HOW IMPORTANT ARE JOB ATTITUDES? META-ANALYTIC COMPARISONS OF INTEGRATIVE BEHAVIORAL OUTCOMES AND TIME SEQUENCES
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
娀 Academy of Management Journal 2006, Vol. 49, No. 2, 305–325. HOW IMPORTANT ARE JOB ATTITUDES? META-ANALYTIC COMPARISONS OF INTEGRATIVE BEHAVIORAL OUTCOMES AND TIME SEQUENCES DAVID A. HARRISON Pennsylvania State University DANIEL A. NEWMAN University of Maryland PHILIP L. ROTH Clemson University Drawing on the compatibility principle in attitude theory, we propose that overall job attitude (job satisfaction and organizational commitment) provides increasingly pow- erful prediction of more integrative behavioral criteria (focal performance, contextual performance, lateness, absence, and turnover combined). The principle was sustained by a combination of meta-analysis and structural equations showing better fit of unified versus diversified models of meta-analytic correlations between those criteria. Overall job attitude strongly predicted a higher-order behavioral construct, defined as desirable contributions made to one’s work role (r ⴝ .59). Time-lagged data also supported this unified, attitude-engagement model. Job attitudes and job performance are perhaps the than they were to in-role performance (e.g., Herz- two most central and enduring sets of constructs in berg, Mausner, Peterson, & Capwell, 1957; Vroom, individual-level organizational research. Yet, a 1964). Subsequent quantitative reviews also failed longstanding debate persists about the nature and to show job attitudes as having strong predictive the strength of relationships between these funda- utility. One meta-analysis reported a lackluster mental predictors and criteria (Austin & Villanova, value (ˆ ⫽ .17) as the best estimate of the correla- 1992; Brief, 1998; Johns, 1998; Judge, Thoreson, tion between satisfaction and performance (Iaffal- Bono, & Patton, 2001). An elemental question re- dano & Muchinsky, 1985). Another review showed mains: How important are job attitudes for predict- organizational commitment bore a weaker relation- ing and understanding job performance in particu- ship to job performance (ˆ ⫽ .14) than to at least lar, and work role– directed behaviors in general? one withdrawal behavior, turnover (ˆ ⫽ ⫺.28; Authors of early qualitative reviews concluded Mathieu & Zajac, 1990). Consequently, the pendu- that only weak support existed for the relationship lum of causal potency has swung away from job between one principal attitude, job satisfaction, attitudes (at least until recently; see Judge et al. and supervisor ratings or output measures of job [2001]). One widely held view is that attitudes are performance (e.g., Brayfield & Crockett, 1955). A inconsistent or epiphenomenal forces in work be- common inference in those reviews was that job havior (e.g., Locke & Latham, 1990): they explain attitudes were more strongly related to absence, only 3–4 percent of performance variance and have turnover, and other forms of work role withdrawal little practical importance for managers. The current article subjects that view to empiri- cal scrutiny via comprehensive and comparative The first two authors contributed equally to the writ- tests. In doing so, we attempt to contribute to man- ing of this article. We would like to acknowledge James agement knowledge in five ways. First, we investi- M. Conway for his help locating studies for the meta- gate and more fully map the individual-level crite- analysis of contextual and focal performance. Coding rion space (i.e., a set of work behaviors valued by assistance was provided by Amanda Yancey, Ilana Goro- organizations [Austin & Villanova, 1992]) by bring- dinskya, Maya Imberman, Rebecca Pedooem (all under a work-study grant from Alliant International University), ing four original meta-analyses to the literature, and Crystal Wiggins. We are also grateful to Kevin Moss- estimating the connections between contextual per- holder, three anonymous reviewers, and Tom Lee for formance and (1) lateness, (2) absence, (3) turnover, helpful and patient comments on drafts. and (4) focal (in-role) performance. Second, we cre- 305
306 Academy of Management Journal April ate a multivariate matrix of meta-analytic correla- In addition to the evidence for a shared concep- tions between pairs of these five behavioral criteria tual domain, there is evidence of these constructs and the two most commonly studied job attitudes: having a great deal of shared variance. Mathieu and job satisfaction and organizational commitment. Zajac (1990) showed that measures of commitment This effort involves combining our new meta-anal- from the Organizational Commitment Question- yses with clarified results of 17 existing meta-anal- naire (OCQ) were more strongly connected to over- yses, a process resulting in 21 estimates of bivariate all satisfaction than to facet-specific (pay, co- relationships in adult working populations. Third, worker, supervision, etc.) satisfaction. Satisfaction we use the ensuing meta-analytic matrix to com- and affective commitment measures have a strong pare the fit of competing theoretical models that correlation (e.g., meta-analytic ˆ ⫽.65 [Meyer, Stan- specify relationships between attitudinal predic- ley, Herscovitch, & Topolnytsky, 2002]). In fact, the tors and (different structures among) behavioral cri- correlation between overall job satisfaction and af- teria. Fourth, we assess the time sequencing among fective commitment is stronger than many of the job attitudes and behaviors, comparing predictive relationships between indicators typically taken as with “postdictive” time-lagged designs. Fifth and representing a single underlying construct. More- most importantly, we attempt to answer the ques- over, the correlation between affective commitment tion posed in our title by estimating links between and job satisfaction is stronger than the correlations predictors and criteria defined at increasingly com- between pairs of (affective, normative, and contin- patible levels of generality, an effort culminating in uance) facets of Meyer and Allen’s (1991) commit- a broad attitude-engagement model that connects ment construct (Meyer et al., 2002), and stronger overall job attitude with overall individual than relationships between indicators of other gen- effectiveness. eral constructs (see du Toit & du Toit, 2001). Thus, it is reasonable to treat job satisfaction and attitu- dinal commitment as specific reflections of a gen- PREDICTORS: JOB SATISFACTION, eral attitude, as each is a fundamental evaluation of ORGANIZATIONAL COMMITMENT, AND one’s job experiences. Hence, we extend the work OVERALL JOB ATTITUDE of Judge and coauthors (2001) and argue we can Job satisfaction, although defined in many ways, conceptualize both job satisfaction and organiza- has often been thought of as an emotional state tional commitment as indicating an underlying resulting from the evaluation or appraisal of one’s overall job attitude. job experiences (Locke, 1976), or as a psychological state simultaneously represented by cognitive and CRITERIA: FOCAL VERSUS CONTEXTUAL affective indicators (Brief & Weiss, 2002; cf. Schlei- PERFORMANCE cher, Watt, & Greguras, 2004). The consensual por- tion of organizational commitment’s definition is Another major issue in a comprehensive test of that it is a feeling of sharing beliefs and values with attitude-behavior relationships at work is the one’s entire organization—itself a positive emo- breadth of the criterion space. For the past two tional state (e.g., Meyer & Allen, 1991). That is, decades, scholars have systematically expanded in- despite conceptual and empirical distinctions (e.g., dividual-level behavioral criteria, responding in Tett & Meyer, 1993), it is clear that job satisfaction part to the early and fairly gloomy reviews of atti- and organizational commitment have theoretical tude-performance connections (e.g., Organ, 1977). and empirical commonalities. Both satisfaction and Organ and his colleagues have defined organiza- commitment are nonspecific with regard to the ac- tional citizenship behavior (OCB) using elements of tions prescribed. In Meyer and Allen’s (1991) three- work activity not fully captured by traditional (fo- dimensional reconceptualization, affective com- cal performance, task completion) concepts (Bate- mitment is the most strongly overlapping in man & Organ, 1983; Organ, 1997; Smith, Organ, & constitutive and operational definition with atti- Near, 1983). Borman and Motowidlo further ab- tude. Indeed, recently it has been termed “attitudi- stracted these behaviors (1993) into contextual per- nal commitment” (Riketta, 2002). Hulin (1991) also formance, a more inclusive criterion dimension. noted considerable theoretical overlap between af- Such behaviors were seen as more interpersonally fective commitment and overall job satisfaction, oriented (Motowidlo, 2000; Van Scotter & Motow- remarking that the only clear difference between idlo, 1996), more discretionary, and more “extra- the two is their conceptual target. The target of job role” (e.g., helping coworkers, encouraging or im- satisfaction is one’s position or work role; the target proving morale, and endorsing, supporting, and of affective commitment is the entire organization defending organizational objectives), than what has (Hulin, 1991: 489). been characterized as “in-role” performance (Or-
