How Promotional Content Changes Ratings: The Impact of Appeals, Humor, and Presentation

Page created by Kimberly Fuller
 
CONTINUE READING
Journal of Applied Communication Research
Vol. 31, No. 3, August 2003, pp. 238–259

       How Promotional Content
           Changes Ratings:
     The Impact of Appeals, Humor,
           and Presentation
   Susan Tyler Eastman, Gregory D. Newton, and Paul D. Bolls

ABSTRACT This study assessed the impact of content—as opposed to structural—fac-
tors on television program ratings, seeking to locate clusters of components that would
identify effective on-air promotion and allocate content a better-defined place within
theoretic models of media priming. Stepwise multiple regression analyses of 1,547 on-air
promos for 155 prime-time programs demonstrated that 5–9% of ratings variance was
accounted for by content appeals, humor, and presentation in promos for comedy
programs. The influence of content variables was greater for familiar than unfamiliar
comedies, and humor and presentation in promos contributed to variance in ratings for
mid-rated but not high- or low-rated comedies.
KEY WORDS: Comedy promotion, content appeals, humor, priming, program pro-
motion, ratings

O     n-air promotion’s importance to prime-time ratings has been beyond ques-
       tion for the last decade. Estimates are that the major broadcast networks
collectively air as many as 30,000 promos yearly (Ferguson, 2002), using up
irreplaceable air time. Touting prime-time programs has become a big-budget
item for the television industry, occupying more than $4 billion in air time that
otherwise could be sold for commercials and that necessitates high in-house
production costs (Eastman & Newton, 1998b). Competition from cable networks
and the internet, hugely increased license fees for series programs, and prolifer-
ating user technologies such as remote control devices, personal video recorders,
and video-on-demand have made effective on-air promotion crucial to network
programming strategy. Nonetheless, scholarly research in this arena has been
limited despite decades of industry practice—leaving questions about how and
why on-air promotion impacts program ratings.
   Only a few programs, such as the Super Bowls, Academy Awards, and
Susan Tyler Eastman is Professor in the Department of Telecommunications at Indiana University in
Bloomington, Indiana; Gregory D. Newton is Assistant Professor in the School of Telecommunications
at Ohio University in Athens, Ohio; Paul D. Bolls is Assistant Professor in the Edward R. Murrow School
of Communication at Washington State University in Pullman, Washington. Direct correspondence to
the first author at the Department of Telecommunications, Indiana University, 1229 E. Seventh Street,
Bloomington, IN 47405-5501 or eastman@indiana.edu.

Copyright 2003, National Communication Association
DOI: 10.1080/0090988032000103458
239

JACR                                                                  AUGUST 2003

Olympics, are considered self-promoting, that is, able to attract a mass audience
without help from on-air spots (promos) or newspaper and program-guide
advertising.1 Most of the prime-time schedule depends on a steady stream of
promotional announcements to elicit audience interest in watching shows as
well as to convey reminders of the day and time they appear. Capturing an
audience for a new series program is almost wholly dependent on having both
a strong lead-in and extended on-air promotion, but continuing programs must
also promote upcoming episodes, guests, and plot twists to maintain audiences
(Eastman, 2000).
   Investigating on-air promotion has importance for two reasons. First, promos
can be viewed as a form of high-stakes advertising. On-air promos are, in
essence, advertisements for a television network’s own product, entertainment
programs. As with advertisements, an effective on-air promo will present the
product in a way that will make its target audience want to sample it. The degree
to which on-air promos are successful at advertising programs is presumed to
affect the programs’ ratings which, in turn, substantially impact the amount of
money the network can charge for advertising time during those programs.
   Second, promos are among the factors that influence viewers’ evaluation of
program content; they affect viewer decision making. Promos entice viewers to
sample unfamiliar shows and new episodes of familiar programs. Moreover,
list-style promos and combined spots promoting multiple programs supply a
memory framework for understanding an evening’s program schedule and the
sequence of the individual programs within it which, in turn, influences tuning
behavior. Clearly, such factors as previous experiences with a program (or genre
or even a particular network) and a viewer’s intended viewing situation (with
family or friends, for example) may also impact viewers’ reactions to specific
promos. Nonetheless, previous studies—using thousands of promos and pro-
grams over long periods of time—have demonstrated that promotion appears to
have a modest impact on viewing above and beyond those factors (Billings,
Eastman, & Newton, 1998; Eastman & Newton, 1998b, 1999; Eastman, Newton, &
Pack, 1996: Walker, 1993).
   In on-air promotion, sets of appeals frame the value to viewers of watching
programs, often called the benefits in industry jargon (Eastman, 2002). When a
program is promoted as “very touching,” for example, the benefit is that the
viewer gets to feel the tender emotion of poignancy and experience the better
side of human nature. Similarly, when a program is promoted as suspenseful, the
benefit is the enjoyable experience of being scared—within the wholly safe
context of a television program. Such appeals are equivalent to what researchers
Vakratsas and Ambler (1999) identify as affect in commercial messages. Thus,
emotional appeals are shorthand for a program’s content, or at least for that
portion of the content picked up for inclusion in promos. Although content
appeals are related to the idea of program quality, the former are identifiable and
measurable, whereas quality (or whatever makes programs popular) generally is
not. In practice, the appeals appearing in promos are selected by creative
producers on a largely intuitive basis from the unwieldy mass of program
content, often under pressure from tight deadlines (Ferguson, 2002). Isolating
appeals that significantly affect ratings should contribute to theoretical and
practical models of the process of building audience size. Viewer decision
making is, after all, the central concern in programming research.
240

PROMOTIONAL CONTENT                                                EASTMAN ET AL.

   As previous research has focused almost exclusively on structural factors in
promotion affecting program ratings (such as location, associated clutter, length,
and so on), this study distinguishes between content and structural components
and separates their relative influences. The purpose of this study is to look for
the factors in promos having measurable impact on comedy viewing, as deter-
mined by changes in Nielsen ratings. Another goal of this line of research is to
locate clusters of components that suggest empirically-derived guidelines for
effective promotion.

