Individual sibling recognition in experimental broods of common tern chicks

Page created by Corey Harmon
 
CONTINUE READING
ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 1999, 58, 375–381
Article No. anbe.1999.1135, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on

           Individual sibling recognition in experimental broods of
                             common tern chicks

                                                     BRIAN G. PALESTIS & JOANNA BURGER
                           Department of Ecology, Evolution and Natural Resources, Rutgers University

                                      (Received 14 January 1999; initial acceptance 23 February 1999;
                                          final acceptance 30 March 1999; MS. number: A8224R)

         Studies of kin recognition in birds have usually examined parent–offspring recognition, while studies of
         sibling recognition are relatively rare. Using choice experiments, we studied the development of sibling
         recognition among common tern, Sterna hirundo, chicks and tested the cues used for recognition. We
         collected newly hatched common tern chicks and raised them in a laboratory in 10 artificial broods of
         three. Chicks showed a significant preference for broodmates (‘siblings’) over familiar nonsiblings
         (nonsiblings from neighbouring broods) when first tested at 4 days posthatching, earlier than previously
         reported. Preferential approach to siblings was most common in broods with low levels of intrabrood
         aggression. Responsiveness of test chicks was highest when test chicks and stimulus chicks could both see
         and hear each other and lowest when they could only hear each other. Sibling-biased approach did not
         depend on stimulus and test chicks seeing each other, only on test chicks seeing (and probably hearing)
         stimulus chicks. Surprisingly, no preference was shown for siblings over strange nonsiblings, suggesting
         that a preference for siblings may involve learning the identity of not only siblings, but also of chicks
         from neighbouring broods.
                                                                                                1999 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour

Kin recognition has been a major focus of behavioural                                 discriminate between nestmates and non-nestmates in
research during the past two decades, and the ability to                              the laboratory, and between recorded sibling and non-
discriminate kin from nonkin has been demonstrated in                                 sibling begging calls in the field. Tern chicks are highly
many species of mammals, birds, anuran amphibians,                                    variable in appearance, particularly in down coloration
social insects, colonial invertebrates and others (Fletcher                           (Buckley & Buckley 1970), and this variation may be used
& Michener 1987; Pfennig & Sherman 1995). Discrimi-                                   by parents to recognize offspring (Shugart 1990). Multiple
nation of kin from nonkin can be important in determin-                               cues and sensory modalities are often used in recognition
ing the distribution of positive and negative interactions                            (Colgan 1983; Halpin 1991). It is therefore possible that
among members of social groups. However, avian kin                                    visual cues also play a role in common tern sibling
recognition studies have focused largely on parent–                                   recognition. Burger et al. (1988), although originally
offspring recognition, and thus studies of recognition of                             demonstrating discrimination among chicks that could
siblings or other collateral kin by birds are relatively rare                         both see and hear each other, only tested the importance
(reviews in Evans 1980; Beecher 1988; Halpin 1991).                                   of auditory cues.
   Tests for sibling discrimination among the Laridae                                    Sibling recognition in colonial birds may help chicks to
(gulls and terns) have consistently demonstrated its                                  locate their own nests quickly, thus avoiding aggression
presence (Evans 1970; Noseworthy & Lien 1976; Burger                                  from unrelated adults and avoiding missed opportunities
et al. 1988; Pierotti et al. 1988; Burger 1998). Preferences                          for parental feeding, in an environment where many
for siblings are learned, as is typical of avian kin recog-                           nests (and thus broods) are found within a small area
nition (Beecher 1988), thus chicks appear to treat nest-                              (Evans 1970; Noseworthy & Lien 1976; Beecher &
mates as siblings regardless of actual relatedness (Burger                            Beecher 1983; unpublished data). Sibling recognition,
et al. 1988; Pierotti et al. 1988). Burger et al. (1988)                              therefore, should develop before the age at which young
demonstrated that common tern, Sterna hirundo, chicks                                 begin wandering from the nest (Evans 1970; Beecher &
                                                                                      Beecher 1983; see also Holmes & Sherman 1982). In
Correspondence: B. G. Palestis, Rutgers University, Department of                     ground-nesting species, like the common tern, the poten-
Ecology, Evolution and Natural Resources, Nelson Biological Labs,                     tial for wandering into a neighbouring territory is high,
604 Allison Road, Piscataway, NJ 08854-8082, U.S.A. (email:                           and thus this proposed benefit of sibling discrimination
palestis@eden.rutgers.edu).                                                           should be particularly important. We have demonstrated
0003–3472/99/080375+07 $30.00/0                                                 375                 1999 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour
376   ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 58, 2

