Intra- and intergroup vocal behavior in resident killer whales, Orcinus orca

Page created by Crystal Elliott
 
CONTINUE READING
Intra- and intergroup vocal behavior in resident killer whales,
Orcinus orca
          Brigitte M. Weißa兲
          Department of Behavioural Biology, University of Vienna, A-1090 Vienna, Austria

          Helena Symonds and Paul Spong
          OrcaLab, P.O. Box 258, Alert Bay, B.C., V0N 1A0, Canada

          Friedrich Ladichb兲
          Department of Behavioural Biology, University of Vienna, A-1090 Vienna, Austria
          共Received 13 June 2007; revised 21 September 2007; accepted 25 September 2007兲
          Vocal communication within and between groups of individuals has been described extensively in
          birds and terrestrial mammals, however, little is known about how cetaceans utilize their sounds in
          their natural environment. Resident killer whales, Orcinus orca, live in highly stable matrilines and
          exhibit group-specific vocal dialects. Single call types cannot exclusively be associated with
          particular behaviors and calls are thought to function in group identification and intragroup
          communication. In the present study call usage of three closely related matrilines of the Northern
          resident community was compared in various intra- and intergroup contexts. In two out of the three
          matrilines significant changes in vocal behavior depending both on the presence and identity of
          accompanying whales were found. Most evidently, family-specific call subtypes, as well as aberrant
          and variable calls, were emitted at higher rates, whereas “low arousal” call types were used less in
          the presence of matrilines from different pods, subclans, or clans. Ways in which the observed
          changes may function both in intra- and intergroup communication.
          © 2007 Acoustical Society of America. 关DOI: 10.1121/1.2799907兴
          PACS number共s兲: 43.80.Ka 关WWA兴                                                            Pages: 3710–3716

I. INTRODUCTION                                                            and reflect genetic relatedness 共Barrett-Lennard 2000兲.
                                                                           Closely related matrilines are referred to as “pods” 共Bigg
      Vocal communication within and between groups of in-
                                                                           et al., 1990兲 and share most or all of their call repertoire.
dividuals has been described extensively in birds and terres-
                                                                           Ford 共1991兲 grouped all pods that share any call types or
trial mammals 共birds: e.g., Kroodsma and Miller, 1996; Rad-
                                                                           subtypes into acoustic clans; subclans further define clans
ford, 2004; Beecher and Campbell, 2005; mammals: e.g.,
Seyfarth, 1987; Boughman and Wilkinson, 1998; McComb                       through use of subclan-specific call types. Relative produc-
et al., 2000兲. However, little is known about how cetaceans                tion rates of different call types and whistles vary with broad
utilize their sounds in their natural environment 共Janik,                  behavioral states of the entire group 共Ford, 1989兲, but in
2000兲, in particular, vocal interactions with conspecifics. Vo-            contrast to the AT1 transients, none of the residents’ call
cal signals relate to behavioral contexts in several species,              types correlate exclusively with any particular activity 共Ford,
mainly in humpback whales, Megaptera novaeangliae,                         1989兲. Rather than reflecting behavioral states, the discrete
bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus 共see review by Ty-                 call repertoires are thought to function primarily to maintain
ack, 2000; Janik, 2000兲, southern right whales, Eubalaena                  cohesion and coordinate activities in intragroup contexts
australis 共Clark, 1982兲, and beluga whales, Delphinapterus                 共Ford, 1989, 1991兲.
leucas 共Belikov and Belkovich, 2003兲. Recently, Saulitis                        There is increasing evidence that the individually dis-
et al. 共2005兲 reported context-specific calls also in the AT1              tinct signature whistles of bottlenose dolphins function as
subpopulation of mammal-eating killer whales or orcas 共Or-                 cohesion calls when individuals of a social group are sepa-
cinus orca兲 in southern Alaska. Unlike “transient” orcas, the              rated 共Janik and Slater, 1998; Watwood et al., 2005兲. Signa-
fish-eating, “resident” orcas of the northeast Pacific live in             ture whistles may even facilitate reunions between separated
exceptionally stable matrilineal units 共hereafter termed                   individuals, especially between calves and their mothers
“matrilines”兲, where offspring of both sexes travel with their             共Smolker et al., 1993兲. Similarly, resident orca matrilines
mothers lifelong 共Bigg et al., 1990; Ford et al., 2000兲. They              were recently found to increase the usage of family-specific
are frequently vocal and possess a complex vocal system                    call types immediately after the births of calves, suggesting
with group-specific dialects 共Ford, 1989, 1991; Yurk et al.,               that family-specific call types are of profound importance for
2002兲 that remain stable over decades 共Deecke et al., 2000兲                maintaining cohesion within the matriline, in particular be-
                                                                           tween mothers and their dependent offspring 共Weiß et al.,
a兲                                                                         2006兲. Also, call type matching in vocal exchanges within
  Current address: Konrad Lorenz Forschungsstelle, Fischerau 11, A-4645
  Grünau im Almtal, Austria. Electronic mail: a9400355@unet.univie.ac.at   matrilines suggests that the discrete call types of residents
b兲
  Electronic mail: Friedrich.Ladich@univie.ac.at                           function in intragroup communication 共Miller et al., 2004b兲.

