INSIDIOUS COGNITIVE DECLINE IN CADASIL - KAARINA AMBERLA, MSC; MINNA WA LJAS, MSC; SUSANNA TUOMINEN, MD; OVE ALMKVIST, PHD; MINNA PO YHO NEN, MD; ...

Page created by Louis Thornton
 
CONTINUE READING
Insidious Cognitive Decline in CADASIL
        Kaarina Amberla, MSc; Minna Wäljas, MSc; Susanna Tuominen, MD; Ove Almkvist, PhD;
          Minna Pöyhönen, MD; Seppo Tuisku, MD; Hannu Kalimo, MD; Matti Viitanen, MD

Background and Purpose—Cerebral autosomal-dominant arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoencephalopathy
  (CADASIL) causes repeated ischemic attacks leading to subcortical vascular dementia. The aim of this study was to
  characterize cognitive function in subjects with a C475T (R133C) mutation in the Notch3 gene, leading to CADASIL.
Methods—Prestroke (n⫽13) and poststroke (n⫽13) mutation carriers and mutation carriers with dementia (n⫽8) were
  compared with healthy noncarriers from the same families using a comprehensive set of neuropsychological tests.
Results—Changes in working memory and executive function were observed in the very early phase of the disease before
  transient ischemic attack (TIA) or stroke. Later, in the poststroke phase, the cognitive impairment concerned also mental
  speed and visuospatial ability. Finally, the subjects with dementia had multiple cognitive deficits, which engaged even
  verbal functions, verbal episodic memory, and motor speed. The 2 mutation carrier groups without dementia and the
  controls could be reliably distinguished using 3 tests that assessed working memory/attention, executive function, and
  mental speed. Episodic memory was relatively well-preserved late in the disease.
Conclusion—A deterioration of working memory and executive function was already observed in the prestroke phase,
  which means that cognitive decline may start insidiously before the first onset of symptomatic ischemic episodes.
  (Stroke. 2004;35:1598-1602.)
                    Key Words: CADASIL 䡲 neuropsychology 䡲 vascular diseases 䡲 small vessel disease

C     erebral autosomal-dominant arteriopathy with subcorti-
      cal infarcts and leukoencephalopathy (CADASIL) is a
generalized small-vessel disease caused by mutations in the
                                                                              carriers compared with controls from the same family.6
                                                                              Another study showed in 2 of 8 symptomatic, but not
                                                                              demented, patients a clear cognitive decline already before
Notch3 gene on chromosome 19. The Notch3 gene encodes                         any major vascular event, suggesting early cognitive impair-
Notch3 receptor protein that is expressed in adults only on                   ment.7 In both studies, the pattern of cognitive decline
vascular smooth muscle cells (SMC). Mutated Notch3 in-                        indicated frontal lobe and subcortical involvement, but the
duces destruction of these SMCs with parallel deposition of                   results are somewhat contradictory with respect to how early
granular osmiophilic material (GOM) and fibrosis.1 These                      the cognitive decline becomes manifest. Also, the low num-
arterial changes result in decreased cerebral blood flow                      ber of subjects has hampered the power of these studies.
(CBF), especially in the white matter,2– 4 and consequent                     Larger subject groups in different phases of the disease need
periventricular white matter degeneration and lacunar infarcts                to be examined to get a more reliable description of the
in deep white matter and basal ganglia.1,4 The natural course                 pattern of cognitive decline in CADASIL. We performed a
of CADASIL is variable. Recurrent transient ischemic attacks                  cross-sectional study of 34 genetically confirmed carriers of
(TIA) and strokes are the main manifestations. One third of                   the same Notch3 mutation. We describe the pattern of
the patients have migraine headache and one fifth have mood                   cognitive function in 3 different phases of the disease. We
disorders, these being mostly depression or anxiety.1                         focused on the early signs of cognitive decline and compared
   The cumulative brain lesions lead to insidious cognitive                   nondemented mutation carriers with 15 healthy nonmutation
decline, the progression of which to subcortical dementia is                  carriers from the same kindreds.
still poorly known. There have been only a few studies
concerning the pattern of decline in cognitive functions in the                                 Subjects and Methods
different phases of the disease. In 1 study, no evidence was                  Thirty-four subjects from 8 Finnish families were genetically con-
found for cognitive decline in 5 asymptomatic mutation                        firmed to carry the same C475T (R133C) mutation in exon 3 of the

