INSIDIOUS COGNITIVE DECLINE IN CADASIL - KAARINA AMBERLA, MSC; MINNA WA LJAS, MSC; SUSANNA TUOMINEN, MD; OVE ALMKVIST, PHD; MINNA PO YHO NEN, MD; ...
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
Insidious Cognitive Decline in CADASIL Kaarina Amberla, MSc; Minna Wäljas, MSc; Susanna Tuominen, MD; Ove Almkvist, PhD; Minna Pöyhönen, MD; Seppo Tuisku, MD; Hannu Kalimo, MD; Matti Viitanen, MD Background and Purpose—Cerebral autosomal-dominant arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoencephalopathy (CADASIL) causes repeated ischemic attacks leading to subcortical vascular dementia. The aim of this study was to characterize cognitive function in subjects with a C475T (R133C) mutation in the Notch3 gene, leading to CADASIL. Methods—Prestroke (n⫽13) and poststroke (n⫽13) mutation carriers and mutation carriers with dementia (n⫽8) were compared with healthy noncarriers from the same families using a comprehensive set of neuropsychological tests. Results—Changes in working memory and executive function were observed in the very early phase of the disease before transient ischemic attack (TIA) or stroke. Later, in the poststroke phase, the cognitive impairment concerned also mental speed and visuospatial ability. Finally, the subjects with dementia had multiple cognitive deficits, which engaged even verbal functions, verbal episodic memory, and motor speed. The 2 mutation carrier groups without dementia and the controls could be reliably distinguished using 3 tests that assessed working memory/attention, executive function, and mental speed. Episodic memory was relatively well-preserved late in the disease. Conclusion—A deterioration of working memory and executive function was already observed in the prestroke phase, which means that cognitive decline may start insidiously before the first onset of symptomatic ischemic episodes. (Stroke. 2004;35:1598-1602.) Key Words: CADASIL 䡲 neuropsychology 䡲 vascular diseases 䡲 small vessel disease C erebral autosomal-dominant arteriopathy with subcorti- cal infarcts and leukoencephalopathy (CADASIL) is a generalized small-vessel disease caused by mutations in the carriers compared with controls from the same family.6 Another study showed in 2 of 8 symptomatic, but not demented, patients a clear cognitive decline already before Notch3 gene on chromosome 19. The Notch3 gene encodes any major vascular event, suggesting early cognitive impair- Notch3 receptor protein that is expressed in adults only on ment.7 In both studies, the pattern of cognitive decline vascular smooth muscle cells (SMC). Mutated Notch3 in- indicated frontal lobe and subcortical involvement, but the duces destruction of these SMCs with parallel deposition of results are somewhat contradictory with respect to how early granular osmiophilic material (GOM) and fibrosis.1 These the cognitive decline becomes manifest. Also, the low num- arterial changes result in decreased cerebral blood flow ber of subjects has hampered the power of these studies. (CBF), especially in the white matter,2– 4 and consequent Larger subject groups in different phases of the disease need periventricular white matter degeneration and lacunar infarcts to be examined to get a more reliable description of the in deep white matter and basal ganglia.1,4 The natural course pattern of cognitive decline in CADASIL. We performed a of CADASIL is variable. Recurrent transient ischemic attacks cross-sectional study of 34 genetically confirmed carriers of (TIA) and strokes are the main manifestations. One third of the same Notch3 mutation. We describe the pattern of the patients have migraine headache and one fifth have mood cognitive function in 3 different phases of the disease. We disorders, these being mostly depression or anxiety.1 focused on the early signs of cognitive decline and compared The cumulative brain lesions lead to insidious cognitive nondemented mutation carriers with 15 healthy nonmutation decline, the progression of which to subcortical dementia is carriers from the same kindreds. still poorly known. There have been only a few studies concerning the pattern of decline in cognitive functions in the Subjects and Methods different phases of the disease. In 1 study, no evidence was Thirty-four subjects from 8 Finnish families