3710   J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 122 共6兲, December 2007         0001-4966/2007/122共6兲/3710/7/$23.00          © 2007 Acoustical Society of America
In social species vocal signals are commonly found to        TABLE I. Life history parameters of the individuals belonging to the three
                                                                  matrilines within the A1 pod in the studied timeframe. ID numbers and
not only serve communication within, but also between
                                                                  demographic data according to Ford et al. 共2000兲.
groups, and call usage and structure frequently change with
the social context 共e.g., Elowson and Snowdon, 1994;              Matriline         ID           Sex           Born–Died         Mother
Smolker and Pepper, 1999; Hopp et al., 2001; Snowdon and
                                                                  A12               A12        Female            1941           unknown
de la Torre, 2002; Baker, 2004; Radford, 2005兲. Group size
                                                                                    A31         Male          1958–1997           A12
and composition are known to affect the use of stereotyped
                                                                                    A33         Male             1971             A12
calls in several highly social species, e.g., African elephants                     A34        Female            1975             A12
共Payne et al., 2003兲 and Northern right whales 共Parks and                           A55         Male             1989             A34
Tyack, 2005兲. Resident orcas are very social and matrilines                         A62        Female            1993             A34
regularly travel and interact together irrespective of related-                     A67       Unknown            1996             A34
ness or degree of call sharing, yet, intergroup communication                       A74       Unknown            2000             A34
has received little attention and has only come into focus        A30               A30        Female            1947             A2
recently. Riesch et al. 共2006兲 described stereotyped whistle                        A6          Male          1964–1999           A30
types that are shared throughout the Northern resident popu-                        A38         Male             1970             A30
lation and potentially serve in vocal communication even                            A39         Male             1975             A30
                                                                                    A50        Female            1984             A30
between members of different acoustic clans. Also, the call
                                                                                    A54        Female            1989             A30
design of several discrete call types suggests that they are
                                                                                    A72       Unknown            1999             A50
long-range communication signals with an active space ex-
                                                                                    A75       Unknown            2001             A54
ceeding by far the distances across which members of a            A36               A36        Female         1947–1997            A1
matriline usually separate 共Miller, 2006兲 and the existence of                      A32         Male             1964             A36
multiple long-range call types suggests a role in intergroup                        A37         Male             1977             A36
communication. We thus suggest a significant role of discrete                       A46         Male             1982             A36
calls not only in intragroup, but also in intergroup, commu-
nication of resident orcas. To test this hypothesis, we ana-
lyzed call use of three Northern resident matrilines in intra-    were within visual range, changes in any of the previous
and intergroup contexts, i.e., matrilines traveling alone or      parameters, as well as times, when the whales passed key
with other matrilines of different relatedness. In particular,    landmarks, were noted.
we tested the following predictions: 共1兲 Call use of focal             Acoustic data were collected with a hydrophone network
matrilines changes with the presence or absence of other          monitored at OrcaLab 24 h a day and year round. Whales
matrilines and 共2兲 changes depend on the identity of the          were recorded on a two-channel audio cassette recorder
other matriline共s兲.                                               共Sony Professional Walkman WM-D6C or Sony TCD-D3兲
                                                                  with up to six radio-transmitting, custom-made hydrophone
                                                                  stations 共overall system frequency response 10 Hz– 15 kHz兲
II. MATERIAL AND METHODS                                          whenever they were vocal 共see Weiß et al., 2006兲. Data col-
                                                                  lection was strictly land based and thus did not interfere with
A. Study animals and data collection                              or disturb the whales.
     Johnstone Strait and adjacent waters off Vancouver Is-
land, British Columbia, form the summer “core area” for the
                                                                  B. Acoustic analyses
Northern resident community of orcas, which consists of
more than 200 individually known orcas in three acoustic               Focal matrilines were frequently observed and recorded
clans 共Bigg et al., 1990兲. The focus in this study was on three   with matrilines from different pods 共closely related
closely related matrilines, A12, A30, and A36, comprising         matrilines兲, subclans, and clans. For investigating the intra-
the most commonly encountered pod, A1 共Ford et al., 2000兲.        and intergroup vocal behavior we selected recordings where
In October 2002, they consisted of 7, 7, and 3 individuals,       focal matrilines were encountered in one of five clearly de-
respectively 共Table I兲.                                           fined social contexts: 共1兲 alone, 共2兲 together with the other
     Visual data were obtained at OrcaLab, located centrally      two A1 matrilines 共“same pod”兲, 共3兲 in the company of
in the study area 共50° 34⬘N and 126° 42⬘W兲, and through a         matrilines belonging to a different pod within the same
network of observers: OrcaLab volunteers stationed at field       acoustic subclan 共“same subclan”兲, 共4兲 in the company of
stations, other independent researchers, and whale watch op-      matrilines belonging to a different subclan within the same
erators. Data from all sources were integrated and summa-         clan 共“other subclan”兲, or 共5兲 in the company of matrilines
rized on a daily basis. The waterways were routinely sur-         belonging to a different clan 共“other clan”兲. “Alone” referred
veyed with spotting scopes; visual observations were done         to situations in which only the focal matriline was seen or
on an opportunistic basis, whenever whales were seen or           heard within the same or adjacent hydrophone range共s兲. A
heard within the vicinity of a station. Upon sighting, the        focal matriline was considered to be in the company of an-
number and identity of individuals 关based on ID catalog           other matriline when both were observed within acoustic
共Ford et al., 2000兲兴, group composition, group cohesion, di-      range of each other, were heading in the same direction and
rection of movement, and behavioral state 共travel, motion-        were engaged in the same behavior. Distances between
less, forage, or socialize兲 were recorded. As long as whales      matrilines traveling in company were estimated with the help