   Received September 17, 2003; accepted March 18, 2004.
   From the Division of Clinical Geriatrics (K.A., O.A., M.V.), Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden; Tampere University Hospital (M.W.),
Department of Neurosurgery, Finland; Department of Psychology (O.A.), Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden; the Department of Medical
Genetics (M.P.), Family Federation of Finland, Helsinki, Finland; the Keski-Pohjanmaa Central Hospital (S.Tuisku), Kokkola, Finland; the Departments
of Neurology (S.Tuominen) and Pathology (H.K.), Turku University Hospital, Turku, Finland; the Department of Pathology (H.K.), Uppsala University
and University Hospital, Uppsala, Sweden; the Department of Pathology (H.K.), University Hospital of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland; and the Department
of Geriatric Medicine (M.V.), University of Turku, Finland.
   Correspondence to Matti Viitanen, Division of Clinical Geriatrics, Karolinska Institutet, Huddinge University Hospital, S-14186 Stockholm, Sweden.
E-mail matti.viitanen@neurotec.ki.se
   © 2004 American Heart Association, Inc.
  Stroke is available at http://www.strokeaha.org                                                       DOI: 10.1161/01.STR.0000129787.92085.0a

                                                                  1598
                               Downloaded from http://stroke.ahajournals.org/ by guest on October 23, 2015
Amberla et al           Cognitive Impairment in CADASIL            1599

                             TABLE 1. Demographic Characteristics of Healthy Controls and the 3 Mutation
                             Carrier Groups
                                                                                       Subjects

                                                            Controls       Prestroke          Poststroke      Dementia
                             N (female/male)                15 (5/10)      13 (5/8)               13 (4/9)     8 (3/5)
                             Age (y)                       39.5⫾10.8      39.3⫾11.0           48.7⫾6.9       54.5⫾5.8
                             Education (y)                 12.1⫾4.3       10.2⫾3.5                9.3⫾3.5     6.5⫾0.9
                             Migraine (frequency)             2/15           7/13                  9/13         3/8
                             Mood change (frequency)           0             5/13                  6/13         3/8