                              Applicable Theories
   The industry invests billions of dollars annually—even in tight economic
times—in promoting television programs on the presumption that audience size
will be affected. At the same time, explaining promotion’s impact on a theoreti-
cal level necessitates accounting for three characteristics of promotional spots:
(a) entertainment promos typically beget very low level, mundane reactions
rather than highly arousing experiences; (b) promos’ impacts on ratings are
generally modest in size; and (c) promos’ impacts commonly occur in spite of
intervening activities and at substantial distances in time from the programs
being promoted, thus any effects occur across hours, days, and weeks. The size
of any collective impact from a large body of promos must necessarily be small
because of the nearly overwhelming impact of inherited viewing (lead-in ratings)
and program popularity (carriage and promoted program ratings), and the ines-
capable fact that most new programs have such low ratings that they will be
canceled within a year. Moreover, most ratings are understood to contain
significant amounts of sampling and data collection error. Nonetheless, decades
of industry assumptions about the critical impact of promos have led to the
widespread and costly practice of extensive promotion of television shows in
advance of their airdates.
   One problematic issue for research has been identifying the parts of promo-
tional messages that contribute to their effects. A growing body of research on
television advertising addresses message effects from a cognitive and emotional
perspective (Hazlett & Hazlett, 1999; Lang, Bolls, Potter, & Kawahara, 1999;
Nelson, Shavitt, Schennum, & Barkmeier, 1997; Rossiter & Silberstein, 2001;
Yoon, Bolls, & Muehling, 1999). If promos are similar to ads, then cognitive and
emotional processes probably act as the mechanisms underlying the effects of
promotional messages. Two likely candidates for providing a theoretical link
between parts of promotional messages and their cognitive and emotional effects
on viewers are excitation transfer and associative priming effects.
   From a theoretical viewpoint, excitation transfer theory holds that content able
to stir viewers’ emotions may lend those emotions to subsequent viewing
(Mundorf, Zillmann, & Drew, 1991; Zillmann & Weaver, 1999). Indeed, research
has produced evidence of emotional effects transferring from programs to com-
mercials as well as from commercials to adjacent programs. Mattes and Cantor
(1982) showed that watching highly arousing programs led to higher levels of
enjoyment of commercials, and Mathur and Chadpadhyay (1991) showed that
program-induced moods transferred to reactions to commercials. Looking for
humor’s transference effects, Perry, Jenzowski, King, Hester, and Yi (1997) found
that the presence of more humorous commercials in a show increased viewer
241

JACR                                                                    AUGUST 2003

enjoyment and the program’s perceived entertainment value, but found no
influence in the opposite direction. In other words, humor in programs was
found to have no influence on commercial enjoyment (Bjorna, Karsal, Vicary,
Wagner, & Perry, 2001).
   Nonetheless, a form of excitation transfer effect has been proposed as a
possible explanation for how advertising works on a target audience. Associa-
tional learning in consumer behavior occurs when a favorable response evoked
by one stimulus is directed or transferred to another (Mullen & Johnson, 1990).
Advertising that is successful at achieving associational learning evokes positive
emotions and thoughts that get transferred to the product itself. In the context of
on-air promotion, this means that emotions evoked during exposure to a promo
could be transferred to the program and have some influence over viewing
decisions.
   It is important to note that proposing excitation transfer as a possible cognitive
and emotional mechanism behind the effects of on-air promos requires a major
extension of the original theory. In studies of television content, excitation
transfer has only been demonstrated for very short lengths of time, and in
laboratory experiments arousal levels return to normal in seconds. This time-
frame excludes promos’ apparent impact over much longer time periods. Apply-
ing excitation transfer to the effects of promos would require the emotions
evoked by a promo either to (a) maintain some level of activation in working
memory until a viewing decision is made, which could be several days after
exposure to the promo, or (b) reside in long-term memory where emotions are
associated with the program. Clearly, the low-level emotions evoked by promos
are unlikely to remain intensely active over the course of days. Recent models of
human cognition, however, suggest that the emotional characteristics of stimuli
get stored in memory in a manner that enables the activation of the same
emotions when a cue related to the original stimulus is encountered again, even
days later (Damasio, 1994). This means that emotions evoked by a promo could
maintain some level of background activation, and be transferred to the pro-
moted program when a viewer is cued to think about the program.
   Zillmann (1971) has defined the excitation transfer effect as a matter of
incomplete decay of arousal, a definition that excludes promos’ apparent impact
over much longer time periods. It seems, nonetheless, that the association of
emotions with a program during exposure to a promo may be explained as a
re-experiencing that occurs when a viewer is cued to think about a program.
Thus, the process could constitute an incomplete decay of arousal that triggers
another excited state that is then transferred to the program. We have named this
the secondary excitation transfer effect. While this notion goes beyond tra-
ditional views of the excitation transfer effect, it seems premature to rule out a
multistep excitation transfer process as the cognitive and emotional mechanism
behind the effects of promos until further experimental research can be conduc-
ted.
   Associative priming theory also provides part of the conceptual explanation of
the promotional process. Research on associative priming, as Jo and Berkowitz
(1994, p. 46) have pointed out, shows that “ideas having emotional significance
are linked associatively” to subsequent behavior. They also note that thinking
about a behavior increases the likelihood of carrying out that behavior, and that
visual stimuli such as video generate relatively high recall. Roskos-Ewoldsen,
242

PROMOTIONAL CONTENT                                              EASTMAN ET AL.