2006 Harrison, Newman, and Roth 307 gan, 1988; Organ & Paine, 1999). We suggest con- means through which employees can withhold in- textual performance is now an important part of puts from an organization. Many foundational the- what Fisher (1980: 607) called the “total set of ories of organizational behavior, including equity work-related behaviors,” and examining this con- theory (Adams, 1965), inducements-contributions struct fulfills recommendations to study broader, theory (March & Simon, 1958), and social exchange more abstract criteria (see Hanisch, Hulin, & theory (Thibault & Kelly, 1959) suggest straightfor- Roznowski, 1998; Judge et al., 2001). ward reasons why individuals contribute or with- Past studies have focused on attitudinal predic- hold such inputs. Under their auspices, we theorize tors of contextual performance (Organ & Ryan, that lateness and absence are often controllable 1995). Research on links between contextual per- forms of input reduction, subject to the same moti- formance and other criterion dimensions (e.g., late- vations for withholding inputs as OCBs, helping ness, absenteeism, and turnover) is more recent. Of behaviors, and other elements of contextual perfor- equal importance, the position of contextual per- mance (cf. Harrison, Johns, & Martocchio, 2000). formance in the temporal progression of behavioral Those who are willing to expend the (extra-role) responses to negative attitudes has not been made effort to engage in contextual performance are less explicit. Below, we review and develop formal hy- apt to reduce their (in-role) effort to meet the focal potheses supporting such links. Those hypotheses demands of their work schedules. Additionally, ab- serve as conceptual bases of four new meta-analy- senteeism and lateness permit an employee to re- ses, which themselves are necessary for completing duce the costs of an aversive job by engaging in the meta-analytic matrix of pairwise correlations more pleasurable activities while still maintaining between all commonly studied behavioral criteria the job’s economic benefits. There are also fewer and job attitudes. opportunities to enact forms of contextual perfor- mance when one spends less time at work (is late or absent). Thus, Contextual Performance and Turnover Hypothesis 2. Contextual performance is neg- Chen, Hui, and Sego (1998) proposed that avoid- atively related to absenteeism. ance of citizenship behavior may be a discretionary and primary means for employees to reduce work Hypothesis 3. Contextual performance is neg- role inclusion. If the morale-building or relation- atively related to lateness. ship-enhancing actions comprising contextual per- formance (Van Scotter & Motowidlo, 1996) are con- Contextual Performance and Focal Performance sidered “prepayment” for eventual good treatment by an employer, then avoidance of contextual per- The connection between contextual and focal formance may signal employees’ intentions to (task) performance has been given more research “write off” these investments in a firm they plan to attention than the connection between contextual leave. Likewise, in their job embeddedness model, performance and withdrawal behaviors (e.g., Con- Mitchell, Holtom, Lee, Sablynski, and Erez (2001) way, 1999; Motowidlo & Van Scotter, 1994; Ro- proposed that a major factor inhibiting turnover is tundo & Sackett, 2002). Task performance is typi- the depth and breadth of interpersonal relation- cally defined as the degree to which an individual ships developed through contextual performance meets or exceeds expectations about focal role re- behaviors. Mossholder, Settoon, and Henagan quirements. Recently, Hunt (2002) argued that (2005) also showed evidence that workers with when employees have a fixed pool of inputs or fewer interpersonal ties were more likely to quit. efforts, a negative relationship should be expected Hence, contextual performance promotes the for- between contextual and focal performance. He re- mal and informal connections that reduce an em- fers to these situations as “Taylorist jobs,” in which ployee’s likelihood of quitting. strict adherence to routinized procedures is advocated. Hypothesis 1. Contextual performance is neg- Most jobs, however, have become less routinized, atively related to turnover. less unidimensional, and less strictly defined (Cas- cio, 1998), reducing the asserted trade-off between contextual and focal performance. Additionally, for Contextual Performance, Absenteeism, and a variety of circumstances, individual difference Lateness variables have been found to produce relatively In formulating ideas about links between contex- high levels of both task performance and citizen- tual performance, absenteeism, and lateness, we ship behavior. These individual difference vari- also note the role of absenteeism and lateness as ables include conscientiousness, emotional stabil-
308 Academy of Management Journal April ity, and agreeableness (Hurtz & Donovan, 2000; In contrast, for the compensatory forms and al- LePine, Erez, & Johnson, 2002). Further, Taylorist ternative forms models, single withdrawal behav- jobs are most likely to produce negative within- iors are assumed to be substitutable in specific person correlations of contextual with task perfor- ways for one another. Rosse and Miller (1984) de- mance, while our current focus is on between-per- scribed them in terms of “water under pressure”; son correlations. Given this pattern of evidence, we the metaphoric flowing water is the urge to with- expect that some individuals bring higher levels of draw from a dissatisfying work environment (see personal resources (time, energy, human capital) to also Johns, 1997). Under the alternate forms model, their jobs, fostering higher levels of focal and con- external constraints on one behavior (the turnover textual performance. faucet is closed) mean that the urge will be expressed Hypothesis 4. Contextual performance is posi- in another behavior (the absenteeism faucet is open). tively related to focal (task) performance. Under compensatory forms, enacting one form of withdrawal will have a tempering (relief valve) effect on dissatisfaction, and therefore lessen the probabil- CRITERIA: WITHDRAWAL BEHAVIORS ity of enacting another form of withdrawal. Both Alongside contextual and focal performance, models are taken to predict negative within-person withdrawal behavior is arguably a third major di- covariance between individual withdrawal behaviors mension of the individual-level criterion space. Ac- over short periods of time (Martocchio & Harrison, tions such as lateness, absenteeism, and turnover 1993). The spillover model connects withdrawal be- have a long history of study in management, and haviors in a positive way (Rosse & Miller, 1984). direct bottom-line implications for firms. Although Engaging in lateness, absence, or turnover is a reflec- researchers have meta-analyzed connections be- tion of a general, underlying propensity to withdraw, tween pairs of withdrawal behaviors (e.g., Mitra, which itself is determined by an overall, negative job Jenkins, & Gupta, 1992), and between each of the attitude. What differentiates the three behaviors is major withdrawal behaviors and job attitudes (e.g., merely the threshold that the underlying attitudinal Hackett, 1989), they have not been examined si- propensity must breach to reveal itself in a particular multaneously or as key components of a broader action (lateness has the lowest threshold [Hulin, criterion space. 