Notch3 gene. All had typical periventricular white matter changes on                                         Results
T2-weighted magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). The controls
(n⫽15) were genetically confirmed to be negative for the mutation           Demographics
and they showed no pathological findings on MRI. Information                In Table 1, the demographic data for the 4 groups are
about the history of TIA and stroke as well as occurrence of migraine       presented. The mean age of the poststroke and dementia
and mood changes was collected through semistructured interviews            groups was significantly higher (P⬍0.001) than that of the
of the subjects and close informants as well as from medical files.
   The mutation carriers were divided into 3 groups defined by              control and prestroke groups. The latter 2 groups did not
occurrence of clinical ischemic symptoms and dementia. The pre-             differ from each other. The mean level of education differed
stroke group (n⫽13) comprised subjects without TIA, stroke, or              significantly (P⬍0.001) between the dementia group versus
dementia. The poststroke group (n⫽13) included subjects who had             control and prestroke and poststroke groups, but there was no
experienced at least 1 cerebrovascular ischemic symptom but were            significant difference between the latter 3 groups.
not demented. Subjects in the dementia group (n⫽8) fulfilled the
DSM-III-R criteria for dementia and 6 of them had had 1 or more
strokes.                                                                    Impairment in the Cognitive Performance in
                                                                            Different Phases of the Disease
Neuropsychological Assessment                                               The prestroke group performed poorer than controls in 3
The neuropsychological examination included the following tests:            tests: digit span forward (P⬍0.05), digit span backward
similarities, block design, digit symbol, digit span total score, digit     (P⬍0.001), and Rey–Osterreith memory test (P⬍0.001). The
span forwards, and digit span backwards from the Wechsler Adult             poststroke CADASIL subjects performed more poorly than
Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-R),8 letter fluency (FAS),9 Trail-
Making Test, parts A and B (TMTA, TMTB;10 number of correct                 prestroke subjects in 4 tests: number correct responses
responses and log-transformed time score), Luria clock-setting and          (P⬍0.01), log time (P⬍0.01) of TMTB, digit symbol
clock-reading test,11 Rey–Osterreith copying12 and Rey–Osterreith           (P⬍0.001), and the block design test (P⬍0.01). Unexpect-
memory, immediate reproduction,10 Rey Auditory Verbal Learning              edly, the poststroke group performed almost as well as the
Test (RAVLT;10 total learning and 30-minute delayed recall), word           controls.
recall (number of correct responses) and recognition (d-prime-
integrated measure of current and false recognitions) from Stock-              In the dementia group, only 11 of the 21 neuropsycholog-
holm Gerontology Research Center (SGRC),13 and finger-tapping               ical tests could be administered because of the severity of the
for dominant, nondominant, and alternating hands.14                         cognitive impairment. In 8 of these 11 tests, the dementia
   Six mutation carriers and 4 controls were tested in the late 1980s       group differed from all the other groups. Only in a single test,
and early 1990s at the Department of Neurology of Oulu University           clock-setting, did the groups not differ from each other. For
Hospital. The remaining 28 mutation carriers and 11 controls were
examined between 1997 to 1999 at the Department of Neurology of             detailed comparisons between the groups, see Table 2.
Keski-Pohjanmaa Central Hospital and Turku University Hospital by
2 neuropsychologists (K.A. and M.W.). Of the mutation carriers, 14          Discriminant Analysis
had a history of mood changes but none of them had severe                   Using all neuropsychological tests, a stepwise discriminant
depression according to clinical evaluation at the time of the              analysis was performed on control, prestroke, and poststroke
neuropsychological investigation. Nineteen of the 34 mutation car-          groups. This analysis resulted in 2 significant discriminant
riers had a history of migraine and 1 had experienced epileptic
seizures.                                                                   functions (P⬍0.001) in 3 tests: Rey–Osterreith memory, digit
   The joint Ethical Committee of Turku University Hospital and             span backwards, and digit symbol, which are primarily
University of Turku approved the study.                                     associated with executive function, working memory, and
                                                                            mental speed. The first discriminant function explained 79%
Statistical Methods                                                         of the variance and was associated with low performance in
Groups were compared by 1-way ANOVA or with ANCOVA when                     2 tests to approximately the same extent (standardized coef-
possible confounding factors (age and education) were included.
Sheffé test was used for the post-hoc analysis. Because of the             ficients 0.63 for Rey–Osterreith memory and 0.69 for digit
exploratory nature of this study, P⬍0.05 was regarded as a differ-          span backwards). The second discriminant function explained
ence and no correction for multiple testing was performed. Stepwise         21% of the variance and was associated with high perfor-
discriminant analysis with Wilks lambda as criterion was used to find       mance in digit symbol (standardized coefficient ⫺0.74)
the best combination of tests to separate groups of nondemented             compared with the performance in Rey–Osterreith memory
mutation carriers and controls. Cross-validation of classification of
cases was performed using jack-knife procedure (each case is                (0.32) and digit span backwards (0.50). The first discriminant
classified by functions derived from all cases other than that case).       function differentiated the poststroke group from the pre-
All analyses were performed using SPSS version 11.0.                        stroke and control groups by low level of cognitive perfor-
                                Downloaded from http://stroke.ahajournals.org/ by guest on October 23, 2015
1600         Stroke        July 2004

                      TABLE 2. Neuropsychological Test Results (meanⴞSD) in Healthy Controls and Prestroke,
                      Stroke, and Dementia Groups of CADASIL Patients
                                                                                              Subjects