were genetically con- found for cognitive decline in 5 asymptomatic mutation firmed to carry the same C475T (R133C) mutation in exon 3 of the Received September 17, 2003; accepted March 18, 2004. From the Division of Clinical Geriatrics (K.A., O.A., M.V.), Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden; Tampere University Hospital (M.W.), Department of Neurosurgery, Finland; Department of Psychology (O.A.), Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden; the Department of Medical Genetics (M.P.), Family Federation of Finland, Helsinki, Finland; the Keski-Pohjanmaa Central Hospital (S.Tuisku), Kokkola, Finland; the Departments of Neurology (S.Tuominen) and Pathology (H.K.), Turku University Hospital, Turku, Finland; the Department of Pathology (H.K.), Uppsala University and University Hospital, Uppsala, Sweden; the Department of Pathology (H.K.), University Hospital of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland; and the Department of Geriatric Medicine (M.V.), University of Turku, Finland. Correspondence to Matti Viitanen, Division of Clinical Geriatrics, Karolinska Institutet, Huddinge University Hospital, S-14186 Stockholm, Sweden. E-mail matti.viitanen@neurotec.ki.se © 2004 American Heart Association, Inc. Stroke is available at http://www.strokeaha.org DOI: 10.1161/01.STR.0000129787.92085.0a 1598 Downloaded from http://stroke.ahajournals.org/ by guest on October 23, 2015
Amberla et al Cognitive Impairment in CADASIL 1599 TABLE 1. Demographic Characteristics of Healthy Controls and the 3 Mutation Carrier Groups Subjects Controls Prestroke Poststroke Dementia N (female/male) 15 (5/10) 13 (5/8) 13 (4/9) 8 (3/5) Age (y) 39.5⫾10.8 39.3⫾11.0 48.7⫾6.9 54.5⫾5.8 Education (y) 12.1⫾4.3 10.2⫾3.5 9.3⫾3.5 6.5⫾0.9 Migraine (frequency) 2/15 7/13 9/13 3/8 Mood change (frequency) 0 5/13 6/13 3/8 Notch3 gene. All had typical periventricular white matter changes on Results T2-weighted magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). The controls (n⫽15) were genetically confirmed to be negative for the mutation Demographics and they showed no pathological findings on MRI. Information In Table 1, the demographic data for the 4 groups are about the history of TIA and stroke as well as occurrence of migraine presented. The mean age of the poststroke and dementia and mood changes was collected through semistructured interviews groups was significantly higher (P⬍0.001) than that of the of the subjects and close informants as well as from medical files. The mutation carriers were divided into 3 groups defined by control and prestroke groups. The latter 2 groups did not occurrence of clinical ischemic symptoms and dementia. The pre- differ from each other. The mean level of education differed stroke group (n⫽13) comprised subjects without TIA, stroke, or significantly (P⬍0.001) between the dementia group versus dementia. The poststroke group (n⫽13) included subjects who had control and prestroke and poststroke groups, but there was no experienced at least 1 cerebrovascular ischemic symptom but were significant difference between the latter 3 groups. not demented. Subjects in the dementia group (n⫽8) fulfilled the DSM-III-R criteria for dementia and 6 of them had had 1 or more strokes. Impairment in the Cognitive Performance in Different Phases of the Disease Neuropsychological Assessment The prestroke group performed poorer than controls in 3 The neuropsychological examination included the following tests: tests: digit span forward (P⬍0.05), digit span backward similarities, block design, digit symbol, digit span total score, digit (P⬍0.001), and Rey–Osterreith memory test (P⬍0.001). The span forwards, and digit span backwards from the Wechsler Adult poststroke CADASIL subjects performed more poorly than Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-R),8 letter fluency (FAS),9 Trail- Making Test, parts A and B (TMTA, TMTB;10 number of correct prestroke subjects in 4 tests: number correct responses responses and log-transformed time score), Luria clock-setting and (P⬍0.01), log time (P⬍0.01) of TMTB, digit symbol clock-reading test,11 Rey–Osterreith copying12 and Rey–Osterreith (P⬍0.001), and the block design test (P⬍0.01). Unexpect- memory, immediate reproduction,10 Rey Auditory Verbal Learning