1991]). This model would be operationalized with all Just as there are debates about the connections of three behaviors serving as congeneric reflections of a job attitudes with performance, there are decades- single withdrawal construct. The compensatory, al- old sets of opposing ideas about the nomological ternative forms, and spillover models also mandate a networks of single- and multiple-behavior forms of separate dimension of withdrawal from task and con- withdrawal (see the summary by Johns [1998]). Hu- textual performance in our meta-analytic model-fit- lin (1984, 1991) suggested that the meanings of ting, as all three models specify a single underlying lateness, absence, and turnover can be found in urge to withdraw that is variously manifested through their patterns of covariation. Rosse and Miller lateness, absenteeism, and turnover. (1984) identified five sets of those patterns, or no- Finally, according to the progression of with- mological networks, as underlying theories of rela- drawal model, positive covariances occur between tionships among withdrawal behaviors themselves, pairs of withdrawal behaviors in a specific, cascad- and between withdrawal behaviors and their pro- ing order (Benson & Pond, 1987; Krausz, Ko- posed antecedents and consequences (also see their slowsky, & Eiser, 1998; Mobley, 1982; Rosse, 1988). reinterpretation by Harrison and Martocchio Under the progression conceptualization, all three [1998]). According to the independent forms model withdrawal behaviors are presumed to be re- of withdrawal, lateness, absenteeism, and turnover sponses to negative job attitudes. But an additional each have a unique etiology. In its extreme form, requirement is that they be connected in a causal this model is taken to predict near-zero covariances chain, generating a simplex pattern of behavior- among uniquely determined withdrawal behaviors specific correlations from lateness to absence, and (Rosse & Miller, 1984). However, a more precise then absence to turnover. characterization of the independent forms model might be that it predicts differential connections of job attitudes to each type of withdrawal behavior. CONNECTING ATTITUDINAL PREDICTORS TO Under an independent forms model of withdrawal, BEHAVIORAL CRITERIA a model fitted to attitude-behavior correlations that The Compatibility Principle keeps lateness, absenteeism, and turnover distinct (and therefore includes no underlying withdrawal Over the past two decades, some researchers construct) should fit best. (e.g., Fisher, 1980; Hulin, 1991) have argued that
2006 Harrison, Newman, and Roth 309 the apparently meager connections between job at- Judge and colleagues (2001) disregarded effects of titudes and job performance (typically measured job facet satisfaction and concluded that overall only as focal performance) are, in fact, consistent satisfaction had a much stronger meta-analytic re- with fundamental principles of attitudes proposed lationship with overall job performance than pre- by social psychologists. These authors contend that viously believed (ˆ ⫽ .30). Their conclusion was job attitudes do not predict job behavior well be- followed by a call for research on relationships of cause behavioral criteria are defined and treated at job attitudes to even broader behavioral criteria: a different level of abstraction than attitudinal pre- “Issues of construct generality and correspondence dictors. These arguments (Fisher, 1980; Fisher & have fundamental effects on the nature and magni- Locke, 1992; Hulin, 1991; Roznowski & Hulin, tude of the relationships between attitudes and be- 1992) are based largely on the theories of Fishbein haviors . . . but have rarely been considered in the and Ajzen (e.g., Ajzen & Fishbein, 1977, 1980; Fish- satisfaction-performance literature” (Judge et al., bein & Ajzen, 1974, 1975), who developed the com- 2001: 392). Responding to this suggestion and tak- patibility principle for attitudes to account for their ing their work a step further, we explicitly consid- apparently inconsistent relationships to behavior ered those issues in the present research. Using (their original term was “congruence,” later re- structural equation modeling of meta-analytic cor- named “compatibility” by Ajzen [1988]). They re- relations between pairs of job attitudes and behav- sponded to extensive disputes in social psychology ioral criteria (Viswesvaran & Ones, 1995), we tested over the role of attitudes in determining and pre- the fit of models of the consequences of overall job dicting behaviors (e.g., Mischel, 1968; Wicker, attitude. As those models move from theorizing 1969), proposing that an attitude impels behavior more specific to more general behavioral criteria, only when the two constructs are compatible in we expected they would show better fit to existing their action, target, context, and time (see Epstein data. In addition to assessing model fit, we exam- [1980] for a parallel explanation with dispositional ined the connection between overall job attitude constructs). Ajzen and Fishbein (1977) theorized and behavioral criteria. Following the compatibil- and showed that attitude-behavior connections ity principle, we expected the attitude-behavior were strongest when the attitude was matched in connection to grow progressively stronger as be- specificity or generality to behavior. More recently, havioral criteria were defined in broader, more in- Kraus (1995) also found support for the compatibil- clusive ways. We termed the most general form of ity principle in nonwork contexts, with meta-ana- these criterion dimensions individual effective- lytic correlations (r’s) of .29 versus .62 under low ness, the tendency to contribute desirable inputs and high compatibility, respectively. toward one’s work role. In applying the compatibility principle to the present question, it should be noted that job satis- Attitude-Behavior Relationships: Specific Models faction and organizational commitment are atti- tudes that connote a broad target, but not an action, As we have noted, withdrawal, contextual per- context, or time. According to attitude theory, such formance, and focal (task) performance have not attitudes should kindle a general, undifferentiated been drawn together in a comprehensive empirical force to engage in (positive or negative) behaviors analysis involving attitudinal predictors. However, that express or manifest the attitude. Such attitudes several theoretical models of the structure of these should therefore predict wide sets or aggregates of behavioral criteria exist. Many comprehensive behaviors directed toward one’s role that are not models (Campbell, 1990; Murphy, 1990; Van Scot- limited to a specific task dimension, social environ- ter & Motowidlo, 1996) do not explicitly address ment, or type of activity at work. This idea, also the possibility that correlations among criterion di- found in Fisher’s assertion about attitude-behavior mensions could reflect a higher-order or more gen- compatibility, that “general satisfaction measures eral effectiveness construct. However, Viswesvaran should be related to the favorableness or unfavor- and Ones (2000) noted that meta-analytic evidence ableness of an individual’s total set of work-related indicated a positive manifold, or sharedness, behaviors” (1980: 607), has not been fully tested. If among the various conceptions of performance di- this idea were correct, as researchers define behav- mensions: task performance, contextual perfor- ioral criteria at increasingly higher levels of ab- mance, counterproductivity, and so on. They inter- straction (e.g., actions contributing to aspects of preted this overlap as a higher-order performance one’s job or work role), the empirical connection or “p-factor” (Arvey, 1986; Viswesvaran, 1993), a between overall job attitudes and such criteria substantively meaningful construct not simply due should become stronger. to idiosyncratic rater halo error (Viswesvaran & In keeping with the compatibility principle, Ones, 2000: 223).
