher mental

distress after the lockdown announcement. We observe here, however, that the upward trend in

mental distress started 30 days before the lockdown date. This result is not surprising given that

many of the policies aimed at containing the spread of the virus occurred before the official

lockdown date (i.e., social distancing policies, school closures, cancelation of public events, etc).

 In Table 3, we present the RDD-DiD estimates. Column 1 shows the estimates associated

with the preferred bandwidth of 17.5 days. The initial lockdown was associated with an increase

in psychological distress of 2.33 units. By focusing on the immediate impact we are relying on

roughly 2,000 observations either side of the lockdown announcement, of which 130

observations correspond to the year of 2020 for the period after the lockdown, and so we note the

wide 95-percent confidence interval surrounding this estimate ( =0.19 to 4.4).

 Both the RDD-DiD and DiD measure different types of effect and so, unsurprisingly, the

estimated effects using RDD-DiD are much larger than the DiD estimates in Table 2. The latter

pick up the average mental health impact of the pandemic between March 23rd and May 31st of

2020, whereas the former pick up the immediate effect of the lockdown in the few days

surrounding the lockdown announcement. Because the immediate impact is much larger than the

 14
full impact observed during March and May 2020, this result is indicative perhaps of some

adaptation towards social distancing measures over time. In Table 3 we present our RDD

estimates using different bandwidths and we note the same general picture: a substantive initial

impact of lockdown on people’s mental health.

 5. Conclusions

 In this paper, we use data from the UKHLS waves 9 and 10 and the COVID-19 survey

waves of April and May of 2020 to study the impact of the pandemic on people’s mental health.

We simultaneously employ two quasi-experimental approaches namely a differences-in-

differences and regression discontinuity design. Such an approach allowed us to present a

snapshot of the initial impact of the first lockdown in the UK for people’s mental health as well

as broader impacts of the pandemic over a longer time period. Considering first the period March

23rd to May 31s of 2020 which corresponds to the period of the first national lockdown in the

UK, we find that the pandemic led to an average increase in GHQ scores of 0.76 units. To put

this into context, this would be approximately half the estimated disutility associated with

unemployment and at least as large as the typical estimated impacts associated with other

negative life events such as divorce and widowhood.

 While the net population impact is substantive we find that it masks significant

heterogeneity across groups. We find, for example, that the mental health burden associated with

the pandemic is much more keenly felt by women than men. The presence of children appears to

be another important moderating factor. Additionally, we find that BAME groups and migrants,

as opposed to whites and natives, are much more likely to suffer mental health consequences,

thus reinforcing many pre-existing inequalities. A particularly important factor appears to be

people’s own subjective feelings about their income as we find that the mental health burden

 15
associated with the pandemic for those who feel under financial stress was particularly

pronounced.

 We observe that the initial impact of the first UK lockdown on mental health was

substantive. Specifically, we observed that the initial lockdown led to a 2.33 increase in our

GHQ measure. The differences between our DiD estimates relating to the period March 23rd and

May 31st of 2020 and these estimates relating to the initial impact of lockdown would indicate

that a substantive part of this initial impact may have dissipated over time.

 In summary, our estimates suggest that the initial impact of the pandemic for mental

health was substantive. These impacts were however not uniformly felt with many groups

experiencing much more substantive increases in psychological distress than others. These

estimates, alongside previous research, provide us with rich insights into the initial consequences

of the pandemic for mental health. Going forward, a useful avenue for future work would be to

supplement this study with more information relating to the likely long-term consequences. This

approach might include an examination of the long-term mental health consequences associated

with the economic and social disruption caused by the pandemic. The possibility of long-term

changes to our behaviour associated with living with this disease indefinitely, though in less

lethal forms, may also be a factor that warrants some consideration. Finally, there may be long-

term scarring effects associated with living under these restrictions and/or general anxiety related

to the possibility of future pandemics.

Acknowledgements: This work was funded by the Economic and Social Research Council

(ESRC), as part of UK Research and Innovation’s rapid response to COVID-19. The underlying

data are available upon request, special license data can be acquired from the UK Data Service.