J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 122, No. 6, December 2007                     Weiß et al.: Orca vocal behavior and group composition        3711
TABLE II. Number of samples, calls and recording days of focal matrilines   avoid a bias towards call types of higher amplitude 共see
in varying social contexts.
                                                                            Miller and Tyack, 2001兲. The remaining data were split into
Matriline                 With          n samples     n calls    n days     samples of 100 calls. Preferably, samples were chosen from
                                                                            different recording days. However, because selection criteria
A12                       ...              10           983         7
                                                                            strongly reduced the number of usable samples in some of
A12                   Other pod             6           570         3
A12                  Other subclan          2           181         1       the defined social contexts, we also included recordings with
A12                   Other clan            2           159         1       less than 100, but a minimum of 75 calls. For the same
A30                       ¯                15          1500        14       reason, we sometimes used multiple samples from the same
A30                   Other pod             4           353         3       day, but as widely separated in time as possible and never
A30                  Other subclan          2           186         1
                                                                            more than three to maximize statistical independence of the
A30                   Other clan            6           550         4
A36                       ¯                10          965         7
                                                                            data 共Table II兲. For each sample, we determined percentages
A36                   Other pod             3           258         3       of call use per call 共sub-兲type as well as the call rate 共n
A36                  Other subclan          4           366         2       calls/minute/individual兲 and the number of different call
A36                   Other clan            9           810         6       types used.
A12+ A30+ A36                              8           783         4             Data were analyzed using the SPSS® statistical pro-
                                                                            gram. As data clearly deviated from normal distribution
of landmarks and were typically well below 1000 m. We                       共Shapiro-Wilk, all parameters p ⬍ 0.02兲, they were tested
only used recordings for further analysis during which the                  nonparametrically. Also, data were tested separately for each
spacing, direction of travel, and behavioral states of the in-              matriline, as basic call use differs somewhat between the
volved matrilines were observed from shore or were reported                 three focal matrilines 共Miller and Bain, 2000; Weiß et al.,
from whale watching boats, and that allowed definite attribu-               2006兲. Frequencies of call types and numbers were compared
tion of calls to the matrilines in a defined situation. This                between single focal matrilines and focal matrilines in com-
excluded night-time recordings as well as those where one or                pany using Mann-Whitney-U tests. Because calls could not
more additional matrilines were seen and/or heard within                    be reliably attributed to the producing matriline when all
range of the same hydrophone as the defined matriline共s兲.                   three focal matrilines were recorded together, we did not
The selected recordings were obtained between 1989 and                      compare recordings of the three matrilines together with
2002 except for one recording of the A30 and B7 matrilines,
                                                                            those of the single matrilines, but rather with several aver-
that was obtained in August 2005. The predominant behav-
                                                                            aged samples 共frequency of a given call type for A12
iors were traveling and/or foraging.
     Calls were classified according to Ford 共1987, 1989,                   + A30+ A36 divided by 3兲. In those cases, where call use did
1991兲 by simultaneous acoustic and visual inspection of son-                differ between the single and the company contexts, we con-
agrams, generated with Cool Edit 2000 共Syntrillium Soft-                    ducted Kruskal-Wallis tests to further test for differences in
ware Corporation兲 or Raven 1.2 共Cornell Lab of Ornithol-                    call use depending on the identity of the company. Compari-
ogy兲. Two call subtypes, N5iii and N9iv 共Weiß et al., 2006兲,                sons of call use between each single social context were not
were additionally distinguished because they were family                    feasible due to an n below 5 in 6 of 9 of the company con-
specific to focal matrilines.                                               texts. We did not consider alpha correction for multiple test-
                                                                            ing, because of an increased risk of type-II error due to small
C. Statistical analyses                                                     sample sizes 共Nakagawa, 2004兲. Call types with rates of oc-
     If more than 5% of calls were not both visually and                    currence below 1% in any context were included in the cat-
acoustically recognizable because of poor signal-to-noise ra-               egory “other” for the given matriline. All statistical tests
tio, recordings were excluded from statistical analysis to                  were two-tailed.