310 Academy of Management Journal April In the present article, we organize these ideas is considered, overall job attitude is predicted to about criterion structure in ways that correspond have unique effects on each criterion dimension with increasing fidelity to the compatibility princi- (see Figure 1, top panel). The independent forms ple reviewed above. Specifically, we move from model of job withdrawal (Rosse & Miller, 1984) conceptualizations of the criterion space that might would fall under this rubric, as it rests on different be characterized as diversified, treating multiple strengths of predictors for lateness, absence, and responses to job attitudes as unique behaviors or turnover (including a version that supposes one sets, to those that might be characterized as unified, behavior is a function of unfavorable job attitude treating all behavioral dimensions as parts of an while the others are not [Johns, 1998]). overall effectiveness construct. The former mod- Model B: Diversified criteria, plus progression els—including those that mandate a distinct crite- of withdrawal. Within the diversified criterion rion dimension for each form of withdrawal behav- model, relationships among single withdrawal be- ior—treat elements of the criterion space as more haviors can be structured to be consistent with one behaviorally specific, and the latter treat them as or more of the withdrawal theories reviewed ear- more general. According to the compatibility prin- lier. In particular, overall job attitude can relate in ciple, the latter models should show stronger con- a unique way to each behavior within the criterion nections between overall job attitude and the space, while the withdrawal behaviors inside that (shared) variance in behavioral criteria. Differences space are interrelated in a predefined way. In keep- between diversified and unified theoretical models ing with the progression of withdrawal hypothesis, stem mainly from how they arrange elements of we propose a model of a diversified criterion space task performance, contextual performance, and that overlays an ordered sequence among with- withdrawal behaviors relative to one another. We drawal behaviors, moving from lateness to absen- describe those models below and show them in teeism to turnover (see the dotted arrows in Figure Figure 1. 1, top panel). Model A: Diversified criteria. Job performance Model C: Diversified criteria, but unitary with- has been defined as behaviors that are under indi- drawal. As suggested in reviews of the conse- vidual control and that affect the goals of the em- quences of job satisfaction and organizational com- ploying organization (Campbell, 1990). As we men- mitment (Herzberg et al., 1957; Mathieu & Zajac, tioned above, a diversified model of the criterion 1990; Vroom, 1964), we specified a model in which space would specify no general, higher-order factor task and contextual performance are distinguished underlying the various dimensions of work behav- from withdrawal as criteria (see Figure 1, middle ior or performance. According to this model, sets of panel). In terms of specific versus general ap- actions such as lateness, absenteeism, turnover, proaches to the criterion space, this intermediate, and contextual performance are determined in dif- three- rather than five-dimensional model, groups ferent ways and in different strengths by job atti- lateness, absenteeism, and turnover together as out- tudes. The criteria do not share a single etiology. croppings of an underlying withdrawal construct Instead, performance-related behaviors reflect how (see Hanisch & Hulin, 1991; Hanisch, Hulin, & Ros- much individual control or discretion each one nowski, 1998; Rosse & Hulin, 1985). This model is entails. Contextual performance is associated with also consistent with withdrawal as spillover (Rosse the level of effort or persistence that an individual & Miller, 1984). exerts beyond what is required. Absenteeism and Model D: Unified criterion. Moving from three lateness, as the reduction of effort, are somewhat dimensions to a single, general effectiveness di- less discretionary, with controls on their expres- mension involves moving from a diversified to a sion that vary across jobs (Johns, 1991). Focal per- unified behavioral criterion. Such a model is con- formance is the least discretionary. Typically in- sistent with the hierarchical perspective of a role or expected, it serves as the basis for the p-factor in the criterion space (Viswesvaran and distribution of formal organizational rewards (Bor- Ones [2000]; see Figure 1, bottom panel). Hulin man & Motowidlo, 1993). The discretionary com- (1982) implied a similar structure decades earlier. ponent of turnover depends upon the external labor The unified model implies that overall job attitude market and information about alternative job is associated with the shared or empirically over- opportunities. lapping portions of behavioral criteria at work. In Such a conceptualization also implies that over- terms of attitude theory, this model represents the all job attitude has a stronger connection to contex- greatest attitude-behavior compatibility (Fishbein tual performance, lateness, and absence, than to & Ajzen, 1997). Both constructs are treated at the focal performance and (perhaps) turnover (e.g., highest level of generality or abstraction. Overall Chen et al., 1998). That is, when individual control job attitude is generic with respect to actions, con-
2006 Harrison, Newman, and Roth 311 FIGURE 1 Models of Relationships between Individual Job Attitudes and Work Behavior
312 Academy of Management Journal April texts, and times. We contend that positive job atti- ples of employed adults. This procedure was con- tude creates a tendency to engage or contribute sistent with previous meta-analyses represented in desirable inputs to one’s work role, rather than our attitude-behavior matrix (Judge et al., 2001). withhold them. Each behavioral criterion is a re- Primary studies sampled people working in natural flection of this general tendency. settings and estimated individual-level effects. Ef- Model E: Unified criterion, plus progression of fect sizes for relationships with turnover measures withdrawal. Finally, it is possible that both the were restricted to those articles and papers report- compatibility principle and the progression of ing actual separations from an organization, rather withdrawal model operate simultaneously (Rosse, than turnover intentions or withdrawal cognitions. 