edly, the poststroke group performed almost as well as the Test (RAVLT;10 total learning and 30-minute delayed recall), word controls. recall (number of correct responses) and recognition (d-prime- integrated measure of current and false recognitions) from Stock- In the dementia group, only 11 of the 21 neuropsycholog- holm Gerontology Research Center (SGRC),13 and finger-tapping ical tests could be administered because of the severity of the for dominant, nondominant, and alternating hands.14 cognitive impairment. In 8 of these 11 tests, the dementia Six mutation carriers and 4 controls were tested in the late 1980s group differed from all the other groups. Only in a single test, and early 1990s at the Department of Neurology of Oulu University clock-setting, did the groups not differ from each other. For Hospital. The remaining 28 mutation carriers and 11 controls were examined between 1997 to 1999 at the Department of Neurology of detailed comparisons between the groups, see Table 2. Keski-Pohjanmaa Central Hospital and Turku University Hospital by 2 neuropsychologists (K.A. and M.W.). Of the mutation carriers, 14 Discriminant Analysis had a history of mood changes but none of them had severe Using all neuropsychological tests, a stepwise discriminant depression according to clinical evaluation at the time of the analysis was performed on control, prestroke, and poststroke neuropsychological investigation. Nineteen of the 34 mutation car- groups. This analysis resulted in 2 significant discriminant riers had a history of migraine and 1 had experienced epileptic seizures. functions (P⬍0.001) in 3 tests: Rey–Osterreith memory, digit The joint Ethical Committee of Turku University Hospital and span backwards, and digit symbol, which are primarily University of Turku approved the study. associated with executive function, working memory, and mental speed. The first discriminant function explained 79% Statistical Methods of the variance and was associated with low performance in Groups were compared by 1-way ANOVA or with ANCOVA when 2 tests to approximately the same extent (standardized coef- possible confounding factors (age and education) were included. Sheffé test was used for the post-hoc analysis. Because of the ficients 0.63 for Rey–Osterreith memory and 0.69 for digit exploratory nature of this study, P⬍0.05 was regarded as a differ- span backwards). The second discriminant function explained ence and no correction for multiple testing was performed. Stepwise 21% of the variance and was associated with high perfor- discriminant analysis with Wilks lambda as criterion was used to find mance in digit symbol (standardized coefficient ⫺0.74) the best combination of tests to separate groups of nondemented compared with the performance in Rey–Osterreith memory mutation carriers and controls. Cross-validation of classification of cases was performed using jack-knife procedure (each case is (0.32) and digit span backwards (0.50). The first discriminant classified by functions derived from all cases other than that case). function differentiated the poststroke group from the pre- All analyses were performed using SPSS version 11.0. stroke and control groups by low level of cognitive perfor- Downloaded from http://stroke.ahajournals.org/ by guest on October 23, 2015
1600 Stroke July 2004 TABLE 2. Neuropsychological Test Results (meanⴞSD) in Healthy Controls and Prestroke, Stroke, and Dementia Groups of CADASIL Patients Subjects Neuropsychological Tests Controls Prestroke Poststroke Dementia Similarities 21.5⫾3.4 21.2⫾3.0 20.9⫾4.1 3 8.0⫾4.9 FAS letter fluency 44.7⫾8.6 36.9⫾12.8 35.8⫾14.6 3 10.0⫾4.2 Clock-reading, # corr 5.0⫾0.0 4.9⫾0.3 4.5⫾1.0 3 3.3⫾0.9 Clock-setting, # corr 4.7⫾0.6 4.4⫾0.8 4.5⫾0.9 3.8⫾1.2 R-O copying 35.2⫾0.8 33.5⫾2.0 32.2⫾4.1 na Block design 31.9⫾2.8 36.0⫾4.4 3 30.0⫾5.4 3 9.9⫾4.7 TMTA, log time (sec) 1.5⫾0.2 1.5⫾0.2 1.7⫾0.2 3 2.5⫾0.2 TMTB, # corr 24.0⫾0.0 23.8⫾0.8 3 17.5⫾6.6 na TMTB, log time (sec) 1.9⫾0.2 2.0⫾0.2 3 2.2⫾0.2 na Digit symbol 42.1⫾10.0 49.0⫾7.6 3 28.0⫾5.3 na Digit span, total 14.9⫾3.8 12.7⫾2.1 13.4⫾2.0 3 7.1⫾2.8 Digit span forward 6.9⫾1.1 3 6.0⫾0.9 6.2⫾0.4 na Digit span backward 5.9⫾1.0 3 4.7⫾0.5 4.3⫾0.8 na RAVL learning 49.1⫾9.3 47.6⫾7.6 43.2⫾9.7 na RAVL retention 10.9⫾2.5 9.9⫾2.3 8.9⫾2.7 na SGRC