 16
References

Banks, J., Fancourt, D., & Xu, X. (2021). Mental health and the COVID-19 pandemic.

 https://ifs.org.uk/publications/15368

Banks, J., & Xu, X. (2020). The Mental Health Effects of the First Two Months of Lockdown during

 the COVID‐19 Pandemic in the UK. Fiscal Studies, 41(3), 685–708.

 https://doi.org/10.1111/1475-5890.12239

Benzeval, M., Burton, J., Crossley, T., Fisher, P., Jackle, A., Low, H., & Read, B. (2020).

 Understanding Society COVID-19 Survey, April Briefing Note: The Economic Effects.

 https://www.understandingsociety.ac.uk/sites/default/files/downloads/general/ukhls_briefingnote

 _covid_economics_final.pdf

Brodeur, A., Clark, A. E., Fleche, S., & Powdthavee, N. (2021). COVID-19, lockdowns and well-

 being: Evidence from Google Trends. Journal of Public Economics, 193, 104346.

 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpubeco.2020.104346

Buyukkececi, Z. (2021). Cross-country differences in anxiety and behavioral response to the Covid-

 19 pandemic. European Societies, 23(S1), S417–S447.

 https://doi.org/10.1080/14616696.2020.1828975

Calonico, S., Cattaneo, M. D., Farrell, M. H., & Titiunik, R. (2017). Rdrobust: Software for

 regression-discontinuity designs. Stata Journal, 17(2), 372–404.

 https://doi.org/10.1177/1536867x1701700208

Cattaneo, M. D., & Vazquez-Bare, G. (2016). The Choice of Neighborhood in Regression

 Discontinuity Designs. In Observational Studies (Vol. 2).

 17
Cheng, Z., Mendolia, S., Paloyo, A. R., Savage, D. A., & Tani, M. (2021). Working parents, financial

 insecurity, and childcare: mental health in the time of COVID-19 in the UK. Review of

 Economics of the Household, 19(1), 123–144. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11150-020-09538-3.

Clark, A.E. and A.J. Oswald, 1994a, Unhappiness and Unemployment, Economic Journal 104, 648-

 659.

Daly, M., Sutin, A., & Robinson, E. (2020). Longitudinal changes in mental health and the COVID-

 19 pandemic: Evidence from the UK Household Longitudinal Study. Psychological Medicine.

 https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291720004432

Etheridge, B., & Spantig, L. (2020). The gender gap in mental well-being during the Covid-19

 outbreak: evidence from the UK. ISER Working Paper Series.

 https://www.iser.essex.ac.uk/research/publications/working-papers/iser/2020-08

Fancourt, D., Bu, F., Mak, H. W., & Steptoe, A. (2020). COVID-19 Social Study. 15.

 http://allcatsrgrey.org.uk/wp/download/public_health/3d9db5_c99f0f8bb89545a6a10040f27949f

 7f9.pdf

Giovanis, E., & Ozdamar, O. (2020). Income Losses and Subjective Well-Being: Gender and Ethnic

 Inequalities during the Covid-19 Lockdown Period in the UK. https://doi.org/10.21203/rs.3.rs-

 132600/v1

Gnambs, T., & Staufenbiel, T. (2018). The structure of the General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-12):

 two meta-analytic factor analyses. Health Psychology Review, 12(2), 179–194.

 https://doi.org/10.1080/17437199.2018.1426484

 18
Goldberg, D. P., & Hillier, V. F. (1979). A scaled version of the General Health Questionnaire.

 Psychological Medicine, 9(1), 139–145. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291700021644

Goolsbee, A., & Syverson, C. (2021). Fear, lockdown, and diversion: Comparing drivers of pandemic

 economic decline 2020. Journal of Public Economics, 193, 104311.

 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpubeco.2020.104311

Gupta, S., Montenovo, L., Nguyen, T., Rojas, F. L., Schmutte, I., Simon, K., Weinberg, B., & Wing,

 C. (2020). Effects of Social Distancing Policy on Labor Market Outcomes.

 https://doi.org/10.3386/w27280.