      (unpublished data) that 4-day-old common tern chicks in          We placed the cages containing the chicks in a chick
      the field are better able to locate their natal nests when    brooder set to 30C for the first week of captivity. After
      siblings are present in the nest than when chicks have no     the first week, we kept chicks warm with space heaters.
      siblings or when siblings have been temporarily removed,      Each cage contained paper bedding, which we changed
      moved to a neighbouring nest, or replaced by a non-           daily, and was always adjacent to the same neighbouring
      sibling. Other proposed benefits of sibling discrimination    cages. Chicks could both see and hear chicks in neigh-
      among larid chicks include prevention of adoption of          bouring broods (typically two or three broods), but could
      unrelated chicks (Holley 1988; but see Pierotti et al.        not see chicks in the remaining broods, separated at first
      1988), avoidance of aggression from nonsiblings (Burger       by the shelves of the brooder and later by cardboard
      1998; Burger et al. 1988), maintenance of cohesive family     barriers.
      groups for efficient provisioning of food (Evans 1970,           We fed the chicks five times per day by hand, with bait
      1980), and approach to sibling begging calls to maximize      fish species (Fundulus spp. and Menidia menidia) that are a
      food intake (Burger et al. 1988). These proposed benefits     part of the natural diet of tern chicks. We weighed the
      of sibling recognition also suggest that discrimination is    chicks periodically, and most grew throughout the study.
      adaptive at an early age. However, Burger et al. (1988)       Any chicks that stopped growing or were in poor con-
      found no evidence for sibling discrimination in common        dition were separated from their broods, and we no longer
      terns at 8 or 9 days of age, when chicks are already highly   used them as either test chicks or stimulus chicks in the
      mobile, but did demonstrate sibling discrimination            experiments outlined below. We aborted additional
      among 12-day-old chicks.                                      experiments when several chicks developed a thiamine
        In this study we confirm that common tern chicks            (vitamin B1) deficiency. Supplementation of the diet with
      learn the identity of their nestmates (‘siblings’) and        crushed vitamin B1 tablets prevented further cases, and
      preferentially approach nestmates over non-nestmates,         led to the rapid recovery of some affected chicks (B. G.
      and we demonstrate that this preference develops earlier      Palestis, J. Burger & M. Gochfeld, unpublished data). On
      than previously reported by Burger et al. (1988). We also     23 July 1997 we killed all surviving chicks under the
      demonstrate the importance of visual cues in sibling          supervision of a veterinarian (R. Harris). We felt that any
      recognition indirectly, by comparing approach responses       chicks raised without exposure to adult terns and released
      when chicks can both see and hear each other to               into the wild would have died of starvation, because tern
      approach responses when only auditory cues are avail-         fledglings must learn how to fish (LeCroy 1972) and
      able. Kin recognition studies usually fail to control for     probably require feeding from their parents even after
      behavioural cues (Gamboa et al. 1991), but the behaviour      fledging (Ashmole & Tovar 1968; Burger 1980).
      of larid chicks may be of overriding importance in               The Rutgers University Institutional Review Board for
      discrimination by parents (Halpin 1991). We test the          the Use and Care of Animals approved the rearing and
      importance of behavioural, nonvocal interactions among        experimental protocol (amendment to protocol 86-016),
      chicks in sibling discrimination with the use of one-way      and chicks were collected under the appropriate state and
      mirrors. Furthermore, we compare discrimination among         federal permits (PRT 674091).
      familiar and unfamiliar individuals to discrimination
      among familiar individuals (true individual recognition).
                                                                    Experiment 1: Visual and Auditory Contact
      We also test whether preferences exist for particular
      individuals within a brood, as occurs among budgerigar,         The first experiment tested whether chicks could dis-
      Melopsittacus undulatus, chicks (Stamps et al. 1990).         criminate siblings from nonsiblings when test and stimu-
      Finally, we compare approach responses among broods           lus chicks could both see and hear each other. Choice
      with varying levels of within-brood competitive               tests began when chicks were 4 days old, and had
      aggression.                                                   therefore been with their siblings for only 3 days. We
                                                                    placed test chicks in the middle of a rectangular box
                                METHODS                             (99.140.6 cm) inside an inverted cup. We placed stimu-
                                                                    lus chicks (one sibling and one nonsibling) in separate,
      We collected 30 newly hatched common tern chicks from         transparent mouse cages 56 cm apart, behind cardboard
      30 different nests, spread over three colonies in Barnegat    dividers. Test chicks and stimulus chicks could hear each
      Bay, New Jersey, on 7 July 1997. All chicks were less than    other and frequently called, but could not see each other
      1 day old, but were no longer wet from hatching. In           until removal of the cardboard dividers. We did not place
      the laboratory, we weighed and banded the chicks, and         siblings consistently on the left or right side of the arena,
      placed them in clear plastic mouse cages in groups of         to control for any direction preferences. All nonsiblings
      three, the natural brood size. Because we did not remove      were familiar nonsiblings (i.e. chicks from neighbouring
      more than one chick from a given nest, to avoid adversely     broods). We hid from view and, after allowing 30 s for
      affecting the productivity of the colonies, broodmates        habituation, removed the cup and the cardboard dividers
      were not true siblings. However, broodmates were raised       remotely. We recorded latency to first movement, any
      together from less than 1 day posthatching, and were          position changes, any calling by test or stimulus chicks
      mostly first-hatched chicks with no experience with their     and the time the test chick spent in each of four equal
      true siblings other than exposure to prenatal vocaliz-        sections of the arena (0–14 cm from sibling’s cage,
      ations (Saino & Fasola 1996). We will refer to laboratory     14–28 cm from sibling’s cage, 14–28 cm from non-
      broodmates as ‘siblings’ throughout this paper.               sibling’s cage, and 0–14 cm from the nonsibling’s cage).
PALESTIS & BURGER: SIBLING RECOGNITION   377