                      Neuropsychological Tests                 Controls           Prestroke              Poststroke          Dementia
                      Similarities                         21.5⫾3.4               21.2⫾3.0               20.9⫾4.1       3    8.0⫾4.9
                      FAS letter fluency                   44.7⫾8.6               36.9⫾12.8              35.8⫾14.6      3   10.0⫾4.2
                      Clock-reading, # corr                    5.0⫾0.0             4.9⫾0.3                4.5⫾1.0       3    3.3⫾0.9
                      Clock-setting, # corr                    4.7⫾0.6             4.4⫾0.8                4.5⫾0.9            3.8⫾1.2
                      R-O copying                          35.2⫾0.8               33.5⫾2.0               32.2⫾4.1               na
                      Block design                         31.9⫾2.8               36.0⫾4.4      3        30.0⫾5.4       3    9.9⫾4.7
                      TMTA, log time (sec)                     1.5⫾0.2             1.5⫾0.2                1.7⫾0.2       3    2.5⫾0.2
                      TMTB, # corr                         24.0⫾0.0               23.8⫾0.8      3        17.5⫾6.6               na
                      TMTB, log time (sec)                     1.9⫾0.2             2.0⫾0.2      3         2.2⫾0.2               na
                      Digit symbol                         42.1⫾10.0              49.0⫾7.6      3        28.0⫾5.3               na
                      Digit span, total                    14.9⫾3.8               12.7⫾2.1               13.4⫾2.0       3    7.1⫾2.8
                      Digit span forward                       6.9⫾1.1    3        6.0⫾0.9                6.2⫾0.4               na
                      Digit span backward                      5.9⫾1.0    3        4.7⫾0.5                4.3⫾0.8               na
                      RAVL learning                        49.1⫾9.3               47.6⫾7.6               43.2⫾9.7               na
                      RAVL retention                       10.9⫾2.5                9.9⫾2.3                8.9⫾2.7               na
                      SGRC recall, # corr                      8.1⫾1.4             7.1⫾1.8                6.8⫾1.0       3    4.8⫾1.0
                      SGRC recognition, d-prime                3.3⫾1.1             3.4⫾0.9                2.6⫾0.9            2.3⫾0.5
                      R-O memory                           27.7⫾2.8       3       20.9⫾5.6               16.8⫾6.5               na
                      Finger-tapping, dominant             57.8⫾10.2              60.2⫾8.2               52.0⫾8.0       3   32.8⫾7.1
                      Finger-tapping, nondominant          50.6⫾7.4               53.0⫾6.0               48.8⫾8.1           40.8⫾14.0
                      Finger-tapping, alternating          87.0⫾13.2              91.0⫾29.0              84.9⫾16.7              na
                         na indicates not assessed; 3, pair-wise difference between the groups; # corr, number of correct reponses; R-O,
                      Rey–Osterreith; TMTA, Trail Making Test A; TMTB, Trail Making Test B; RAVL, Rey Auditory Verbal Learning; SGRC,
                      Stockholm Gerontology Research Center.