Howley, P. and Knight, S. (2021) Staying down with the Joneses: Neighbourhood differences in the

 well-being effects of unemployment. Work, Employment and Society,

 https://doi.org/10.1177/09500170211003483

Jackson, C. (2007). The General Health Questionnaire. Occupational Medicine, 57(1), 79–79.

 https://doi.org/10.1093/occmed/kql169

Kapteyn, A., Angrisani, M., Bennett, D., de Bruin, W. B., Darling, J., Gutsche, T., Liu, Y., Meijer, E.,

 Perez-Arce, F., Schaner, S., Thomas, K., & Weerman, B. (2020). Tracking the Effect of the

 COVID-19 Pandemic on American Households. Survey Research Methods, 14(2), 179–186.

 https://doi.org/10.18148/srm/2020.v14i2.7737

Lee, D. S., & Lemieux, T. (2010). Regression Discontinuity designs in economics. Journal of

 Economic Literature, 48(2), 281–355. https://doi.org/10.1257/jel.48.2.281

McGinty, E. E., Presskreischer, R., Han, H., & Barry, C. L. (2020). Psychological Distress and

 Loneliness Reported by US Adults in 2018 and April 2020. In JAMA - Journal of the American

 19
Medical Association (Vol. 324, Issue 1, pp. 93–94). American Medical Association.

 https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2020.9740

Miles, D. K., Stedman, M., & Heald, A. H. (2021). “Stay at Home, Protect the National Health

 Service, Save Lives”: A cost benefit analysis of the lockdown in the United Kingdom.

 International Journal of Clinical Practice, 75(3), e13674. https://doi.org/10.1111/ijcp.13674

Niedzwiedz, C. L., Green, M. J., Benzeval, M., Campbell, D., Craig, P., Demou, E., Leyland, A.,

 Pearce, A., Thomson, R., Whitley, E., & Katikireddi, S. V. (2021). Mental health and health

 behaviours before and during the initial phase of the COVID-19 lockdown: Longitudinal

 analyses of the UK Household Longitudinal Study. Journal of Epidemiology and Community

 Health, 75(3), 224–231. https://doi.org/10.1136/jech-2020-215060.

ONS, 2020a, ‘Personal and economic well-being in Great Britain: May 2020’, London: Office for

 National Statistics.

ONS, 2020b, “Coronavirus and depression in adults, Great Britain: June 2020”, Office for National

 Statistics.

Proto, E., & Quintana-Domeque, C. (2021). COVID-19 and mental health deterioration by ethnicity

 and gender in the UK. PLOS ONE, 16(1), e0244419.

 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0244419

Rojas, F. L., Jiang, X., Montenovo, L., Simon, K., Weinberg, B., & Wing, C. (2020). Is the Cure

 Worse than the Problem Itself? Immediate Labor Market Effects of COVID-19 Case Rates and

 School Closures in the U.S. https://doi.org/10.3386/w27127

 20
Shen, J., & Bartram, D. (2021). Fare differently, feel differently: mental well-being of UK-born and

 foreign-born working men during the COVID-19 pandemic. European Societies, 23(sup1),

 S370–S383. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616696.2020.1826557

Swaziek, Z., & Wozniak, A. (2020). Disparities Old and New in US Mental Health during the

 COVID‐19 Pandemic*. Fiscal Studies, 41(3), 709–732. https://doi.org/10.1111/1475-

 5890.12244

Zamarro, G., & Prados, M. J. (2021). Gender differences in couples’ division of childcare, work and

 mental health during COVID-19. Review of Economics of the Household, 19(1), 11–40.

 https://doi.org/10.1007/s11150-020-09534-7

 21
Figures and Tables

 Figure 1: Mental distress before and after the lockdown announcement between January and
 May of 2020 and 2019

 22
Figure 2: Mental distress before and after the lockdown announcement around the selected
 bandwidth (17.5 days)

 23
Table 1: Summary statistics of the analytical sample

 DiD Sample
 From January to May
Year 2019 2020

Psychological Distress (GHQ-12) 11.39 12.23
 (5.60) (5.92)
Female (%) 0.55 0.58
 (0.50) (0.49)
BAME (%) 0.07 0.08
 (0.26) (0.28)
Not Born in UK (%) 0.08 0.10
 (0.27) (0.30)
Children in household 0.51 0.48
 (0.92) (0.87)
Subjective Financial Health (SFH): Living
comfortably (%) 0.31 0.35
 (0.46) (0.48)
Subjective Financial Health (SFH): Alright (%) 0.40 0.43
 (0.49) (0.50)
Subjective Financial Health (SFH): Getting By (%) 0.20 0.17
 (0.40) (0.37)
Subjective Financial Health (SFH): Difficult (%) 0.06 0.04
 (0.24) (0.19)
Subjective Financial Health (SFH): Very Difficult (%) 0.02 0.01
 (0.15) (0.12)

Observations 17,918 23,205

Note: statistics represent the mean unless otherwise specified. Standard deviations
 in parenthereses.

