In search of 'incels': Can social exclusion push sexist men towards misogynistic right-wing groups?
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
In search of ‘incels’: Can social exclusion push sexist men towards misogynistic right-wing groups? Zach Loughery1, Emma Renström2, Hanna Bäck1, & Holly Knapton1 1 Lund University, 2University of Gothenburg Abstract The present study sought to investigate the potential predictors that might lead men to join misogynistic, politically extreme right-wing groups. The aims of this research were inspired by the ‘incels’ subculture found within the online manosphere, a group of men who claim to have been sexually and romantically rejected by women. They present as highly sexist and often strongly endorse far-right ideologies. Relating the incels struggle for acceptance and potential for violence to the quest for significance (Kruglanski, Chen, Dechesne, Fishman, & Orehek, 2009), this research aimed to examine the relationship between social exclusion, sexism, and the desire to join a right-wing group. A sample of American men (N=298) participated in an online study. Following a manipulation in which half of the participants were subject to exclusion, all participants were presented with promotional material for a fake right-wing ‘pop culture media’ community, and measured on how interested they were in joining this group. The results suggest that whilst social exclusion itself was not significant in predicting the participants joining intentions, an interaction between exclusion and sexism was, with rejected participants who already harbored sexist notions being more likely to want to join the fake group. This finding is in line with past research arguing that excluded individuals are more likely to turn to radical groups if they share some of that group’s attitudes to begin with (Bäck, Bäck, Altermark, & Knapton, 2018). Limitations and implications for future research are discussed within. Introduction Somewhere on the internet, you’ve probably encountered an incel before, even if you didn’t realize it. You’ll find them in YouTube comment sections, Reddit, or in sexist memes doing the rounds on Facebook. The term incel, meaning ‘involuntarily celibate’, is not particularly new, having first originated in 1993 when a Canadian student created a website to detail her frustration over her lack of sexual relationships. Yet the adoption of the term as a moniker for
angry, isolated men is a much more recent development, and it is the troubling growth of this subculture that has begun to face media scrutiny in the recent years. Alek Minassian, perpetrator of the Toronto van attack that killed 10 people in 2018, openly identified himself as an incel. Shortly before the attack began, Minassian made a Facebook post, in which he declared, “The Incel Rebellion has already begun! We will overthrow all the Chads and Stacys! All hail the Supreme Gentleman Elliot Rodger!” Elliot Rodger, the man referenced here, murdered six people in the Isla Vista killings of 2014, and released a lengthy manifesto in which he blamed his lack of sexual fulfilment for his crimes. Incels are characterized by their loneliness, fueled by a belief that they will forever be unable to obtain a sexual relationship. This loneliness often comes accompanied by misogyny, misanthropy, narcissism, racism, and resentment. Their intragroup dynamics consist of processes which reinforce their existing beliefs, akin to an echo chamber. It is not uncommon for those who identify as incels to post pictures of themselves for other incels to rate, resulting in overwhelmingly negative appraisals that further strengthen the individual’s sense of self-loathing and pity (Ging, 2017). The incels emergence in the public sphere is no coincidence. In the last several years, there has been a major shift towards populism in the politics of the Western world. Typified by the results of the 2016 US Election and the UK’s Brexit referendum of the same year, there has been a marked development in political tribalism, fueled by the growth of energetic new movements and groups that have taken advantage of the internet to proliferate their ideologies and grow their numbers (Bryden & Silverman, 2019). Perhaps the most prominent of these new groups to emerge is the ‘alt-right,’ and the numerous subcultures that have emerged from within its sphere. The exponential growth of the alt-right accompanied the surge in popularity of present US president Donald Trump, and their efforts to support the Republican candidate during the election became the focus of worldwide media (Heikkila, 2017). Whilst the growth of the alt-right doubtlessly assisted in Trump’s election victory, it is their broader influence on society that is of particular interest. Loosely organized, the alt-right’s ideology seems to be inherently flexible, uniting right-wing groups across a broad spectrum, everything from card-carrying neo-Nazi’s to more typical American conservatives (Love, 2017).