                                                                                                           FIG. 1. Call use of all three focal
                                                                                                           matrilines traveling on their own or
                                                                                                           with each other. ab, aberrant and var,
                                                                                                           variable. Bars show median percent-
                                                                                                           age of total calls and first and third
                                                                                                           quartiles. Asterisks mark significance
                                                                                                           levels: * = p ⬍ 0.05; ⫻ = p ⬍ 0.06; and
                                                                                                           n = 8 共alone兲 and 9 共together兲.

3712    J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 122, No. 6, December 2007                            Weiß et al.: Orca vocal behavior and group composition
FIG. 2. Call use of the A12 matriline
                                                                                                 traveling on its own or with whales
                                                                                                 from different pods or 共sub-兲clans. ab.,
                                                                                                 aberrant; var., variable; and imit., imi-
                                                                                                 tation. Bars show median percentage
                                                                                                 of total calls and first and third quar-
                                                                                                 tiles. Asterisks mark significance lev-
                                                                                                 els: * = p ⬍ 0.05; * * = p ⬍ 0.01; and n
                                                                                                 = 10 共alone兲 and 10 共with company兲.

III. RESULTS                                                      the A12 matriline, the family-typical call subtype, N9iv, was
     Altogether, 81 samples totaling 7664 calls of focal          used significantly more often in the presence of other groups,
matrilines on their own or in the company of other groups         which was also the case for variable calls 共Mann-Whitney U
were of sufficient quality for statistical analysis 共Table II兲.   test, N9iv: n = 26, U = 25, p = 0.003, variable: n = 26, U
Call patterns when matrilines were alone were comparable to       = 21.5, p = 0.001兲. On the other hand, N1, N3, N7, N8 and
those described by Miller and Bain 共2000兲 and Weiß et al.         N9i calls made up significantly higher portions of the call
共2006兲 for the three focal matrilines.                            repertoire when the matriline was recorded on its own
     With the exception of N3 calls and N10 calls, mean call      共Mann-Whitney U test, N1: n = 26, U = 41.5, p = 0.041, N3:
use of the single A1 matrilines was very similar to that of the   n = 26, U = 24, p = 0.002, N7: n = 26, U = 33, p = 0.012, N8: n
A1 matrilines traveling together 共Fig. 1兲. N3 calls made up a     = 26, U = 32, p = 0.01, N9i: n = 26, U = 34.5, p = 0.014兲. With
significantly smaller proportion of calls when all three A1       up to 10% change in call use, differences were more pro-
matrilines were together 共Mann-Whitney U test, n = 17, U
                                                                  nounced in the A36 matriline than in the A12 matriline.
= 9, p = 0.008兲, while the use of N10 calls tended to increase
                                                                       The use of some call types also differed depending on
in such situations 共Mann-Whitney U test, n = 17, U = 16, p
= 0.059兲.                                                         which group accompanied a focal matriline 共Fig. 5, Kruskal-
     In the presence of more distantly or unrelated matrilines    Wallis test, A12—aberrant: n = 10, H = 5.595, p = 0.061,
from other pods/clans, the A12 matriline was found to in-         A36—N7: n = 10, H = 5.854, p = 0.054, A36—variable: n
crease the use of the family-typical N5iii, as well as aberrant   = 10, H = 6.014, p = 0.049兲, which suggests that changes in
and imitation calls 共Fig. 2, Mann-Whitney U test, N5iii: n        call use may not only have been affected by the presence of
= 20, U = 22.5, p = 0.035, aberrant: n = 20, U = 20, p = 0.023,   other groups, but also by their identity. However, sample
imitation: n = 20, U = 15, p = 0.007兲. In each case, changes in   sizes in the different conditions were low and more detailed
call use were below 5%. Although we did not find any sig-         analyses were not feasible.
nificant differences in call use of the A30 matriline traveling        Focal matrilines did not differ in their call rate 共n calls/
with or without company 共Fig. 3兲, they used the family typi-      minute/individual兲 nor in the number of different call types
cal N47 call 2–3 times more often in the presence of
                                                                  used in any of the contexts 共Kruskal-Wallis test, all p ⬎ 0.1,
B-subclan for which two samples were available. The A36
                                                                  Mann-Whitney U test, all p ⬎ 0.1兲.
matriline showed a number of distinct changes 共Fig. 4兲. As in

                                                                                                 FIG. 3. Call use of the A30 matriline
                                                                                                 traveling on its own or with whales
                                                                                                 from different pods or 共sub-兲clans. ab.,
                                                                                                 aberrant and var., variable. Bars show
                                                                                                 median percentage of total calls and
                                                                                                 first and third quartiles. n = 15 共alone兲
                                                                                                 and 12 共with company兲.