 24
Table 2: DiD estimation results of the pandemic effect on mental health

 (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7)
 Psychological Distress (GHQ-12)

Interview Post Lockdown
(IPL)*Year=2020 0.76*** 0.24 0.65*** 0.72*** 0.63*** 0.57*** -0.01
 (0.14) (0.15) (0.14) (0.14) (0.14) (0.14) (0.15)
Interview Post Lockdown (IPL) -0.06 -0.05 -0.05 -0.07 -0.06 -0.00 -0.02
 (0.08) (0.08) (0.08) (0.08) (0.08) (0.08) (0.08)
Year=2020 0.25** 0.25** 0.25** 0.24** 0.23* 0.29*** 0.29***
 (0.12) (0.12) (0.12) (0.12) (0.12) (0.11) (0.11)
Female 1.11*** 1.03***
 (0.08) (0.07)
IPL*Year=2020*Female 0.83*** 0.87***
 (0.10) (0.11)
BAME 0.23 0.23
 (0.16) (0.18)
IPL*Year=2020*BAME 1.12*** 0.61**
 (0.22) (0.25)
Not Born in UK -0.41*** -1.04***
 (0.14) (0.16)
IPL*Year=2020*Not UK born 0.60*** 0.34
 (0.19) (0.22)
Children in household (#) 0.17*** -0.10**
 (0.04) (0.04)
IPL*Year=2020*Children 0.23*** 0.12*
 (0.07) (0.07)
 Robust standard errors in parentheses
 *** p
Table 2 (Cont.): DiD estimation results of the pandemic effect on mental health

 (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7)
 Psychological Distress (GHQ-12)

Subjective Financ Health (SFH): Alright 1.12*** 1.14***
 (0.08) (0.08)
SFH: Getting By 3.28*** 3.30***
 (0.11) (0.11)
SFH: Difficult 6.29*** 6.34***
 (0.21) (0.21)
SFH: Very Difficult 9.20*** 9.28***
 (0.40) (0.40)
IPL*Year=2020*SFH: Alright 0.59*** 0.47***
 (0.10) (0.11)
IPL*Year=2020*SFH: Getting by 0.81*** 0.72***
 (0.15) (0.17)
IPL*Year=2020*SFH: Difficult 0.94*** 0.50
 (0.31) (0.35)
IPL*Year=2020*SFH: Very Difficult 2.43*** 1.30**
 (0.59) (0.64)
Constant 11.42*** 10.80*** 11.40*** 11.45*** 11.33*** 9.69*** 9.23***
 (0.06) (0.07) (0.06) (0.06) (0.06) (0.07) (0.08)

Observations 50,812 50,812 50,812 50,812 41,180 50,748 41,123
R-squared 0.01 0.03 0.01 0.01 0.01 0.13 0.14
 Robust standard errors in parentheses
 *** p
Table 3: RDD-DiD estimation results of the immediate effect of the lockdown on mental health

 (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6)
 Psychological Distress (GHQ-12)
 [-11.77, [-11.77,
 Bandwidth in elapsed days: +/-17.53 +/-10.2 +/-12.29 +/-21.13 12.29] 12.39]