1988). In this specification, individual withdrawal Identification of studies. Studies for the present behaviors owe a major portion of their covariation meta-analyses were located through electronic and to the general effectiveness criterion. At the same manual searches of databases, bibliographies from time, there is a dependency structure between pairs quantitative and qualitative reviews, and confer- of withdrawal behaviors (see Figure 1, bottom ence proceedings. Initially, we searched the panel, dashed arrows). PsycINFO, ERIC, and ABI/Inform databases from 1983 (the first year in which a paper on OCB was published) through October 2004, using the subject METHODS terms “citizenship behavior,” “contextual perfor- To test the compatibility principle, and therefore mance,” “prosocial behavior,” and “extra-role be- estimate the importance of overall job attitude for havior.” The focus of the literature review was on predicting a higher-order job behavior construct, published articles and chapters, unpublished doc- we applied the models described above to a meta- toral dissertations, conference papers, and cited but analytic matrix of relationships among specific job unpublished manuscripts. The abstracts from all attitudes and behaviors that have frequently ap- studies identified by the databases were searched peared in past research. These behaviors included manually for references to absenteeism, sick leave focal performance (task or in-role performance, typ- use, attendance, turnover, retention, quitting, late- ically measured by supervisor ratings), contextual ness, promptness, tardiness, performance, and in- performance (typically measured as OCB), lateness, role behavior. We also posted messages on listservs absenteeism, and turnover. Although published (e.g., RMNet, HRDivNet) asking for unpublished or meta-analytic estimates were available for bivariate forthcoming studies. Even with this broad search, relationships between attitudes (job satisfaction there were relatively few studies to cumulate that and organizational commitment) and each specific correlated contextual performance with each of the criterion dimension, one of the contributions of our three withdrawal behaviors. However, we were study is to review and estimate meta-analytic rela- able to obtain at least five independent samples for tionships between contextual performance and other each new estimate, a number that compares favor- criteria. We derived meta-analytic correlations be- ably to those in other recent meta-analyses (e.g., tween contextual performance and turnover (Hypoth- Martocchio, Harrison, & Berkson, 2000). Table 1 esis 1), absenteeism (Hypothesis 2), lateness (Hypoth- reports the number of studies (k) and total number esis 3), and focal performance (Hypothesis 4). of individuals (N) for each new meta-analytic esti- In many of the primary studies included in our mate. Because one of the prior meta-analytic esti- search, contextual and task performance ratings mates (lateness with focal performance, from Ko- were taken from the same source (e.g., supervisors). slowski, Sagie, Krausz, and Singer [1997]) was Therefore, to be commensurate with the other based on fewer than five original studies, we also meta-analytic values that were not subject to bias report an updated estimate for it in Table 1. by common method variance or percept-percept Meta-analytic procedures. Following the inflation (cf. Organ & Ryan, 1995), we separated method used to derive the prior correlations in- original studies on the basis of whether data for the cluded in our eventual meta-analytic matrix of at- two variables came from a common source. Non- titude-behavior connections, we employed Hunter common source estimates were used in our tests of and Schmidt’s (2004) corrections for attenuation competing models. due to unreliability. Such corrections are often viewed as conservative (Hunter & Schmidt, 2004). Many of the primary studies did not report corre- Meta-Analyses of Links between Contextual lations with contextual performance, but only Performance and Other Job Behaviors facet-specific correlations (e.g., with the “sports- Rules for inclusion. We selected studies for con- manship” facet of OCB). We combined correla- textual performance meta-analyses that used sam- tions from these dimensional measures to form a
2006 Harrison, Newman, and Roth 313 TABLE 1 Results of Meta-Analyses for Contextual Performance Percentage of Variance Uncorrected Total Corrected Accounted for by r k N 95% Confidence Interval r Sampling Error Contextual performance Turnover ⫺.20 5 1,619 ⫺.12 to ⫺.36 ⫺.22 21% Lateness ⫺.11 5 578 ⫺.03 to ⫺.28 ⫺.15 70 Absenteeism ⫺.22 8 957 ⫺.16 to ⫺.27 ⫺.26 100 Focal performance .20 24 9,912 .16 to .24 .23 20 Lateness Focal performance ⫺.20 7 1,879 ⫺.13 to ⫺.50 ⫺.26 12 unit-weighted composite correlation between all correlation matrix can produce incorrect standard dimensions of contextual performance (e.g., errors when the standard deviation varies across OCB) and the respective criterion. input variables (Cudeck, 1989). Fortunately, these In contrast to the other job behaviors, turnover standard errors are often overestimated (Cudeck, was treated as having a reliability of unity (Griffeth, 1989: 323), making significance tests of individual Hom, & Gaertner, 2000). Interrater or internal con- parameters conservative. sistency estimates of reliability were often not re- ported for lateness, absenteeism, and in-role perfor- Other Adjustments to the Meta-Analytic mance. We substituted the average reliability Correlation Matrix estimate from the other studies that did provide it, which is a form of imputation. One limitation we discovered in previous meta- analyses involving job attitudes and behavioral crite- ria was failure to appropriately specify the level of Analytic Framework for Fitting and Comparing analysis of primary studies involved in the calcula- Diversified versus Unified Models tion of the final meta-analytic estimate (Ostroff & Har- Data for estimating the fit of models A through E rison, 1999). For example, one of the prior meta- were obtained by bringing together the four new analytic estimates in the literature was based on a meta-analytic estimates described above with 17 total N of approximately 3,000, but 1,244 of the data other meta-analyses estimating bivariate relation- points were from Angle and Perry’s (1981) unit-level ships between job attitudes and work behaviors. Use rather than individual-level analysis. In several cases, of meta-analytically derived matrices for structural inclusion of this correlation created significant bias in equation models was advised by Viswesvaran and the published estimate. Thus, we removed Angle and Ones (1995) and Shadish (1996) and has been re- Perry’s (1981) result from the meta-analytic estimates ported many times in the human resources and or- that included it, permitting our final model to reflect ganizational behavior literatures (e.g., Bhaskar- individual-level relationships. Additionally, for the Shrinivas, Harrison, Shaffer, & Lau, 2005). A series of correlation between organizational commitment and meta-analyses published since 1989 provided esti- job satisfaction, we combined