mance in general. In the second discriminant function, there                         digit span backwards (P⬍0.01 and P⬍0.05, respectively).
was a contrast between high performance in the test of mental                        Among the prestroke subjects, 3 were misclassified (2 were
speed (digit symbol) and low performance in the 2 tests                              misclassified as controls because they performed as well as
assessing aspects of executive function and working memory                           the those in the control group in the Rey–Osterreith memory
(Rey–Osterreith memory and digit span backwards). This                               test; P⬍0.001). In summary, misclassification of individuals
function primarily differentiated the control group with intact                      was related to patterns of relative performance as demon-
cognitive performance from the prestroke group, which had                            strated by a scatter-plot of all prestroke and poststroke
lower performance in the tests of executive function.                                subjects and the controls in terms of the 2 discriminant
   The combination of the 3 tests (Rey–Osterreith memory,                            functions (Figure 1).
digit span backwards, and digit symbol) correctly classified
85% of all subjects included. After cross-validation, 78%
                                                                                                                      Discussion
were correctly classified (Table 3). When correctly and                              Cognitive decline could be observed very early during the
incorrectly classified subjects were compared, 3 of 15 con-                          course of CADASIL as shown in the prestroke and poststroke
trols were misclassified as prestroke subjects because of                            groups. The performance of the prestroke group was poorer
poorer performance in the Rey–Osterreith memory test and                             than that of the controls in 3 cognitive domains: short-term
                                                                                     memory (digit span forwards), working memory (digit span
TABLE 3. Classification (Original/Cross-Validated) of Subjects                       backwards), and executive function (Rey–Osterreith mem-
According to Discriminant Analysis of CADASIL Subjects
                                                                                     ory), because of poor strategy and planning in completing the
Without Dementia and the Controls
                                                                                     task. The cognitive domain, which distinguished prestroke
                                              Classification                         from the poststroke group, was mental slowing as verified by
Diagnostic Group       Controls           Prestroke      Poststroke       Total      poor results in digit symbol and TMTB time and set-shifting
                                                                                     ability (TMTB, number correct responses). This pattern of
Control subjects         12/11              3/4                 0/0        15
                                                                                     cognitive decline was also confirmed by the discriminant
Prestroke patients        2/3              10/9                 1/1        13
                                                                                     analysis in which tests of working memory (digit span
Poststroke patients       0/0               0/1                13/12       13
                                                                                     backwards), executive function (Rey–Osterreith memory),
                                     Downloaded from http://stroke.ahajournals.org/ by guest on October 23, 2015
Amberla et al      Cognitive Impairment in CADASIL                 1601