We ended each trial when the test chick had been greater        Observations of Intrabrood Aggression
than 14 cm away from either cage for 2 consecutive min
(scored as unresponsive) or stayed within 14 cm from              In addition to the choice experiments described above,
either cage for 2 consecutive min (scored as choice made        we also observed aggression among siblings within each
for sibling or nonsibling). A trial also ended if the test      brood. During feeding, we usually separated siblings from
chick walked back and forth between the two stimulus            each other to minimize aggression and to facilitate
chicks repeatedly for 3 min without making a choice             unbiased feeding of all three chicks. During one feeding
(scored as ambivalent). We also calculated the proportion       session per day, however, siblings were not separated and
of time the test chick spent in each of the four parts of the   observations of aggression were recorded. We recorded
arena.                                                          the number of pecks and bites directed towards siblings
   We used each 4-day-old chick as a test chick once,           for 1 min before feeding and 1 min after feeding. If a
resulting in 30 total trials. We repeated this experiment       chick grabbed another chick’s leg or wing without letting
on the following day, using each chick as a test chick          go, then the two chicks were briefly separated. No injuries
again, but with a different combination of sibling and          resulted from this aggressive behaviour. If a chick was
familiar nonsibling stimulus chicks. We analysed the            removed from a brood due to poor condition or lack of
data for each day separately, to avoid pooling non-             growth, then observations of aggression were no longer
independent data (repeated observations on the same             made on that brood, to eliminate differences due to
individuals), a common error in kin recognition studies         brood size.
(Gamboa et al. 1991). Data from the two trials were only
combined when comparing preferences among broods,                                        RESULTS
but were averaged to give only one data point per brood
(N=10), thus decreasing statistical power. We followed          Sibling discrimination was already present when chicks
the same experimental protocol when the chicks were 10          were first tested at 4 days of age, with both auditory and
days old, but used unfamiliar nonsiblings instead of            visual contact among stimulus and test chicks. Signifi-
familiar nonsiblings.                                           cantly more test chicks chose siblings than familiar non-
                                                                siblings at this age (Table 1). These chicks also spent
                                                                significantly more time in the section of the test arena
Experiment 2: No Visual Cues                                    closest to their siblings than in the section closest to
   To examine the importance of visual cues indirectly, we      familiar nonsiblings (Fig. 1; Wilcoxon signed-ranks test,
prevented test and stimulus chicks from seeing each             sample sizes given in Table 1: Z=3.03, P
378   ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 58, 2

                   Table 1. Results of common tern sibling versus nonsibling (familiar, strange) choice tests with auditory and visual
                   contact among test and stimulus chicks, no visual contact among chicks and unidirectional visual contact
                                                     Auditory/visual contact                               No visual contact             One-way mirror