recall, # corr 8.1⫾1.4 7.1⫾1.8 6.8⫾1.0 3 4.8⫾1.0 SGRC recognition, d-prime 3.3⫾1.1 3.4⫾0.9 2.6⫾0.9 2.3⫾0.5 R-O memory 27.7⫾2.8 3 20.9⫾5.6 16.8⫾6.5 na Finger-tapping, dominant 57.8⫾10.2 60.2⫾8.2 52.0⫾8.0 3 32.8⫾7.1 Finger-tapping, nondominant 50.6⫾7.4 53.0⫾6.0 48.8⫾8.1 40.8⫾14.0 Finger-tapping, alternating 87.0⫾13.2 91.0⫾29.0 84.9⫾16.7 na na indicates not assessed; 3, pair-wise difference between the groups; # corr, number of correct reponses; R-O, Rey–Osterreith; TMTA, Trail Making Test A; TMTB, Trail Making Test B; RAVL, Rey Auditory Verbal Learning; SGRC, Stockholm Gerontology Research Center. mance in general. In the second discriminant function, there digit span backwards (P⬍0.01 and P⬍0.05, respectively). was a contrast between high performance in the test of mental Among the prestroke subjects, 3 were misclassified (2 were speed (digit symbol) and low performance in the 2 tests misclassified as controls because they performed as well as assessing aspects of executive function and working memory the those in the control group in the Rey–Osterreith memory (Rey–Osterreith memory and digit span backwards). This test; P⬍0.001). In summary, misclassification of individuals function primarily differentiated the control group with intact was related to patterns of relative performance as demon- cognitive performance from the prestroke group, which had strated by a scatter-plot of all prestroke and poststroke lower performance in the tests of executive function. subjects and the controls in terms of the 2 discriminant The combination of the 3 tests (Rey–Osterreith memory, functions (Figure 1). digit span backwards, and digit symbol) correctly classified 85% of all subjects included. After cross-validation, 78% Discussion were correctly classified (Table 3). When correctly and Cognitive decline could be observed very early during the incorrectly classified subjects were compared, 3 of 15 con- course of CADASIL as shown in the prestroke and poststroke trols were misclassified as prestroke subjects because of groups. The performance of the prestroke group was poorer poorer performance in the Rey–Osterreith memory test and than that of the controls in 3 cognitive domains: short-term memory (digit span forwards), working memory (digit span TABLE 3. Classification (Original/Cross-Validated) of Subjects backwards), and executive function (Rey–Osterreith mem- According to Discriminant Analysis of CADASIL Subjects ory), because of poor strategy and planning in completing the Without Dementia and the Controls task. The cognitive domain, which distinguished prestroke Classification from the poststroke group, was mental slowing as verified by Diagnostic Group Controls Prestroke Poststroke Total poor results in digit symbol and TMTB time and set-shifting ability (TMTB, number correct responses). This pattern of Control subjects 12/11 3/4 0/0 15 cognitive decline was also confirmed by the discriminant Prestroke patients 2/3 10/9 1/1 13 analysis in which tests of working memory (digit span Poststroke patients 0/0 0/1 13/12 13 backwards), executive function (Rey–Osterreith memory), Downloaded from http://stroke.ahajournals.org/ by guest on October 23, 2015
Amberla et al Cognitive Impairment in CADASIL 1601 in basal ganglia.5 These changes are in accordance with the more severe of the cognitive impairment in later phases of CADASIL. The frontal involvement became more accentu- ated in the poststroke subjects, as indicated by poorer perfor- mance in tests measuring mental speed (digit symbol) and set-shifting ability (TMTB, number correct). Also in the subjects with dementia, cognitive impairment still showed predominantly frontal–subcortical involvement, whereas cog- nitive functions associated with posterior regions were less affected. This type of cognitive decline is also seen in other diseases with frontal subcortical involvement, such as other stroke-related dementias, Huntington disease, and multiple sclerosis.22 In previous clinical studies of CADASIL, the focus has mostly been on the poststroke and dementia phases of the disease.6,7,23–25 Of the 2 studies mentioned, Taillia et al7 showed results concordant with ours. Their 8 symptomatic Scatter-plot of root values from a discriminant analysis of all nondemented subjects. *Two controls