                                                                       in basal ganglia.5 These changes are in accordance with the
                                                                       more severe of the cognitive impairment in later phases of
                                                                       CADASIL. The frontal involvement became more accentu-
                                                                       ated in the poststroke subjects, as indicated by poorer perfor-
                                                                       mance in tests measuring mental speed (digit symbol) and
                                                                       set-shifting ability (TMTB, number correct). Also in the
                                                                       subjects with dementia, cognitive impairment still showed
                                                                       predominantly frontal–subcortical involvement, whereas cog-
                                                                       nitive functions associated with posterior regions were less
                                                                       affected. This type of cognitive decline is also seen in other
                                                                       diseases with frontal subcortical involvement, such as other
                                                                       stroke-related dementias, Huntington disease, and multiple
                                                                       sclerosis.22
                                                                          In previous clinical studies of CADASIL, the focus has
                                                                       mostly been on the poststroke and dementia phases of the
                                                                       disease.6,7,23–25 Of the 2 studies mentioned, Taillia et al7
                                                                       showed results concordant with ours. Their 8 symptomatic
Scatter-plot of root values from a discriminant analysis of all
nondemented subjects. *Two controls with the same values.
                                                                       (TIA, stroke, migraine or seizures) CADASIL patients with-
                                                                       out dementia had significantly impaired performance in
                                                                       Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, trail-making test, and Rey–
and mental speed (digit symbol) determined the classification
                                                                       Osterreith copying. Two of them showed cognitive decline
of the prestroke and poststroke subjects and controls.
                                                                       without having had TIA or stroke. This is in accordance with
   It was unexpected that motor speed (finger-tapping domi-
                                                                       the results for our prestroke group. In accordance with our
nant, nondominant, and alternating hands) did not distinguish
                                                                       study, the decline began with deterioration in tests associated
the prestroke and poststroke groups from the control group,
                                                                       with executive function, attention, and mental speed. How-
because motor slowing is considered to be a common
                                                                       ever, the results of Trojano et al6 differ from those of our
symptom in stroke-related dementia disorders.15 In concor-
                                                                       prestroke subjects. In a 2-year follow-up study, Trojano et al6
dance with other vascular dementias, verbal episodic memory
                                                                       did not find any significant differences in cognitive perfor-
was not significantly impaired in the prestroke and poststroke
                                                                       mance between the controls and 5 asymptomatic CADASIL
groups compared with the controls. This is in contrast to its
                                                                       patients.
early appearance in Alzheimer disease.
                                                                          In our study, the groups differed significantly with respect
   The dementia group demonstrated considerable decline in             to age and length of education. The age difference between
multiple cognitive domains, which is in accordance with the            the patient groups is explained by the progressive nature of
clinical picture of CADASIL.16,17 Interestingly, in 3 tests            the disease; therefore, the 2 symptomatic groups are older.
(SGRC word recognition, finger-tapping dominant hand, and              The difference in years of education was caused by a cohort
clock-setting) there was no difference compared with the               effect, with the oldest subjects being less educated as com-
other groups, suggesting that episodic memory, motor speed,            pared with the younger ones. However, when age and
and visuospatial ability are relatively spared late in the             education were controlled for, the group differences remained
disease development. The cognitive impairment in the de-               unchanged. Importantly, the prestroke group and controls did
mentia group corresponded to early frontal–subcortical vas-            not differ for age or education, thus supporting our findings of
cular dementia, in which episodic memory is relatively                 subclinical cognitive impairment in CADASIL without con-
well-preserved, showing predominantly difficulties of re-              founding effects of demographic variables. In our material,
trieval and less decline in encoding and storage. This is in           frequencies of mood change (39%) and migraine (54%) were
agreement with previous research.7,18                                  somewhat higher than reported earlier.17,26 However, none of
   Hypothetically, the pattern of cognitive impairment in              our subjects had depression according to clinical evaluation at
various phases of CADASIL can be associated with dysfunc-              the time of the neuropsychological investigation. Although
tion in certain brain regions. The early decline in Rey–               there is some evidence of cognitive impairment in persons
Osterreith memory and digit span forwards and backwards                with life-long migraine,27,28 in our patients, history of mi-
may reflect a common underlying insufficiency of working               graine or mood change did not significantly associate with
memory and executive function. Experimental studies have               cognitive performance. There was also an overrepresentation
demonstrated an association between prefrontal structures              of men among our subjects (approximately two thirds), which
and working memory capacity.19 Deterioration of executive              deviates from that generally seen in CADASIL.17 The de-
function has also been associated with decreased activity in           mentia group was small and the subjects were severely
the prefrontal cortex.20 Concordantly, the cerebral blood flow         impaired and not able to perform all the tests. This may limit
in frontal white matter is decreased already in the prestroke          the power of comparison with the other groups.
phase and degeneration of the white matter is already detect-             In conclusion, our findings indicate that the cognitive
able by MRI.4,21 In the progression of the disease, white              decline in CADASIL begins before the first TIA or stroke. It
matter lesions were more extended in the periventricular               can be detected as an impaired performance in neuropsycho-
areas. Lacunar infarcts are also seen in deep white matter and         logical tests of working memory, executive function, and
                              Downloaded from http://stroke.ahajournals.org/ by guest on October 23, 2015
1602        Stroke         July 2004