Roskos-Ewoldsen, and Carpentier (2002) summarize research showing that me-
dia influence later behavior by priming aggression, stereotyping, and political
judgments, and thus, it follows that an arousing television promo that urges
viewers to tune in at a later time to experience a program might well have an
impact on some viewers. While the content may be important because of its
arousing nature, however, any impact on viewing occurs long after particular
jokes and plot moments are forgotten. It may be that secondary arousal transfers
to behavior when the proper occasion (another promo or perhaps merely the
right date and time) occurs. Nonetheless, the explanations falter inasmuch as
situation comedy promos typically generate only the most modest of emotional
reactions of any kind, yet they apparently provoke subsequent viewing (by some
people some of the time). Roskos-Ewoldsen et al. hold that “people have myriad
mental models stored in long-term memory” (2002, p. 112) and that the media
can prime these memories, meaning that media content will make these memor-
ies readily accessible. Extending this mental model view of priming into promo-
tional priming leads to the proposition that promos evoke viewers’ mental
models of television programs or, conversely, that the beginning of a program (or
its title, music, teaser) evokes previously viewed promos that are stored in
long-term memory.
   The model of promotional salience developed by Eastman and Newton (1998b,
1999) adds another level of explanation to this discussion. Salience theory
proposes that on-air promos with specific attributes—those that make promos
prominent in their environments—are more likely to have an impact on viewing
than promos with fewer of those attributes (or that have the attributes but in a
non-salient condition). The idea is that certain conditions of variables—first or
last position in breaks, within-program location, little clutter in breaks, and a
dozen others—are structural factors that enhance attention to promos and conse-
quently generate their impact on program ratings. By extension, salience theory
suggests that aspects of content, such as the appeals, humor, and presentation
(execution in industry jargon) that are the focus of this study, can effectively
create expectations about programs under some conditions and, therefore, have
an impact even without high levels of emotional arousal. Thus, associative
priming and secondary transference may together account for the relationship
between the promo and the program, while salience theory may identify the
conditions of attributes creating the expectations.
   A first step in testing priming along with secondary transference as possible
cognitive and emotional mechanisms behind promotional effects is to find out
whether a relationship between promos’ content components and program
ratings can be isolated. However, programming scholars already know that the
single biggest component of program ratings is viewing inherited from the
preceding program (Webster & Phalen, 1997).2 This study looks only at situation
comedies in order to include the component of humor in promos, but when aired
in prime time, situation comedies are nearly always blocked in groups for the
first hour or two. The consequence is that inheritance should be particularly
strong from comedy to comedy, irrespective of the amount and type of pro-
motion received by the individual programs. While inheritance and other struc-
tural characteristics of promos have previously been shown to affect program
ratings (see Eastman & Bolls, 2000 for a summary), this study provides the first
243

JACR                                                                      AUGUST 2003

direct test of the assumption that certain aspects of content also impact audience
size.

  H1: In addition to structural components, promos’ content components will
      significantly affect the ratings of promoted comedies.

  Turning to the factor of program popularity as measured by Nielsen ratings,
Eastman and Newton (1998a, 1998b) showed that promotion’s impact was
greatest on the mid-rated shows, probably because the top-rated hits were
already about as high in audience size as they could go, and the bottom-rated
losers would be promoted less and less until they were canceled. The Eastman
and Newton findings led to the practice of assessing promotion’s impact on the
two mid quartiles (that is, the middle half) of programs as subdivided by their
ratings. The consistency of previous findings for structural variables suggests a
second hypothesis.

  H2: Content-related promotional variables will account for more variance in ratings
      for mid-rated comedies than for high-rated or low-rated comedies.

  Practitioners concerned with the effects of advertising have been asking how
ads work for decades. Based on a meta-analysis of advertising studies, Vakratsas
and Ambler (1999) proposed a three-dimensional model of advertising effective-
ness with components of cognition, affect, and experience. They concluded that
the cognitive aspects of ads were more important than their affective dimensions
for high-involvement products, but that the affective aspects were more import-
ant for low-involvement products; they also found that experience mattered most
for mature, familiar products. Although their model was derived from studies of
ads for consumer goods, it can be adapted to program promotion: If promos are
indeed low-involvement messages, then they are most likely to be most effective
when they emphasize affect. Nevertheless, structural factors must be accounted
for, leading to this hypothesis:

  H3: After the structural variables of lead-in ratings and carriage program ratings,
      appeals in comedy promos will contribute more to ratings variance than
      presentation or humor.

   Walker (1993), however, showed that the familiarity of the program was key to
effective promotion, and that more frequent promotion of continuing (but not
new) programs was positively related to their ratings. Taking Vakratsas and
Ambler’s (1999) and Walker’s conclusions a step further, when promos are for
familiar programs, affect will be the crucial component, whereas in promos for
unfamiliar programs, structural variables will dominate and, consequently, con-
tent variables should have relatively little impact. The question then shifts back
to the sources of affect in program promos. It may be that the appeals in on-air
promos, especially those alleging benefits for viewers, will most impact viewing
of familiar programs. Paraphrased, these extensions lead to this hypothesis:

  H4: The ratings for familiar comedy programs will be more affected than the ratings
      for unfamiliar comedies by the affective appeals contained within the promos.
244

PROMOTIONAL CONTENT                                               EASTMAN ET AL.

                        Prior Research About Humor
   Another factor, related to appeals and likely to be crucial when evaluating
promos for situation comedies, is comedy or humor—a widely-used indicator of
arousal. A reasonable presumption in most studies was that more humor implies
greater likely impact. According to Speck (1990), scholars have identified several
basic processes in humor, three of which apply to television commercials. The
first of these, arousal-safety (or Ahhh humor), is a mechanism that allows relief
from strain or from the need to repress feelings. Laughter usually occurs when
a person experiences increased arousal but evaluates the stimulus (typically
another person) as safe, cute, or inconsequential, as in much family humor. The
second type of humor, identified as incongruity-resolution (or Ah-Ha humor),
occurs when two ideas cannot be assimilated using the same schema or, in other
words, when the outcome of a story or event doesn’t fit with the audience’s
expectations, as occurs in puns, punch lines, comic reversals, understatement,
and exaggeration. The third type of humor Speck calls humorous disparagement
(or Ha-Ha humor), referring to censure, detraction, denunciation, or in present-
day slang, dissing. Much hostile or aggressive humor falls into this category.
Because writers and producers generally want the audience to feel good after
jokes or laugh at put-downs, they avoid getting mean or ugly in promos, and
because clips are very rarely short (usually excerpted from the program) and
directed at broad audiences, promos are very subtle or witty. All three types of
humor (and perhaps several others) probably appear within comedy programs,
but only certain types will be selected by the producers or programming and
promotion executives to appear within promos. In preparation for this study, a
preliminary survey of adult reactions to sitcom promos (Eastman & Bolls, 1999)
showed that virtually all the humor in a large promo sample was identified by
viewers as one of these three types. Because measuring the quantitative amount
of humor proved unreliable in pilot studies, this analysis focuses on the type of
humor and investigates the relationship between type and impact.
   An additional factor is the presentation of the promo. Eastman and Bolls
(2000) analyzed open-ended responses from nearly 2,000 adult television view-
ers about why they might or might not watch a new program based solely on
viewing a promo. Analysis showed that more than one-third of comments related
to the promos (37%), rather than to the actors, storylines, or networks, and
focused on humor and presentation (music, effects) within the promo.
   The advertising literature contains many studies of execution that look at the
way commercials (and by extension promos, if considered as advertisements for
programs) are implemented. Some studies have examined how the viewer feels
about the presentation of the product being pitched. Lang (1990), for example,
has demonstrated that physiological responses to structural features of commer-
cials are intensified by mild emotional content. Two feelings can be dis-
tinguished: (a) how the viewer feels about the product based on purchase, use,
price, quality, and so on, and (b) how the viewer feels the product is being
presented in a commercial (Laskey, Fox, & Crask, 1994; Stewart & Furse, 1986).
Although viewers’ feelings about television shows are part of the equation
leading to ratings, this study focuses on the second meaning of presentation (or
execution) to assess that characteristic of on-air promos. Lang, Geiger, Strick-
werda, and Sumner (1993) and Yoon, Bolls, and Lang (1998) examined aspects of
245