with the same values. (TIA, stroke, migraine or seizures) CADASIL patients with- out dementia had significantly impaired performance in Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, trail-making test, and Rey– and mental speed (digit symbol) determined the classification Osterreith copying. Two of them showed cognitive decline of the prestroke and poststroke subjects and controls. without having had TIA or stroke. This is in accordance with It was unexpected that motor speed (finger-tapping domi- the results for our prestroke group. In accordance with our nant, nondominant, and alternating hands) did not distinguish study, the decline began with deterioration in tests associated the prestroke and poststroke groups from the control group, with executive function, attention, and mental speed. How- because motor slowing is considered to be a common ever, the results of Trojano et al6 differ from those of our symptom in stroke-related dementia disorders.15 In concor- prestroke subjects. In a 2-year follow-up study, Trojano et al6 dance with other vascular dementias, verbal episodic memory did not find any significant differences in cognitive perfor- was not significantly impaired in the prestroke and poststroke mance between the controls and 5 asymptomatic CADASIL groups compared with the controls. This is in contrast to its patients. early appearance in Alzheimer disease. In our study, the groups differed significantly with respect The dementia group demonstrated considerable decline in to age and length of education. The age difference between multiple cognitive domains, which is in accordance with the the patient groups is explained by the progressive nature of clinical picture of CADASIL.16,17 Interestingly, in 3 tests the disease; therefore, the 2 symptomatic groups are older. (SGRC word recognition, finger-tapping dominant hand, and The difference in years of education was caused by a cohort clock-setting) there was no difference compared with the effect, with the oldest subjects being less educated as com- other groups, suggesting that episodic memory, motor speed, pared with the younger ones. However, when age and and visuospatial ability are relatively spared late in the education were controlled for, the group differences remained disease development. The cognitive impairment in the de- unchanged. Importantly, the prestroke group and controls did mentia group corresponded to early frontal–subcortical vas- not differ for age or education, thus supporting our findings of cular dementia, in which episodic memory is relatively subclinical cognitive impairment in CADASIL without con- well-preserved, showing predominantly difficulties of re- founding effects of demographic variables. In our material, trieval and less decline in encoding and storage. This is in frequencies of mood change (39%) and migraine (54%) were agreement with previous research.7,18 somewhat higher than reported earlier.17,26 However, none of Hypothetically, the pattern of cognitive impairment in our subjects had depression according to clinical evaluation at various phases of CADASIL can be associated with dysfunc- the time of the neuropsychological investigation. Although tion in certain brain regions. The early decline in Rey– there is some evidence of cognitive impairment in persons Osterreith memory and digit span forwards and backwards with life-long migraine,27,28 in our patients, history of mi- may reflect a common underlying insufficiency of working graine or mood change did not significantly associate with memory and executive function. Experimental studies have cognitive performance. There was also an overrepresentation demonstrated an association between prefrontal structures of men among our subjects (approximately two thirds), which and working memory capacity.19 Deterioration of executive deviates from that generally seen in CADASIL.17 The de- function has also been associated with decreased activity in mentia group was small and the subjects were severely the prefrontal cortex.20 Concordantly, the cerebral blood flow impaired and not able to perform all the tests. This may limit in frontal white matter is decreased already in the prestroke the power of comparison with the other groups. phase and degeneration of the white matter is already detect- In conclusion, our findings indicate that the cognitive able by MRI.4,21 In the progression of the disease, white decline in CADASIL begins before the first TIA or stroke. It matter lesions were more extended in the periventricular can be detected as an impaired performance in neuropsycho- areas. Lacunar infarcts are also seen in deep white matter and logical tests of working memory, executive function, and Downloaded from http://stroke.ahajournals.org/ by guest on October 23, 2015