mental speed. The most sensitive tests to detect cognitive                   10. Lezak M. Neuropsychological Assessment. New York: Oxford University
decline in CADASIL appear to reflect frontal subcortical                         Press; 1995.
                                                                             11. Christensen AL. Luria’s Neuropsychological Investigation. Copenhagen:
involvement. Cognitive impairment begins with deterioration                      Munksgaard; 1979.
of executive functions and working memory/attention in the                   12. Rey A. L’examen psychologique dans les cas d’encéphalopathie trau-
asymptomatic prestroke phase, an impairment of mental                            matique. Archives de Psychologie. 1941;28:286 –340.
                                                                             13. Bäckman L, Forsell Y. Episodic memory functioning in a
speed and visuospatial ability are seen in the poststroke                        community-based sample of very old adults with major depression:
phase. Finally, multiple deficits of cognition including verbal                  utilisation of cognitive support. J Abnorm Psychol. 1994;103:361–370.
functions, verbal episodic memory, and motor speed are                       14. Iregren A, Gamberale F, Kjellberg A. SPES: a psychological test system
observed in the patients who fulfill the criteria of dementia.                   to diagnose environmental hazards. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 1996;18:
                                                                                 485– 491.
                                                                             15. Cummings JL. Vascular subcortical dementias: clinical aspects.
                      Acknowledgments                                            Dementia. 1994;5:177–180.
The study was financially supported by Gamla Tjänarinnor, Gros-             16. Chabriat H, Vahedi K, Iba-Zizen MT, Joutel A, Nibbio A, Nagy TG,
chinsky, and Bertil Stohne foundations, the Academy of Finland,                  Krebs MO, Julien J, Dubois B, Ducrocq X, Levasseur M, Homeyer P,
project 50046, Sigrid Juselius Foundation, EVO research Funds of                 Mas JL, Lyon-Caen O, Tournier Lasserve E, Bousser MG. Clinical
Turku University Hospital, Turku University Foundation, Neurology                spectrum of CADASIL: a study of 7 families. Cerebral autosomal
Foundation, Finnish Cultural Foundation. We express our gratitude                dominant arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoencephalopathy.
to Professor Vilho Myllylä, MD and Helinä Mononen, MSc, Depart-                Lancet. 1995;346:934 –939.
ment of Neurology, Oulu University Hospital for their engagement             17. Dichgans M, Mayer M, Uttner I, Bruning R, Muller-Hocker J, Rungger
                                                                                 G, Ebke M, Klockgether T, Gasser T. The phenotypic spectrum of
in our study and for providing us with valuable material
                                                                                 CADASIL: clinical findings in 102 cases. Ann Neurol. 1998;44:731–739.
and information.
                                                                             18. Wentzel C, Darvesh S, MacKnight C, Shea C, Rockwood K. Inter-rater
                                                                                 reliability of the diagnosis of vascular cognitive impairment at a memory
                            References                                           clinic. Neuroepidemiology. 2000;19:186 –193.
 1. Kalimo H, Ruchoux MM, Viitanen M, Kalaria RN. CADASIL: a                 19. Levy R, Goldman-Rakic PS. Segregation of working memory functions
    common form of hereditary arteriopathy causing brain infarcts and            within the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Exp Brain Res. 2000;133:23–32.
    dementia. Brain Pathol. 2002;12:371–384.                                 20. Cabeza R, Nyberg L. Imaging cognition II: an empirical review of 275
 2. Chabriat H, Pappata S, Ostergaard L, Clark CA, Pachot-Clouard M,             PET and fMRI studies. J Cogn Neurosci. 2000;12:1– 47.
    Vahedi K, Jobert A, Le Bihan D, Bousser MG. Cerebral hemodynamics        21. Chabriat H, Pappata S, Poupon C, Clark CA, Vahedi K, Poupon F,
    in CADASIL before and after acetazolamide challenge assessed with            Mangin JF, Pachot-Clouard M, Jobert A, Le Bihan D, Bousser MG.
    MRI bolus tracking. Stroke. 2000;31:1904 –1912.                              Clinical severity in CADASIL related to ultrastructural damage in white
 3. Bruening R, Dichgans M, Berchtenbreiter C, Yousry T, Seelos KC, Wu           matter. In vivo study with diffusion tensor MRI. Stroke. 1999;30:
    RH, Mayer M, Brix G, Reiser M. Cerebral autosomal dominant arteri-           2637–2643.
    opathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoencephalopathy: decrease in    22. Butters MA, Goldstein G, Allen DN, Shemansky WJ. Neuropsycho-
    regional cerebral blood volume in hyperintense subcortical lesions           logical similarities and differences among Huntington’s disease, multiple
    inversely correlates with disability and cognitive performance. Am J         sclerosis, and cortical dementia. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1998;13:
    Neuroradiol. 2001;22:1268 –1274.                                             721–735.
 4. Tuominen S, Miao Q, Kurki T, Tuisku S, Pöyhönen M, Kalimo H,           23. Adair JC, Hart BL, Kornfeld M, Graham GD, Swanda RM, Ptacek LJ,
    Viitanen M, Sipilä HT, Bergman J, Rinne JO. Positron emission               Davis LE. Autosomal dominant cerebral arteriopathy: neuropsychiatric
    tomography examination of cerebral blood flow and glucose metabolism         syndrome in a family. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol.
    in young CADASIL patients. Stroke. 2004;35:1063–1067.                        1998;11:31–39.
 5. Chabriat H, Levy C, Taillia H, Iba-Zizen MT, Vahedi K, Joutel A,         24. Desmond DW, Moroney JT, Lynch T, Chan S, Chin SS, Shungu DC,
    Tournier-Lasserve E, Bousser MG. Patterns of MRI lesions in CADASIL.         Naini AB, Mohr JP. CADASIL in a North American family: clinical
    Neurology. 1998;51:452– 457.                                                 pathologic and radiologic findings. Neurology. 1998;51:844 – 849.
 6. Trojano L, Ragno M, Manca A, Caruso G. A kindred affected by cerebral    25. Harris JG, Filley CM. CADASIL: neuropsychological findings in three
    autosomal dominant arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoen-       generations of an affected family. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2001;7:
    cephalopathy (CADASIL): a 2-year neuropsychological follow-up.               768 –774.
    J Neurol. 1998;245:217–222.                                              26. Desmond DW, Moroney JT, Lynch T, Chan S, Chin SS, Mohr JP. The
 7. Taillia H, Chabriat H, Kurtz A, Verin M, Levy C, Vahedi K, Tournier-         natural history of CADASIL. A pooled analysis of previously published
    Lasserve E, Bousser MG. Cognitive alterations in non-demented                cases. Stroke. 1999;30:1230 –1233.
    CADASIL patients. Cerebrovasc Dis. 1998;8:97–101.                        27. Waldie KE, Hausmann M, Milne BJ, Poulton R. Migraine and cognitive
 8. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Revised: Manual. New          function. A life-course study. Neurology. 2002;59:904 –908.
    York: Harcourt Brace, Jovanovich; 1981.                                  28. Calandre EP, Bembibre J, Arnedo ML, Becerra D. Cognitive disturbances
 9. Spreen O, Benton AL. Neurosensory Center Comprehensive Examination           and regional cerebral blood flow abnormalities in migraine patients: their
    for Aphasia. Victoria, BC: University of Victoria Neuropsychological         relationship with the clinical manifestations of the illness. Cephalalgia.
    Laboratory; 1977.                                                            2002;22:291–302.