Under the alt-right’s umbrella, extremists can find shelter and even admiration. It is here where the incels have carved out their own little niche. Whilst they appear to share many traits with the more conventional alt-right, the incels glorification of extreme acts of spree violence and intense misogyny make them one of the most radical, and perhaps the most dangerous, of communities housed under the alt-right umbrella. The pathways that might lead to an individual identifying with such an extreme group have yet to be explored in the radicalization literature. As attempting to directly access such a fringe group would create a number of practical and ethical issues for empirical study, any such exploration must instead take a broader approach by examining the processes at work that might nudge a vulnerable individual in the direction of groups such as these. The present study aimed to take such an approach. Whilst there is a great deal of potential underlying issues to unpack from the incel conundrum, the most obvious characteristics appear to be feelings of exclusion, particularly by the opposite sex, and misogynistic attitudes. Sexism has been previously identified as highly prevalent amongst the alt-right, particularly hostile sexism, which was found to predict voting outcomes in favor of Donald Trump in the 2016 US Election (Ratliff, Redford, Conway, & Smith, 2019). Sexist attitudes amongst Trump supporters have been observed to have increased after the election, suggesting that Trump’s election success has reinforced existing attitudes or emboldened their expression (Georgeac, Rattan, & Effron, 2019). The other piece of the puzzle, social exclusion, has been examined at length in past research as a potential factor in drawing isolated individuals towards radical groups. It has been suggested that ostracized individuals are more likely to engage with extreme groups, expressing openness to recruitment (Hales & Williams, 2018), and a willingness to take part in protest actions (Bäck, Bäck, & Knapton, 2015; Knapton, Bäck, & Bäck, 2015). Excluded individuals are more likely to view themselves with self-loathing or pity, a characteristic that appears to be common in incel discourse. The most violent manifestation of this self-loathing could be a desire to ‘be someone,’ to make the world understand their plight. The quest for significance model (Kruglanski, Chen, Dechesne, Fishman, & Orehek, 2009; Kruglanski et al., 2013) is built entirely on this motivation, and past research supports the significance motive as influencing an excluded individual’s efforts to adapt their attitudes to fit in with radical groups (Bäck, Bäck, Altermark, & Knapton, 2018).
As the quest for significance describes an attempt to resolve the crisis between an individual’s belief in their own worth, and the lack of acknowledgement from the external world of this worthiness (Jasko, LaFree & Kruglanski, 2017), the ‘dark trait’ of narcissism (Paulhus & Williams, 2002) appears to have a great deal of relevance as a potential control. Incels seem drawn almost exclusively to right-wing ideologies and commonly express authoritarian beliefs, suggesting that right-wing authoritarianism (Altemeyer, 1981) should also be factored in as a control. In this research, we sought to test the influence of sexism and exclusion on an individual’s willingness to join a right-wing misogynistic group. It was predicted that exclusion and sexism would act as independent predictors of the participant’s intentions to join an extreme group, and that an interaction between exclusion and sexism would also significantly predict intentions to join, with excluded participants high in sexism being more likely to want to do so. Method Participants Two hundred and ninety-eight men (Mean age = 34) were recruited from the online participant pool provided by Prolific Academic. The sample was limited to users currently living in the United States at the time of the study, and was further restricted to participants aged eighteen years or older. Design & Procedure The study utilized a between-subjects design in its approach to examining the effects of sexism and exclusion on the participant’s willingness to join a right-wing, sexist group. The primary independent variables measured were social exclusion, manipulated via experimental condition (excluded/included) and sexism. In addition, right wing authoritarianism (RWA; Altemeyer, 1981) and narcissism (Paulhus & Williams, 2002) were measured as controls given an expected association with the participants openness to joining such a group. The dependent variable was a measurement of the participant’s desire to join the group. Upon self-selecting to participate in the study via Prolific Academic, participants were redirected to an initial questionnaire hosted through Qualtrics, in which they were measured on