1602 Stroke July 2004 mental speed. The most sensitive tests to detect cognitive 10. Lezak M. Neuropsychological Assessment. New York: Oxford University decline in CADASIL appear to reflect frontal subcortical Press; 1995. 11. Christensen AL. Luria’s Neuropsychological Investigation. Copenhagen: involvement. Cognitive impairment begins with deterioration Munksgaard; 1979. of executive functions and working memory/attention in the 12. Rey A. L’examen psychologique dans les cas d’encéphalopathie trau- asymptomatic prestroke phase, an impairment of mental matique. Archives de Psychologie. 1941;28:286 –340. 13. Bäckman L, Forsell Y. Episodic memory functioning in a speed and visuospatial ability are seen in the poststroke community-based sample of very old adults with major depression: phase. Finally, multiple deficits of cognition including verbal utilisation of cognitive support. J Abnorm Psychol. 1994;103:361–370. functions, verbal episodic memory, and motor speed are 14. Iregren A, Gamberale F, Kjellberg A. SPES: a psychological test system observed in the patients who fulfill the criteria of dementia. to diagnose environmental hazards. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 1996;18: 485– 491. 15. Cummings JL. Vascular subcortical dementias: clinical aspects. Acknowledgments Dementia. 1994;5:177–180. The study was financially supported by Gamla Tjänarinnor, Gros- 16. Chabriat H, Vahedi K, Iba-Zizen MT, Joutel A, Nibbio A, Nagy TG, chinsky, and Bertil Stohne foundations, the Academy of Finland, Krebs MO, Julien J, Dubois B, Ducrocq X, Levasseur M, Homeyer P, project 50046, Sigrid Juselius Foundation, EVO research Funds of Mas JL, Lyon-Caen O, Tournier Lasserve E, Bousser MG. Clinical Turku University Hospital, Turku University Foundation, Neurology spectrum of CADASIL: a study of 7 families. Cerebral autosomal Foundation, Finnish Cultural Foundation. We express our gratitude dominant arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoencephalopathy. to Professor Vilho Myllylä, MD and Helinä Mononen, MSc, Depart- Lancet. 1995;346:934 –939. ment of Neurology, Oulu University Hospital for their engagement 17. Dichgans M, Mayer M, Uttner I, Bruning R, Muller-Hocker J, Rungger G, Ebke M, Klockgether T, Gasser T. The phenotypic spectrum of in our study and for providing us with valuable material CADASIL: clinical findings in 102 cases. Ann Neurol. 1998;44:731–739. and information. 18. Wentzel C, Darvesh S, MacKnight C, Shea C, Rockwood K. Inter-rater reliability of the diagnosis of vascular cognitive impairment at a memory References clinic. Neuroepidemiology. 2000;19:186 –193. 1. Kalimo H, Ruchoux MM, Viitanen M, Kalaria RN. CADASIL: a 19. Levy R, Goldman-Rakic PS. Segregation of working memory functions common form of hereditary arteriopathy causing brain infarcts and within the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Exp Brain Res. 2000;133:23–32. dementia. Brain Pathol. 2002;12:371–384. 20. Cabeza R, Nyberg L. Imaging cognition II: an empirical review of 275 2. Chabriat H, Pappata S, Ostergaard L, Clark CA, Pachot-Clouard M, PET and fMRI studies. J Cogn Neurosci. 2000;12:1– 47. Vahedi K, Jobert A, Le Bihan D, Bousser MG. Cerebral hemodynamics 21. Chabriat H, Pappata S, Poupon C, Clark CA, Vahedi K, Poupon F, in CADASIL before and after acetazolamide challenge assessed with Mangin JF, Pachot-Clouard M, Jobert A, Le Bihan D, Bousser MG. MRI bolus tracking. Stroke. 2000;31:1904 –1912. Clinical severity in CADASIL related to ultrastructural damage in white 3. Bruening R, Dichgans M, Berchtenbreiter C, Yousry T, Seelos KC, Wu matter. In vivo study with diffusion tensor MRI. Stroke. 