J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 122, No. 6, December 2007                   Weiß et al.: Orca vocal behavior and group composition        3713
FIG. 4. Call use of the A36 matriline
                                                                                                              traveling on its own or with whales
                                                                                                              from different pods or 共sub-兲clans. ab.,
                                                                                                              aberrant and var., variable. Bars show
                                                                                                              median percentage of total calls and
                                                                                                              first and third quartiles. Asterisks mark
                                                                                                              significance levels: * = p ⬍ 0.05. * *
                                                                                                              = p ⬍ 0.01; and n = 10 共alone兲 and 16
                                                                                                              共with company兲.

IV. DISCUSSION                                                                tent changes were increases in the use of family-specific call
                                                                              subtypes as well as variable and aberrant calls in the pres-
     This study presents evidence that resident orca
                                                                              ence of orcas from other groups. Changes in call use were
matrilines change their vocal behavior in intergroup contexts.
                                                                              typically well below 10% and thus less pronounced than the
Consistent with our predictions, percentages of call types
                                                                              changes observed in call use in another social context, i.e.,
used by focal matrilines depended on the presence or ab-
                                                                              after the birth of a calf 共Weiß et al., 2006兲.
sence of additional matrilines in two out of three studied
                                                                                   The focal matrilines differed considerably in the number
matrilines; findings for the third matriline were also consis-
                                                                              of call types with emission rates affected by the social con-
tent in key respects, though sample sizes were small and the
                                                                              text. The A36 matriline showed significant changes in call
changes were not statistically significant. The most consis-
                                                                              use in half of their repertoire 共7 of 14 call types兲, the A12
                                                                              matriline to a considerably lesser extent 共3 of 15 call types兲,
                                                                              and there were no statistically significant changes at all in the
                                                                              A30 matriline. However, their family typical N47 call was
                                                                              used 2–3 times as often in the presence of B-subclan
                                                                              matrilines and interestingly, changes in the A36s’ acoustic
                                                                              behavior were also strongest in the presence of matrilines
                                                                              from B-subclan.
                                                                                   To some extent, the differences in call use may reflect
                                                                              low sample sizes for some matrilines in some contexts; how-
                                                                              ever, it is also possible that they reflect different social roles
                                                                              arising from differences in associations and movement pat-
                                                                              terns. At least one of the three focal matrilines is present in
                                                                              the study area, almost daily, during each summer and fall
                                                                              共Symonds and Spong, private communication兲. Changes in
                                                                              call use exhibited by the A1 matrilines thus may reflect dif-
                                                                              ferences in the manner in which each of the matrilines re-
                                                                              sponds to intergroup situations involving the other, less com-
                                                                              monly visiting Northern resident groups in the Johnstone
                                                                              Strait area, and they may underscore possible differing social
                                                                              roles within their own pod and community. Just how each of
                                                                              the A1 matrilines performs its role and specifically what
                                                                              these roles might be is beyond the scope of this study, how-
                                                                              ever, the indication that matrilines within a pod have possible
                                                                              different roles should encourage further investigation.
                                                                                   It also seems possible that some of the changes we ob-
                                                                              served reflect differences in the age and sex composition of
                                                                              individual matrilines. Indeed, the three focal matrilines do
                                                                              differ in this respect; the A36 matriline consisted of only one
                                                                              female and her three adult sons prior to the matriarch’s death
FIG. 5. Use of 共a兲 N7 calls and 共b兲 variable calls by the A36 matriline in    in 1997, and since then only of adult males, whereas the
different social contexts 共focal matriline alone, with matrilines from the
                                                                              other two matrilines each added a new generation during the
same subclan, another subclan or another clan兲. Bars show median percent-
age of calls and first and third quartiles. Numbers of samples appear above   study period. Studies of other species show that vocal signals
the bars.                                                                     can convey not only individual or group-specific informa-