JACR                                                                      AUGUST 2003

presentation in advertising research that include pacing in terms of length and
number of editing cuts, narrative structure, recognition of background scenes,
and the degree to which the message informs users about the things they want
to know. When their findings are applied to promos, the use of quick cuts or
relatively slow-paced camera changes (apparent pacing) might be expected to
transfer to viewers’ expectations about promoted programs and thus impact
ratings to some small degree. As Eastman (2002) points out, all measurable
impacts are likely to be small but significant because tiny shifts in ratings affect
millions of dollars in annual advertising revenue.
   The focus of this study was not the appeals, humor, and presentation in the
programs themselves. Instead, it was expected that within 5-, 10-, 15- and
30-second promos the inclusion of certain kinds of appeals or representations of
certain kinds of humor might have close association with certain types of
presentation and thus, as a group, affect ratings (in addition to the well-recog-
nized elements of inherited viewing and the structural variables in promotion
identified in previous studies). If such combinations could be identified, they
would further refine the current models of advertising and promotional effective-
ness and suggest empirically-based guides for industry practitioners. Because so
little research has been published about the role of content in on-air promotion,
in addition to the four hypotheses already identified, this study posed one broad
research question. It serves as a first step towards forming a model to guide
practitioners and subsequent research into promotion:

  RQ1: Can combinations of certain kinds of presentation, specific appeals, and types
       of humor be identified within promos that associate with significant increases
       in comedy program ratings?

                                      Method
   Because prime time is the most watched daypart and because half-hour
programs maximized the number of programs in the database, this study looked
only at prime-time promotion. It analyzed only on-air promos for half-hour
situation comedies (live or animated); humor was included and genre-related
variation excluded. The prime-time hours on all six broadcast networks were
videotaped by the researchers for six weeks: two weeks in October 1998, two
weeks in May 1998, and two weeks in April/May 1999.3 These purposive
samples encompass the highly-produced promos for new program introductions
(October), where many programs would be unfamiliar, big-budget cliffhangers
comprising the culminating episodes of many series (April), and the end-of-sea-
son program specials (May); they also avoid the extreme fluctuations in audience
size occurring between winter and summer and reflect a mix of sweeps and
nonsweeps months. Promotional messages of less than three seconds in length
were excluded, and no promos longer than 30 seconds were reported.

Coders
  In addition to the three researchers, three classes of student coders worked on
this project. One class pretested the coding sheet in two iterations, resulting in
the rewording of confusing or redundant appeals, reducing the number of
246

PROMOTIONAL CONTENT                                               EASTMAN ET AL.

options for narrative structure, and eliminating measures of the quantity or
degree of humor because reliabilities were low. Two subsequent classes of 22
and 31 students analyzed the videotaped promos, and all 218 tapes were coded
at least twice, each time by different students. The first class analyzed the four
weeks of 1998 tapes; the second class reanalyzed the four weeks of 1998 as well
as the two weeks of 1999 tapes. The students undertook the coding for a grade
as part of an upper-division seminar in electronic media promotion, and they
were required to write a separately-graded paper about the research process. Two
class periods were devoted to training each class of coders, focusing on distin-
guishing promos for network sitcoms from local rerun sitcom spots, identifying
appeals and types of humor, utilizing the semantic-differential scales, and
counting shot changes. Stopwatches were supplied to measure promo length.
  In the training and written instructions, coders were repeatedly directed to
give their attention to the promo and its content and presentation, and not to
consider their reactions to whatever program was being promoted. Because they
were enrolled on a course specifically about media promotion and marketing, not
programming, focusing on the promos was a reasonable expectation.

Coding Instrument
   The coding sheets requested nine items of structural and sorting data, includ-
ing (a) identification of the type (or absence) of humor, (b) an assessment of the
promoted program’s familiarity to the coder, (c) 14 ratings of specific appeals,
and (d) four measures of presentation of the promo’s content. Following
specification of the network, date and time, and name of the program containing
the promo (the carriage program), coders were asked to code the name, date and
time of the sitcom being promoted, the construction of the spot (operationalized
as the number of named programs included in the promo), and the distance
between the promo and its program (operationalized as the next show; later
tonight; tomorrow night; later in the same week; next week—between seven and
14 days away; and much later—with space to supply a date if one was given).
   Coders classified the predominant type of humor used in the promo by
choosing one of three options: relief/cute (followed by a definition and the words
“arousal-safety or Ahhh!”), surprise/mismatch (definition and “unexpected in-
congruity or Ah-Ha!”), and put-down/sarcasm (definition and “dissing and satire
or Ha-Ha!”). Pretesting using promos not in the sample had demonstrated that
the coders could consistently distinguish between types of humor and identify
one dominant type even when more than one type appeared in 30-second spots.
In a few cases, promos for sitcoms had no humor component, which was
recorded.
   The coding instrument listed 14 appeals to rate on a nine-step semantic
differential, anchored by not very or weak on the low end and very or
strong on the high end. Following the heading question “How does the promo
make the show seem to look?” were seven phrases anchored from negative to
positive, as in not very funny to very funny, along with not very realistic, not
very new, not very uplifting, not very hip/trendy, not very critically acclaimed,
and not very suspenseful. Following the heading question “What does the promo
make the show seem to have?” were seven more appeals: a not very puzzling
situation (to a very puzzling situation, and so on), weak star appeal, weak sex
247