the results of Mathieu mates for the input correlation matrix (see the foot- and Zajac (1990) with those of Meyer and colleagues notes for Table 2). In this approach, the structural (2002), as those meta-analyses were based on inde- equation model uses manifest indicators without pendent sets of original studies. correction for measurement error, as these correc- tions have already been accomplished through Time Sequencing meta-analysis. We acknowledge that comparisons between non- Our use of directional arrows in Figure 1 carries nested models are somewhat arbitrary, and their with it a set of implicit and often disregarded hy- use prohibited us from pinpointing specific paths potheses about the temporal ordering among vari- that accounted for between-model differences in ables (Mitchell & James, 2001). All of the models fit. Further, these models are limited by our neces- implicitly specify that job attitudes are temporally sary reliance on correlation (rather than covari- prior to behavioral criteria, and the progression of ance) estimates as input for the modeling proce- withdrawal models (models B and E) implies a dure. Structural equation models based on a temporal sequence from lateness to absence to turn-
314 Academy of Management Journal April TABLE 2 Meta-Analytic Correlations between Job Attitudes and Job-Related Behaviorsa Construct 1 2 3 4 5 6 1. Job satisfaction 2. Organizational commitment .60 b k studies 112 N total observations 39,187 3. Focal performance .30 c .18 d k studies 312 87 N total observations 54,471 20,973 4. Contextual performance .28 e .25 f .23 g k studies 32 42 24 N total observations 16,348 10,747 9,912 5. Turnover ⫺.19 h ⫺.22 i ⫺.15 j ⫺.22 k k studies 67 66 72 5 N total observations 24,566 26,296 25,234 1,619 6. Lateness ⫺.11 l ⫺.15 m ⫺.26 n ⫺.15 o .09 p k studies 15 7 7 5 5 N total observations 3,767 1,896 1,879 578 1,310 7. Absenteeism ⫺.17 q ⫺.16 r ⫺.29 s ⫺.26 t .30 u .38 v k studies 25 30 49 8 33 24 N total observations 4,741 5,748 15,764 957 5,316 6,769 a All correlations are disattenuated for unreliability. Turnover correlations are not corrected for base rate. If more than one meta-analysis reported on the same relationship, we used the estimate reflecting the greatest amount of data (in all cases, this was the most recent estimate). When more than one set of estimates was given, correlations with absence frequency were used. The letter superscripts in the body of the table indicate the sources of the meta-analytic correlations as follows: “s,” Bycio (1992); “h,” “i,” “j,” Griffeth et al. (2000); “q,” Hackett (1989); “c,” Judge et al.; “l,” “m,” “v,” “p,” Koslowsky et al. (1997); “e,” “f,” LePine et al. (2002); “b,” “r,” Mathieu and Zajac (1990), composited with Meyer et al. (2002); “u,” Mitra et al. (1992); and “d,” Riketta (2002). Original analyses include “g,” “k,” “n,” “o,” and “t.” A correlation from Angle and Perry (1981) was removed from estimates “i,” “m,” “p,” and “v.” over. Our review of relationships between contex- expectations, contextual performance was nega- tual performance and withdrawal behavior (Hy- tively related to all three withdrawal behaviors potheses 1–3) further implies that citizenship and positively related to focal performance. Cor- behaviors are withheld prior to the decision to miss rected (for unreliability) estimates were moderate work (Chen et al., 1998). in size for relationships with turnover (ˆ ⫽ ⫺.22), Because the published meta-analyses on which lateness (ˆ ⫽ ⫺.15), absenteeism (ˆ ⫽ ⫺.26), and our meta-matrix is based did not distinguish pri- focal performance (ˆ ⫽ .23). All 95% confidence mary studies with regard to time (except Bycio intervals for these estimates excluded zero, sup- [1992]), we returned to the original data to differ- porting Hypotheses 1– 4. We note again that these entiate studies with predictive designs (e.g., job results are not biased by percept-percept infla- satisfaction measured before absenteeism) from tion (Doty & Glick, 1998; Harrison & McLaughlin, those with postdictive designs (e.g., absenteeism 1996; Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Lee, & Podsakoff, measured before job satisfaction). A finding of 2003) as we included only those studies for stronger predictive than postdictive effect sizes which correlations were taken from noncommon would be consistent with the temporal order im- sources. plied by models A to E. For two of the hypothesized links (contextual performance with turnover and with focal perfor- RESULTS mance), there was considerable variability in effect sizes not accounted for by sampling error. This Hypotheses 1–4: Links between Contextual variability implied the presence of potential mod- Performance and Other Job Behaviors erators, which will be discussed later. Our goal for Meta-analytic results for tests of Hypotheses 1 the present study was to provide the best summary through 4 appear in Table 1. In keeping with estimates of the links between contextual perfor-
2006 Harrison, Newman, and Roth 315 mance and other behavioral criteria across settings. dex (TLI; also known as the nonnormed fit index Having done so, we could proceed with tests of the [Tucker & Lewis, 1973]), Jöreskog and Sörbom’s compatibility principle. (1989) adjusted goodness-of-fit index (AGFI), and Steiger’s (1990) root-mean-square error of approxi- mation (RMSEA). The last is really a “badness-of- Models A–E: Increasingly Compatible Job fit” index, with larger values indicating greater Attitude–Job Behavior Constructs misfit. We tested the compatibility principle by compar- Two trends are apparent in the Table 3 results. ing structural equation models (Edwards, 2001) fit- First, models with more general or more unified ted to the meta-analytic, corrected correlations conceptualizations of behavioral criteria fit better shown in Table 2. Path coefficients for each model than models with more diversified criterion con- are shown in Figure 1. Fit indexes for each model ceptualizations, as evidenced by all indexes. That are reported in Table 3. As correlations in each cell is, model D (unified model: CFI ⫽ .92, TLI ⫽ .86, reflected different sample sizes, the model fit in- AGFI ⫽ .93, and RMSEA ⫽ .10) fits better than dexes were based on the harmonic mean across model C (diversified model, but unitary with- meta-analytic cells (Viswesvaran & Ones, 1995). As drawal construct: CFI ⫽ .87, TLI ⫽ .79, AGFI ⫽ .89, shown in Figure 1, organizational commitment and and RMSEA ⫽ .12), which in turn fits better than job satisfaction were modeled as a single, higher- model A (diversified model: CFI ⫽.77, TLI ⫽ .65, order, overall attitude construct in all cases. AGFI ⫽ .81, and RMSEA ⫽ .16). This finding, based Models of the criterion space took several forms. on a very large sample of employees in natural These included diversified models A and B, with settings, supports