demographic information and presented a battery of scales intended to create a baseline reading for sexism, RWA, and the dark traits. Following the completion of this questionnaire, they were redirected to an additional website, on which an artificial social media environment was hosted (Wolf et al., 2015). On this website, participants were tasked with giving themselves a username, selecting an avatar from a list of pre-selected silhouettes, and writing a brief profile about themselves and their interests. They were informed that they would be sharing their profile with other participants in the study, and would have the opportunity to ‘like’ profiles that they found interesting, just as they too could receive ‘likes’ in return. Participants were led to believe that the other profiles they were reading were real, but in actuality, they were pre-written by the researchers. The participants were randomly sorted between the inclusion and exclusion conditions prior to their redirection to the social media environment. Those who were excluded received only a single like during the three minute ‘sharing’ session that took place after they had constructed their profiles, whilst those who were included received seven. When the allotted three minutes were up, the participants were then redirected back to Qualtrics to continue with the survey segment of the study. Upon completion of this manipulation check, participants were informed that, based on the answers they had provided thus far, they had been selected to receive information concerning a ‘third-party website’ interested in obtaining their opinions. All participants received this message regardless of any of their previous answers. They were then redirected once more to a survey supposedly provided by this third-party website, a group identifying themselves as the “Men of America”. Describing themselves as a ‘brand new media website with a focus on pop culture and society’, the group’s description was written to be colorful and blatant with regards to their political stance, taking a ‘men first’ stance and decrying political correctness and ‘leftist crazies.’ Although presented as a real group, they were in fact entirely fictional, devised for the purposes of the study. The Men of America component of the study presented participants briefly with some relevant questions specific to the group’s content, such as gauging the participants interests in different forms of media (e.g Movies, Comic Books, Video Games), or asking them how much they agreed or disagreed with statements made concerning trends they might have noticed in media,
such as “Beloved franchises like Star Wars are being ruined because the filmmakers are trying to send political messages.” Following their propaganda package and the questions that followed, participants were measured on how interested they would be on joining the group. The procedure followed those outlined by Bäck and colleagues (2018). Once the group survey had been completed, participants were redirected to a final debrief section. Further manipulation checks were used to ensure that the experimental social media environment worked correctly, such as asking the participants if they were able to read the profiles of other participants, or see their avatars. They were also asked directly whether they thought they were ignored, excluded, or if they thought the other ‘participants’ had liked the profile they had written. Participants were offered a chance to provide initial feedback prior to the debrief. Afterwards, they were fully informed of the deception involved throughout the study, and given a second chance to give feedback in light of this new information. Materials Social exclusion was measured using a condition-based experimental approach, conducted through the use of an artificial social media environment, Ostracism Online (Wolf et al., 2015). This environment was hosted on a website managed by the researchers. Participants were randomly split between the inclusion and exclusion condition. Dummy coding was then used during the analysis to represent this variable (1 = Exclusion, 0 = Inclusion). Sexism was measured using the eight-item Modern Sexism Scale (Swim, Aikin, Hall, & Hunter, 1995; α = .89). Presented alongside the items measuring RWA, the participants were asked to indicate how much they agreed on several statements concerning a variety of societal issues. These items were measured on a seven-point scale, ranging from 1 = “Strongly disagree” to 7 = “Strongly agree”, and example items include “Discrimination against women is no longer a problem in the United States”, or “Women often miss out on good jobs due to sexual discrimination.” An eighteen-item measurement (α = .91) of right-wing authoritarianism (RWA) accompanied the sexism scale described previously, with the same instructions. These items were adapted from the original right-wing authoritarianism scale devised by Altemeyer (1981), and measured on a seven-point scale, ranging from “Strongly disagree” to “Strongly agree.” Sample items include
“Our prisons are a shocking disgrace. Criminals are unfortunate people who deserve much better care, instead of so much punishment”, or “The “old-fashioned ways” and “old-fashioned values” still show the best way to live.” Narcissism was measured using the narcissism inventory on the Short Dark Triad scale (SD3; Jones & Paulhus, 2014). The trait was measured using nine items (α = .80), and participants were simply instructed to indicate how much they agree with the statements presented, using a five- point scale ranging from “Disagree strongly” to “Agree strongly.” Example items include “I know that I am special because everyone keeps telling me so” or “I have been compared to famous people.” The participant’s desire to join the group was measured using a single item, “How interested would you be in joining Men of America?”, with responses made on a five-point scale, ranging from “Not interested” to “Very interested.” Results Descriptive statistics for all of the continuous variables used in the study are presented in Table 1. In order to assess whether or not participants had been appropriately affected by the exclusion manipulation, a t-test was performed using the condition (exclusion/inclusion) as the grouping variable, and two manipulation check items, “I was ignored” and “I was excluded.” The results support the effectiveness of the manipulation, with excluded participants expressing significantly higher feelings of being ignored (M = 3.06, SD = 1.35, t(225) = 12.27, p =