                                                Familiar      Familiar    Strange                         Familiar       Familiar      Familiar        Familiar
                       Age (days)                  4             5           10                              8             12             7              11

                       Test chick
                         Chose sibling*         16           14            6                               5              5            11               7
                         Chose nonsibling        4            7            6                               3              2             2               2
                         Ambivalent              1            3            4                               2              2             1               0
                         Unresponsive            9            6            7                              15             12            16              13
                       Total                    30           30           23                              25             21            30              22
                       Probability†              0.005        0.055        0.226                           0.219          0.164         0.01            0.07
                                                (0.01)       (0.117)      (0.122)                         (0.246)        (0.246)       (0.022)         (0.07)

                   *We considered a choice to be made when the test chick spent 2 consecutive min in the end (one-fourth of arena
                    area) of the test arena nearest a stimulus chick. Unresponsive chicks remained in the centre of the test arena for
                    2 consecutive min, and ambivalent chicks walked back and forth between siblings and nonsiblings for 3 min
                    without making a clear choice.
                   †Binomial probabilities, comparing choice of sibling versus choice of nonsibling. The number in parentheses is the
                    binomial probability if sibling choice is compared to responsive chicks that did not choose their sibling (i.e. chicks
                    that chose nonsiblings plus ambivalent chicks).

      Test chicks spent a significantly larger proportion of                     (Fig. 2). There was no trend for test chicks that had been
      time near siblings than near nonsiblings in both trials                    used previously as stimulus chicks on a given day to be
      (Fig. 1; age 7 days: Z=2.95, P
PALESTIS & BURGER: SIBLING RECOGNITION   379

                 0·7                                                                        0·8

                                                                                            0·7
                 0·6
                                                                                            0·5
Responsiveness

                                                                        Time near sibling
                 0·5                                                                        0·5

                                                                                            0·4
                 0·4                                                                        0·3

                                                                                            0·2
                 0·3
                                                                                            0·1

                 0·2                                                                         0
                       2   4   6       8        10    12        14
                                   Age (days)                                         –0·1
                                                                                         1·5      2   2·5      3    3·5    4    4·5   5     5·5    6
Figure 2. The mean±SE responsiveness of test chicks equals the                                              Prefeeding aggression/min
proportion of time spent near the sibling plus the proportion of
time spent near the nonsibling. h: Auditory/visual contact, includes   Figure 3. Mean±SE proportion of time members of each common
treatments with familiar (ages 4 and 5 days) and strange nonsiblings   tern brood spent in the section of the test arena nearest their sib-
(10 days) and choice among siblings (6 and 9 days); ◆: no visual       lings in relation to mean intrabrood, prefeeding aggression/min.
contact; j: one-way mirror.                                            Y= −0.10X+0.76.

see and hear each other, and mean responsiveness was                   chicks can recognize their siblings at this age. This is also
relatively high in both trials (Fig. 2).                               the age at which common tern chicks first discriminate
   There was significantly more within-brood aggression                between the calls of their parents and those of other
in the minute before feeding than in the minute after                  adults (Stevenson et al. 1970). Four-day-old common tern
feeding (paired t test: t9 =6.86, P
380   ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 58, 2