3714    J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 122, No. 6, December 2007                               Weiß et al.: Orca vocal behavior and group composition
tion, but also age and sex-specific cues that may remain dis-         functions in intra- and intergroup communication, such as
tinguishable even when transmitted over greater distances             directionality cueing and thus indicating one’s location and
共e.g. Green, 1981; Rendall et al., 2004; Blumstein and Mu-            direction of movement 共Miller, 2002兲.
nos, 2005兲. Animals may respond differently to calls of                    In conclusion, the presence and identity of accompany-
males and females 共e.g., Vicario et al., 2001; Miller et al.,         ing matrilines significantly affected calling behavior of resi-
2004a兲 or may respond differently to signals depending on             dent orca matrilines. The observed changes seem to reflect
their own sex 共Rogers et al., 2006兲. It is plausible, therefore,      call functions in both intragroup, as well as intergroup, com-
that the differences in call use among our focal matrilines           munication, and differences between matrilines hint at pos-
stemmed partly from different age or sex distributions in the         sible different social roles within the community. To get a
focal or the accompanying matrilines. However, as virtually           better understanding of these roles, we will need extensive
nothing is known about age or sex differences in the call use         data sets of individual calling behavior along with precise
of wild orcas, this idea remains purely speculative.                  behavioral observations. Both are currently extremely diffi-
     At least in some call types, changes in use seemed not           cult to obtain, but provide potentially fruitful challenges for
only dependent on the mere presence of nonfocal matrilines,           future research.
but to some degree on whether these nonfocal whales were
                                                                      ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
from a pod within the same subclan, from a different sub-
clan, or from an entirely different clan, i.e., whales that differ         The authors wish to thank Anna Spong and OrcaLab
both in the degree of relatedness and of call type sharing.           assistants for continuous recording and observation efforts.
Although the focal matrilines share almost all call types with        Also, they are grateful for the contributions of observers,
whales within the same subclan, they share few with those             researchers, and whale-watchers in the Johnstone Strait area,
from a different subclan and none with whales from a differ-          especially the late Michael Bigg, Graeme Ellis, and John
ent clan 共Ford, 1987, 1991兲. Studies in other species have            Ford. The study was supported by a doctoral scholarship of
shown that the degree of vocal sharing may play an impor-             the Austrian Academy of Sciences to B.M.W.
tant role in acoustic communication between groups and in-
dividuals. For instance, song sparrows, Melospiza melodia,            Baker, M. C. 共2004兲. “The chorus song of cooperatively breeding laughing
                                                                       kookaburras 共Coraciiformes, Halcyonidae: Dacelo novaeguineae兲: charac-
were more likely to perceive a song as directed at them if the         terization and comparison among groups,” Ethology 110, 21–35.
song was shared than if it was unshared 共Beecher and Camp-            Barrett-Lennard, L. G. 共2000兲. “Population structure and mating patterns of
bell, 2005兲; and great tits, Parus major, responded differ-            killer whales, Orcinus orca, as revealed by DNA analysis,” Ph.D. thesis,
                                                                       University of British Columbia, Vancouver.
ently to song types shared with neighbors and strangers               Beecher, M. D., and Campbell, S. E. 共2005兲. “The role of unshared songs in
共Stoddard, 1996兲. Observations of primates are consistent              singing interactions between neighbouring song sparrows, Anim. Behav.
with those for birds. Lemasson and Hausberger 共2004兲 ob-               70, 1297–1304.
                                                                      Belikov, R. A., and Belkovich, V. M. 共2003兲. “Underwater vocalization of
served more vocal exchanges in Campbell’s monkeys, Cer-
                                                                       the beluga whales 共Delphinapterus leucas兲 in a reproductive gathering
copithecus campbelli, that shared calls and suggested sharing          during different behavioral situations,” Okeanologiya 43, 118–126.
to be important in advertising bonds.                                 Bigg, M. A., Olesiuk, P. F., and Ellis, G. M. 共1990兲. “Social organization and
     Complex vocal signals can serve multiple functions in             genealogy of resident killer whales 共Orcinus orca兲 in the coastal waters of
                                                                       British Columbia and Washington State,” Rep. Int. Whal. Comm., Spec.
inter-group behavior, such as cooperation, inter- and intra-           Issue, 12, 383–405.
sexual assessment between groups 共e.g., Seddon, 2002兲, and            Blumstein, D. T., and Munos, O. 共2005兲. “Individual, age- and sex-specific
call repertoire and complexity are often parameters used in            information is contained in yellow-bellied marmot alarm calls,” Anim.
                                                                       Behav. 69, 353–361.
mate choice 共see McGregor, 1992兲. The higher percentages              Boughman, J. W., and Wilkinson, G. S. 共1998兲. “Greater spear-nosed bats
of variable, aberrant and in part also imitation calls in inter-       discriminate group mates by vocalizations,” Anim. Behav. 55, 1717–1732.
group contexts may reflect some of these vital aspects of             Clark, C. W. 共1982兲. “The acoustic repertoire of the southern right whale, a
social interactions. Variable and aberrant calls have been as-         quantitative analysis,” Anim. Behav. 30, 1060–1071.
                                                                      Deecke, V. B., Ford, J. K. B., and Spong, P. 共2000兲. “Dialect change in
sociated with situations of high arousal, e.g. during socializ-        resident killer whales 共Orcinus orca兲: Implications for vocal learning and
ing 共Ford, 1989, 1991兲 and may reflect a more sexually                  cultural transmission,” Anim. Behav. 60, 629–638.
charged situation, since whales from disparate groups are             Elowson, A. M., and Snowdon, C. T. 共1994兲. “Pygmy marmosets, Cebuella
                                                                        pygmaea, modify vocal structure in response to changed social environ-
more likely to mate 共Barrett-Lennard, 2000兲. Ford 共1989兲               ment,” Anim. Behav. 47, 1267–1277.
also reported whales to be highly vocal when engaged in               Ford, J. K. B. 共1987兲. “A catalogue of underwater calls produced by killer
socializing 共i.e. showing physical contact, aerial displays            whales 共Orcinus orca兲 in British Columbia,” Canadian Data Report of
                                                                       Fisheries and Aquatic Science No. 633.
etc.兲. However, the mere presence of other groups that were
                                                                      Ford, J. K. B. 共1989兲. “Acoustic behaviour of resident killer whales 共Orcinus
not engaged in active socializing, did not lead to an increase         orca兲 off Vancouver Island, British Columbia,” Can. J. Zool. 67, 727–745.
in the call rates of our focal matrilines. In contrast to states of   Ford, J. K. B. 共1991兲. “Vocal traditions among resident killer whales 共Orci-
high arousal, “low arousal” calls like N3 and N7 calls are             nus orca兲 in coastal waters of British Columbia,” Can. J. Zool. 69, 1454–
                                                                       1483.
heard most often during behaviours such as resting 共Ford,             Ford, J. K. B., Ellis, G. M., and Balcomb, K. C. 共2000兲. Killer Whales: The
1989; Symonds and Spong, private communication兲, so their              Natural History and Genealogy of Orcinus Orca in British Columbia and
tendency to occur more frequently in matrilines on their own           Washington, 2nd ed. 共UBC Press, Vancouver兲.
                                                                      Green, S. M. 共1981兲. “Sex differences and age gradations in vocalizations of
might reflect a similar state of “low arousal” in this particular
                                                                        Japanese and lion-tailed monkeys 共Macaca fuscata and Macaca silenus兲,”
social context. Finally, the lack of changes in the majority of         Am. Zool. 21, 165–183.
discrete call types may indicate that they serve the same             Hopp, S. L., Jablonski, P., and Brown, J. L. 共2001兲. “Recognition of group