1999;30: RH, Mayer M, Brix G, Reiser M. Cerebral autosomal dominant arteri- 2637–2643. opathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoencephalopathy: decrease in 22. Butters MA, Goldstein G, Allen DN, Shemansky WJ. Neuropsycho- regional cerebral blood volume in hyperintense subcortical lesions logical similarities and differences among Huntington’s disease, multiple inversely correlates with disability and cognitive performance. Am J sclerosis, and cortical dementia. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1998;13: Neuroradiol. 2001;22:1268 –1274. 721–735. 4. Tuominen S, Miao Q, Kurki T, Tuisku S, Pöyhönen M, Kalimo H, 23. Adair JC, Hart BL, Kornfeld M, Graham GD, Swanda RM, Ptacek LJ, Viitanen M, Sipilä HT, Bergman J, Rinne JO. Positron emission Davis LE. Autosomal dominant cerebral arteriopathy: neuropsychiatric tomography examination of cerebral blood flow and glucose metabolism syndrome in a family. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol. in young CADASIL patients. Stroke. 2004;35:1063–1067. 1998;11:31–39. 5. Chabriat H, Levy C, Taillia H, Iba-Zizen MT, Vahedi K, Joutel A, 24. Desmond DW, Moroney JT, Lynch T, Chan S, Chin SS, Shungu DC, Tournier-Lasserve E, Bousser MG. Patterns of MRI lesions in CADASIL. Naini AB, Mohr JP. CADASIL in a North American family: clinical Neurology. 1998;51:452– 457. pathologic and radiologic findings. Neurology. 1998;51:844 – 849. 6. Trojano L, Ragno M, Manca A, Caruso G. A kindred affected by cerebral 25. Harris JG, Filley CM. CADASIL: neuropsychological findings in three autosomal dominant arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoen- generations of an affected family. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2001;7: cephalopathy (CADASIL): a 2-year neuropsychological follow-up. 768 –774. J Neurol. 1998;245:217–222. 26. Desmond DW, Moroney JT, Lynch T, Chan S, Chin SS, Mohr JP. The 7. Taillia H, Chabriat H, Kurtz A, Verin M, Levy C, Vahedi K, Tournier- natural history of CADASIL. A pooled analysis of previously published Lasserve E, Bousser MG. Cognitive alterations in non-demented cases. Stroke. 1999;30:1230 –1233. CADASIL patients. Cerebrovasc Dis. 1998;8:97–101. 27. Waldie KE, Hausmann M, Milne BJ, Poulton R. Migraine and cognitive 8. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Revised: Manual. New function. A life-course study. Neurology. 2002;59:904 –908. York: Harcourt Brace, Jovanovich; 1981. 28. Calandre EP, Bembibre J, Arnedo ML, Becerra D. Cognitive disturbances 9. Spreen O, Benton AL. Neurosensory Center Comprehensive Examination and regional cerebral blood flow abnormalities in migraine patients: their for Aphasia. Victoria, BC: University of Victoria Neuropsychological relationship with the clinical manifestations of the illness. Cephalalgia. Laboratory; 1977. 2002;22:291–302. Downloaded from http://stroke.ahajournals.org/ by guest on October 23, 2015
Insidious Cognitive Decline in CADASIL Kaarina Amberla, Minna Wäljas, Susanna Tuominen, Ove Almkvist, Minna Pöyhönen, Seppo Tuisku, Hannu Kalimo and Matti Viitanen Stroke. 2004;35:1598-1602; originally published online May 13, 2004; doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000129787.92085.0a Stroke is published by the American Heart Association, 7272 Greenville Avenue, Dallas, TX 75231 Copyright © 2004 American Heart Association, Inc. All rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0039-2499. Online ISSN: 1524-4628 The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, is located on the World Wide Web at: http://stroke.ahajournals.org/content/35/7/1598 Permissions: Requests for permissions to reproduce figures, tables, or portions of articles originally published in Stroke can be obtained via RightsLink, a service of the Copyright Clearance Center, not the Editorial Office. Once the online version of the published article for which permission is being requested is located, click Request Permissions in the middle column of the Web page under Services. Further information about this process is available in the Permissions and Rights Question and Answer document. Reprints: Information about reprints can be found online at: http://www.lww.com/reprints Subscriptions: Information about subscribing to Stroke is online at: http://stroke.ahajournals.org//subscriptions/ Downloaded from http://stroke.ahajournals.org/ by guest on October 23, 2015
You can also read