of which are found in Table 2. Social exclusion is represented in the model by the condition variable, which is dummy coded for the analysis (1 = Exclusion, 0 = Inclusion). Narcissism and RWA are added as controls, with two-way interactions between the condition, sexism, and RWA added in the second stage of the model. The final model accounted for 26% of the variance in the dependent variable. Exclusion was non-significant in both stages of the model, whilst sexism is significant in the first stage but becomes non-significant once the interactions are accounted for. Narcissism and RWA both emerge as positive significant predictors of the participant’s intentions to join the Men of America, regardless of the interactions. We also find a significant effect for the interaction between exclusion and sexism, presented in Figure 1, which suggests that sexist participants were more likely to want to join the group if they had been rejected during the experimental manipulation. The interaction between exclusion and RWA is non-significant. Figure 1. Effect of sexism moderated by exclusion on group joining intentions
Table 2. Hierarchical regression model to predict participant’s desire to join Men of America group Step and Predictor Variable B SE B β p R2 Adj. Step 1. Controls and Main Effects .25* Condition -.08 .13 -.03 .51 Sexism .19** .05 .20** .00 Narcissism .34** .08 .19** .00 RWA .36** .07 .20** .00 Step 2. Interactions .26* Condition -.14 .47 -.05 .75 Sexism .07 .07 .07 .34 Narcissism .35** .08 .20** .00 RWA .48** .10 .40** .00 Condition x Sexism .26* .11 .41* .02 Condition x RWA -.26 .14 -.38 .07 * p < .05, **p < .01. Discussion The aim of this research was to investigate the potential influence of sexism and social exclusion on men’s desire to join right-wing, misogynistic groups, inspired by the rhetoric commonly employed by ‘incels.’ The results suggest that these factors in and of themselves do not seem to significantly influence this desire, but that an interaction between them does. Men who already harbored sexist ideas were significantly more likely to wish to join the ‘Men of America’ group once they had been subject to exclusion. This follows the argument made by past research that rejection alone is not sufficient to drive a ‘normal’ person towards an extreme group, but that instead they must have some existing attitudes that direct them towards a group where these attitudes could be not only accepted, but reinforced (Bäck, Bäck, Altermark, & Knapton, 2018). These results reflect the complexity of the underlying processes that lay behind the new wave of right-wing radicalization. An individual’s recruitment into an extreme right-wing group is likely to involve some element of self-radicalization, which may come down to seeking out websites, social media groups, and other sources of information that either confirm the individual’s worldview, or pushes them further down the rabbit hole towards yet more hateful beliefs (Johnson, 2018). In this study, participants were presented with one such source of information, a highly misogynistic source, but only those who were already sexist were drawn towards it.
The moderating effect of exclusion in this study is a finding that supports those made by similar past research concerning ostracism, rejection, and openness to extreme groups (Hales & Williams, 2018; Knapton, Bäck, & Bäck, 2015; Bäck, Bäck, & Knapton, 2015). As exclusion seems to be the primary grievance expressed by the subject of the study’s interest, the ‘incels,’ this finding highlights the potential importance of ostracism in unravelling the major pathways to far-right radicalization that vulnerable individuals may take. Limitations It is important to note that the present study cannot draw any concrete conclusions regarding the incels subculture described throughout, simply because the participants were not drawn directly from a population that identifies themselves as incels. As described in the introduction, accessing such a group for empirical research presents numerous challenges, and there exists almost no published literature to date that could set a precedent for study. Concerning the mechanism of exclusion explored in the current study, it should also be considered that incels seem to be primarily frustrated with their romantic rejection by women. The manipulation used in this study did not deal with romantic rejection in any sense and instead explored feelings of ostracism through a social media simulation, without any particular intention to make the participants feel like they had been rejected by a prospective romantic partner. Future manipulations could attempt to correct this limitation either through direct or indirect means, although good ethical practice should be considered. The inherent challenges and limitations offered by using an online participant pool should also be noted. Whilst Prolific Academic has systems in place for validating their userbase and blacklisting spurious responders, there is no way of guaranteeing that every data entry found in the sample was made by a serious participant. Any survey-based method of sample collection is prone to the risk of participants skimming through instructions, missing items, or providing false data, but these risks are somewhat more pronounced in an online environment where monetary incentives are provided for survey completion. In the present study, this limitation was addressed through extensive cleaning of the data set to remove error-strewn entries.