the compatibility principle. It five separate criteria; diversified model C, with also empirically attests to the viability of individual three separate criteria (lateness, absenteeism, and effectiveness as a higher-order criterion for overall turnover reflecting a higher-order withdrawal con- job attitude. It is reasonable to think of job satisfac- struct); and unified models D and E, represented tion and organizational commitment not as unique with a single, higher-order effectiveness construct predictors of specific performance criteria or with- for all the behavioral criteria. Model B was nested drawal tendencies, but as predictors of a general within A, and model E, within D, so that chi-square response that involves the overall engagement differences could be used to test changes in fit with, or contribution of favorable efforts to, one’s within these pairs (Hu & Bentler, 1999). The se- work role. quence of all five models was not nested; therefore, A second trend evident in Table 3 is the superi- comparisons were made on other indexes. Because ority of models with progression of withdrawal. the various indexes differ in specific assumptions, Model B fits much better than model A (⌬2 ⫽ the use of multiple indexes is recommended (Jöres- 648.7, df ⫽ 2, p ⬍ .01), and model E fits consider- kog & Sörbom, 1989). We included Bentler’s (1990) ably better than model D (⌬2 ⫽ 185.6, df ⫽ 2, p ⬍ comparative fit index (CFI), the Tucker-Lewis in- .01). Even when an effectiveness construct is spec- TABLE 3 Attitude-Behavior Model Comparisonsa Number of Performance Models Dimensions 2 df RMSEA TLI CFI AGFI Model A: Diversified 5 1,144.42** 14 .16 .65 .77 .81 criteria Model B: Diversified 5 495.68** 12 .11 .81 .89 .90 criteria, progression of withdrawal Model C: Diversified 3 596.42** 13 .12 .79 .87 .89 criteria, unitary withdrawal construct Model D: Unified criterion 1 380.76** 13 .10 .86 .92 .93 Model E: Unified criterion, 1 195.15** 11 .07 .92 .96 .96 progression of withdrawal a Harmonic mean N ⫽ 3,120. ** p ⬍ .01
316 Academy of Management Journal April ified as the behavioral criterion for overall job atti- the predictive effect sizes were larger than postdic- tude, fit improves as models include an ordered tive effect sizes (binomial p ⫽ .003). These trends, sequence of relationships from the unique variance however, should not be taken as a definitive test of of lateness to absenteeism to turnover. In fact, temporal sequence, as confidence intervals for pre- model E, which has the unified effectiveness crite- dictive and postdictive estimates overlapped (with rion and progression of withdrawal, demonstrates the exception of relationships in which absentee- fit that might be considered a close approximation ism was a consequence of job attitudes). In sum, to true structural relationships in the population our synthesis of 221 primary studies that employed (Hu & Bentler, 1999: CFI ⫽.96, TLI ⫽ .92, AGFI ⫽ time-lagged designs gave initial evidence about at- .96, and RMSEA ⫽ .07). The significant chi-square titude-behavior sequencing. It revealed a statisti- suggests there is still room for improvement, but cally significant overall trend favoring temporal this statistic is sensitive to trivial model departures precedence for attitudes and a progression of with- in our very large samples, and it is also inflated by drawal behaviors. the nonnormal distributions of turnover, lateness, and absenteeism (Harrison & Hulin, 1989). DISCUSSION Despite the inclusion of progression of with- drawal, the unified criterion model E remains con- This study addresses several theoretical ques- sistent with the compatibility principle of attitude tions that previous empirical research has left un- theory. In this model, the structural path that joins answered. We provide the first large-scale empiri- overall job attitude with effectiveness is rather cal test of the compatibility principle (Fishbein & strong (␥standardized ⫽ .59, p ⬍ .01). As this model is Ajzen, 1975) for job attitudes and work behavior, based on only a single predictor and a single crite- following conjectures from theorists such as Fisher rion, the standardized structural path can be taken (1980) and Hulin (1991). By noting that job satis- as a correlation between latent constructs. This la- faction and organizational commitment are atti- tent correlation is markedly stronger than even the tudes that specify a target but do not specify any recently updated job satisfaction–performance esti- particular action, we hypothesized and demon- mate from Judge et al. (2001; ˆ ⫽ .30). Therefore, to strated that a general set of actions at work—not answer the question in our paper’s title, overall job specific behaviors—serves as the best criterion con- attitude has considerable importance for under- struct for overall job attitudes. According to com- standing behavioral outcomes. peting theoretical positions, job attitudes should preferentially predict withdrawal behaviors (Vroom, 1964), or job attitudes serve simply as a Time Sequencing of Job Attitudes and Job common cause for a variety of otherwise unrelated Behaviors behaviors (Johns, 1998). Instead, results of our To test the time ordering between variables im- meta-analytic study support a unified criterion plied by the directed arrows in models A through E, model on the basis of its relative and absolute fit, we meta-analyzed primary studies using time- and show that when attempting to understand pat- lagged designs. All correlations involving turnover terns of work behavior from attitudes such as job were predictive (turnover was always a lagged cri- satisfaction and organizational commitment, re- terion). Our literature search found 564 of the orig- searchers should conceptualize the criterion at a inal 667 primary effects from the published and high level of abstraction. A general job attitude is new meta-analyses. Because 50 of these failed to strongly linked to a general behavioral criterion. In report enough information on the time ordering our conceptualization, the higher-order effective- between measures, 514 were eventually coded for ness construct might be defined as a general ten- time sequencing. Overall, 82 effects were predic- dency of employees to contribute desirable inputs tive, 139 effects were postdictive, and 292 effects toward their work roles rather than withhold those were concurrent. Table 4 presents results of this inputs. A simple label for such a conceptualization analysis. might be the “attitude-engagement” model of job Of the 14 bivariate relationships shown in the attitudes and behaviors. table, 2 (focal performance with lateness and job An alternative, formative view of these behav- satisfaction with contextual performance) provided ioral criteria might characterize our models as mis- no data to permit comparisons of predictive versus specified (e.g., Jarvis, MacKenzie, & Podsakoff, postdictive designs. Of the remaining 12 relation- 2003). In such a view, each behavior does not “re- ships, 10 were in the hypothesized direction (i.e., flect” an underlying construct (as in the conven- consistent with the directions of arrows in Figure tional approach to constructs [Edwards & Bagozzi, 1). That is, for 10 out of 12 bivariate relationships, 2000]), but instead, all behaviors add together to