JACR                                                                   AUGUST 2003

appeal, characters not easy to identify with, not very realistic characters, a not
very intriguing situation, and no network popularity.
   Presentation was operationalized in four ways: as background scene, narrative
structure, informational value, and the pacing of the promo. For the primary
background scene, the options were bar/restaurant, workplace, school, home/
family, singles’ apartment, and other. Based on the results of pretesting, three
options for the promo’s narrative structure were provided: clip, with or without
a voice over (a short scene from the show with dialogue); announcement (a cast
member speaking directly to the audience, sometimes accompanied by a brief
voiceover); and other. The informational value of each promo was assessed with
the question: “How informative was this promo?” Coders responded from not at
all informative to very informative on a nine-step semantic differential.
   Pacing was assessed in two ways. Coders were asked to count the number of
cuts (shot changes) in a promo (or in a segment dealing with one program during
a spot promoting multiple shows) and to record the clock length in seconds of
the promo (or individual segment). Pacing was then calculated as the number of
cuts divided by length. Coders also made their own assessment of the pacing on
a five-step semantic differential, ranging from very slow to very fast. Finally, for
sorting purposes, a global measure of the coder’s familiarity with the promoted
program was also obtained using a nine-step semantic differential anchored by
the terms not very familiar and very familiar.
   Subsequent to coding, four additional kinds of information were added to the
database by the researchers. The frequency of promotion of each program was
calculated from the database, and the published ratings for each carriage and
promoted program were added, in the latter case utilizing only the rating for the
episode closest in time to the promo’s appearance. To account for the impact of
inheritance, the rating for the lead-in program was also entered. When unavail-
able because the sitcom was scheduled as a prime-time lead-off, the mean of all
lead-ins in that two-week period was substituted for the missing data, thus
maintaining sample size and utilizing all identified promos and situation come-
dies.4

Ratings Analysis
  For the analyses dealing only with mid-rated comedies, researchers identified
the range of high to low ratings separately for each network because the range of
ratings varies even within the “Big Three” (ABC, CBS, NBC) and widely outside
them (Fox, UPN, WB). One network’s ratings, for example, might have a high of
17.5 and low of 5.5, while another has a high of only 4.9 and a low of 1.9. The
middle half of each network’s ratings was calculated independent of the other
networks by computer subroutine.

Coding Reliability
  The researchers looked for coding errors at five stages in the research process.
During an initial examination of completed coding sheets, they discarded those
with blank or off-scale responses, and scheduled the tapes for recoding. In
addition, absence on a training day, and in-class responses or project papers
showing misunderstandings of the protocols, triggered recoding of a student’s
248

PROMOTIONAL CONTENT                                                EASTMAN ET AL.

tapes. After the second tentatively approved coding, the two analyses were
compared on administrative items and factual items about the carriage and
promoted programs, spot construction, distance, pacing, length, background
scene, and narrative structure. Any discrepancies in these items led to a third (or
fourth) coding. During data entry by the researchers, the coding sheets were
again examined for missing or inconsistent data. Whenever the ratings of the 14
appeals and one scaled information item differed by more than two steps on the
nine-step scales, a recoding was undertaken (blind to the original evaluations).
Because the number of students in the second participating class was not an
exact multiple of the number of tapes to be coded, students were available for
repeated recodings for class credit. In addition, one student was hired to recode
the promos in 16 of the 218 tapes. When two codings of the appeals agreed
within two degrees (within two steps on each of the 15 items) and on all other
factual items, one of the codings, selected at random, was subsequently retained
in the database. Finally, during statistical analysis, high measures of inconsis-
tency on some appeals flagged two students’ work and led to recoding of all four
of their tapes. As a final check, comparisons were made of 12 randomly selected
pairs of final coding sheets. Following Holsti’s (1969) method, calculations
showed 93% reliability on the 14 appeals (in other words, an average of five of
the 126 decision steps differed by up to two degrees per pair).

                                     Results
   Altogether, 1,547 promos for 155 different situation comedies (for more than
300 different episodes) were located. Results showed that two-thirds (68%) of
the promos were multiple spots promoting more than one program (in the
patterns of two to four sitcoms together), leaving just one-third as single spots
(32%). More than one third of the promos (37%) were for programs scheduled
later the same week; 20% were for the same night; 17% for the next night; and
23% for the following week (generally the next episode); only 3% were for a
program more than one week away. The promos’ narrative structure was similar
from program to program as well as from network to network because of the
current style of using a clip from the program with a voice-over narration,
although occasionally, a star spoke directly to the audience (LL Cool J of In the
House, and Wayans Brothers). Thus, lacking variance, no distinctions in impact
could emerge for the narrative aspect of promo presentation. About one third
(34%) of the comedy promos had a home or family situation as the primary
situation, the second most frequent location was the workplace (31%), with a
mix of bars/restaurants (15%), singles’ apartments (9%), and mixed other loca-
tions (11%) having progressively smaller proportions of promo backgrounds.
   The number of cuts in the promos varied from less than three (33%), four to
five (26%), six to eight (21%), to more than eight (20%). In general, coders
assessed the promos’ pacing as fast (36%) or in the middle (36%), rather than
very fast (15%) or slow or very slow (13%). The three kinds of humor were fairly
well distributed across the sample, with 38% using cute type, 36% using
surprise, 24% using sarcasm, and just 2% not funny.
   The variable of promo length was measured as the total clock time (in seconds)
devoted to a single program. A 30-second multiple spot might have portions of
differing lengths devoted to its programs. The distribution of lengths was as
249

JACR                                                                      AUGUST 2003

follows: 27% were of 5 seconds or less, 39% were of 6–10 seconds, 15% were
of 11–15 seconds, 10% were of 16–20 seconds, and 9% were of 21–30 seconds.5

Promo Content to Program Impact
   The first and most important question driving this study was whether reac-
tions to content significantly impacted program ratings, and as Tables 1 to 3
show, they certainly appeared to do so. Although multiple regression analyses
revealed that the well-established inheritance and structural variables related to
promotion predicted the greatest amounts of variance in most analyses, several
content-related variables also appeared to contribute significantly to subsequent
program ratings. For example, Table 1 reports the analysis of the 1,547 promos
for the total database of all 155 programs airing in 1998 and 1999.
   In the overall analysis, the combination of inheritance, structural variables
(carriage program rating, number of promos aired), and five appeal and presen-
tation variables accounted for 78% of the variance in program ratings (adj.
R2 ⫽ .781). The content variables—fostering character identification, surprise
humor, (not) seeming new, suspenseful, and promo length—produced changes
in R2 totaling .062, a relatively small but still significant contribution to the total
target program ratings in a business where tenths of a ratings point are treated as
important because they are financially valuable. This result supports the first
hypothesis—that content matters significantly, in addition to structure, in com-
edy promotion.