      them from seeing neighbouring broods, and the arrange-          in the frequency of competitive aggression successfully
      ment of broods was randomly changed daily so that the           predicts much of the variation among broods in the
      identity of neighbours was constantly changing. If sibling      proportion of time spent near siblings in the choice tests.
      discrimination does indeed involve comparing familiar           That chicks in highly aggressive broods are less likely to
      nestmates to familiar neighbours, then the rearing proto-       approach their siblings than chicks in less aggressive
      col of Burger et al. (1988), which was intended to reduce       broods may suggest an adaptive response by chicks to
      familiarity with neighbours, may have actually led to           avoid aggressive siblings. Failure to approach a sibling
      discrimination developing late. The lack of a preference        and/or a nonsibling in a choice test may not always be
      for siblings over strangers needs replication, particularly     evidence of a recognition error, but instead may reflect
      because this experiment was performed on a different day        accurate recognition and avoidance of the sibling. Dis-
      than the trials using familiar nonsiblings, and thus used       crimination of kin is expected to be context dependent,
      test chicks of a different age.                                 and therefore recognition ‘errors’ are also to be expected
         We have also demonstrated the importance of visual           (Blaustein & Porter 1990; Sherman et al. 1997). However,
      cues in common tern sibling discrimination. Burger et al.       the behaviour of the test chicks suggested that siblings or
      (1988) demonstrated that common tern chicks could               nonsiblings were approached, not that one chick was
      recognize their siblings based on playback of begging calls     avoided. This hypothesis could be tested experimentally
      alone. We found no evidence here for discrimination             by using only one stimulus chick at a time. It is also
      when test chicks could not see stimulus chicks, but,            possible that sibling recognition simply did not develop
      although calls were present in all visual isolation trials,     fully in all of the broods, resulting in high levels of
      the calls were not necessarily begging calls and calling        aggression in these broods. This nonadaptive hypothesis
      was often infrequent. We found chicks to be more                is suggested by the fact that the levels of aggression
      responsive, and thus more likely to approach siblings,          observed in these artificial broods is much higher than is
      when they had full auditory and visual contact with             observed in the field.
      stimulus chicks than when they could only hear stimulus           Sibling recognition can have important ecological
      chicks. This comparison is complicated by differences in        consequences in colonial, ground-nesting birds. We have
      the age of the chicks, but responsiveness of test chicks        demonstrated that sibling recognition in common terns
      when stimulus and test chicks could both see and hear           develops early enough for it to benefit chicks when they
      each other was relatively high at all ages tested (4, 5, 6, 9   become mobile, and that more than one sensory
      and 10 days), while responsiveness of test chicks visually      modality is used in discrimination of siblings from non-
      isolated from stimulus chicks was very low at both ages         siblings. Whether discrimination requires comparison
      tested (8 and 12 days) (see Fig. 2). That auditory and          among familiar individuals should be tested rigorously, as
      visual cues combined are more effective than auditory           should the effect of age and relative age on interactions
      cues alone has been demonstrated previously for parent–         among siblings.
      offspring recognition in razorbills, Alca torda (Ingold
      1973) and ring-billed gulls, Larus delawarensis (Conover
      et al. 1980), and for approach responses towards other                                Acknowledgments
      chicks in ring-billed gulls (Evans 1970). Whether visual        The authors thank H. John-Alder, T. McGuire, J.
      cues alone can allow sibling recognition among common           Spendelow, R. Trivers and the referees for comments on
      tern chicks has not been demonstrated. However, in the          the manuscript, and M. Gochfeld and R. Harris for advice
      auditory and visual contact experiments described here,         on the care of chicks. B.G.P. is supported by a Marion
      there were six cases in which neither stimulus chick            Johnson Fellowship and a teaching assistantship from
      called, and, in all three cases when the test chick made a      Rutgers University, and J.B. is supported by the Penn
      choice, the chick chose its sibling.                            Foundation through the Trust for Public Lands, CRESP
         In the trials using one-way mirrors to separate test and     (Department of Energy, AI DE-FC01-95EW55084) and
      stimulus chicks, we demonstrated that nonvocal behav-           NIEHS (ESO 5022).
      ioural interactions among chicks, although important
      in maintaining responsiveness, are not necessary for
      sibling discrimination. Approach responses to stimulus                                      References
      chicks decreased, but, when approach did occur, the
      bias towards siblings remained. These trials also gave          Ashmole, N. P. & Tovar, S. H. 1968. Prolonged parental care in
                                                                        royal terns and other birds. Auk, 85, 90–100.
      additional evidence for the importance of visual cues.
                                                                      Beecher, M. D. 1988. Kin recognition in birds. Behavior Genetics, 18,
      Stimulus chicks could not see test chicks, but could hear         465–482.
      them. Despite approach and calling by test chicks, stimu-
                                                                      Beecher, I. M. & Beecher, M. D. 1983. Sibling recognition in
      lus chicks sat quietly and did not noticeably react to the        bank swallows (Riparia riparia). Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 62,
      presence of the test chicks.                                      145–150.
         Aggression among siblings was significantly higher           Blaustein, A. R. & Porter, R. H. 1990. The ubiquitous concept of
      before than after feeding, demonstrating that intrabrood          recognition with special reference to kin. In: Interpretation and
      aggression can result from competition over food. By              Explanation in the Study of Animal Behavior. Vol. 1 (Ed. by M. Bekoff
      examining each brood separately, instead of averaging             & D. Jamieson), pp. 123–148. Boulder, Colorado: Westview Press.
      aggression levels across broods, we were able to examine        Bradshaw, R. H. 1991. Discrimination of group members by laying
      differences among broods. This variation among broods             hens Gallus domesticus. Behavioural Processes, 24, 143–151.
PALESTIS & BURGER: SIBLING RECOGNITION       381