                                 Downloaded from http://stroke.ahajournals.org/ by guest on October 23, 2015
Insidious Cognitive Decline in CADASIL
Kaarina Amberla, Minna Wäljas, Susanna Tuominen, Ove Almkvist, Minna Pöyhönen, Seppo
                       Tuisku, Hannu Kalimo and Matti Viitanen

              Stroke. 2004;35:1598-1602; originally published online May 13, 2004;
                           doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000129787.92085.0a
      Stroke is published by the American Heart Association, 7272 Greenville Avenue, Dallas, TX 75231
                    Copyright © 2004 American Heart Association, Inc. All rights reserved.
                               Print ISSN: 0039-2499. Online ISSN: 1524-4628

The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, is located on the
                                       World Wide Web at:
                                 http://stroke.ahajournals.org/content/35/7/1598

 Permissions: Requests for permissions to reproduce figures, tables, or portions of articles originally published
 in Stroke can be obtained via RightsLink, a service of the Copyright Clearance Center, not the Editorial Office.
 Once the online version of the published article for which permission is being requested is located, click
 Request Permissions in the middle column of the Web page under Services. Further information about this
 process is available in the Permissions and Rights Question and Answer document.

 Reprints: Information about reprints can be found online at:
 http://www.lww.com/reprints

 Subscriptions: Information about subscribing to Stroke is online at:
 http://stroke.ahajournals.org//subscriptions/

                     Downloaded from http://stroke.ahajournals.org/ by guest on October 23, 2015
You can also read