Implications As the present study only focused on two potential factors that might motivate an individual to join an extreme group, it stands to reason that there could be many more. As the alt-right and its subcultures, particularly incels, express many different beliefs and ideologies, it stands to reason that there may be many more attitudinal factors in play than simply sexism. Racism, nationalism, a belief in conspiracy theories, and more could all play a role. Demographic factors such as age and race are also worth considering, as are socioeconomic factors and mental health. To build on the present study’s results, future research should look to ostracism as a moderator and continue to explore the factors it may interact with to motivate intentions to join or participate in hate-filled groups. Novel methods of manipulation exclusion may prove especially useful for experimental designs. Conclusion This research sought to conduct the first exploration of its kind into the elusive ‘incels,’ a fringe group of alt-right men characterized by their loneliness and their loathing of women. We found that whilst sexist attitudes and feelings of rejection alone don’t seem to be enough to drive an individual towards a misogynistic group, the interaction between these two does. Although only scratching the surface of the problem, the present study serves as a solid foundation upon which future research could build.
References Bäck, E. A., Bäck, H. & Knapton, H. (2015). Group Belongingness and Collective Action: Effects of Need to Belong and Rejection Sensitivity on Willingness to Participate in Protest Activities. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 56(5), 537-544. Bryden, J. & Silverman, E. (2019). Underlying socio-political processes behind the 2016 US election. PLoS ONE, 14(4). Georgeac, O. A. M., Rattan, A., & Effron, D. A. (2019). An Exploratory Investigation of Americans' Expression of Gender Bias Before and After the 2016 Presidential Election. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 10(5), 632–642. Ging, D. (2017). Alphas, Betas, and Incels: Theorizing the Masculinities of the Manosphere. Men and Masculinities. Hales, A., & Williams, K. (2018). Marginalized Individuals and Extremism: The Role of Ostracism in Openness to Extreme Groups. Journal Of Social Issues, 74(1), 75-92. Heikkila, N. (2017). Online Antagonism of the Alt-Right in the 2016 Election. European Journal of American Studies, 12(2). Johnson, J. (2018). The Self-Radicalization of White Men: "Fake News" and the Affective Networking of Paranoia. Communication, Culture, and Critique, 11(1), 100-115. Jones, D., & Paulhus, D. (2014). Introducing the Short Dark Triad (SD3). Assessment, 21(1), 28- 41. Knapton, H., Bäck, H., & Bäck, E. A. (2015). The Social Activist: Conformity to the Ingroup Following Rejection as a Predictor of Political Participation. Social Influence, 10(2), 97-108. Love, N.S (2017). Back to the Future: Trendy Fascism, the Trump Effect, and the Alt-Right. New Political Science, 39(2), 263-268. Paulhus, D. L., & Williams, K. M. (2002). The Dark Triad of personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism and psychopathy. Journal of Research in Personality, 36(6), 556-563. Pollard, T. (2018). Alt-Right Transgressions in the Age of Trump. Perspectives on Global Development and Technology, 17(1-2), 76-88.