J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 122, No. 6, December 2007                        Weiß et al.: Orca vocal behavior and group composition             3715
membership by voice in Mexican jays, Aphelocoma ultramarina,” Anim.              Anim. Behav. 70, 1227–1234.
  Behav. 62, 297–303.                                                            Rendall, D., Owren, M. J., Weerts, E., and Hienz, R. D. 共2004兲. “Sex dif-
Janik, V. M. 共2000兲. “Food-related bray calls in wild bottlenose dolphins          ferences in the acoustic structure of vowel-like grunt vocalizations in ba-
  共Tursiops truncatus兲,” Proc. R. Soc. London, Ser. B 267, 923–927.                boons and their perceptual discrimination by baboon listeners,” J. Acoust.
Janik, V. M., and Slater, P. J. B. 共1998兲. “Context-specific use suggests that     Soc. Am. 115, 411–421.
  bottlenose dolphin signature whistles are cohesion calls,” Anim. Behav.        Riesch, R., Ford, J. K. B., and Thomsen, F. 共2006兲. “Stability and group
  56, 829–838.                                                                     specificity of stereotyped whistles in resident killer whales, Orcinus orca,
Kroodsma, D. E., and Miller, E. H. 共1996兲. Ecology and evolution of acous-         off British Columbia,” Anim. Behav. 71, 79–91.
  tic communication in Birds 共Cornell University Press, Ithaca, N.Y.兲.           Rogers, A. C., Mulder, R. A., and Langmore, N. E. 共2006兲. “Duet duels: sex
Lammers, M. O., Schotten, M., and Au, W. W. L. 共2006兲. “The spatial                differences in song matching in duetting eastern whipbirds,” Anim. Behav.
  context of free-ranging Hawaiian spinner dolphins 共Stenella longirostris兲        72, 53–61.
  producing acoustic signals,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 119, 1244–1250.               Saulitis, E. L., Matkin, C. O., and Fay, F. H. 共2005兲. “Vocal repertoire and
Lemasson, A., and Hausberger, M. 共2004兲. “Patterns of vocal sharing and            acoustic behavior of the isolated AT1 killer whale subpopulation in south-
  social dynamics in a captive group of Campbell’s monkeys 共Cercopithecus          ern Alaska,” Can. J. Zool. 83, 1015–1029.
  campbelli campbelli兲,” J. Comp. Psych. 118, 347–359.                           Seddon, N. 共2002兲. “The structure, context and possible functions of solo,
McComb, K., Moss, C., Sayialel, S., and Baker, L. 共2000兲. “Unusually               duets and choruses in the subdesert mesite 共Monias benschi兲,” Behaviour
  extensive networks of vocal recognition in African elephants,” Anim. Be-         139, 645–676.
  hav. 59, 1103–1109.                                                            Seyfarth, R. M. 共1987兲. “Vocal communication and its relation to language,”
McGregor, P. K. 共1992兲. Playback and studies of animal communication,”             in Primate Societies, edited by B. B. Smuts, D. L. Cheney, R. M. Seyfarth,
  NATO ASI Series, Series A: Life Sciences, Vol. 228 共Plenum, New York兲.           R. W. Wrangham, and T. T. Struhsaker 共University of Chicago Press,
Miller, P. J. O. 共2002兲. “Mixed-directionality of killer whale stereotyped         Chicago兲, pp. 440–451.
  calls: a direction of movement cue?,” Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 52, 262–
                                                                                 Smolker, R. A., Mann, J., and Smuts, B. B. 共1993兲. “Use of signature
  270.
                                                                                   whistles during separations and reunions by wild bottlenose dolphin moth-
Miller, P. J. O. 共2006兲. “Diversity in sound pressure levels and estimated
                                                                                   ers and infants,” Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 33, 393–402.
  active space of resident killer whale vocalizations,” J. Comp. Physiol. 关A兴
                                                                                 Smolker, R. A., and Pepper, J. W. 共1999兲. “Whistle convergence among