Program Popularity
  The second hypothesis posited a greater impact of content-related promotional
variables for mid-rated comedies compared to high-rated or low-rated comedies.
Table 2 shows the results of a stepwise regression analysis for the two middle
quartiles of promoted programs, containing 85 mid-rated programs and 837
promos. Table 3 reports the results of a similar analysis of the 70 programs and
710 promos in the high- and low-rated quartiles. The overall difference between
the two groups was substantial, as 7% more variance was explained for the
mid-rated programs (adj. R2 ⫽ .840) than for the high- and low-rated shows (adj.
R2 ⫽ .762). Moreover, more content variables, including the elements of humor
and presentation, were significant factors in the mid-quartile ratings than in the
extreme quartiles. However, contrary to Hypothesis 2, the content variables
alone accounted for more of the ratings variance for the high- and low-rated
shows (9% came from relating to characters, suspenseful, and seems new) while
predicting less for the mid-quartile programs (just 6% came from star appeal,
hip, cute humor, and length). Thus, Hypothesis 2 was not supported.
  Looking at the content variables, however, reveals some interesting differences
between mid-rated programs and (1) those much more or much less successful
and (2) as compared with the overall analysis reported in Table 1. Fewer separate
content variables were significant predictors for the high- and low-rated shows,
but one appears to be particularly important. Fostering identification with
characters explains more than 4% of the variance for those programs, nearly
twice as much as any other content variable in any of the regressions. The
250

PROMOTIONAL CONTENT                                                                EASTMAN ET AL.

                                             TABLE 1
                Hierarchical (Stepwise) Regression Analysis of Appeal, Presentation,
                               and Humor Variables for All Programs

          Variable                      B           SE B           B          R2        Adj. R2

Step 1                                                                        .661         .658
  Lead-in rating                        .948         .055        .813*
Step 2                                                                        .044         .700
  Lead-in rating                        .810         .059        .694*
  Carriage program rating               .257         .054        .240*
Step 3                                                                        .019         .717
  Lead-in rating                        .799         .057        .685*
  Carriage program rating               .250         .053        .233*
  Relate to characters                  .399         .125        .137**
Step 4                                                                        .014         .729
  Lead-in rating                       .761          .058        .652*
  Carriage program rating              .237          .052        .221*
  Relate to characters                 .358          .123        .123**
  n of promos                         6.788E-02      .024        .124**
Step 5                                                                        .014         .742
  Lead-in rating                       .745          .057        .639*
  Carriage program rating              .238          .051        .222*
  Relate to characters                 .332          .121        .114**
  n of promos                         7.344E-02      .024        .134**
  Surprise humor                      1.061          .370        .119**
Step 6                                                                        .009         .749
  Lead-in rating                       .746          .056        .640*
  Carriage program rating              .219          .051        .204*
  Relate to characters                 .377          .121        .130**
  n of promos                         6.864E-02      .024        .126**
  Surprise humor                      1.074          .365        .120**
  Program seems new                  ⫺ .282          .123      ⫺ .096***
Step 7                                                                        .011         .759
  Lead-in rating                       .743          .055        .638*
  Carriage program rating              .210          .050        .196*
  Relate to characters                 .280          .124        .096***
  n of promos                         7.474E-02      .023        .137**
  Surprise humor                       .913          .362        .102***
  Program seems new                  ⫺ .343          .122      ⫺ .117**
  Program seems suspenseful            .326          .121        .116**
Step 8                                                                        .009         .768
  Lead-in rating                       .758          .054        .650*
  Carriage program rating              .172          .051        .160*
  Relate to characters                 .266          .122        .091***
  n of promos                         8.328E-02      .023        .152*
  Surprise humor                       .822          .358        .092***
  Program seems new                  ⫺ .382          .121      ⫺ .130**
  Program seems suspenseful            .335          .119        .119**
  Promo length                         .550          .220        .104***

Note: n of programs ⫽ 155; n of promos ⫽ 1,547; *p ⬍ .001; **p ⬍ .01; ***p ⬍ .05

negative coefficients for both appeals to hipness and cute humor suggest that
contemporary audiences (at least younger ones, the avowed target audiences for
most comedies) may respond best to spots with some edge but not to overt
messages such as “look how cool this show is.”
  The results also provide some support for Hypothesis 3, which predicted that
appeals would matter more than presentation or type of humor. Based on either
the number of times appeals appear in the analyses, or on total predictive power,
certain appeals (especially fostering character identification) explain more vari-
251

JACR                                                                                   AUGUST 2003

                                            TABLE 2
               Hierarchical (Stepwise) Regression Analysis of Appeal, Presentation,
                          and Humor Variables for Mid-Rated Programs

        Variable                        B           SE B           B            R2       Adj. R2

Step 1                                                                          .641        .636
  Lead-in rating                        .883         .073        .800*
Step 2                                                                          .130        .765
  Lead-in rating                        .623         .070        .564*
  Carriage program rating               .257         .038        .431*
Step 3                                                                          .025        .788
  Lead-in rating                        .544         .071        .493*
  Carriage program rating               .255         .036        .428*
  n of promos                          7.44E-02      .024        .174**
Step 4                                                                          .016        .802
  Lead-in rating                       .561          .069        .509*
  Carriage program rating              .228          .036        .383*
  n of promos                         6.809E-02      .023        .159**
  Star appeal                          .212          .082        .132***
Step 5                                                                          .017        .817
  Lead-in rating                       .531          .067        .481*
  Carriage program rating              .204          .036        .342*
  n of promos                        6.3551E-02      .022        .149**
  Star appeal                          .288          .084        .179*
  Program seems hip                  ⫺ .331          .120      ⫺ .146**
Step 6                                                                          .014        .830
  Lead-in rating                       .539          .065        .488*
  Carriage program rating              .179          .036        .300*
  n of promos                         5.871E-02      .021        .137**
  Star appeal                          .379          .088        .236*
  Program seems hip                  ⫺ .392          .118      ⫺ .173*
  Cute humor                         ⫺ .776          .292      ⫺ .131**
Step 7                                                                          .011        .840
  Lead-in rating                       .562          .063        .510*
  Carriage program rating              .152          .036        .256*
  n of promos                         6.107E-02      .021        .143*
  Star appeal                          .366          .085        .288*
  Program seems hip                  ⫺ .387          .114      ⫺ .171*
  Cute humor                         ⫺ .719          .284      ⫺ .122***
  Length                               .434          .180        .113***

Note: n of programs ⫽ 85; n of promos ⫽ 837; *p ⬍ .001; **p ⬍ .01; ***p ⬍ .05

ance than the type of humor employed or the presentation (where only length
may be significant). Nonetheless, humor and presentation did contribute
significantly to the variance in promos for mid-rated program ratings but not to
the promos for highly popular or unpopular programs.