Ratliff, K. A., Redford, L., Conway, J. & Smith, C. T. (2019). Engendering support: Hostile sexism predicts voting for Donald Trump over Hillary Clinton in the 2016 US presidential election. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 22(4), 578–593. Swim, J. K., Aikin, K. J., Hall, W. S., & Hunter, B. A. (1995). Sexism and racism: Old-fashioned and modern prejudices. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68(2), 199-214. Wolf, W., Levordashka, A., Ruff, J. R., Kraaijeveld, S., Lueckmann, J. M., & Williams, K. D. (2015). Ostracism Online: A social media ostracism paradigm. Behavior Research Methods, 47(2), 361-373. Altemeyer, B. (1981). Right-wing authoritarianism. University of Manitoba Press. Bäck, E. A., Bäck, H. & Knapton, H. (2015). Group Belongingness and Collective Action: Effects of Need to Belong and Rejection Sensitivity on Willingness to Participate in Protest Activities. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 56(5), 537-544. Bäck, E. A., Bäck, H., Altermark, N., & Knapton, H. M. (2018). The quest for significance: Attitude adaption to a radical group following social exclusion. International Journal of Developmental Science, 12, 25-36. Bryden, J. & Silverman, E. (2019). Underlying socio-political processes behind the 2016 US election. PLoS ONE, 14(4). Georgeac, O. A. M., Rattan, A., & Effron, D. A. (2019). An Exploratory Investigation of Americans' Expression of Gender Bias Before and After the 2016 Presidential Election. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 10(5), 632–642. Ging, D. (2017). Alphas, Betas, and Incels: Theorizing the Masculinities of the Manosphere. Men and Masculinities. Hales, A., & Williams, K. (2018). Marginalized Individuals and Extremism: The Role of Ostracism in Openness to Extreme Groups. Journal Of Social Issues, 74(1), 75-92. Heikkila, N. (2017). Online Antagonism of the Alt-Right in the 2016 Election. European Journal of American Studies, 12(2).
Jasko, K. , LaFree, G. & Kruglanski, A. (2017), Quest for Significance and Violent Extremism: The Case of Domestic Radicalization. Political Psychology, 38, 815-831.Altemeyer, B. (1981). Right-wing authoritarianism. University of Manitoba Press. Johnson, J. (2018). The Self-Radicalization of White Men: "Fake News" and the Affective Networking of Paranoia. Communication, Culture, and Critique, 11(1), 100-115. Jones, D., & Paulhus, D. (2014). Introducing the Short Dark Triad (SD3). Assessment, 21(1), 28- 41. Knapton, H., Bäck, H., & Bäck, E. A. (2015). The Social Activist: Conformity to the Ingroup Following Rejection as a Predictor of Political Participation. Social Influence, 10(2), 97-108. Kruglanski, A. W., Belanger, J. J., Gelfand, M., Gunaratna, R., Hettiarachchi, M., & Sharvit, K. (2013). Terrorism - a (self) love story: Redirecting the significance quest can end violence. American Psychologist, 68, 559-575. Kruglanski, A. W., Chen, X., Dechesne, M., Fishman, S., & Orehek, E. (2009). Fully committed: Suicide bombers' motivation and the quest for personal significance. Political Psychology, 30, 331-557. Love, N.S (2017). Back to the Future: Trendy Fascism, the Trump Effect, and the Alt-Right. New Political Science, 39(2), 263-268. Paulhus, D. L., & Williams, K. M. (2002). The Dark Triad of personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism and psychopathy. Journal of Research in Personality, 36(6), 556-563. Pollard, T. (2018). Alt-Right Transgressions in the Age of Trump. Perspectives on Global Development and Technology, 17(1-2), 76-88. Ratliff, K. A., Redford, L., Conway, J. & Smith, C. T. (2019). Engendering support: Hostile sexism predicts voting for Donald Trump over Hillary Clinton in the 2016 US presidential election. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 22(4), 578–593. Swim, J. K., Aikin, K. J., Hall, W. S., & Hunter, B. A. (1995). Sexism and racism: Old-fashioned and modern prejudices. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68(2), 199-214.
Wolf, W., Levordashka, A., Ruff, J. R., Kraaijeveld, S., Lueckmann, J. M., & Williams, K. D. (2015). Ostracism Online: A social media ostracism paradigm. Behavior Research Methods, 47(2), 361-373.
You can also read