2006 Harrison, Newman, and Roth 317 TABLE 4 Attitude-Behavior (A-B) and Behavior-Behavior (B1-B2) Meta-Analytic Correlations by Temporal Order of Constructsa Percentage of Variance Uncorrected Total 95% Confidence Corrected Accounted for by Relationship r k N Interval r Sampling Error Job satisfaction (A) with focal performance (B) Predictive: A measured, then B .17 23 3,251 .14 to .20 .28 30% Concurrent: A and B measured at same .18 130 23,045 .17 to .19 .30 28 time; separate sources Postdictive: B measured, then A .19 47 8,713 .17 to .21 .31 17 Organizational commitment (A) with focal performance (B) Predictive .12 9 2,439 .08 to .16 .16a 34% Concurrent, same source .17 17 4,634 .14 to .20 .23 17 Concurrent, separate sources .12 28 7,682 .10 to .15 .17 86 Postdictive .11 16 3,602 .08 to .14 .15a 90 Job satisfaction (A) with absenteeism (B) Predictive ⫺.15 7 997 ⫺.21 to ⫺.09 ⫺.24a 91% Concurrent, same source ⫺.31 2 435 ⫺.39 to ⫺.22 ⫺.49 n.a. Concurrent, separate sources ⫺.14 1 139 ⫺.30 to .03 ⫺.22 n.a. Postdictive ⫺.06 17 2,312 ⫺.10 to ⫺.02 ⫺.10a 39% Organizational commitment (A) with absenteeism (B) Predictive ⫺.12 10 3,484 ⫺.15 to ⫺.09 ⫺.18a 38% Concurrent, same source .03 1 252 ⫺.09 to .15 .04 n.a. Concurrent, separate sources ⫺.21 1 114 ⫺.38 to ⫺.03 ⫺.30 n.a. Postdictive ⫺.05 14 1,501 ⫺.10 to .00 ⫺.07a 37% Focal performance (B1) with absenteeism (B2)b Predictive: B1 measured, then B2 ⫺.33 8 1,134 ⫺.45 to ⫺.21 ⫺.57a 54% Concurrent: B1, B2 measured at the same ⫺.20 15 7,749 ⫺.26 to ⫺.15 ⫺.35 53 time; same source Concurrent: B1, B2 measured at the same ⫺.26 5 722 ⫺.33 to ⫺.19 ⫺.45 38 time; separate sources Postdictive: B2 measured, then B1 ⫺.11 19 4,463 ⫺.18 to ⫺.05 ⫺.20a 25 Job Satisfaction (A) with lateness (B) Predictive ⫺.16 2 677 ⫺.23 to ⫺.08 ⫺.19a n.a. Concurrent (same source) ⫺.09 5 1,455 ⫺.14 to ⫺.04 ⫺.11 21% Postdictive ⫺.14 7 1,235 ⫺.19 to ⫺.08 ⫺.17a 52% Organizational commitment (A) with lateness (B) Predictive ⫺.20 1 402 ⫺.29 to ⫺.10 ⫺.21a n.a. Concurrent n.a. n.a. n.a. n.a. n.a. n.a. Postdictive ⫺.07 4 1,059 ⫺.13 to ⫺.01 ⫺.08a 27% Focal performance (B1) with lateness (B2) Predictive n.a. n.a. n.a. n.a. n.a. n.a. Concurrent (same source) ⫺.02 2 545 ⫺.11 to .06 ⫺.03 n.a. Postdictive ⫺.25 2 1,325 ⫺.29 to ⫺.19 ⫺.30 n.a. Lateness (B1) with absenteeism (B2) Predictive .31 1 324 .21 to .41 .44a n.a. Concurrent (same source) .48 4 880 .42 to .53 .67 14% Concurrent (separate sources) .32 7 2,295 .28 to .35 .45 18% Postdictive .24 1 324 .14 to .34 .34a n.a. Job satisfaction (A) with contextual performance (B) Predictive .22 4 807 .20 to .23 .27 100% Concurrent, separate sources .26 14 4,492 .22 to .30 .32 53% Postdictive n.a. n.a. n.a. n.a. n.a. n.a.
318 Academy of Management Journal April TABLE 4 (Continued) Percentage of Variance Uncorrected Total 95% Confidence Corrected Accounted for by Relationship r k N Interval r Sampling Error Organizational commitment (A) with contextual performance (B) Predictive .21 9 2,231 .13 to .29 .28a 27% Concurrent, same source .19 24 6,288 .15 to .22 .25 66 Concurrent, separate sources .19 16 4,060 .15 to .23 .25 67 Postdictive .16 3 363 .10 to .22 .21a 100 Contextual performance (B1) with focal performance (B2) Predictive .20 3 1,102 .09 to .31 .23a 26% Concurrent, separate sources .21 16 8,182 .17 to .25 .25 18 Postdictive .07 5 698 ⫺.02 to .16 .09a 67 Contextual performance (B1) with lateness (B2) Predictive ⫺.14 3 291 .02 to ⫺.31 ⫺.19a 47% Concurrent, separate sources ⫺.10 1 94 n.a. ⫺.13 n.a. Postdictive ⫺.07 1 193 n.a. ⫺.09a n.a. Contextual performance (B1) with absenteeism (B2) Predictive ⫺.21 2 339 ⫺.20 to ⫺.22 ⫺.24 100% Concurrent, separate sources ⫺.18 3 331 ⫺.17 to ⫺.19 ⫺.21 100 Postdictive ⫺.26 3 287 ⫺.12 to ⫺.40 ⫺.30 59 a Predictive-postdictive difference is in hypothesized direction. b Estimates taken from Bycio (1992). “form” a new construct. In the present context, (see Locke, 1970), but the combined evidence from such an argument would also maintain that overall dozens of time-lagged studies tends to favor the job attitude first contributes to individual tenden- attitude-behavior mechanism. cies to engage in specific behaviors and that these Work behaviors also appear to display a theoret- behaviors then coalesce or combine (no positive ically meaningful time sequence. Although we con- covariation is necessary) to cause the general be- ceptualized effectiveness as a unified tendency, ac- havioral criterion. It is crucial for us to note that tions reflecting this tendency come about in a way this formulation is theoretically at odds with the that suggests progression of withdrawal (Rosse & compatibility principle, which posits that the gen- Miller, 1984). Time-lagged data show that lateness eral tendency to behave favorably toward an object tends to precede absence, and absence predicts (in this case, one’s work role) is caused by an over- turnover. Additionally, as Chen and coauthors all, positive evaluation of that object. Our conten- (1998) explained, the withholding of contextual tion is that we tested substantive models specifying performance behaviors may be a part of this pro- that (1) there is a general construct of effectiveness gression sequence (as the first signal of reduction in and (2) job behaviors reflect it, but not in identical work role inclusion), invoked prior to tardiness or ways (specific work criteria are congeneric, not par- quitting. The time-lagged studies concur, again allel). Both of these statements are consistent with showing a (slight) tendency for contextual perfor- attitude theory (Ajzen, 1988). mance to precede lateness. To further integrate this In addition to validating a unified criterion for concept with our theoretical models, we tried a job attitudes, we found that attitudes and behaviors modification to model E, specifying contextual per- may occur according to a particular time-ordered formance as the first element in the progression sequence. For six out of seven attitude-behavior sequence among disturbance terms (i.e., contextual pairings, predictive correlations (attitude to behav- performance to lateness, lateness to absence, and ior) were stronger than postdictive correlations (be- absence to turnover) and found a slight improve- havior to attitude). We cannot rule out the possibil- ment in overall model fit (⌬2 ⫽ 5.1, df ⫽ 1, p ⬍ ity that reciprocal causal processes are operating .05). Thus, both our static model and our lagged
You can also read