Impact of Familiarity
  The fourth hypothesis predicted that appeals would impact the ratings for
familiar comedies more than for unfamiliar comedies. Using familiarity as a
binary (familiar/unfamiliar) sorting variable, the results reported in Table 4 and
Table 5 indicate that although all of the variables accounted for nearly 20% more
variance in the ratings for familiar (adj. R2 ⫽ .927) than for unfamiliar (adj.
R2 ⫽ .759) programs, the contributions made by appeals were very similar.
However, the effective appeals were very different for the two categories of
programs: Structural variables and appeals contributed most significantly to the
252

PROMOTIONAL CONTENT                                                                        EASTMAN ET AL.

                                            TABLE 3
               Hierarchical (Stepwise) Regression Analysis of Appeal, Presentation,
                     and Humor Variables for High-and Low-Rated Programs

          Variable                        B             SE B               B             R2     Adj. R2

Step 1                                                                                   .653      .648
  Lead-in rating                         .967             .085           .808*
Step 2                                                                                   .046      .690
  Lead-in rating                         .911             .082           .761*
  Relate to characters                   .760             .239           .219*
Step 3                                                                                   .027      .713
  Lead-in rating                         .767             .097           .641*
  Relate to characters                   .680             .232           .196**
  Carriage program rating                .394             .156           .207***
Step 4                                                                                   .017      .727
  Lead-in rating                         .746             .096           .623*
  Relate to characters                   .610             .229           .175**
  Carriage program rating                .426             .153           .224**
  Program seems suspenseful              .422             .203           .133***
Step 5                                                                                   .026      .750
  Lead-in rating                         .735             .091           .614*
  Relate to characters                   .628             .218           .181**
  Carriage program rating                .420             .146           .221**
  Program seems suspenseful              .554             .200           .175**
  Program seems new                    ⫺ .615             .229           .167**
Step 6                                                                                   .014      .762
  Lead-in rating                         .707             .090            .591*
  Relate to characters                   .544             .218            .157***
  Carriage program rating                .394             .143            .207**
  Program seems suspenseful              .601             .197            .190**
  Program seems new                    ⫺ .604             .223          ⫺ .164**
  n of promos                           7.583E-02         .038            .127***

Note: n of programs ⫽ 70; n of promos ⫽ 710; *p ⬍ .001; **p ⬍ .01; ***p ⬍ .05

ratings for familiar programs. Presentation (length) was a factor only for unfam-
iliar shows, whereas type of humor (surprise) was significant only for familiar
show ratings.
   Table 4 shows that for the 45 familiar programs promoted in 492 promos, two
structural variables (inheritance and carriage program rating) accounted for

                                            TABLE 4
               Summaries of Hierarchical (Stepwise) Regression Analysis of Appeal,
                   Presentation, and Humor Variables for Familiar Programs

                      Variable                      B            SE B                B

              Lead-in rating                       .729          .053               .668*
              Carriage program rating              .102          .025               .203*
              Network popularity                   .329          .075               .194*
              Surprise humor                      1.050          .224               .195*
              Program seems hip                  ⫺ .405          .109             ⫺ .174*
              Star appeal                          .189          .075               .114***

              R2 ⫽ .773 for Step 1; .079 for Step 2; .024 for Step 3; .031 for Step 4; .021
              for Step 5; and .011 for Step 6.
              Adjusted R2 ⫽ .767 for Step 1; .844 for Step 2; .866 for Step 3; .897 for Step
              4; .917 for Step 5; and .927 for Step 6.
              Note: n of programs ⫽ 45; n of promos ⫽ 492; *p ⬍ .001; **p ⬍ .01;
              ***p ⬍ .05
253

JACR                                                                                  AUGUST 2003

                                         TABLE 5
            Summaries of Hierarchical (Stepwise) Regression Analysis of Appeal,
               Presentation, and Humor Variables for Unfamiliar Programs

                    Variable                     B          SE B              B

          Lead-in rating                        .720         .071          .610*
          Carriage program rating               .289         .087          .204*
          Identify with characters              .376         .159          .120***
          Program seems suspenseful             .461         .154          .152**
          Program seems new                   ⫺ .432         .160        ⫺ .134**
          N of promos                          7.943E-02     .030          .137**
          Length of promo                       .625         .280          .108***

          R2 ⫽ .654 for Step 1; .043 for Step 2; .026 for Step 3; .012 for Step 4; .016
          for Step 5; .012 for Step 6; and .011 for Step 7.
          Adjusted R2 ⫽ .651 for Step 1; .692 for Step 2; .716 for Step 3; .725 for Step
          4; .739 for Step 5; .749 for Step 6; and .759 for Step 7.
          Note: n of programs ⫽ 110; n of promos ⫽ 1,055; *p ⬍ .001; **p ⬍ .01;
          ***p ⬍ .05

approximately 84% of the ratings variance. Three appeals (network popularity,
seeming hip, and star power) added an additional 5%. The remainder (3%) was
attributed to the use of surprise humor.
   In contrast, Table 5 shows that the conventional structural variables account
for only 70% of the ratings variance for the 110 unfamiliar programs, and
presentation (length) adds an additional 1%. The significant appeals for unfam-
iliar shows were identifying with characters, suspense, and (negatively) seeming
new, which collectively added 5%. Thus, Hypothesis 4 was not supported
because the size of variance accounted for by the appeals was identical for both
familiar and unfamiliar program ratings. The differences lay in the contributions
of humor and presentation. Nonetheless, appeals and humor together accounted
for 8% for familiar programs, whereas appeals and presentation accounted for
just 6% for unfamiliar programs.