  192, 449–459.
                                                                                   allied male bottlenose dolphins 共Delphinidae, Tursiops sp.兲,” Ethology
Miller, P. J. O., and Bain, D. E. 共2000兲. “Within-pod variation in the sound
                                                                                   105, 595–617.
  production of a pod of killer whales, Orcinus orca,” Anim. Behav. 60,
                                                                                 Snowdon, C. T., and de la Torre, S. 共2002兲. “Multiple environmental con-
  617–628.
Miller, P. J. O., and Tyack, P. L. 共2001兲. “Mixed-directionality of O. orca        texts and communication in pygmy marmosets 共Cebuella pygmaea兲,” J.
  stereotyped calls: a design feature promoting social cohesion,” Proceed-         Comp. Psychol. 116, 182–188.
  ings of the 14th Biennal Conference on the Biology of Marine Mammals,          Stoddard, P. K. 共1996兲. “Vocal recognition of neighbors by territorial passe-
  Vancouver.                                                                       rines,” in Ecology and Evolution of Acoustic Communication in Birds,
Miller, C. T., Scarl, J., and Hauser, M. D. 共2004a兲. “Sensory biases underlie      edited by D. E. Kroodsma and E. H. Miller 共Cornell University Press,
  sex differences in tamarin long call structure,” Anim. Behav. 68, 713–720.       Ithaca, NY兲, pp. 356–374.
Miller, P. J. O., Shapiro, A. D., Tyack, P. L., and Solow, A. R. 共2004b兲.        Tyack, P. L. 共2000兲. “Functional aspects of cetacean communication,” in
  “Call-type matching in vocal exchanges of free-ranging resident killer           Cetacean Societies: Field Studies of Dolphins and Whales, edited by J.
  whales, Orcinus orca,” Anim. Behav. 67, 1099–1107.                               Mann, R. C. Connor, P. L. Tyack, and H. Whitehead 共University of Chi-
Nakagawa, S. 共2004兲. “A farewell to Bonferroni: the problems of low sta-           cago Press, Chicago兲, pp. 270–307.
  tistical power and publication bias,” Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 15, 1044–        Vicario, D. S., Naqvi, N. H., and Raksin, J. N. 共2001兲. “Sex differences in
  1045.                                                                            discrimination of vocal communication signals in a songbird,” Anim. Be-
Parks, S. E., and Tyack, P. L. 共2005兲. “Sound production by North Atlantic         hav. 61, 805–817.
  right whales 共Eubalaena glacialis兲 in surface active groups,” J. Acoust.       Watwood, S. L., Owen, E. C. G., Tyack, P. L., and Wells, R. S. 共2005兲.
  Soc. Am. 117, 3297–3306.                                                         “Signature whistle use by temporarily restrained and free-swimming
Payne, K. B., Thompson, M., and Kramer, L. 共2003兲. “Elephant calling               bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus,” Anim. Behav. 69, 1373–1386.
  patterns as indicators of group size and composition: The basis for an         Weiß, B. M., Ladich, F., Spong, P., and Symonds, H. 共2006兲. “Vocal behav-
  acoustic monitoring system,” Afr. J. Ecol. 41, 99–107.                           ior of resident killer whale matrilines with newborn calves: The role of
Radford, A. N. 共2004兲. “Vocal Coordination of Group Movement by Green              family signatures,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 119, 627–635.
  Woodhoopoes 共Phoeniculus purpureus兲,” Ethology 110, 11–20.                     Yurk, H., Barrett-Lennard, L., Ford, J. K. B., and Matkin, C. O. 共2002兲.
Radford, A. N. 共2005兲. “Group-specific vocal signatures and neighbour–             “Cultural transmission within maternal lineages: Vocal clans in resident
  stranger discrimination in the cooperatively breeding green woodhoopoe,”         killer whales in southern Alaska,” Anim. Behav. 63, 1103–1119.

3716    J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 122, No. 6, December 2007                                    Weiß et al.: Orca vocal behavior and group composition
You can also read