Buckley, P. A. & Buckley, F. G. 1970. Color variation in the soft parts   Holley, A. J. F. 1988. Intergenerational conflict in gulls. Animal
  and down of royal tern chicks. Auk, 87, 1–13.                             Behaviour, 36, 619–620.
Burger, J. 1980. The transition to independence and postfledging          Holmes, W. G. & Sherman, P. W. 1982. The ontogeny of kin
  parental care in seabirds. In: Behavior of Marine Animals. Vol. 4:        recognition in two species of ground squirrels. American Zoologist,
  Marine Birds (Ed. by J. Burger, B. L. Olla & H. E. Winn),                 22, 491–517.
  pp. 367–447. New York: Plenum.                                          Ingold, P. 1973. Zur lautlichen beziehung des Elters zu seinem
Burger, J. 1998. Effects of lead on sibling recognition in young            Kueken bei Tordalken (Alca torda). Behaviour, 45, 154–190.
  herring gulls. Toxicological Sciences, 43, 155–160.
                                                                          LeCroy, M. 1972. Young common and roseate terns learning to fish.
Burger, J. & Gochfeld, M. 1990. Early experience and vegetation
                                                                            Wilson Bulletin, 84, 201–202.
  preferences in common tern chicks. Wilson Bulletin, 102, 328–333.
Burger, J., Gochfeld, M. & Boarman, W. I. 1988. Experimental              Noseworthy, C. M. & Lien, J. 1976. Ontogeny of nesting habitat
  evidence for sibling recognition in common terns (Sterna hirundo).        recognition and preference in neonatal herring gull chicks, Larus
  Auk, 105, 142–148.                                                        argentatus Pontoppidan. Animal Behaviour, 24, 637–651.
Colgan, P. W. 1983. Comparative Social Recognition. New York:             Pfennig, D. W. & Sherman, P. W. 1995. Kin recognition. Scientific
  J. Wiley.                                                                 American, 272, 98–103.
Conover, M. R., Klopfer, F. D. & Miller, D. E. 1980. Stimulus             Pierotti, R., Brunton, D. & Murphy, E. C. 1988. Parent–offspring
  features of chicks and other factors evoking parental protective          and sibling–sibling recognition in gulls. Animal Behaviour, 36,
  behavior in ring-billed gulls. Animal Behaviour, 28, 29–41.               620–621.
Evans, R. M. 1970. Imprinting and mobility in young ring-billed           Saino, N. & Fasola, M. 1996. The function of embryonic vocaliz-
  gulls, Larus delawarensis. Animal Behaviour Monographs, 3,                ation in the little tern (Sterna albifrons). Ethology, 102, 265–271.
  193–248.                                                                Sherman, P. W., Reeve, H. K. & Pfennig, D. W. 1997. Recognition
Evans, R. M. 1980. Development of behavior in seabirds: an                  systems. In: Behavioural Ecology: an Evolutionary Approach (Ed. by
  ecological perspective. In: Behavior of Marine Animals. Vol. 4:           J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies), pp. 69–96. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific.
  Marine Birds (Ed. by J. Burger, B. L. Olla & H. E. Winn),
                                                                          Shugart, G. W. 1990. A cue-isolation experiment to determine if
  pp. 271–322. New York: Plenum.
Fletcher, D. J. C. & Michener, C. D. (Eds) 1987. Kin Recognition in         caspian tern parents learn their offspring’s down color. Ethology,
                                                                            84, 155–161.
  Animals. New York: J. Wiley.
Gamboa, G. J., Reeve, H. K. & Holmes, W. G. 1991. Conceptual              Stamps, J., Kus, B., Clark, A. & Arrowood, P. 1990. Social relation-
  issues and methodology in kin-recognition research: a critical            ships of fledgling budgerigars, Melopsitticus undulatus. Animal
  discussion. Ethology, 88, 109–127.                                        Behaviour, 40, 688–700.
Halpin, Z. T. 1991. Kin recognition cues of vertebrates. In: Kin          Stevenson, J. G., Hutchinson, R. E., Hutchinson, J. B., Bertram,
  Recognition (Ed. by P. G. Hepper), pp. 220–258. Cambridge:                B. C. R. & Thorpe, W. H. 1970. Individual recognition by auditory
  Cambridge University Press.                                               cues in the common tern (Sterna hirundo). Nature, 226, 562–563.
You can also read