                                        Discussion
  First, and most important, these findings strongly support the presumption
that promos have a significant and measurable impact on program ratings.
Second, the content—not just the structure—of promos was demonstrated to
impact program ratings in this study. Third, affective appeals contributed sub-
stantially more to promotional impact than the forms of humor or execution in
the promos. Fourth, the impact of promo content was greater for familiar than
unfamiliar programs. Fifth, the impact of humor and presentation were greater
for promos promoting mid-rated than for top- or bottom-rated comedies. At the
same time, the results also confirm the expected powerful impact of inherited
viewing on program ratings and the influential role of scheduling of promotional
messages to reach a wide audience. The consistency of such findings with the
long tradition of programming research adds credence to these results.
  Particularly new in this study was the identification of some of the specific
appeals that made a difference. For television comedies, the key content ele-
ments appear to be: having characters that can be identified with; having
dramatic suspense and realism; using big stars; avoiding self-identified trendi-
254

PROMOTIONAL CONTENT                                                EASTMAN ET AL.

ness and newness in appeals; and avoiding cute humor. While these results may
not generalize to other program genres, and some key appeals may have been
omitted, these findings provide the first examination of promotional content
outside the boundaries of sexual and violent content. The importance of foster-
ing identification with characters in hit programs will be no surprise to program
producers and writers, but what emerges here is its importance to successful
promotion. The ability of audiences to relate to characters, and to feel some
element of suspense related to the program episode, appear to be significant
factors in the effectiveness of promotion for both high-rated and low-rated
programs. Successful promotion of mid-rated shows, in contrast, seems to be
driven to a greater degree by structural factors, by appealing to the star power of
the actors, and by allotting sufficient time to the spots (or to the individual
programs in a multiple spot) to get the promotional message across (promo
length). Further, these findings offer preliminary evidence that the content with
which programs are promoted affects viewing decisions. This significant rela-
tionship should give experimental researchers the confidence to test such
specific mechanisms as excitation transfer and priming that could be behind the
relationship.
   On the theoretical level, these findings add strength to the presumption that
even the very modest interest stirred by on-air promotion measurably primes
behavior related to program ratings. This occurs despite the low level of
arousal—if that is the appropriate word for such a mundane effect—created by
promos. It occurs despite the regular presence of intervening activities including
other programming and the extended time lapse between the airing of promos
and the airing of promoted programs. For familiar shows in particular, the
affective and humor components are effective (and more important), probably
because they aid viewers’ recall of positive experiences and build anticipation
through their desire for subsequent similar experiences. To be maximally effec-
tive, then, promos should contain cognitive and emotional elements that trigger
viewer recall of previous experiences with a program and emphasize the success-
ful nature of the show. As examples, one might point to the very successful
“must-see-TV” theme and the widespread use of variants of “Last Sunday, 30
million Americans saw. . . .”

Limitations and Future Research
  As previously mentioned, executional (presentational) variables are commonly
used in laboratory and marketing studies. Their infrequent appearance in these
analyses may be measurement artifacts or may represent real differences between
advertising and promotional messages. The variable of promo length, an aspect
of pacing, becomes of particular interest because it was the only presentational
variable to show up in the overall and mid-quartile regression analyses as a
significant factor in promos’ impact on program ratings.
  Another qualification is that the regression method employed lacks the rigor of
laboratory experimentation for deducing causal explanations, while simul-
taneously being too demanding for situations where the variance is small (as in
the smaller networks’ ratings). Nonetheless, while more extended tests of
specific findings are highly desirable, along with using evaluators (coders)
reflecting a wider range of demographics, these results have remarkable simi-
255

JACR                                                                  AUGUST 2003

larity to the findings of some previous studies of structural variables in pro-
motion (Eastman & Newton, 1998a, 1998b). Thus, despite limitations, the
findings confirm some general outlines that should be expected to continue to
hold up in subsequent research into promotion.
   In addition, given that the affective appeals and content pacing were rated by
college students and were necessarily somewhat subjective (or at least, time
bound), generalization from these findings to a wider range of age groups is not
warranted. While the specific appeals probably differ for demographic groups
other than young adults, these finding do provide the first steps toward isolating
the powerful content elements in promos. Moreover, college students are part of
the demographic groups that programmers want to attract as viewers (adults
18–34 and 18–49) and are an ideal target for building loyalty (bonding) to
specific programs, one of the other key goals of promotion.
   While the collective contribution of the content variables to the variance
accounted for in this study remains small, it must be kept in mind that the
end-of-season differences in ratings from the number one to number two network
may be as small as one-tenth of a ratings point. This was indeed the case in May
1999, the end of that year’s 36-week season. CBS was ahead by one-tenth of a
ratings point (9.0 to 8.9, season-to-date) and was equal in share points (at 15), a
situation that was unchanged for much of the year. ABC, the third-ranked
network, was usually less than one ratings point behind throughout that year.
Thus, the amount of variance available to be found is small but vital to the
industry because it represents millions of dollars in advertising revenue, and
important to scholars in demonstrating the applicability of priming and salience
theories.
   Future applied research needs to assess the kinds of content factors salient to
appeals in promos for other genres of programs, as well as the ideal and
maximum distance of promos from various genres of programs. It can be
reasonably expected, for example, that effects would occur over greater lengths
of time for specials and movies but only over relatively shorter lengths of time
for episodic promos for series. Future theoretical research in laboratory experi-
ments needs to pinpoint the exact mechanism that accounts for the secondary
excitation transfer effect or priming effect.

Practical Applications
  The importance of on-air promotion when assessing the sources of prime-time
ratings lends practical value to this study. The findings have implications for
both television programming executives and scholars interested in teaching and
researching program promotion because they provide empirical evidence to
guide the design of effective comedy promos. First, the results illustrate how
similar program promotion is to product advertising. As in advertising, the type
of appeal used matters a great deal. Another similarity is that, as in product
advertising, brand (or program) familiarity and popularity impact the roles of
appeals in determining promotion’s effectiveness. The pattern of results in this
study should give television executives added confidence in drawing on the
knowledge gained from advertising research when designing effective strategies
for program promotion. Second, the study shows the need to distinguish pro-
grams by their ranking in the ratings when creating promos and probably to
You can also read