Interview Post Lockdown
(IPL)*Year=2020 2.33** 2.68** 3.31** 1.50 2.64** 2.64**
 (1.09) (1.35) (1.40) (1.16) (1.26) (1.26)
Interview Post Lockdown (IPL) 0.09 -0.09 -0.06 -0.01 0.02 0.02
 (0.37) (0.49) (0.44) (0.33) (0.45) (0.45)
Year=2020 2.73*** 3.46*** 3.86*** 2.78*** 3.20*** 3.20***
 (0.84) (0.85) (1.07) (0.90) (0.88) (0.88)
Running Variable Intervew date 0.01 0.01 0.02 0.02 0.00 0.00
 (0.02) (0.06) (0.04) (0.02) (0.05) (0.05)
Running_Var*Lockdown -0.02 0.03 -0.01 -0.02 0.01 0.01
 (0.03) (0.08) (0.06) (0.03) (0.06) (0.06)
Running_Var*Year=2020 0.15* 0.26* 0.36* 0.15** 0.19 0.19
 (0.08) (0.15) (0.20) (0.06) (0.15) (0.15)
IPL*Year=2020*Running_Var -0.08 -0.28 -0.33 -0.22** -0.16 -0.16
 (0.10) (0.25) (0.24) (0.10) (0.20) (0.20)
Constant 24.66*** 24.66*** 24.72*** 24.72*** 24.64*** 24.64***
 (0.27) (0.36) (0.33) (0.25) (0.34) (0.34)

Observations 4,535 2,522 3,231 5,590 3,056 3,056
R-squared 0.0025 0.003 0.0028 0.0026 0.0026 0.0026
 Robust standard errors in parentheses
 *** p
Appendix

 Table A1: Probit point estimates balance-check of the demographic characteristics between the
 2019 and 2020 sample

 (1) (2) (3) (4)
 Dependent Variable: Year=2020
 Same Bandwidth Above & Below the Cutoff
 Bandwidth in elapsed days: +/-10.2 +/-12.292 +/-17.526 +/-21.131

Female -0.11 -0.08 -0.08 -0.09*
 (0.08) (0.08) (0.06) (0.05)
BAME -0.09 -0.13 0.01 0.07
 (0.18) (0.17) (0.14) (0.12)
Not Born in UK -0.09 -0.09 -0.07 -0.07
 (0.16) (0.15) (0.13) (0.11)
Children in household (#) 0.03 0.04 0.05 0.00
 (0.04) (0.04) (0.03) (0.03)
Subjective Financ Health (SFH): Alright 0.25** 0.21** 0.14* 0.11*
 (0.10) (0.09) (0.08) (0.06)
SFH: Getting By -0.22 -0.07 0.05 0.01
 (0.14) (0.12) (0.09) (0.08)
SFH: Difficult -0.17 -0.26 -0.01 -0.05
 (0.24) (0.22) (0.15) (0.13)
SFH: Very Difficult -0.49 -0.54 -0.40 -0.43*
 (0.40) (0.38) (0.29) (0.24)
Constant -1.67*** -1.72*** -1.70*** -1.58***
 (0.08) (0.08) (0.07) (0.06)

Observations 2,656 3,420 4,793 5,881
 Robust standard errors in parentheses
 *** p
Table A1 (Cont.): Probit point estimates balance-check of the demographic characteristics
 between the 2019 and 2020 sample

 (5) (6) (7) (8)
 Dependent Variable: Year=2020
 Different Bandwidth Above & Below the Cutoff
 [-11.77, [-11.77, [-20.23, [-20.23,
 Bandwidth in elapsed days:
 12.29] 12.39] 21.13] 21.3]

Female -0.09 -0.09 -0.08 -0.08
 (0.08) (0.08) (0.05) (0.05)
BAME -0.13 -0.13 0.08 0.08
 (0.18) (0.18) (0.12) (0.12)
Not Born in UK -0.05 -0.05 -0.06 -0.06
 (0.16) (0.16) (0.12) (0.12)
Children in household (#) 0.05 0.05 0.00 0.00
 (0.04) (0.04) (0.03) (0.03)
Subjective Financ Health (SFH): Alright 0.19** 0.19** 0.11* 0.11*
 (0.09) (0.09) (0.06) (0.06)
SFH: Getting By -0.15 -0.15 0.02 0.02
 (0.13) (0.13) (0.08) (0.08)
SFH: Difficult -0.25 -0.25 -0.03 -0.03
 (0.23) (0.23) (0.13) (0.13)
SFH: Very Difficult -0.52 -0.52 -0.42* -0.42*
 (0.39) (0.39) (0.24) (0.24)
Constant -1.72*** -1.72*** -1.59*** -1.59***
 (0.08) (0.08) (0.06) (0.06)

Observations 3,229 3,229 5,744 5,744
 Robust standard errors in parentheses
 *** p
You can also read